- Feb 2018
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2013 Nov 15, George McNamara commented:
The direct link to the supplemental files is http://bloodjournal.hematologylibrary.org/content/107/7/2643/suppl/DC1 The supplemental files includes two movies. My rule of thumb is you should make popcorn before watching movies.
An additional movie from this project is available at http://works.bepress.com/gmcnamara/21/ Shows a triplet cell division. That is, a cell that divided three ways. I have discussed this with other researchers, a few of whom have observed triplet cell divisions as well.
Several of the authors have moved from City of Hope - Mike Jensen is in Seattle. I am with Laurence Cooper at M.D. Anderson Cancer Center in Houston.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2015 Feb 23, Ivan Oransky commented:
None
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY. -
On 2015 Feb 23, Ivan Oransky commented:
Although this paper has been retracted, it has been cited 52 times since, with only two of those citations mentioning the retraction: http://retractionwatch.com/2015/02/18/evidence-scientists-continue-cite-retracted-papers/
(Deleted first of these comments as they posted twice.)
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Feb 21, Tom Kindlon commented:
MDDm should be resolved for more than 5 years before a CFS diagnosis can be given
(This was originally posted here http://www.biomedcentral.com/1741-7015/3/19/comments#285572 but the formatting has gone from that page)
In this paper, it says: "Following recommendations of the International CFS Study Group, only current MDDm was considered exclusionary for CFS." However, part of the specific recommendations of the International CFS Study Group [1] was that MDDm had to have been resolved for more than 5 years: "The 1994 case definition stated that any past or current diagnosis of major depressive disorder with psychotic or melancholic features, anorexia nervosa, or bulimia permanently excluded a subject from the classification of CFS ... we now recommend that if these conditions have been resolved for more than 5 years before the onset of the current chronically fatiguing illness, they should not be considered exclusionary." It might not be important to point this out for definitions for some illnesses: however if one looks at table 2, 6 of the 16 who are said to have CFS using the "current classification" of CFS, had been diagnosed with MDDm at a previous assessment which suggests it is important in this context.
Reference:
[1] Reeves WC, Lloyd A, Vernon SD, Klimas N, Jason LA, Bleijenberg G, Evengard B, White PD, Nisenbaum R, Unger ER, International Chronic Fatigue Syndrome Study Group: Identification of ambiguities in the 1994 chronic fatigue syndrome research case definition and recommendations for resolution.BMC Health Services Research 2003, 3:25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1472-6963-3-25
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY. -
On 2014 Feb 21, Tom Kindlon commented:
Data from another population study found scores on the RE subscale are similar in CFS patients to those found in healthy controls
(This was originally posted here http://www.biomedcentral.com/1741-7015/3/19/comments#285575 but the formatting has gone from that page)
I previously said that I questioned the value of using the Role Emotional (RE) subscale to satisfy functional impairment criteria (http://www.biomedcentral.com/1741-7015/3/19/comments#284561). Researchers deciding whether to follow the method of operationalizing the Fukuda [1] used in this study, might be interested at looking at Table 2 in Jason et al [2].
The subjects were also obtained from a random-digit population study. Here is what the authors said in the text on this part of the results: "A MANCOVA for the Medical Outcomes Study SF-36 Health Survey (controlling for the effects of work status) revealed significant differences in gradations of disability across the diagnostic categories of CFS only, MCS only, FM only, more than one diagnosis, and no diagnosis on seven of the eight subscales (F(4,208) = 1.82, p < .05). The role-emotional scale was the only scale that did not reveal significant differences between the groups (see Table 2). Significant post hoc tests revealed that individuals with CFS demonstrated greater disability than those with no diagnosis on the role-physical; bodily pain; vitality; and social functioning scales. Individuals with MCS demonstrated greater disability than the no diagnosis group on the physical functioning; role-physical; bodily pain; general health; vitality; social functioning; and mental health scales. Individuals with FM demonstrated greater disability than the no diagnosis group on the physical functioning; role-physical; bodily pain; and social functioning scales. In addition, individuals with more than one diagnosis demonstrated greater disability than those in the no diagnosis group on the physical functioning; role-physical; bodily pain; vitality; and social functioning scales. Means for each of the Medical Outcomes Study subscales are reported in Table 2."
This issue of how the Fukuda criteria [1] are operationalized is not a trivial matter. Using the previous method of operationalizing the criteria, a CDC team found a prevalence for CFS of 235 per 100,000 [3]. Using the method of operationalizing the criteria outlined in this study, the prevalence rate for CFS was found to be 2.54% or 2540 per 100,000 [4] or 10.81 times the previous prevalence rate!
References
[1] Fukuda, K., Straus, S.E., Hickie, I., Sharpe, M.C., Dobbins, J.G., & Komaroff, A. (1994). The chronic fatigue syndrome: A comprehensive approach to its definition and study. Annals of Internal Medicine, 121 (12):953-959. http://www.annals.org/cgi/content/full/121/12/953
[2] Jason, L.E., Taylor, R.R., & Kennedy, C.L. "Chronic Fatigue Syndrome, Fibromyalgia, and Multiple Chemical Sensitivities in a Community-Based Sample of Persons With Chronic Fatigue Syndrome-Like Symptoms." Psychosomatic Medicine 62:655-663 (2000). http://www.psychosomaticmedicine.org/cgi/reprint/62/5/655
[3] Reyes M, Nisenbaum R, Hoaglin DC, Unger ER, Emmons C, Randall B, Stewart JA, Abbey S, Jones JF, Gantz N. Prevalence and incidence of chronic fatigue syndrome in Wichita, Kansas. Arch Intern Med. 2003;163:1530–1536. doi: 10.1001/archinte.163.13.1530.
[4] Reeves WC, Jones JF, Maloney E, Heim C, Hoaglin DC, Boneva RS, Morrissey M, Devlin R. Prevalence of chronic fatigue syndrome in metropolitan, urban, and rural Georgia. Population Health Metrics 2007, 5:5 doi:10.1186/1478-7954-5-5
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY. -
On 2014 Feb 28, Tom Kindlon commented:
This may not be a representative group of those who would be diagnosed in a random sample using the "standardized clinically empirical criteria"
(This was originally posted here http://www.biomedcentral.com/1741-7015/3/19/comments#290587 but the formatting has gone from that page)
This "empirical" method of operationalizing the CDC 1994 CFS criteria[1] has subsequently been used in a population study[2]. It found a prevalence rate for CFS of 2540 per 100,000 persons 18 to 59 years of age[2].
This is considerably higher than the prevalence rates found in earlier studies.
For example, a previous study using this cohort using a "previous" method of operationalizing the CDC 1994 CFS criteria[1] found a prevalence rate of 235 per 100,000[3]. Given the way the cohort in this current study was drawn up, using 58 people who had previously been diagnosed using a "previous" method of operationalizing the CDC 1994 CFS criteria, the group satisfying the new method of operationalizing the CDC 1994 CFS criteria, the "empirical" criteria, in this study may well not be the same sort of people that would show up if the method was used on a random sample of the population. So for example the results in Table 6 may not be similar to the results one can get in a random sample.
Unfortunately the paper giving the prevalence rate for Georgia[2] does not give the same pieces of information as is in Table 6 in this study. However we do have a paper which uses a group from the Georgia cohort[4]. Table 1 of this study[4] includes similar data. Some of the numbers are somewhat similar. However one that particularly stands out is the Role Emotional score. It was 35.6 (95% CI: 26.3-44.8). That compares to the value in this paper of 55.8+/-42.2.
Perhaps other data will be published in time. The main point of this comment is to point out or remind people that the data presented in this paper may not be representative of those that would be diagnosed using the empirical criteria.
References:
[1] Fukuda K, Straus SE, Hickie I, Sharpe MC, Dobbins JG, & Komaroff A. (1994). The chronic fatigue syndrome: A comprehensive approach to its definition and study. Annals of Internal Medicine, 121 (12):953-959. http://www.annals.org/cgi/content/full/121/12/953
[2] Reeves WC, Jones JF, Maloney E, Heim C, Hoaglin DC, Boneva RS, Morrissey M, Devlin R. Prevalence of chronic fatigue syndrome in metropolitan, urban, and rural Georgia. Population Health Metrics 2007, 5:5 doi:10.1186/1478-7954-5-5http://www.pophealthmetrics.com/content/5/1/5
[3] Reyes M, Nisenbaum R, Hoaglin DC, Unger ER, Emmons C, Randall B, Stewart JA, Abbey S, Jones JF, Gantz N, Minden S, Reeves WC: Prevalence and incidence of chronic fatigue syndrome in Wichita, Kansas. Arch Int Med 2003, 163:1530-1536.
[4] Nater UM, Maloney E, Boneva RS, Gurbaxani BM, Lin JM, Jones JF, Reeves WC, Heim C. Attenuated Morning Salivary Cortisol Concentrations in a Population-based Study of Persons with Chronic Fatigue Syndrome and Well Controls. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2007 Dec 26
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY. -
On 2014 Feb 28, Tom Kindlon commented:
Why is this definition being referred to as an "empirical definition"?
(This was originally posted here http://www.biomedcentral.com/1741-7015/3/19/comments#301566 but the formatting has gone from that page)
I believe most people's understanding of "empirical criteria" or an "empirical definition" would be that the data would speak for itself; it "would decide" the cut-off points through methods such as cluster analysis (for example).
Indeed this would seem to have been William Reeves' understanding of an empirical definition. For example, in a presentation on the CDC's CFS research program (to a Task Force Meeting on the Epidemiology of Interstitial Cystitis)[1], he said: "The problem with the CFS criteria was that they were not specific enough and not empiric-based. For example, one of the criteria stated that the research subject must have at least four of eight symptoms, among them, impaired concentration or memory and postexertional worsening of physical or mental fatigue. "The accompanying symptoms need to be defined in and of themselves," Dr. Reeves said.
The 1994 International Study Group also hypothesized that fatigue led to patients' symptoms rather than the reverse. The CDC is currently conducting population studies to develop an empiric definition of CFS that is based on statistical modeling."At the inaugural meeting of the US Department of Health and Human Services' Chronic Fatigue Syndrome Advisory Committee (CFSAC), Dr Reeves said the CDC team of research would "derive an empirical case definition based on data".[2]
The definition presented here does not seem to have been based either on "statistical modeling" or "data". It seems to involve relatively arbitrary cut-off points; for example, of the 8 subscales of the SF-36, four are chosen and, for each of these, the 25th percentile of the published US population is chosen as a cut-off point. A patient is required to be in the bottom quartile for just one of these subscales to satisfy the criteria. Where did this cut-off point come from? There is no mention of it in the paper that suggested the use of the SF-36[3]; nor is there any mention that these particular subscales should be chosen or that one would be sufficient. One of the authors of the paper[3] has confirmed that cut-off points were never chosen nor was it decided which sub-scales would be used.
Given that the CDC's definition of CFS tends to go on to be used in numerous studies, would it not be better to investigate which thresholds give a "better" definition e.g. with a higher specificity and sensitivity - for example, for some of the SF-36 subscales, perhaps (say) the 13th, 15th, 20th or even 30th percentiles may be more appropriate.
The cut-off points suggested in this paper may or may not be useful. But is it really accurate to suggest that they are "empirically" derived?
References:
[1] Epidemiology of Interstitial Cystitis - Executive Committee Summary and Task Force Meeting Report October 29th, 2003. http://www.niddk.nih.gov/fund/reports/ic/task_force_summary.pdf
[2] US Department of Health and Human Services - Chronic Fatigue Syndrome Advisory Committee (CFSAC). Inaugural Meeting. September 29th, 2003Meeting Summary. http://www.hhs.gov/advcomcfs/CSFAC_mins_2003.09.29R.pdf
[3] Reeves WC, Lloyd A, Vernon SD, Klimas N, Jason LA, Bleijenberg G, Evengard B, White PD, Nisenbaum R, Unger ER, International Chronic Fatigue Syndrome Study Group: Identification of ambiguities in the 1994 chronic fatigue syndrome research case definition and recommendations for resolution. BMC Health Services Research 2003, 3:25
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY. -
On 2014 Feb 28, Tom Kindlon commented:
Using two MFI scales ("General Fatigue" or "Reduced Activity") to ensure patients satisfying the definition have "severe fatigue"
(This was originally posted here http://www.biomedcentral.com/1741-7015/3/19/comments#304567 but the formatting has gone from that page)
Initially when I read this paper, where it says "we defined severe fatigue as >= medians of the MFI general fatigue (>=13) or reduced activity (>=10) scales", I thought this referred to medians of the general population.Hearing other people commenting on it, that's how some other people have been interpreting it also. It is probably somewhat natural to do this as the sentence before reads: "We defined substantial reduction in occupational, educational, social, or recreational activities as scores lower than the 25th percentile of published US population [11] on the physical function (<=70), or role physical (<=50), or social function (<=75), or role emotional (<=66.7) subscales of the SF-36."
However from looking at the scores for controls in other papers, these MFI scores do not look like medians for the whole US population but in fact are medians for this particular group of patients. This seems a strange way to set cut-off points for a CFS definition that is used for numerous studies into the illness, given the cohort that is being used as a basis: "This population-based case control study enrolled 227 adults identified from the population of Wichita with: (1) CFS (n = 58); (2) non-fatigued controls matched to CFS on sex, race, age and body mass index (n = 55); (3) persons with medically unexplained fatigue not CFS, which we term ISF (n = 59); (4) CFS accompanied by melancholic depression (n = 27); and (5) ISF plus melancholic depression (n= 28)." i.e. this is not a random sample of the US population but a group of people selected for a specific purpose (or purposes) (not necessarily to design a definition, but as a follow-up study of people previously diagnosed with CFS or given some other label).
Some of the groups are of different sizes - if the relative size of these groups had been changed, with relatively more people taken from some classification groups and less people taken from other groups, the median scores would likely have been different.
It should also be remembered that in this context the categories listed in the last paragraph refer to their classification when they evaluated years before (from 1997 to 2000), and not necessarily at the time when they were evaluated in this study (December 2002 to July 2003) (as is clear from the tables in this paper).
I thought it would be interesting to look at MFI scores in some other papers on CFS that did not use the "empirical definition".
I don't claim this is a definitive list but, at the same time, mean MFI scores with standard deviations only seem to be listed in a small percentage of papers. The papers use cohorts from a variety of locations: England [3], The Netherlands [4], Germany [5] and the USA (New Jersey) [6]. I did not see any ranges given which would be useful given the task at hand (selecting cut-off points for a definition).Unfortunately not all of the papers I found used the Fukuda [1] definition for CFS; some also used the Sharpe [2] definition for CFS. I indicate which definition is used in each case.
MFI: General Fatigue (i) Sample/(ii) Sample Size/(iii) Mean/(iv) SD/ (v) (Mean - 13)/SD/ (vi) Definition
Weatherley-Jones [3] 53 18.4 1.7 3.176470588 Sharpe (1991)
Vermeulen (Group 1) [4] 30 18.6 1.9 2.947368421 Fukuda (1994)
Vermeulen (Group 2) [4] 30 18.4 1.8 3 Fukuda (1994)
Vermeulen (Group 3) [4] 30 19.1 1.4 4.357142857 Fukuda (1994)
Gaab [5] 21 17.7 0.5 9.4 Sharpe (1991) and Fukuda (1994)
Brimacombe [6] 65 18.41 2.02 2.678217822 Fukuda (1994)Combining these give a sample of 229 patients with a mean "General Fatigue" score of 18.45655022.
This data suggests that a threshold of >=13 will have a very very high sensitivity. This would suggest that another measure would not be necessary (unless it was being used as an extra criterion to increase the specificity, which isn't done with this definition).
However for completeness, I'm including the "Reduced Activity" data from the same papers:Reduced activity (MFI) (i) Sample / (ii) Sample Size / (iii) Mean Score / (iv) SE (v) (Mean-10)/SD (vi) Definition
Weatherley-Jones [3] 53 16.1 3.1 1.967741935 Sharpe(1991)
Gaab [5] 21 15 0.7 8.714285714 Sharpe (1991) and Fukuda(1994)
Brimacombe [6] 65 15.93 4.55 1.340659341 Fukuda 1994
Combining these give a sample of 139 patients with a mean Reduced Activity score of 15.85431655.
Note: the Vermeulen paper[4] did not collect the MFI scores for Reduced Activity, just "the fatigue axes of the Multidimensional Fatigue Inventory" (which they defined as the MFI scores for General fatigue, Physical fatigue, Mental fatigue).
It seems strange in the definition of Chronic Fatigue Syndrome defined in this paper (i.e. Reeves et al) that the "severe fatigue" criterion can be satisfied by a patient having a low score on a subscale of the MFI testing activity levels (as opposed to one of the 3 subscales measuring fatigue), especially when the function of the SF-36 is to "measure functional impairment". Just because someone is inactive doesn't mean they have severe fatigue. Allowing patients to be included if they simply have a "Reduced Activity" score of 10 or more (without necessarily having a low score on one of the fatigue axes of the MFI) risks reducing the specificity of the definition.
References:
[1] Fukuda K, Straus SE, Hickie I, Sharpe MC, Dobbins JG, Komaroff A. The chronic fatigue syndrome; a comprehensive approach to its definition and study. Ann Int Med 1994, 121:953-959.
[2] Sharpe MC, Archard LC, Banatvala JE, Borysiewicz LK, Clare AW, David A, Edwards RH, Hawton KE, Lambert HP, Lane RJ, et al. A report--chronic fatigue syndrome: guidelines for research. J R Soc Med. 1991 Feb;84(2):118-21.
[3] Weatherley-Jones, E., Nicholl, JP., Thomas, KJ., Parry, GJ., McKendrick, MW., Green, ST., Stanley, PJ and Lynch, SPJ. A randomised, controlled, triple-blind trial of the efficacy of homeopathic treatment for chronic fatigue syndrome. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 2004, 56, 2, 189-197.
[4] Vermeulen, RCW and Scholte, HR. Exploratory open label, randomized study of acetyl- and propionylcarnitine in chronic fatigue syndrome. Psychosomatic Medicine, 2004, 66, 276-282.
[5] Gaab J, Hüster D, Peisen R, Engert V, Heitz V, Schad T, Schürmeyer TH, Ehlert U. Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis reactivity in chronic fatigue syndrome and health under psychological, physiological, and pharmacological stimulation.Psychosom Med. 2002 Nov-Dec;64(6):951-62.
[6] Brimacombe, Michael; Lange, Gudrun; Bisuchio, Kim; Ciccone, Donald S.; Natelson, Benjamin. Cognitive Function Index for Patients with Chronic Fatigue Syndrome Journal of Chronic Fatigue Syndrome, 2004, vol 12; number 4, pages 3-24
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY. -
On 2016 Jul 21, Tom Kindlon commented:
Another study raises serious questions about the use of the role emotional subscale of the SF-36 in the diagnosis of CFS
A CDC group (Reeves et al (2005)) proposed a way to operationalise chronic fatigue syndrome (CFS) criteria including subscales of the SF-36[1,2]: "We defined substantial reduction in occupational, educational, social, or recreational activities as scores lower than the 25th percentile of published US population [] on the physical function (≤ 70), or role physical (≤ 50), or social function (≤ 75), or role emotional (≤ 66.7) subscales of the SF-36." This method has been used in numerous subsequent CDC CFS studies but not by external groups, apart from Leonard Jason and colleagues who have examined the effect of this operationalisation of the criteria.
A newly published study looked at using the SF 36 subscales to define reduced activity in CFS (and ME)[3]. Six of the 8 subscales (Physical Functioning, Role-Physical, Bodily Pain, General Health, Vitality and Social Functioning) correlated with the sum of current hours of activity. Two subscales did not correlate (Role-Emotional and Mental Health).
The same 6 subscales (negatively) correlated with percentage reduction in the total hours of activity. Again, two subscales did not correlate (Role-Emotional and Mental Health).
Also, a "ROC analysis was used to assess which of the SF-36 subscales best discriminated between patients with ME and CFS and healthy controls. The ROC analysis used a plot of sensitivity versus 1-specificity for scores on the SF-36. The area under the curve (AUC) assessed the degree to which each subscale was able to discriminate between patients and controls. The closer the AUC is to 1, the better discriminatory power that SF-36 subscale has." The same 6 subscales (Physical Functioning, Role-Physical, Bodily Pain, General Health, Vitality and Social Functioning) had AUCs of 0.89 or better. By comparison, the Mental Health Functioning subscale had a AUC of 0.59. The AUC for the Role Emotional subscale was even worse at 0.47.
Each of these results demonstrates that the Role Emotional subscale is not suitable to be used to operationalise criteria for CFS.
References:
[1] Reeves WC, Wagner D, Risenbaum R, et al. Chronic fatigue syndrome – a clinically empirical approach to its definition and study. BMC Med. 2005;3(19):1–9.
[2] Ware JE, Sherbourne CD. The MOS 36-item short-form health survey (SF-36): conceptual framework and item selection. Med Care. 1992;30:473–483
[3] Thorpe T, McManimen S, Gleason K, Stoothoff J, Newton J, Strand EB, Jason LA (2016). Assessing current functioning as a measure of significant reduction in activity level. Fatigue: Biomedicine, Health & Behavior. Published online: 19 Jul 2016 doi: 10.1080/21641846.2016.1206176
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Jun 01, Ivan Oransky commented:
This paper has been retracted: http://retractionwatch.com/2014/05/27/oncology-researcher-getzenberg-notches-seventh-retraction/
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Dec 10, Kath Wright commented:
Other search filters are available from the InterTASC Information Specialists' Sub-Group Search Filter Resource at https://sites.google.com/a/york.ac.uk/issg-search-filters-resource/home
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov
-
On 2015 Aug 27, Bernard Friedenson commented:
For more information please see the article entitled "Mutations in Breast Cancer Exome Sequences Predict Susceptibility to Infection and Converge on the Same Signaling Pathways" This article is available at http://la-press.com/article.php?article_id=5029
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov
-
On 2014 Oct 12, EDWARD BERRY commented:
The structure of the 3NPA-Arg adduct proposed here has been confirmed in the case of E. coli fumarate reductase, and a mechanism for its formation has been proposed, by Tomasiak et al. (J Biol Chem. 2011 286:3047-56) Pubmed ID 21098488 . Correction: The heterocyclic ring of carboxin is modeled with the wrong orientation in 2fbw, shown in Figure 6 here. Ruprecht et al.(J Biol Chem. 2009 284:29836-46) PMID 19710024. have deposited a corrected structure as 2wqy. The original authors agree with this correction as indicated here. Edward A. Berry 21:52, 21 May 2014 (IDT)
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 May 06, John Roger Andersen commented:
Green Open Access to the authors final version: click here.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2017 Dec 23, Adriano Aguzzi commented:
If anything, it should read "E uno plures".
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Mar 29, Jonathan Wren commented:
URL relocated: http://www.genenames.org/
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Dec 15, Jim Stewart commented:
Please note, this article has been retracted. The "full text" link will take you to the retraction notice.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY. -
On 2018 Jan 08, Alexander Kraev commented:
Please note that this is a peculiar case of retraction, since while there are indeed problems with certain images, the authors stated the following in the retraction notice: "Nevertheless, although we feel that the scientific conclusions of the paper (that loss of NNT impairs insulin secretion and impairs glucose tolerance in C57BL/6J mice) still stand, given the conclusion of scientific misconduct against the first author, the authors think the most responsible course is to retract the paper." It is important to understand that nuance especially in view that thousands of publications state that they used "C57BL6" without making a crucial distinction between C57BL/6J and C57BL/6N.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov
-
On 2014 Dec 10, Kath Wright commented:
Other search filters are available from the InterTASC Information Specialists' Sub-Group Search Filter Resource at https://sites.google.com/a/york.ac.uk/issg-search-filters-resource/home
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2015 Jan 21, Anders von Heijne commented:
We have just seen a case (young woman;in Stockholm, Sweden) with the exakt same type of vascular malformation and no obvious etiology, but with episodes of left sided pleuritic pain.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Nov 20, Morten Oksvold commented:
An investigation committee at Wayne State University (WSU) recommends that 42 articles from Fazlul Sarkar to be retracted (report finished August 31, 2015). This article represents one of them.
This information was published by Retraction Watch (November 17, 2016) and you can find a link to the full report here:
http://retractionwatch.com/2016/11/17/details-of-investigative-report-into-sarkar-released-by-aclu/
This article should therefore no longer be cited.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Jan 09, Andrew Brown commented:
This article was corrected in 2006, but does not appear to be linked in PubMed. Correction: Are fast food restaurants an environmental risk factor for obesity? http://www.ijbnpa.org/content/pdf/1479-5868-3-35.pdf
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2015 Oct 27, Peter Gøtzsche commented:
This review concludes that, “The three cholinesterase inhibitors are efficacious for mild to moderate Alzheimer’s disease.” I believe these drugs don’t work. The improvement in cognitive function was 2.7 points, in the midrange of a 70-point scale. This is less than the 4 points the FDA considers the minimally relevant clinical chan¬ge (1). We can also compare with the smallest effect that can be per¬ceived on the Hamilton scale for depression, which is 5-6 (2), although the maximum on this scale is only 52.
The placebo controlled trials have not been effectively blinded, as cholinesterase inhibitors have conspicuous side effect. This lack of blinding can in itself easily have caused the very minor effect that was noted in the trials (3).
A long-term trial of 565 patients with mild to moderate Alz¬heimer’s disease that compared donepezil with placebo found no meaningful effects whatsoever, and the authors concluded that do¬nepezil isn’t cost-effective, with benefits below minimally relevant thresholds (4). In contrast to other trials, it was publicly funded. This trial was excluded from the Cochrane review, for no good reason, as far as I can see. The outcomes after three years were similar on drug and placebo with respect to institutionalisation, progression of disability, and behavioural and psychological symptoms.
The author of the Cochrane review wrote that “donepezil appears to have no serious or common side effects.” This sentence is highly misleading. The harms are both common and serious, and she documents in her review that 29% of the patients left the drug group on account of adverse events, as compared to only 18% in the placebo groups. The most common side effects of donepezil are nausea, diarrhoea, not sleeping well, vomiting, muscle cramps, feeling tired, and not wanting to eat. I believe this is not what we would want for an old person who might already have problems with not sleeping well, feeling tired, and not wanting to eat.
The list of frequent side effects in Pfizer’s product information for Aricept (donepezil) is very long (5). Hypotension and syncope occurs in more than 1% and when old people fall, there is a considerable risk that they break their hip and die. A large Canadian cohort study showed that people who took anti-dementia drugs had almost a doubled risk of hospitalisation for syncope compared to demented people who didn’t take these drugs, and they had more pacemakers inserted and more hip fractures (6). Most astonishingly, more than half the patients who were admitted to hospital for bradycardia were retreated with the drug.
There are many good reasons not to use anti-dementia drugs.
1 Molnar FJ, Man-Son-Hing M, Fergusson D. Systematic review of measures of clinical significance employed in randomized controlled trials of drugs for de¬mentia. J Am Geriatr Soc 2009;57:536-46.
2 Leucht S, Fennema H, Engel R, et al. What does the HAMD mean? J Affect Disord 2013;148:243-8.
3 Gøtzsche PC. Deadly psychiatry and organised denial. Copenhagen: People’s Press; 2015.
4 Courtney C, Farrell D, Gray R, et al. Long-term donepezil treatment in 565 patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD2000): randomised double-blind trial. Lancet 2004;363:2105-15.
5 ARICEPT ® (donepezil hydrochloride) tablets. http://labeling.pfizer.com/ShowLabeling.aspx?id=510.
6 Syncope with cholinesterase inhibitors. Rev Prescrire 2011;31:434.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Feb 12, Daniel Schwartz commented:
This tool can be used online and in mobile apps here:
https://qxmd.com/calculate/mayo-early-screen-for-discharge-planning
Conflict of interest: Medical Director, QxMD
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Jan 05, Morten Oksvold commented:
Please note that this article contains fraudulent data, which was concluded from an investigation by the University of Alabama at Birmingham in 2009 (!):
http://www.uab.edu/reporterarchive/71570-uab-statement-on-protein-data-bank-issues
The case was recently discussed at Retraction Watch: http://retractionwatch.com/2016/01/04/nature-retracts-paper-six-years-after-it-was-flagged-for-fraud/
PNAS was informed about this case in 2009, but as far as I can see there has still not been published any retraction. In light of the fact that this single article has been cited more than 40 times since 2009, and all the wasted work and resources, correction of the literature is highly important.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Feb 01, Raphael Levy commented:
I have written a blog post where I identify this influential article (cited 5000 times) as one that introduces the (misleading) idea that nanoparticles can "penetrate cell membrane". Comments very much welcome, here or at the blog:
NANOPARTICLES & CELL MEMBRANES: HISTORY OF A (SCIENCE) FICTION? https://raphazlab.wordpress.com/2016/02/01/nanoparticles-cell-membranes-history-of-a-science-fiction/
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Nov 23, Harri Hemila commented:
A comment on the study is available at: http://hdl.handle.net/10250/8081
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Dec 10, Kath Wright commented:
Other search filters are available from the InterTASC Information Specialists' Sub-Group Search Filter Resource at https://sites.google.com/a/york.ac.uk/issg-search-filters-resource/home
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2013 Nov 24, John Sotos commented:
The death of “Mr. Abbott” (*) whose overlooked aortic dissection was misdiagnosed as an acute coronary syndrome, illuminates more than just the demise of the physical examination. It also illustrates Goethe`s precept “What one knows, one sees” (1).
A patient writhing because of chest pain should immediately be suspected to have aortic dissection (2)(3). Patients with chest discomfort due to coronary events more characteristically lie motionless (3), as noted in older (4), but not newer (5) cardiology textbooks.
Today`s highly specific imaging and biochemical tests have changed the role of physical examination from hypothesis confirmation to hypothesis generation. However, these tests have not, in the words of Dr. Joseph Bell (the model for Sherlock Holmes), changed the obligation of each physician to know “the features of disease… as precisely as you know the features, the gait, the tricks of manner of your most intimate friend” (3).
(*) Jauhar S. The demise of the physical exam. N Engl J Med. 2006; 354: 548-551.
(1) DeGowin RL. DeGowin & DeGowin`s Bedside Diagnostic Examination. 5th ed. New York: Macmillan, 1987; 37.
(2) Slater EE. Aortic dissection: presentation and diagnosis. In: Doroghazi RM, Slater EE, Aortic Dissection. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1983; 62.
(3) Sotos JG. Zebra Cards. Philadelphia: American College of Physicians, 1989; page 19 and card HE-011.
(4) Pasternak RC, Braunwald E, Sobel BE. Acute myocardial infarction. In: Heart Disease. 3rd ed. Braunwald E (ed.). Philadelphia: WB Saunders, 1988; 1235.
(5) Antman EM, Braunwald E. Acute myocardial infarction. In: Heart Disease. 5th ed. Braunwald E (ed.). Philadelphia: WB Saunders, 1997; 1198.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Jul 14, Pedro Reche commented:
This tool is now maintained at the Facultad de Medicina of the University Complutense of Madrid (http://imed.med.ucm.es/dachis/)
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov
-
On 2014 Nov 14, Robert Eibl commented:
This was the very first report on VLA-1 / VCAM-1 interactions measured at the single-molecule level with AFM and at the same time between two living mammalian cells. Two later AFM reports on VLA-4/VCAM-1 lack citation and discussion of my original work, although they were clearly induced or even initiated by myself; interestingly, my years of initiating and pioniering such AFM experiments as independent and unpaid guest PI and exclusive sharing of know-how was neither acknowledged, nor cited in related reviews.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Apr 24, Stuart RAY commented:
NOTE: Sequence features reported prominently in this report have been found in a subsequent report (http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21702696) to have been erroneous, due to alignment error and other issues.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Aug 21, Morten Oksvold commented:
Please note that this article represents one of eleven publications which were found to contain false data, after an investigation led by ORI in 2015:
https://ori.hhs.gov/content/case-summary-dasmahapatra-girija
This article should therefor not be cited.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Dec 10, Kath Wright commented:
Other search filters are available from the InterTASC Information Specialists' Sub-Group Search Filter Resource at https://sites.google.com/a/york.ac.uk/issg-search-filters-resource/home
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Jul 29, Noel O'Boyle commented:
I believe that the description of the structure as cyclo-(D-NMePhe-L-Leu-L-Ile-L-NMeTyr-L-Phe-NMeGly-L-Pro) is incorrect, as the convention is to write the residues from the N terminus to the C terminus. This is a cyclic peptide, and thus missing the terminii, but there is still a directionality from the N of a residue towards its carbonyl.
In short, to be consistent with the X-ray and structural depiction, I believe that it should be written instead as cyclo-(L-Pro-NMeGly-L-Phe-L-NMeTyr-L-Ile-L-Leu-D-NMePhe).
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2015 Oct 29, Vinodh Srinivasasainagendra commented:
PowerAtlas web-application has a new URL http://poweratlas.ssg.uab.edu , which replaces our previous homepage http://www.poweratlas.org.
Thanks, PowerAtlas Team
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Nov 20, Morten Oksvold commented:
An investigation committee at Wayne State University (WSU) recommends that 42 articles from Fazlul Sarkar to be retracted (report finished August 31, 2015). This article represents one of them.
This information was published by Retraction Watch (November 17, 2016) and you can find a link to the full report here:
http://retractionwatch.com/2016/11/17/details-of-investigative-report-into-sarkar-released-by-aclu/
This article should therefore no longer be cited.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Sep 15, Dr Yasmin Momin commented:
a rare event --intresting
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Nov 20, Morten Oksvold commented:
An investigation committee at Wayne State University (WSU) recommends that 42 articles from Fazlul Sarkar to be retracted (report finished August 31, 2015). This article represents one of them.
This information was published by Retraction Watch (November 17, 2016) and you can find a link to the full report here:
http://retractionwatch.com/2016/11/17/details-of-investigative-report-into-sarkar-released-by-aclu/
This article should therefore no longer be cited.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Oct 19, Richard Kunert commented:
The central claim of this paper is not supported by the numbers reported in the paper.
p. 142:
Participants exhibited the core personality features of psychopathy (Factor 1) to a greater extent than the core behavioral features of psychopathy (Factor 2).
In contradiction to this central claim of the paper, Factor 2 scores at 7.1 (the behavioural features of psychopathy) are actually higher than the Factor 1 scores at 5.2 (the personality features of psychopathy). The numbers tell the exactly opposite story to the words.
The numbers are given twice in the paper making a typo unlikely (p. 138 and p. 139). Adjusting the scores for the maxima of the scales that they are from (factor 1 x/xmax = 0.352 < factor 2 x/xmax=0.394) or the sample maximum (factor 1 x/xmaxobtained = 0.433 < factor 2 x/xmaxobtained = 0.44375) makes no difference. No outlier rejection is mentioned in the paper.
In sum, it appears as if DeMatteo and his co-authors interpret their numbers in a way which makes intuitive sense but which is in direct contradiction to their own data.
This issue was first raised publicly on Brain's Idea. The first author (DeMatteo) and the editor of the journal (Ewing) have been invited to respond via e-mail on 12/8/2016. Now, more than two months later, there is still no response.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Feb 18, Kristina Hanspers commented:
A version of the pathway in figure 6a is available in the "Open Access Publication" collection at WikiPathways: http://www.wikipathways.org/index.php/Pathway:WP2293. This pathway can be downloaded for use in network analysis tools such as Cytoscape and PathVisio.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Nov 23, Harri Hemila commented:
A manuscript version is available at HDL
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2013 Nov 18, Lior Pachter commented:
The GeneMapper software is no longer supported or maintained.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Jan 09, Tom Kindlon commented:
Why is the 11-item bimodal Chalder Fatigue Scale being used as a primary outcome measure?
(This was originally posted here http://www.biomedcentral.com/1741-7015/4/9/comments in 2008)
Stouten [1] has analysed some commonly used fatigue scales in the CFS area: the Chalder Fatigue Scale, the Checklist Individual Strength and the Krupp Fatigue Severity. He calculated the lower bound for the number and percentage of items with the maximum score for several studies and found that "extreme scoring" was common in studies in the field using these instruments. What is clear from this analysis is that the bimodal Chalder Fatigue Scale comes out worst in this regard.
Two of the authors of the current study should have been "intimately" aware of this problem as they were involved in one [2] of the two studies examined by Stouten [1] that used the 11-item bimodal Chalder Fatigue Scale. Here is the data from that study[2]: Baseline assessment for four intervention groups: Mean (95% Confidence Interval): 10.6 (10.4 to 10.9); 10.4 (10.0 to 10.7); 9.9 (9.2 to 10.6); 10.2 (9.9 to 10.6).
In percentage terms, this means that the lower bounds for the number of items with the maximum score for intervention groups were: 96%;95%;90%;93%. One can also calculate a lower bound for the percentage of patients who scored a maximum of 11 out of 11 for the items in the four intervention groups: 60%; 40%; 0%; 20%.
It should be remembered that these are lower bounds and the actual figure is likely to be higher (unless the authors give this data one can't calculate it from the mean). This can be illustrated by calculating the lower bound for the percentage of maximum (11 out of 11) scores for one study of an outpatient clinic in London[3] and comparing it to the actually percentage with the maximum score that was given. The Mean Score on the 11-item bimodal Chalder Fatigue Scale was 9.9. This translates to a lower bound for the percentage of patients who scored a maximum of 11 out of 11 of 0% (this could be achieved, for example, by 90% of the patients scoring 10/11 and 10% scoring 9/11). However the actual percentage of patients who scored the maximum (11/11) was 58%.
This shows that the 11-item bimodal Chalder Fatigue Scale doesn't just have a low ceiling for each individual question but also for the total score when applied to Chronic Fatigue Syndrome patients. Why is this important? Well, as the authors point out, some surveys of patient groups have found patients reporting being made worse by interventions such as Graded Exercise Therapy and Cognitive Behavioural Therapy (CBT).
For example, the results of a large survey with 2338 respondents were published in a report for the Chief Medical Officer[4]: it found that, of 285 who had done Cognitive Behavioural Therapy, 26% reported being made worse by the program, compared to 7% who said they were helped and 67% who said it made no difference. Of 1214 people who had done a graded exercise program, 50% had been been made worse by it (compared to 34% who said it helped and 16% who said it made no difference).These are not once-off results. For example, a recently published report of 2763 patients with ME or CFS in the UK[5] which asked about people's experiences of treatments over the last three years, found that of 699 who said they'd tried Graded Exercise Therapy, 34% said they'd been made worse by it compared to 45% who said they'd been helped and 21% who said it made no difference. The contention that people would not have being made worse by a treatment if they had done the treatment under specialist supervision, is not backed up by the data from this study[5]. Patients were asked who provided the GET treatment. Of the 567 who answered this question, 181 (31.92%) said it had made them worse compared to 276 (48.68%) who said it helped and 110 (19.40%) who said it made no difference; these are very similar percentages to the subgroup of 335 patients who had done the management strategy under an "NHS specialist": 111 (31.27%) of this group said they'd been made worse compared to 162 (45.63%) who said they'd been helped and 82 (23.10%) who said it made no difference. Again these don't appear to be once-off figures. In 2003, Action for ME did a smaller survey of 550 members asking about their experiences of Treatments[6]. 354 (64%) replied. The numbers for this study were small but if you combined the data from those who had done GET under a Physiotherapist, Occupational Therapist, Doctor or Behavioural Therapist, the results are: Negative 22 (56.41%) Neutral 2 (5.13%) Positive 15 (38.46%). [These don't compare favorably to the small group who did GET under no professional: Negative 1 (8.33%), Neutral 4 (33%) and Positive 7 (58%)]. A large percentage of the patients also reported being made by made worse by CBT in this study. Of 55 who had done CBT under a CBT Therapist/psychologist, Doctor/Psychiatrist, CPN/Other Mental Health, Counsellor/Psychotherapist, OT or Nurse specialist, 19 (34.55%) said it had made them worse compared to 24 (43.64%) who had been helped and 12 (21.82%) who said it made no difference.
The reason this is important is that if somebody already has a score of 11/11, they can't come up on the 11-item bimodal Chalder Fatigue Scale as being made worse on the treatment. Indeed, once they improved on one item, it would be marked as an improvement overall even if they actually felt worse on one or more of the other 10 items. Of course, this doesn't just apply to patients who score 11 out of 11; a patient could score 10/11 (say), feel worse on several items they'd already scored the maximum but come out as an improver once they improved on one idea. Saying all that, it would be good if the authors reported how many patients in each branch of the study scored the maximum.
Some of the items on the 11-item Chalder Fatigue Scale may also not be good as a measure of severity of CFS or ME. For example, somebody could be severe but not answer positively to the question, 'do you feel sleepy or drowsy'. Similarly a patient could disimprove and still not answer positively to this question. So a patient who scores 11 may not necessarily be more severely affected on a patient scoring 10. Saying all this, the generally very high scores (i.e. close to 11 on average) found in previous studies in the field suggest that the Likert method (0,1,2,3) of scoring does appear preferable. However it too also suffers from a ceiling effect although not to the same extent[1]. Also as previously pointed out, because of questions such as 'do you feel sleepy or drowsy', (which many if not most would feel are not intrinsic to Chronic Fatigue Syndrome or ME), even the likert version of the Chalder Fatigue Scale is not ideal for measuring severity of the condition.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY. -
On 2014 Jan 09, Tom Kindlon commented:
Further comments on the outcome measures being used and suggestions for other outcome measures that could be useful in such trials
(This was originally posted here http://www.biomedcentral.com/1741-7015/4/9/comments in 2009)
In the protocol, the authors say the following: "A 2001 systematic review of all treatments for CFS/ME concluded that cognitive behaviour therapy (CBT) and graded exercise therapy (GET) were the most promising treatments for CFS/ME, but that owing to the small number of studies available for review, the generalisability of these results could not be assured [1]*. The authors recommended that further studies be carried out using standardised outcome measures."
They authors neglect to say that the authors of that review also recommended the use of more objective outcome measures: e.g. "Outcomes such as 'improvement,' in which participants were asked to rate themselves as better or worse than they were before the intervention began, were frequently reported. However, the person may feel better able to cope with daily activities because they have reduced their expectations of what they should achieve, rather than because they have made any recovery as a result of the intervention. A more objective measure of the effect of any intervention would be whether participants have increased their working hours, returned to work or school, or increased their physical activities."
It is very disappointing that the organisers of this trial have not taken this on board with this study. Given the cost of the trial (over £1m), the cost of some actometers (for example) would have virtually neglible.
Existing research has some interesting findings on the issue. For example, one study (on a single patient)[2] found "using a 26-session graded activity intervention involved gradual increases in physical activity" that "from baseline to treatment termination, the patient’s self-reported increase in walk time from 0 to 155 min a week contrasted with a surprising 10.6% decrease in mean weekly step counts."The authors of the current trial refer to the Prins (2001) study[3] as an example of a study which supposedly found that hospital-based hospital-based CBT was an "effective treatment" for CFS. Judging by some of the questionnaire data, it does look like CBT has had an effect. However the actometer data from this study subsequently became available[4) and the increases in activity were minimal. For instance, the baseline average for the group which received CBT was 67.9, which increased to 68.8 after treatment and to 72.2 at follow-up. About 4 points. Not unlike the medical care controls, who went from 64.9 to 68.7 in the same period.
One of the aims of CBT (for CFS) has been said to be "increased confidence in exercise and physical activity"[5]. Thus it may be the case that when asked questions about one's ability to do things, such as in the physical functioning subscale of SF-36 (one of the three primary outcome measures in the FINE Trial), the patients might say that they are "Limited A Little" or "Not Limited At All" but may be just as limited as patients in other arms of study who say "limited a lot".
The physical functioning subscale is the primary outcome measure that is also being used to measure "clinically significant improvement" ("An improvement of 50% or more on the SF-36 physical functioning scale, or a score of 75% or more on that scale, will be considered a clinically significant improvement"). It is not an objective instrument, particularly in a psychosocial trial.
In my two previous comments, I have criticised the use of the bimodal Chalder Fatigue Scale as an outcome measure in a trial of patients with "CFS/ME". Recently another trial[6] was published involving CFS patients in the UK. The mean score was not given but the median mark was 11. That is to say, at least 50% of the people scored the maximum mark before the intervention. These people can not "get worse" on the scale using the scale even if they feel worse.
The third and final primary outcome measure being used is a quality of life measure. Although it may be useful to measure the quality of life, the findings of a recent study[7] make for interesting reading. It used 73 patients, also with a diagnosis of CFS according to the Oxford criteria, from UK clinics. It involved using principal-component analysis to analyse various bits of questionnaire. The Principal-component analysis of all scale scores revealed 2 distinct components, explaining 53% of the total variance. The results are summarised in the following extract: "The perceived incapacity in fulfilling social and physical roles may be best captured by the subscales of the SF-36 on social and physical functioning. The scores on these subscales are associated with vitality and inversely with one of the defining symptoms of CFS, i.e. fatigue (Chalder Fatigue Scale, Fatigue Visual Analogue Scale). They are also associated with other physical symptoms (SDQ, SCL-90-R subscale ‘somatization’), but not with psychological symptoms such as depression (Beck Hopelessness Scale, SCL-90-R subscale ‘depression’) and anxiety (Spielberger Trait Anxiety Questionnaire, SCL-90-R subscale ‘anxiety’). These psychological symptoms are linked to a generic measure of quality of life (MANSA), reflecting satisfaction with life in general and life domains, and to emotional role functioning and mental health (SF-36, subscale)."
Of course, the instrument to measure quality of life is different in this study so the relevance of this study is unclear at this time. But like the other two outcome measures (Chalder Fatigue Scale and SF-36 PF), the Euroqol is subjective.
It is disappointing that there apart from checking for the presence or absence of the CDC criteria, there appears to be no measurement of other symptoms apart from fatigue. (And of course I've already pointed out the problems with using the bimodal Chalder Fatigue Scale e.g. it's hard for some patients to score "worse" scores using the scale). But most researchers do not think CFS = fatigue. Even if some patients have few other symptoms because the Oxford criteria is being used, this should not have mattered.
(contd.)
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY. -
On 2014 Jan 09, Tom Kindlon commented:
Comment on the discussion of the effectiveness of interventions for CFS
(This was originally posted here http://www.biomedcentral.com/1741-7015/4/9/comments in 2009)
The authors state that, in this study, "effect sizes and confidence intervals will be reported for each group", which is to be welcomed. As well as giving the protocol for the FINE Trial, this paper also gives information and data from some previous studies in the area including saying some treatments have been shown to be "effective". However, it does not report effect sizes.
Readers of this paper may be interested to know about a recent meta-analysis of the efficacy of CBT for CFS[1]. The studies involved a total of 1371 patients. It involved calculating the size of an effect measure, the Cohen's d value. They calculated d using the following method: "Separate mean effect sizes were calculated for each category of outcome variable (e.g., fatigue self- rating) and for each type of outcome variable (mental, physical, and mixed mental and physical). Studies generally included multiple outcome measures. For all analyses except those that compared different categories or types of outcome variables, we used the mean effect size of all the relevant outcome variables of the study." d was calculated to be 0.48.
For anyone unfamiliar with Cohen's d values, they are not bounded by 1; also, the higher the score, the bigger the "effect size" i.e. the more "effective" a treatment was found to be. Cohen's d values are considered to be a small effect size at 0.2, a moderate effect size at 0.5, and a large effect size at 0.8[2]. CBT had a more general definition in this paper and included some papers on GET. For example, the current paper says that, "Graded exercise therapy has also been shown to be effective in randomised controlled trials with selected hospital patients[3] and in our own previous study with a more general sample of hospital patients[4]." Malouff et al[1] calculated the d value for these studies as 0.46 (95% CI: 0.03 -0.95) and 0.17 (95% CI: -0.30 - +0.65) respectively (For the latter study, Malouff et al calculated the figures by comparing drug (i.e. antidepressant) treatment plus CBT to drug treatment without CBT).
References:
[1] Malouff, J. M., et al., Efficacy of cognitive behavioral therapy for chronic fatigue syndrome: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review (2007), doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.004
[2] Cohen J: Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences. Edited by: 2. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1988.
[3] Fulcher KY, White PD: Randomised controlled trial of graded exercise in patients with the chronic fatigue syndrome. BMJ 1997, 314:1647-1652.
[4] Wearden, A. J., Morriss, R. K., Mullis, R., Strickland, P. L., Pearson, D. J., Appleby, L., et al. (1998). Randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled treatment trial of fluoxetine and graded exercise for chronic fatigue syndrome. British Journal of Psychiatry, 172, 485?490.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Oct 17, Arturo Martí-Carvajal commented:
Reading this abstract, I asked myself two questions:
- What is the relation between title and conclusions of this RCT?
- TWAUC cholesterol from week 4 decreased more in the pravastatin group (-0.8 +/- 1.0 versus -0.3 +/- 0.9 mmol/L/week; P = 0.04) Query: mean (SE) or (SD)?
Does somebody have the answer please? Arturo
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Feb 01, Ferenc Zsila commented:
None
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY. -
On 2014 Feb 12, Ferenc Zsila commented:
This text is shown on the page 24, in the paper of Ma et al. (see above):
"Figure 2C shows the change in intensity of CD bands in the presence of 1.0 mol equiv of Cu<sup>2+</sup> at 252, 312, and 514 nm. Relatively strong CD bands are often for d-d transitions of Cu<sup>2+</sup> tetragonal complexes.<sup>29</sup> However, these complexes involve main-chain amide coordination as well as histidine coordination via the imidazole ring. In these cases, the dominant contribution to optical activity observed is due to the vicinal contributions resulting from the asymmetric alpha carbon held in a chelate ring between two chelating donor atoms, such as adjacent main-chain amides.<sup>43</sup> At physiological pH and below, the lack of optical activity from the d-d transition of the complex suggests that backbone amide coordination is absent. At pH 8.5 and above, amide proton is deprotonated, which promotes copper coordination by the main chain, resulting in a negative CD band appearing at 514 nm. Clearly, Aβ(1-16) forms a type II square-planar coordination geometry with Cu<sup>2+,</sup> and the coordination geometry is 3N1O at pH 7.5. Both visible absorption and CD spectra indicate that the coordinating ligands are strongly pH dependent, a mixed species is present at physiological pH, and main-chain amide coordination is absent at lower pH values."
The following text is shown on the page 18171, in the paper of Syme et al. Syme CD, 2004:
"The insets in Fig. 2 show the change in the intensity of CD bands in the presence of 1 eq of Cu<sup>2+</sup> at 252, 312, and 514 nm. Relatively strong CD bands are often observed for d-d transitions of Cu<sup>2+</sup> tetragonal complexes (36, 37). However, these complexes involve main-chain amide coordination as well as histidine coordination via the imidazole ring. In these cases, the dominant contribution to optical activity observed is due to the vicinal contributions resulting when the asymmetric α-carbon is held in a chelate ring between two chelating donor atoms (e.g. adjacent main-chain amides) (38). At physiological pH and below, the lack of optical activity from the d-d transition of the Cu-Aβ complex suggests that backbone amide coordination is not taking place. It is likely that raising the pH above 8 promotes amide deprotonation and copper coordination by the main chain, resulting in a CD band being observed at 514 nm. In summary, it is clear that Aβ forms a TypeII square-planar coordination geometry with Cu<sup>2+,</sup> and both EPR and CD measurements indicate that the coordinating ligands are highly pH-dependent, a mixed species is present at physiological pH, and main-chain amide coordination is not present at lower pH values."
[Syme CD, Nadal RC, Rigby SE, Viles JH. Copper binding to the amyloid-β (Aβ) peptide associated with Alzheimer's disease: folding, coordination geometry, pH dependence, stoichiometry, and affinity of Abeta-(1-28): insights from a range of complementary spectroscopic techniques. J. Biol. Chem. 2004 (279) 18169-18177.]
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov
-
On 2014 Nov 23, Harri Hemila commented:
The comment is available at DOI
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Sep 21, Morten Oksvold commented:
The published erratum of this article does not mention that a committee at McGill University has completed an investigation and concluded that several articles, included this one, were subjected to research misconduct. More specifically they found that two figures in this study “were intentionally contrived and falsified,”.
Please see a discussion at Retraction Watch: http://retractionwatch.com/2013/01/25/mcgill-committee-says-nature-figures-were-intentionally-contrived-and-falsified/
More recently a study by Vande Walle et al. (Nature, Brief communication) is questioning the conclusions in this article.
Please follow link:
http://www.nature.com/nature/journal/v534/n7605/full/nature17649.html
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2013 Oct 29, Hilda Bastian commented:
The results of this systematic review are contradicted by a more recent and more comprehensive analysis of the methodologically more rigorous trials of viscosupplementation of the knee (Rutjes AW, 2012). In that more recent review, Rutjes and colleagues identify significant publication bias as a contributing factor in previous over-estimations of the benefit of intra-articular injections.
Rutjes and colleagues conclude that the intervention has only a clinically irrelevant benefit on pain, no statistically significant effect on function and is associated with serious unexplained adverse events. They discourage the use of the intervention and suggest an individual patient data meta-analysis would be needed to explore the issue of adverse events.
Doubts about the potential for viscosupplementation of the knee to do more good than harm were also expressed in another systematic review published after that by Bellamy and colleagues (Samson DJ, 2007).
UPDATE: A network meta-analysis by Bannuru RR, 2015 was able to analyze intra-inarticular injections, intra-articular placebos, and oral placebos, as well as a range of active treatments. It found a clinically meaningful benefit from intra-articular hyaluronic acid injections, in large part attributable to the effects of intra-articular injections per se.
(I discussed the implications of the 2015 review in a February 2015 blog post.)
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov
-
On 2016 May 02, Hiten Patel commented:
WAT. No other trials?
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov
-
On 2014 Dec 10, Kath Wright commented:
Other search filters are available from the InterTASC Information Specialists' Sub-Group Search Filter Resource at https://sites.google.com/a/york.ac.uk/issg-search-filters-resource/home
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2013 Jun 28, Rafael Irizarry commented:
I highly recommend reading this paper. It presents the statistical motivation for the widely used limma package. Although originally developed for microarrays, I have used the ideas presented here for other high throughput data include next generation sequencing. Of particular importance is the idea of shrinking sample standard deviations before computing signal to noise summaries such as the t-test.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Apr 20, Daniel Corcos commented:
There is a serious bias in this study, as the mutation status for the cases was determined after diagnosis, whereas the controls were at-risk women. The large majority of the cancers were not diagnosed by screening mammography, indicating that screening was not performed efficiently in this group. On the contrary, being at-risk implicates implementation of screening, and, as a consequence, more mammograms in this group.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Nov 20, Morten Oksvold commented:
An investigation committee at Wayne State University (WSU) recommends that 42 articles from Fazlul Sarkar to be retracted (report finished August 31, 2015). This article represents one of them.
This information was published by Retraction Watch (November 17, 2016) and you can find a link to the full report here:
http://retractionwatch.com/2016/11/17/details-of-investigative-report-into-sarkar-released-by-aclu/
This article should therefore no longer be cited.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Aug 20, Peter Gøtzsche commented:
This meta-analysis, which was based on observational studies, found that a history of depression doubled the risk of developing Alzheimer’s disease later in life. The authors speculate that vascular disease and inflammation may be risk factors for both diseases. Nowhere in the paper do they alert their readers to the most obvious explanation. Virtually all people with a history of depression have been treated with antidepressant drugs and it might very well be the drugs that cause dementia.
We know for sure that antipsychotics cause permanent brain damage, and many of us suspect that this is also the case for other psychotropic drugs. Animal studies have been particularly worrying in this respect. However, the psychiatrists have learned from the drug industry never to blame the drug but always to blame the disease (1-4). There are countless studies in depression and countless statements by official bodies representing psychiatry about how dangerous untreated depression is, about visible deterioration on brain scans, etc, when in actual fact these opinions are built on studies of patients who were treated with antidepressant drugs (3). This makes no sense.
Peter C Gøtzsche Professor and Director, DrMedSci, MSc, MD Nordic Cochrane Centre Rigshospitalet Copenhagen
Conflicts of interest: none.
Healy D. Let Them Eat Prozac. New York: New York University Press; 2004.
Whitaker R. Anatomy of an Epidemic. New York: Broadway Paperbacks; 2010.
Raven M. Depression and antidepressants in Australia and beyond: A critical public health analysis. PhD thesis, University of Wollongong, Australia; 2012 (http://ro.uow.edu.au/theses/3686/, accessed 13 August 2014).
Gøtzsche PC. Deadly medicines and organised crime: How big pharma has corrupted health care. London: Radcliffe Publishing, 2013.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Aug 29, Jaime A. Teixeira da Silva commented:
The listed authorship on PubMed is incorrect. It should be Esashi Y, Nagao M. The affiliation for Nagao is also missing, and should be: 2 Biological Institute, Faculty of Science, Tohoku University, Aobayama, Sendai, Japan
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2015 Jul 08, Bill Cayley commented:
A good example of when "less" is "more": https://lessismoreebm.wordpress.com/2015/07/08/comparison-of-the-surgical-outcomes-of-punch-incision-and-elliptical-excision-in-treating-epidermal-inclusion-cysts-a-prospective-randomized-study/
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Dec 10, Kath Wright commented:
Other search filters are available from the InterTASC Information Specialists' Sub-Group Search Filter Resource at https://sites.google.com/a/york.ac.uk/issg-search-filters-resource/home
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Dec 10, Kath Wright commented:
Other search filters are available from the InterTASC Information Specialists' Sub-Group Search Filter Resource at https://sites.google.com/a/york.ac.uk/issg-search-filters-resource/home
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Nov 23, Harri Hemila commented:
A comment on the paper is available at: http://hdl.handle.net/10250/8080
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov
-
On 2013 Oct 28, DAVID SANDERS commented:
See the earlier articles "Sulfhydryl involvement in fusion mechanisms" [2000]Sanders DA, 2000 and "Ancient viruses in the fight against HIV" [2003] Sanders DA, 2003 for the prediction of the results published here.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2015 Mar 09, Salzman Lab Journal Club commented:
Interesting science! This paper provides the first evidence which suggests that the major RNA isoform produced by the muscleblind gene in Drosophila is a circular RNA that may be developmentally regulated.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2013 Dec 16, Andrew Davison commented:
Source code for the model described in this article is available at ModelDB with accession number 64261
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Jun 29, Ivan Oransky commented:
This paper has been retracted, one of three by the same group: http://retractionwatch.com/2014/06/23/cancer-genetics-group-retracts-three-papers-for-inappropriate-presentation-of-data/
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Dec 10, Kath Wright commented:
Other search filters are available from the InterTASC Information Specialists' Sub-Group Search Filter Resource at https://sites.google.com/a/york.ac.uk/issg-search-filters-resource/home
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2015 May 27, Anne Niknejad commented:
Error in Table 2: pEGFP-N1 585.5 ± 2.5
Text reports ∼35% palmitic acid fold-increase, by comparing EGFP-BGR containing fractions and controls (COS-7 cells transfected with pEGFP-N1), so the value should be '58.5'
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Dec 10, Kath Wright commented:
Other search filters are available from the InterTASC Information Specialists' Sub-Group Search Filter Resource at https://sites.google.com/a/york.ac.uk/issg-search-filters-resource/home
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2013 Oct 22, Rafael Najmanovich commented:
Genome-wide metabolic reconstructions have numerous uses. One of which is predicting what would be the effect of knocking out one or more genes or inhibiting the function of the corresponding protein(s). Once a gene is deleted or the protein inhibited, matter flows around other available metabolic routes. Such rearrangements may drastically disrupt the production of biomass or altogether prevent it, signifying that the gene/protein is fundamental or essential. Such proteins represent potential therapeutic targets.
Flux Balance Analysis (FBA) is widely used to perform predictions of gene essentiality Orth JD, 2010. Wunderlich and Mirny introduce Synthetic Accessibility (SA) as an alternative method that is based solely on the topology of the metabolic network. The idea is derived from synthetic chemistry labs where the difficulty in creating a new molecule is measured as the number of synthetic steps necessary to produce the molecule starting from available starting materials. In the case of metabolic networks, the idea is to calculate the number of steps necessary to produce biomass compounds from input metabolites.
The validity of the SA approach in predicting essential genes was verified in E. coli and S. cerevisiae. When a gene is knocked out or its protein inhibited in silico, the SA will necessarily either remain unchanged or increase (even infinitely) reflecting the longer path (or the absence thereof) necessary to reach output compounds using alternate metabolic routes.
SA and FBA are equivalent in terms of accuracy, around 60% and 80% respectively for E. coli and S. cereviseae. We implemented both SA and FBA in our lab and independently verified these results. Furthermore we also tested B. subtilis where a metabolic network exists Oh YK, 2007 and the full list of essential genes is known Kobayashi K, 2003, obtaining a success rate of 92% with SA (equivalent to the 94% obtained by Oh et al. Oh YK, 2007 with FBA). Wunderlich and Mirny point that the equivalent success rates between FBA and SA suggests the success of the former should be attributed mainly to network topology.
Some advantages of SA over FBA involve the simplicity of the approach (in terms of implementation and execution), not requiring any knowledge of the stoichiometry of reactions (or initial ranges for reaction rates). The latter in my opinion is a very interesting aspect of SA that allows its application to mixed networks that integrate gene regulatory networks, metabolic networks and other cellular processes that are more difficult to define in terms of stoichiometry and reaction rates.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2017 Jun 06, Egon Willighagen commented:
A third paper about the CDK was published today in the J. Cheminform.: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13321-017-0220-4
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov
-
On 2014 Dec 10, Kath Wright commented:
Other search filters are available from the InterTASC Information Specialists' Sub-Group Search Filter Resource at https://sites.google.com/a/york.ac.uk/issg-search-filters-resource/home
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2015 Jun 26, Bill Cayley commented:
A good example of when less is more: https://lessismoreebm.wordpress.com/2015/06/26/improving-womens-experience-during-speculum-examinations-at-routine-gynaecological-visits-randomised-clinical-trial/
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Mar 04, Chris Del Mar commented:
Between 1 - 5/1,000 children aged <5 years in developed countries may need hospital admission each year because of influenza infections, (PMID: 26111238) -- much lower (e.g. 0.6/1,000 children when based on laboratory confirmation of influenza, (http://www.eurosurveillance.org/ViewArticle.aspx?ArticleId=19365). The benefits of influenza vaccination in children are a NNTB of 28 to prevent one child age aged >6 years getting influenza, but there is no effect on any downstream consequences such as secondary infections, nor benefit for children aged <2 years, from a Cochrane review (PMID 22895945). This systematic review estimates a rate of fevers of 6-7%, and of febrile convulsions at ~1/1,000 from inactivated vaccine fever in children aged >6 years (NNTH of 16 and 1,000 respectively) -- in the same range as those who might be prevented from a post-influenza admission. This suggests the decision about influenza vaccination needs discussion with parents to balance benefits against harms, perhaps using shared decision making (patient decision aid might be ideal), rather than public health pronouncements. Peer Collingnon collignon.peter@gmail.com; Chris Del Mar cdelmar@bond.edu.au
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov
-
On 2013 Nov 04, Jamie Horder commented:
In a conference abstract, Berl and colleagues suggested that the striking results presented in this paper may have been the result of an artifact, observed in some MRI DTI sequences.
They argued that although the vibration signal-loss artifact in question is best known as an issue on Siemens MRI scanners, GE scanners, such as the one used in the Voss study, are not immune.
Berl et al wrote:
"The artifact may be the basis for a clinical misinterpretation that has been cited as evidence to change policy and practice [i.e the Voss study]. The speed that this article was propagated was assisted by the inherent interest of the case details; however, it also illustrates the danger of premature clinical interpretation of DTI data.
We suggest that repositioning the patient by adjusting the roll would be a simple and practical method to determine if such findings were indeed axonal regrowth or artifact."
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY. -
On 2014 Feb 14, Henning Voss commented:
We were aware of the possibility of artefacts in DTI at the time of publishing of [1], and therefore as noted in our study [1] we tested for increased residuals in the tensor fits, which would be a sign of the artefact described in [2] and [3]. We did not find increased residuals in the medial parietal occipital region or the cerebellar vermis. In particular for the vermis, later we also created plots corresponding to [3], Figure 2, on higher quality data of the same subject obtained with a headcoil unavailable at the time of first timepoint of study (not published) which showed the same effect. We did not find any hint for the artefact there, either. We also compared this case with 20 healthy control subjects scanned on the same equipment with the same protocol and the patient had the highest anisotropy in the medial parietal occipital region.
[1] Voss HU, Uluç AM, Dyke JP, Watts R, Kobylarz EJ, McCandliss BD, Heier LA, Beattie BJ, Hamacher KA, Vallabhajosula S, Goldsmith SJ, Ballon D, Giacino JT, Schiff ND. Possible axonal regrowth in late recovery from the minimally conscious state. J Clin Invest. 2006 Jul;116(7):2005-11. [2] Berl M, Walker L, Sarlls J, and Pierpaoli, C. Investigation of vibration induced artifacts in clinical diffusion weighted imaging of the brain. Proc. Intl. Soc. Mag. Reson. Med. 20, 3740 (2012). [3] Gallichan D, Scholz J, Bartsch A, Behrens TE, Robson MD, Miller KL. Addressing a systematic vibration artifact in diffusion-weighted MRI. Hum Brain Mapp. 2010 Feb;31(2):193-202.
H.U. Voss and N.D. Schiff
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Aug 19, Morten Oksvold commented:
Please note that this article has been retracted and should therefore not be cited in the future:
http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ijc.30268/full
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2015 Jun 16, Bill Cayley commented:
A great example of when "Less" is more: https://lessismoreebm.wordpress.com/2015/06/16/written-action-plans-for-asthma-in-children/
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2017 Jan 05, Robert Badgett commented:
A subsequent systematic review (Mathieson S, 2015) reports that the ID Pain using a cutoff of 2 or more to suggest neuropathic pain, "demonstrated satisfactory hypothesis testing and reliability" but sensitivity and specificity were not reported.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2017 May 15, Eric Yarnell commented:
Authors names are incorrect, being listed by their given names not their family names. Should be Bao XY, Wong CK, Li EKM, Tam LS, Leung PC, Yin YB, and Lam CWK.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Dec 10, Kath Wright commented:
Other search filters are available from the InterTASC Information Specialists' Sub-Group Search Filter Resource at https://sites.google.com/a/york.ac.uk/issg-search-filters-resource/home
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Nov 18, Morten Oksvold commented:
An investigation committee at Wayne State University (WSU) recommends that 42 articles from Fazlul Sarkar to be retracted (report finished August 31, 2015). This article represents one of them.
This information was published by Retraction Watch (November 17, 2016) and you can find a link to the full report here:
http://retractionwatch.com/2016/11/17/details-of-investigative-report-into-sarkar-released-by-aclu/
This article should therefore no longer be cited.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Sep 28, Guillem Feixas commented:
Could anyone tell me whether fibromyalgia could be considered a NCD? Thanks.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2017 Jun 26, Guangcun Huang commented:
The third author's name, Guangcun Huang, has been incorrectly spelled as Quangcun Huang.
http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1872-034X.2007.00295_1.x/full
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov
-
On 2016 Jun 11, Egon Willighagen commented:
The website is inaccessible. Is the database discontinued or is there a new website?
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Dec 10, Kath Wright commented:
Other search filters are available from the InterTASC Information Specialists' Sub-Group Search Filter Resource at https://sites.google.com/a/york.ac.uk/issg-search-filters-resource/home
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Sep 06, Jakob Suckale commented:
PubSum: The stem cells that can generate our body's specialized cell types take cues from the stiffness of their environment to decide which cell to become; soft generates nerve-like cells, hard generates bone-like cells. To arrive at this hypothesis researchers grew human stem cells on surfaces of varying stiffness and analyzed how they changed over time.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov
-
On 2014 Jun 15, Jorge H Ramírez commented:
Related articles:
Ramírez JH, Palacios M, Tamayo O, et al. Acute and subacute toxicity of Salvia scutellarioides in mice and rats. J Ethnopharmacol 2007;109:348–53. URL: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16978817 Full text article available in English
Ramírez JH, Palacios M, Gutiérrez O. Implementation of the isolated vascular tissue model as a device for the validation of medicinal plants: Study of the vasodilator activity of Salvia scutellarioides. Colomb. Med. 2007;38:28–39. URL: http://colombiamedica.univalle.edu.co/index.php/comedica/article/view/471/480 Full text article available in English and Spanish.
Ramírez JH, Palacios M, Ocampo HH, et al. Lack of association between blood pressure, target organ damage and retinopathy in the L-NAME rat hypertension model: Are new animal models of hypertension required?. Colomb. Med. 2006;37:328–31. URL: http://colombiamedica.univalle.edu.co/index.php/comedica/article/view/465/471 Full text article available in Spanish. Abstract available in English.
Ramírez JH, Palacios M, Gutiérrez O. Evaluation of the antihypertensive effect of Salvia scutellarioides in a rat model of hypertension. Colomb Med 2006; 37: 53-60. URL: http://colombiamedica.univalle.edu.co/index.php/comedica/article/view/412/417 Full text article available in Spanish. Abstract available in English.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Sep 14, Daniel Haft commented:
EpsH family proteins in bacteria are now called exosortase. Their very distant homologs in the archaea are termed archaeosortase. Exosortases and archaeosortases belong to an extended superfamily, exclusive to prokaryotes, whose members can be detected by TIGRFAMs model TIGR04178. An updated description of the superfamily occurs in PMID:22037399 (Haft, et al., 2012). The variety of archaeosortase and exosortase systems should recall the situation with sortases and their substrates, "An embarrassment of sortases - a richness of substrates?" (PMID:11239768, Pallen, et al. 2001).
The first characterized member of the extended family is archaeosortase A, ArtA, from Haloferax volcanii. Its primary target is the major cell surface glycoprotein, which forms the S-layer. This target has been a model for studying post-translational modification in the archaea. Knocking out ArtA blocks removal of the PGF-CTERM sorting signal and causes a variety of phenotypic differences include S-layer defects. See Abdul Halim, et al., 2013, PMID:23651326.
The work on archaeosortase suggests that both exosortases and archaeosortases may be transpeptidases. The S-layer glycoprotein was previously known to have a large prenyl-derived lipid, attached somewhere toward the C-terminus. Its purpose was unclear because the final C-terminal transmembrane helix seemed sufficient to anchor the protein to membrane. However, proof that the PGF-CTERM sorting signal is removed during maturation suggests that the lipid replaces it as the anchor. Transpeptidase activity would allow removal of the C-terminal sorting signal and attachment of the lipid to occur simultaneously.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Aug 23, Ben Goldacre commented:
This trial has the wrong trial registry ID associated with it on PubMed: both in the XML on PubMed, and in the body of the text of the article. The ID given is NCT00092791. We believe the correct ID, which we have found by hand searching, is NCT00092781.
This comment is being posted as part of the OpenTrials.net project<sup>[1]</sup> , an open database threading together all publicly accessible documents and data on each trial, globally. In the course of creating the database, and matching documents and data sources about trials from different locations, we have identified various anomalies in datasets such as PubMed, and in published papers. Alongside documenting the prevalence of problems, we are also attempting to correct these errors and anomalies wherever possible, by feeding back to the originators. We have corrected this data in the OpenTrials.net database; we hope that this trial’s text and metadata can also be corrected at source, in PubMed and in the accompanying paper.
Many thanks,
Jessica Fleminger, Ben Goldacre*
[1] Goldacre, B., Gray, J., 2016. OpenTrials: towards a collaborative open database of all available information on all clinical trials. Trials 17. doi:10.1186/s13063-016-1290-8
* Dr Ben Goldacre BA MA MSc MBBS MRCPsych<br> Senior Clinical Research Fellow<br> ben.goldacre@phc.ox.ac.uk<br> www.ebmDataLab.net<br> Centre for Evidence Based Medicine<br> Department of Primary Care Health Sciences<br> University of Oxford<br> Radcliffe Observatory Quarter<br> Woodstock Road<br> Oxford OX2 6GG
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Nov 23, Harri Hemila commented:
The published version is available at DOI.
A manuscript version is available at https://helda.helsinki.fi/handle/10138/228095
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2017 Jul 07, Morten Oksvold commented:
This article should have been retracted after an investigation by The University of Maryland found this article to contain "compromised" data (a total of 26 articles in 11 journals were affected). The journal Cancer Research was informed in August 2016, according to Retraction Watch.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2013 Nov 24, John Sotos commented:
The courageous effort by Emanuel to outline a new pre-medical and medical curriculum has one contradiction and two omissions.
First, after advocating a year of biochemistry in the pre-medical curriculum, he rightly states that knowledge of the Krebs cycle generally has no practical use at the bedside. This contradiction suggests that devoting a year to biochemistry is excessive.
Second, there is a fundamental, yet unspoken truth about medicine: as an intellectual endeavor, it is extremely easy. While the hard sciences require detailed understanding and nuanced application of difficult quantitative principles, medical textbooks simply demand memorization on a massive scale. One could argue that mnemonic training is the greatest omission in medical teaching and that, of all pre-medical requirements, organic chemistry is the greatest developer of memorization skills.
Finally, I wish there were some way to teach humility more effectively and more permanently. Any physician not cowed by their own ignorance should be drummed out of the profession.
(1) Emanuel EJ. Changing premed requirements and the medical curriculum. JAMA. 2006 Sep 6;296(9):1128-1131. Pubmed 16954492
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2013 Jun 13, Robert Tibshirani commented:
The interested reader should be sure to look at the published comments to this paper:
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17872427 http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17872429 http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17872428
All three groups found similar, serious flaws in the analyses
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2013 Nov 15, George McNamara commented:
For fluorescence spectra data, please see the current PubSpectra download site at
http://works.bepress.com/gmcnamara/9/
For spectra graphing web sites, please see my comment in PubMed 16969821, McNamara et al 2006 Spectral imaging microscopy web sites and data. Cytometry A 69:863-871.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2013 Nov 15, George McNamara commented:
The PubSpectra dataset of over 2,000 fluorescence spectra is now (2013) downloadable in the Excel XLSX file inside a zip archive downloadable from
http://works.bepress.com/gmcnamara/9/
The Boswell spectra graphing site described in this paper is defunct and has been replaced by Urs Utzinger and Carl Boswell's University of Arizona Spectra site
http://www.spectra.arizona.edu/
Urs has added additional spectra - especially 2-photon excitation spectra - to his web site.
Several vendors have spectral graphing sites, including (but not limited to)
http://www.semrock.com/searchlight-welcome.aspx
http://www.chroma.com/spectra-viewer
LifeTech/Invitrogen/Molecular Probes http://www.lifetechnologies.com/us/en/home/life-science/cell-analysis/labeling-chemistry/fluorescence-spectraviewer.html instructions: http://www.lifetechnologies.com/us/en/home/references/molecular-probes-the-handbook/technical-notes-and-product-highlights/using-the-fluorescence-spectraviewer.html
Leica http://www.leica-microsystems.com/fluoscout/ (filter sets are from Chroma, so may be simpler to use Chroma’s web site)
BD Biosciences http://www.bdbiosciences.com/research/multicolor/spectrum_viewer/index.jsp
Most of the confocal microscope companies have spectral viewers in their software. Zeiss ZEN acknowledges PubSpectra as the source of data.
Re-using data: This 2006 paper includes a section,
Data Is Not Copyrightable During the course of developing this data, one of us had an epiphany while reading in Lessig (18) about a U.S. Supreme Court decision: data is not subject to copyright (14). Text and commentary about Feist can be found on many legal web sites by doing a Google search. Indeed, the broad availability of the text of Supreme Court decisions is because they are not subject to copyright. The Feist decision reaffirmed the U.S. Copyright act of 1976 that "there can be no copyright in facts". The basis for the Feist decision can be found in the U.S. Constitution. 14. Feist Publications, Inc. v. Rural Tel. Serv. Co. 1991;499 U.S. 340. 18. Lessig L. The Future of Ideas. New York: Random House; 2001. p 368. For those interested in reference 17, Multi-Probe Microscopy, it is available for download at http://works.bepress.com/gmcnamara/2/
Now in 2013, I want to reinforce in this PubMed Comment, that: 1. Data is not copyrightable (in the United States). 2. I encourage re-use of PubSpectra instead of you starting from scratch. 3. If anyone wants to "take over" adding data, please go ahead and do so. I would love for someone to find money and organizational skills to set up a village in India or China - or downtown Troy NY or Detroit MI - to hire people to unscan spectra graphs, and add it to "New PubSpectra".
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Jun 15, Jorge H Ramírez commented:
Related articles:
Ramírez JH, Palacios M, Gutiérrez O. Diuretic effect of an infusion of the herbal plant, Salvia scutellarioides, in rats. Biomedica 2006;26:145–9. URL: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16929912 Full text article available in Spanish. Abstract available in English.
Ramírez JH, Palacios M, Gutiérrez O. Implementation of the isolated vascular tissue model as a device for the validation of medicinal plants: Study of the vasodilator activity of Salvia scutellarioides. Colomb. Med. 2007;38:28–39. URL: http://colombiamedica.univalle.edu.co/index.php/comedica/article/view/471/480 Full text article available in English and Spanish.
Ramírez JH, Palacios M, Ocampo HH, et al. Lack of association between blood pressure, target organ damage and retinopathy in the L-NAME rat hypertension model: Are new animal models of hypertension required?. Colomb. Med. 2006;37:328–31. URL: http://colombiamedica.univalle.edu.co/index.php/comedica/article/view/465/471 Full text article available in Spanish. Abstract available in English.
Ramírez JH, Palacios M, Gutiérrez O. Evaluation of the antihypertensive effect of Salvia scutellarioides in a rat model of hypertension. Colomb Med 2006; 37: 53-60. URL: http://colombiamedica.univalle.edu.co/index.php/comedica/article/view/412/417 Full text article available in Spanish. Abstract available in English.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2015 May 19, Stephen Maher commented:
According to Br J Radiol a retraction has been issued for this article. http://www.birpublications.org/doi/full/10.1259/bjr.20139004
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov
-
On 2016 Aug 29, Jaime A. Teixeira da Silva commented:
The abstract describes the study as being about "rude" medicinal material. What is this? Also, the results describe tentacles being found in the rhizomes. At first glance, this sounds like a paper on octopii. Unable to view the full text, and presumably the full text being in Chinese, one wonders how this type of research has the luxury of being listed in PubMed. Such sloppily (botanically) written abstract call into question the rigor of peer review and the validity - at first glance - of the findings.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2013 Oct 23, Andrew R Kniss commented:
This paper (on which I am a co-author) was an early report based solely on greenhouse and laboratory studies. Glyphosate applied to glyphosate-resistant sugarbeet increased disease under greenhouse conditions in this work. Our conclusions with respect to field management of the disease went beyond what the limited data could support. Subsequent research has shown over five field seasons, two growing regions, and 6 sugarbeet cultivars that the effect presented here is unlikely to occur in the field. Based on evidence presented in these subsequent works (outlined below), it appears that adoption of glyphosate-resistant sugarbeet is likely to have had no impact, or possibly even a positive impact, on Rhizoctonia root and crown rot management in sugarbeet in the US.
In an M.S. thesis by Youdon (http://search.proquest.com/docview/899255899), field studies using 2 sugarbeet cultivars over 3 field seasons failed to find any significant effect of glyphosate on Rhizoctonia root and crown rot in sugarbeet. Youdon's work further showed that the timing of glyphosate application in relation to disease infection had no impact on disease severity or incidence. Similar to the Larson et al. paper, glyphosate increased Rhizoctonia root and crown rot under greenhouse conditions. However, conventional sugarbeet herbicides caused greater disease than glyphosate. Therefore even if it is true that glyphosate can increase sugarbeet susceptibility to this disease, the conventional herbicides that glyphosate replaced would likely have even greater impact. A conventional herbicide control was not used in the Larson et al. paper, and therefore the conclusions were not relevant to real-world growing conditions.
In preliminary greenhouse studies similar to those conducted in the Larson paper, Barnett et al. (2012) http://www.bioone.org/doi/abs/10.1614/WS-D-11-00027.1 observed mixed results; glyphosate increased Rhizoctonia in one sugarbeet cultivar, decreased Rhizoctonia in a different cultivar, and had no effect on a third cultivar. A second set of greenhouse studies using 4 commercial sugarbeet cultivars showed no effect of glyphosate on Rhizoctonia severity. Glyphosate treatments also had no observable effect on Rhizoctonia root and crown rot in any of four sugarbeet cultivars conducted over two years in field studies.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Mar 15, Kristina Hanspers commented:
The pathway in figure 2 is available as a pathway in the Open Access Publication Collection at WikiPathways: http://wikipathways.org/index.php/Pathway:WP1591. These pathways are available for download in multiple formats and can be used for analysis and visualization in tools like PathVisio and Cytoscape.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2013 Jul 03, Jessie Tenenbaum commented:
This is such a seminal paper in this area, and Connectivity Map data has proven very valuable in many follow-on studies by other authors- a great model for how data sharing can work.
I remember being struck by this sentence when I first read this paper- "There is no standard approach for estimating the statistical significance of the connections observed." I wonder if the authors would revise that 6 years later? Have they developed, or seen work by others, any methods to advance this capabilities? Certainly this flavor of analysis has been done many times. FDR seems to be the statistic of choice, though often applied somewhat differently on a case by case basis.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2015 Jan 31, Stephen Jacobs commented:
8 Years after this publication, the prevailing autoimmune explanation of celiac disease still has not been fleshed out. We have known for 10 years that gluten fragments activate T cells, but there remains no explanation of why that should cause a localized failure of gut function (often within half an hour), nor of why that should cause substantial remodeling of the epithelium. The blocked recognition hypothesis explains the nature and timing of the celiac response to gluten in some detail, and remains consistent with newer results.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Jun 18, Madhusudana Girija Sanal commented:
This paper in Nature Genetics - 38, 1323 – 1328, 2006 is rather "unique" in its findings. I wonder someone has made similar observations/reproduced these results in the past 8 years (2014)! Even the original observations by Briggs (Phili) and King (Bronx, New York) suggest nucleus from immature (less differentiated) cells are better for SCNT compared to more mature (or differentiated) cells. In their classic experiments in frogs they used nucleus from early embryonic cells to perform SCNT. Refer Briggs Robert, and King Thomas Joseph. “The transplantation of living nuclei from blastula cells into enucleated frogs’ eggs.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 38 (1952): 455–463. However later many tried with nucleus from more mature frog cells but failed. In 1958, Gurdon (Oxford,UK), successfully cloned a frog using intact nuclei from the somatic cells (gut epitheial cells) of a tadpole and won the Nobel prize together with Yamanaka. These experiments are well documented and part of the history. Going by logic one would think, reprogramming an immature cell is easier compared to a differentiated and mature cell which has undergone extensive epigenetic rearrangements and condensation of chromosomes. It is also reported that generating iPSC or embryonic cells through SCNT from older adults is more difficult compared to infants or children. Moreover, isolation, purification of haematopoietic stem cells by FACS or Magnetic beads (involving seven markers) might have damaged these cells more compared to granulocytes which were isolated by anti-Gr-1 and scatter. The abundance and ease of isolation of granulocytes might have contributed to this observation which was interpreted by the authors as suggested by the title "Differentiated cells are more efficient than adult stem cells for cloning by somatic cell nuclear transfer"!
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Mar 27, Tom Kindlon commented:
Additional information on/context for the walking test results (from the full text):
The abstract doesn't put the walking test results in context.
In the full text, the authors say:
"The ISWT, used as a physical performance measure, has normative reference data described by Taylor and colleagues.7 Their sample of 122 healthy subjects (mixed gender and age) walked a mean of 67 × 10-m shuttles."
Contrast this figure of 67 with the results achieved:
The CBT group started at: 24.3. After six months, were at 28.5 and at 12 months were 28.9.
The changes are thus minimal compared to normal functioning.
The full text also refers to outliers being excluded:
"Five clear outlying observations were omitted from the analysis of shuttles walked. Three were very low values (0 or 2) and two were amongst the highest values (60 and 75), but were from a patient with a low baseline score (9). If these outliers were retained, the SEs increased and difference between CBT and SMC was no longer statistically significant (p = 0.17)."
Whatever about excluding the low values, I question excluding the values of 60 and 75 which are the type of scores one sees in the normal population. It is not widely accepted that nobody with CFS ever gets back to normal functioning (I don't even think the authors believe this either). It is understandable to exclude scores that don't look like they represent the real values, but there is no evidence that the scores of 60 and 75 are anything other than the real, or valid, values.
Also, the wording, "two were amongst the highest values", suggests that there were other high values but these weren't excluded.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2013 Oct 24, Alexander J. Stein commented:
.<br> A subsequent, longer and more detailed article on the potential impact and cost-effectiveness of Golden Rice has been published in a journal that is not included in the PubMed database because it has more of a development focus:
- Stein A.J., Sachdev H.P.S., Qaim M. 2008. Genetic engineering for the poor: Golden Rice and public health in India.<br> World Development 36 (1): 144-158. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.worlddev.2007.02.013
Similarly, another article that is not listed discusses the value of the health benefits of Golden Rice vs. the value of agronomic benefits of other GM rice in the R&D pipeline:
- Demont M., Stein A.J. 2013. Global value of GM rice: a review of expected agronomic and consumer benefits.<br> New Biotechnology 30 (5): 426-436. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nbt.2013.04.004<br> .
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Mar 15, George McNamara commented:
The Gu et al correction is about details in the FLIPPi-260n sensor. Corrected sequences are available at http://www.addgene.org/13552/ FLIPPi-260n http://www.addgene.org/13556/ FLIPPi-30m and entries in between.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Nov 18, Morten Oksvold commented:
An investigation committee at Wayne State University (WSU) recommends that 42 articles from Fazlul Sarkar to be retracted (report finished August 31, 2015). This article represents one of them.
This information was published by Retraction Watch (November 17, 2016) and you can find a link to the full report here:
http://retractionwatch.com/2016/11/17/details-of-investigative-report-into-sarkar-released-by-aclu/
This article should therefore no longer be cited.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov
-
On 2016 Aug 23, Ben Goldacre commented:
This trial has the wrong trial registry ID associated with it on PubMed: both in the XML on PubMed, and in the body of the text of the article. The ID given is NCT001168532. We believe the correct ID, which we have found by hand searching, is NCT00168532.
This comment is being posted as part of the OpenTrials.net project<sup>[1]</sup> , an open database threading together all publicly accessible documents and data on each trial, globally. In the course of creating the database, and matching documents and data sources about trials from different locations, we have identified various anomalies in datasets such as PubMed, and in published papers. Alongside documenting the prevalence of problems, we are also attempting to correct these errors and anomalies wherever possible, by feeding back to the originators. We have corrected this data in the OpenTrials.net database; we hope that this trial’s text and metadata can also be corrected at source, in PubMed and in the accompanying paper.
Many thanks,
Jessica Fleminger, Ben Goldacre*
[1] Goldacre, B., Gray, J., 2016. OpenTrials: towards a collaborative open database of all available information on all clinical trials. Trials 17. doi:10.1186/s13063-016-1290-8
* Dr Ben Goldacre BA MA MSc MBBS MRCPsych<br> Senior Clinical Research Fellow<br> ben.goldacre@phc.ox.ac.uk<br> www.ebmDataLab.net<br> Centre for Evidence Based Medicine<br> Department of Primary Care Health Sciences<br> University of Oxford<br> Radcliffe Observatory Quarter<br> Woodstock Road<br> Oxford OX2 6GG
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov
-
On 2016 Aug 23, Ben Goldacre commented:
This trial has the wrong trial registry ID associated with it on PubMed: both in the XML on PubMed, and in the originating journal article. The ID given is NCT0033272. We believe the correct ID, which we have found by hand searching, is NCT00332722.
This comment is being posted as part of the OpenTrials.net project<sup>[1]</sup> , an open database threading together all publicly accessible documents and data on each trial, globally. In the course of creating the database, and matching documents and data sources about trials from different locations, we have identified various anomalies in datasets such as PubMed, and in published papers. Alongside documenting the prevalence of problems, we are also attempting to correct these errors and anomalies wherever possible, by feeding back to the originators. We have corrected this data in the OpenTrials.net database; we hope that this trial’s text and metadata can also be corrected at source, in PubMed and in the accompanying paper.
Many thanks,
Jessica Fleminger, Ben Goldacre*
[1] Goldacre, B., Gray, J., 2016. OpenTrials: towards a collaborative open database of all available information on all clinical trials. Trials 17. doi:10.1186/s13063-016-1290-8 PMID: 27056367
* Dr Ben Goldacre BA MA MSc MBBS MRCPsych<br> Senior Clinical Research Fellow<br> ben.goldacre@phc.ox.ac.uk<br> www.ebmDataLab.net<br> Centre for Evidence Based Medicine<br> Department of Primary Care Health Sciences<br> University of Oxford<br> Radcliffe Observatory Quarter<br> Woodstock Road<br> Oxford OX2 6GG
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY. -
On 2016 Sep 19, Laxmaiah Manchikanti commented:
Dr Ben Goldacre BA MA MSc MBBS MRCPsych Senior Clinical Research Fellow ben.goldacre@phc.ox.ac.uk www.ebmDataLab.net Centre for Evidence Based Medicine Department of Primary Care Health Sciences University of Oxford Radcliffe Observatory Quarter Woodstock Road Oxford OX2 6GG
Dr. Goldacre:
Thank you so much for bringing this error to our attention. You are absolutely correct in your research to assess the correct National Clinical Trial number for our research.
The proper number is NCT00332722.
We have immediately corrected the open access version of this manuscript available online. The pdf available on the Pain Physician site now reflects the correct Clinical Trial numbers. We are also checking all our manuscript for potential errors.
We will run an erratum in the next issue of Pain Physician journal and will also ask that the error be corrected on PubMed.
Again, we appreciate your taking the time to point out this error to us.
Regards,
Laxmaiah Manchikanti, MD Chairman of the Board and Chief Executive Officer, ASIPP and SIPMS Medical Director, Pain Management Center of Paducah Clinical Professor Anesthesiology and Perioperative Medicine University of Louisville, Kentucky 67 Lakeview Drive Paducah, KY 42001 E-mail: drm@asipp.org
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Sep 10, Gerard Ridgway commented:
This is one of two papers that independently introduced the concept of voxel-specific design matrices to neuroimaging, for the purpose of multi-modal analysis. The other is Oakes TR, 2007.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Nov 20, Morten Oksvold commented:
An investigation committee at Wayne State University (WSU) recommends that 42 articles from Fazlul Sarkar to be retracted (report finished August 31, 2015). This article represents one of them.
This information was published by Retraction Watch (November 17, 2016) and you can find a link to the full report here:
http://retractionwatch.com/2016/11/17/details-of-investigative-report-into-sarkar-released-by-aclu/
This article should therefore no longer be cited.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Feb 02, Jan Tunér commented:
It is interesting to note that the authors themselves state the origin of "burning mouth" as multifactor, psychosomatic or psychogenic. Thus, there would be no actual injury to the tissue, and laser, like any other modality, would have no effect. The negative 5-HIAA measurement in the laser group may indeed confirm the inappropriate inclusion parameters and laser energies in this study.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov
-
On 2014 Apr 29, Richard Jahan-Tigh commented:
I think instead of a clear-cell acanthoma, this is actually a verruciform xanthoma. There are abundant lipid-laden macrophages in Figures 7 and 8. They are known to occur in the groin, and it has the correct clinical appearance.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2015 Jun 02, thomas samaras commented:
Additional information is available on height, CHD, and longevity from these publications.
Samaras TT. Shorter height is related to lower cardiovascular disease risk—A narrative review. Indian Heart Journal 2013; 65: 66-71.
Samaras, TT. Is short height really a risk factor for coronary heart disease and stroke mortality? A review. Med Sci Monit 2004; 10(4): RA63-76.
Samaras TT. Evidence from eight different types of studies showing that smaller body size is related to greater longevity. Journal of Scientific Research & Reports. 2014: 3 (16): 2150-2160, 2014; article no. JSRR.2014.16.003.
Samaras TT. Human Scaling and Body Mass Index. In: Samaras TT (ed): Human Body Size and the Laws of Scaling: Physiological Performance, Growth, Longevity and Ecological Ramifications. New York: Nova Science Pub; 2007: pp 17-32
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Dec 10, Kath Wright commented:
Other search filters are available from the InterTASC Information Specialists' Sub-Group Search Filter Resource at https://sites.google.com/a/york.ac.uk/issg-search-filters-resource/home
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov
-
On 2014 Jan 09, Tom Kindlon commented:
Accumulating evidence that CFS patients were actually more active on average than controls before becoming ill
(This is a comment originally posted in 2008. However, the change meaning it's not formatted on the site http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-244X/6/53/comments means few will read it there. Also, many may not see it there)
The main finding in this study is that "patients with chronic, unexplained fatigue rated themselves as more active before their illness (p < 0.001)" and that "these differences remained significant for the subset of patients who met strict criteria for chronic fatigue syndrome or fibromyalgia"[1]. The authors then spend a lot of time speculating about whether this could have been an "overestimation of previous activity" and giving the "altered perception" hypothesis.
Given that they only had self-report data to go on, it is interesting to read a recent prospective population study on the illness[2]. It followed 4779 people from birth for the first 53 years of their lives. At age 53, 34 reported a diagnosis of CFS. Amongst other things, it found that "increased levels of exercise throughout childhood and early adult life and a lower body mass index were associated with an increased risk of later CFS." As it was a prospective study, there was no issue of recall bias. It also wasn't simply self-rated, as it also involved reporting by a teacher at age 13. Also they used the subject's BMI index - patients who went on to have CFS at age 53 had a (statisically significant) lower BMI than those who did not go on to develop CFS at ages 36 and 43 (before they had CFS). The authors say this "this may provide some indirect but objective evidence of increased levels of activity at these ages, especially as this difference had resolved by the age of 53 years" (when the people with CFS were no longer more active).
In the current study[1], the authors say "the only prospective cohort study of risk factors for CFS found that sedentary behavior at 10 years of age doubled the risk of self-reported CFS in adulthood"[3]. I thought I would give more information on that finding as it only related to a small percentage of the "CFS/ME" patients and doesn't show that on average patients were more inactive: in response to the question, about the amount of sport played in spare time at 10 years, 16% of the people who ever had CFS/ME by age 30 were in the "never or hardly ever" category compared to 8% in the people who didn't go on to have CFS/ME (so 8% more than expected - but it still meant that 84% in the "sometimes" and "often" categories). However the authors of the study[3] didn't combine this data in some way with a question on sports played within school: for the category "Played >2 hours/week of sport at school at 10 years", 52% of those who ever had CFS/ME by age 30 were in this category compared to 44% of those who never had CFS/ME by age 30. The unadjusted OR for this at 1.4 nearly reached statistically significance (95% confidence intervals: 0.9 to 2.2). Some sort of combination of the two pieces of data would have been preferable - a person's body doesn't distinguish between activity done within schools hours and in their spare time.
In the current study[1], the authors point out that their "findings are congruent with those of 3 retrospective studies reporting that CFS patients perceived themselves as more active before their illness began than healthy controls"[4-6]. They also said that "the high levels of physical activity reported by patients have been corroborated by their spouses, partners, or parents."[7]
With all this evidence from various sources about pre-morbid levels, researchers perhaps need to start showing more evidence before they will convince many of us of any speculative theories about CFS patients misperceiving how much activity and exercise they used to do.
References:
[1] Smith WR, White PD, Buchwald D: A case control study of premorbid andcurrently reported physical activity levels in chronic fatigue syndrome. BMCPsychiatry 2006, 6:53. http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-244X/6/53
[2] Harvey SB, Wadsworth M, Wessely S, Hotopf M: Etiology of Chronic FatigueSyndrome: Testing Popular HypothesesUsing a National Birth Cohort Study. Psychosom Med. 2008 Mar 31
[3] Viner R, Hotopf M: Childhood predictors of self reported chronic fatiguesyndrome/myalgic encephalomyelitis in adults: national birth cohort study.BMJ 2004, 329:941. http://www.biomedcentral.com/pubmed/15469945
[4] Riley MS, O'Brien CJ, McCluskey DR, Bell NP, Nicholls DP: Aerobic workcapacity in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome. BMJ 1990, 301:953-6.
[5] Van Houdenhove B, Onghena P, Neerinckx E, Hellin J: Does high"action-proneness" make people more vulnerable to chronic fatigue syndrome?A controlled psychometric study. J Psychosom Res 1995, 39:633-40.
[6] MacDonald KL, Osterholm MT, LeDell KH, White KE, Schenck CH, Chao CC,Persing DH, Johnson RC, Barker JM, Peterson PK: A case-control study toassess possible triggers and cofactors in chronic fatigue syndrome. Am J Med1996, 100:548-54.
[7] Van Houdenhove B, Neerinckx E, Onghena P, Lysens R, Vertommnen H:Premorbid "overactive" lifestyle in chronic fatigue syndrome andfibromyalgia: an etiological relationship or proof of good citizenship? JPsychosom Res 2001, 51:571-6.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov
-
On 2016 Jul 08, Daniel Haft commented:
The novel macrolide 2'-phosphotransferase introduced in this paper, CAJ98570.1, called mph(E) here, has been renamed mph(F). The name mph(E) has been assigned to ABI20451.1 (see PMID:17261525). The revised nomenclature for macrolide phosphotransferases can be found in table 4 of PMID:23162539, by Roberts, et al.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Dec 10, Kath Wright commented:
Other search filters are available from the InterTASC Information Specialists' Sub-Group Search Filter Resource at https://sites.google.com/a/york.ac.uk/issg-search-filters-resource/home
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Mar 29, Jonathan Wren commented:
Appears to be relocated: http://carolina.imis.athena-innovation.gr/index.php?r=mirgenv3
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Jan 08, Tom Kindlon commented:
New or "Unusual" definition for CFS used in this study
(This was originally posted on the journal's website here: http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-2377/6/41/comments. However, the formatting has been removed meaning few may read it there)
People reading this study need to be aware that it uses a new or "unusual" definition of Chronic Fatigue Syndrome (CFS)[1] so the results may not apply to CFS cohorts as usually defined[2].This definition selects a group covering 2.54% of the adult population[3]. This is much higher than previous estimates of the prevalence of CFS. For example, members of the team in this study have previously estimated the prevalence as 0.235%[4] i.e. the prevalence rate using this definition is 10.8 times the rate found using the more usual CFS definition[2].
There has been some criticism of this new definition[5]. Unlike previous times when the CDC produced definitions for CFS[2,6], the definition used in this study is generally only being used by the CDC-funded CFS research team.
References:
[1] Reeves WC, Wagner D, Nisenbaum R, Jones JF, Gurbaxani B, Solomon L, Papanicolaou DA, Unger ER, Vernon SD, Heim C: Chronic Fatigue Syndrome – A clinically empirical approach to its definition and study. BMC Medicine 2005, 3:19 (15 December 2005)
[2] Fukuda, K., Straus, S.E., Hickie, I., Sharpe, M.C., Dobbins, J.G., & Komaroff, A. (1994). The chronic fatigue syndrome: A comprehensive approach to its definition and study. Annals of Internal Medicine, 121 (12):953-959. http://www.annals.org/cgi/content/full/121/12/953
[3] Reeves WC, Jones JF, Maloney E, Heim C, Hoaglin DC, Boneva RS, Morrissey M, Devlin RPrevalence of chronic fatigue syndrome in metropolitan, urban, and rural Georgia. Population Health Metrics 2007, 5:5 (8 June 2007)
[4] Reyes M, Nisenbaum R, Hoaglin DC, Unger ER, Emmons C, Randall B, Stewart JA, Abbey S, Jones JF, Gantz N, Minden S, Reeves WC: Prevalence and incidence of chronic fatigue syndrome in Wichita, Kansas. Arch Int Med 2003, 163:1530-1536.
[5] Jason Leonard: Issues with CDC Empirical Case Definition and Prevalence of CFS. IACFS website http://tinyurl.com/2qdgu4 i.e. http://www.iacfsme.org/IssueswithCDCEmpiricalCaseDefinitionandPrev/tabid/105/Default.aspx
[6] Holmes GP, Kaplan JE, Gantz NM, Komaroff AL, Schonberger LB, Straus SE, et al. Chronic fatigue syndrome: a working case definition. Ann Intern Med. 1988; 108:387-9.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Jan 25, Dylmitr Rittoo commented:
Potentially interesting study. Need to know exactly which type of cardiac Troponin was compared with CK-MB.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Sep 10, Gerard Ridgway commented:
This is one of two papers that independently introduced the concept of voxel-specific design matrices to neuroimaging for the purpose of multi-modal analysis. The other is Casanova R, 2007.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Nov 20, Morten Oksvold commented:
An investigation committee at Wayne State University (WSU) recommends that 42 articles from Fazlul Sarkar to be retracted (report finished August 31, 2015). This article represents one of them.
This information was published by Retraction Watch (November 17, 2016) and you can find a link to the full report here:
http://retractionwatch.com/2016/11/17/details-of-investigative-report-into-sarkar-released-by-aclu/
This article should therefore no longer be cited.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov
-
On 2014 Jan 10, Helmi BEN SAAD commented:
None
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY. -
On 2014 Jan 10, Helmi BEN SAAD commented:
The second author name is "BEN SAAD H" not "BENSAAD H"
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Aug 19, Morten Oksvold commented:
Please note that this article has been retracted and should therefore not be cited in the future.
http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ijc.30269/full
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2017 Jun 15, Ahmet Erdemir commented:
Data on elastomeric foams investigated as part of this study are now available at https://simtk.org/projects/elastomericfoam
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Nov 23, Harri Hemila commented:
A comment on the paper is available at: http://hdl.handle.net/10250/135144
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2013 Nov 07, Pierre Lindenbaum commented:
!DEPRECATED http://research.imb.uq.edu.au/rnadb/ "In June 2012, the RNAdb 2.0 database was officially retired"
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2017 Aug 17, Randy Blakely commented:
For those citing this paper, please note that the name of Susan L Andersen was misspelled. Please change this in your citation managers
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov
-
On 2017 Jun 28, Daniel Corcos commented:
The discrepancy may be related to the fact that women in the screened arm who have dropped out the screening are not willing to report their cancer because they think they have broken the trial.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2017 Mar 23, Daniel Haft commented:
Note that this paper is about YdgR. Several mentions of YgdR (a lipoprotein, not a transporter) appear to represent typographical errors.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov
-
On 2014 Feb 04, Martine Crasnier-Mednansky commented:
Comment concerning the AUTHOR’S CORRECTION:
In 1942, Jacques Monod indeed reported that associating any compound A (Glucose, Mannose, Fructose, Mannitol) with any compound B (the 'less preferred' carbon source) invariably resulted in diauxic growth with Bacillus subtilis. With Escherichia coli, he reported a diauxic growth with Glucose or Mannitol in combination with the 'less preferred' carbon source, but not with Fructose or Mannose. Therefore, Monod labeled Fructose and Mannose as 'indifférents' sugars for E. coli.
However, in his 1947 publication entitled 'The phenomenon of enzymatic adaptation' Monod emphasized what was true for B. subtilis was "equally true with many Escherichia coli strains, although with others, Fructose and Mannose are often found not to produce diauxic growth in any combination."
Researchers should therefore be aware that with E. coli production of diauxie is most likely strain-dependent when Fructose or Mannose is used in combination with a 'less preferred carbon source'. The strains used by the present reviewer did not produced diauxie with Fructose and a 'less preferred carbon source' (in agreement with Monod 1942 publication) but diauxie was observed with fruR strains (lacking the fructose repressor) in the presence of Fructose and a 'less preferred carbon source'. Therefore the widely used E. coli strain MC4100 which has been reported to be disrupted for fruR Ferenci T, 2009 should exhibit diauxie with Fructose and any compound B.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2018 Jan 31, Denise N Slenter commented:
The pathway outlined in Figure 2, is available as free machine readable data in WP4210 in WikiPathways: https://www.wikipathways.org/index.php/Pathway:WP4210 . This pathway can be used for data analysis with e.g. Pathvisio and Cytoscape!
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Dec 10, Kath Wright commented:
Other search filters are available from the InterTASC Information Specialists' Sub-Group Search Filter Resource at https://sites.google.com/a/york.ac.uk/issg-search-filters-resource/home
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2015 Jul 16, Md. Shahidul Islam commented:
An important issue is whether TRPV1(+) pancreatic sensory neurons are present in the human islets. Immunohistochemistry of human islets and human insulinomas does not show presence of any TRPV1 positive cells in these tissues.
Islets. 2012 Jan-Feb;4(1):56-63. doi: 10.4161/isl.18915. Epub 2012 Jan 1. Insulin-secreting INS-1E cells express functional TRPV1 channels. Fågelskiöld AJ, Kannisto K, Boström A, Hadrovic B, Farre C, Eweida M, Wester K, Islam MS.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov
-
On 2014 Mar 26, Tom Kindlon commented:
I posted some comments on, and extracts from, this paper here: http://forums.phoenixrising.me/index.php?threads/managing-chronic-fatigue-syndrome-in-u-k-primary-care-challenges-and-opportunities-wearden-06.29212/#post-445534
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Dec 10, Kath Wright commented:
Other search filters are available from the InterTASC Information Specialists' Sub-Group Search Filter Resource at https://sites.google.com/a/york.ac.uk/issg-search-filters-resource/home
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Dec 10, Kath Wright commented:
Other search filters are available from the InterTASC Information Specialists' Sub-Group Search Filter Resource at https://sites.google.com/a/york.ac.uk/issg-search-filters-resource/home
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Aug 23, Ben Goldacre commented:
This trial has the wrong trial registry ID associated with it on PubMed: both in the XML on PubMed, and in the originating journal article. The ID given is NCT002770192. We believe the correct ID, which we have found by hand searching, is NCT00270192.
This comment is being posted as part of the OpenTrials.net project<sup>[1]</sup> , an open database threading together all publicly accessible documents and data on each trial, globally. In the course of creating the database, and matching documents and data sources about trials from different locations, we have identified various anomalies in datasets such as PubMed, and in published papers. Alongside documenting the prevalence of problems, we are also attempting to correct these errors and anomalies wherever possible, by feeding back to the originators. We have corrected this data in the OpenTrials.net database; we hope that this trial’s text and metadata can also be corrected at source, in PubMed and in the accompanying paper.
Many thanks,
Jessica Fleminger, Ben Goldacre*
[1] Goldacre, B., Gray, J., 2016. OpenTrials: towards a collaborative open database of all available information on all clinical trials. Trials 17. doi:10.1186/s13063-016-1290-8 PMID: 27056367
* Dr Ben Goldacre BA MA MSc MBBS MRCPsych<br> Senior Clinical Research Fellow<br> ben.goldacre@phc.ox.ac.uk<br> www.ebmDataLab.net<br> Centre for Evidence Based Medicine<br> Department of Primary Care Health Sciences<br> University of Oxford<br> Radcliffe Observatory Quarter<br> Woodstock Road<br> Oxford OX2 6GG
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Mar 16, Levi Waldron commented:
This study is now literally a textbook example of confounding between batches and the primary observational unit: http://genomicsclass.github.io/book/pages/confounding.html
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Mar 28, Michelle Kraft commented:
https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8821-4828 Other medical library articles by this author
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Dec 10, Kath Wright commented:
Other search filters are available from the InterTASC Information Specialists' Sub-Group Search Filter Resource at https://sites.google.com/a/york.ac.uk/issg-search-filters-resource/home
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov
-
On 2014 Jan 08, Brett Snodgrass commented:
Dear Author,
Thank you for the excellent article. Naming this is the "modified Nail Psoriasis Severity Index" when there was already a published "modified Nail Psoriasis Severity Index," may cause some confusion.
The modified NAPSI was published in 2005. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16198816/
In contrast to your scale, the "2005, mNAPSI" was not validated.
Perhaps referring to your article as the "2007, mNAPSI" may reduce some confusion...
Comments and suggestions are welcome.
Thank you kindly.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Nov 23, Harri Hemila commented:
A manuscript version is available at handle
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov
-
On 2017 Nov 10, Anne Niknejad commented:
GenBank reference (fig.1) for Bacillus subtilis YcsK (LipC) should be BSU04110, not BSU041110
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2017 Jan 01, Donald Forsdyke commented:
PURINE LOADING AS A THERMAL ADAPTATION (This comment was original posted as a reader response to the original PLOS CompBiol article 20 Feb 2008.)
This paper draws conclusions tending to oppose those of myself and coworkers (cited). A "key question" is held to be: "Which factor - amino acid or nucleotide composition - is primary in thermal adaptation and which is derivative?" Previous evidence is considered "anecdotal." Now there is evidence for "an exact and conclusive" relationship, based on an "exhaustive study" that provides a "complete picture." A set of amino acids - IVYWREL - correlates well with growth temperature. It is noted:
"Signatures of thermal adaptation in protein sequences can be due to the specific biases in nucleotide sequences and vice versa. ... One has to explore whether a specific composition of nucleotide (amino acid) sequences shapes the content of amino acid (nucleotide) ones, or thermal adaptation of proteins and DNA (at the level of sequence compositions) are independent processes."
In other words, are primary adaptations at the nucleic acid level driving changes at the protein level, or vice- versa? To what extent are the two processes independent? Their conclusion:
"Resolving the old-standing controversy, we determined that the variation in nucleotide composition (increase of purine-load, or A + G content with temperature) is largely a consequence of thermal adaptation of proteins."
Thus, the superficial reader of the paper, while noting the purine-richness of some of the codons corresponding to the IVYWREL amino acids, will conclude that the "independent processes" alternative has been excluded. Reading the paper (e.g. Figure 7) one can question the validity of this conclusion. Many of the IVYWREL amino acids have purine-poor alternative codons (especially IYLV, which at best can only change one purine unit in their codons). One of the IVYWREL amino acids has relatively purine-rich alternative codons (R, which at best can change two purine units). Two (EW) are always purine-rich, and there are no alternatives.
Displaying more EW's as the temperature got hotter would satisfy a need both for more purines and for more tryptophan and glutamate, so here there is no discrimination as to whether one "shapes" the organism’s content of the other. Displaying more IYLVs gives only minimal flexibility in accommodating a purine-need. Most flexibility is provided by R codons.
The authors do not give statistics for the differences between the slopes of Figs. 7a (unshuffled codons) and 7b (shuffled codons), but they appear real, presumably reflecting the choice biologically of purine-rich codons, a choice the organisms might not have to make if there were no independent purine-loading pressure. Thus, the authors note, but only in parenthesis, that the slopes "are somewhat different suggesting that codon bias may be partly responsible for the overall purine composition of DNA."
An analogy may help. Passing from winter to summer, you change your outdoor attire. Many people (Imelda Marcos excepted) have a wider range of shirts and sweaters than of footwear. There may be items which provide more reliable indices of outside temperature than others. For example, the weight of your shirt + sweater might more finely correlate with temperature than the weight of your shoes. If you wanted to predict outside temperature from attire, you would choose shirt-sweater weights, rather than shoe weights. But if your clothes (shirt + sweater) weigh more, you might need sturdier shoes to cope with the extra weight. Thus, shoe-weight would depend on clothes-weight. The latter would be primary and the former would be secondary ("derivative"). But showing a better correlation with temperature in the case of shirt + sweater, does not establish a dependence of shoe-weight on shirt-sweater weight. More likely, you wear lighter shoes in warm weather because they keep your feet cooler! Both depend on temperature. It just so happens that the correlation with temperature is coarser and not so finely tuned for shoe weight, as for shirt-sweater weight.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Nov 17, Raphael Levy commented:
There is a detailed discussion of this paper at PubPeer, including the publication of technical comment that was considered, but not published by Science.
More broadly, the evidence behind the structure and special properties of “striped” nanoparticles has been challenged by Cesbron Y, 2012. The publication in 2012 of Cesbron Y, 2012 took three years and has been followed by post-publication peer review of the various existing and new stripy articles on my blog, PubPeer, etc.
A detailed analysis of this body of work is published today in PloS One by Stirling et al; from the abstract: “through a combination of an exhaustive re-analysis of the original data with new experimental measurements of a simple control sample comprising entirely unfunctionalised particles, we conclusively show that all of the STM evidence for striped nanoparticles published to date can instead be explained by a combination of well-known instrumental artefacts, strong observer bias, and/or improper data acquisition/analysis protocols.”
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2015 May 02, Martine Crasnier-Mednansky commented:
The role of cAMP in the glucose-acetate diauxie was not taken into account by the authors however if it is taken into account, interesting possibilities arise.
Acetate utilization by the fast-switchers is suppressed during growth on glucose as shown by a biphasic growth curve indicating preferential use of glucose over acetate (Figure 1A, blue circles), and a nearly constant concentration of acetate in the medium to the switch point (Figure 1C, blue circles). In contrast, an extended diauxic lag is observed with the slow-switchers (Figure 1A, red circles). Both switchers however show identical growth rate during the first growth phase (red and blue circles in Figure 1A), same switching point in time, and same glucose use (red and blue circles in Figure 1B).
A diauxic lag disappearance and biphasic growth are typically associated with addition of cAMP to the growth medium (Ullmann A, 1968). It was established that, during a downshift from glucose to acetate, the CRP-cAMP complex peaked during the first hour of transition, which correlated with an increase in cAMP (Kao KC, 2004). Therefore addition of cAMP to a typical glucose-acetate diauxie most likely will affect the diauxic lag. Moreover, a cAMP-dependent regulation of the aceBAK operon was previously inferred by the presence of CRP-binding sites within the operon and a strong glucose repression, as indicated by transcriptome data (Table 1 in Zhang Z, 2005).
So, what if the fast-switchers are no longer 'dependent' on the cAMP signal upon entry in the second phase of growth on acetate (in other words there is no need for CRP-cAMP in the absence of IclR for the transcription of the aceBAK operon). It is then expected that the fast-switchers with an 'ancestral' iclR will exhibit a typical diauxic growth, which is what is observed (Figure 4A).
As regards the slow-switchers, their growth pattern does not significantly vary from the growth pattern of the ancestor. They both resume growth on acetate after an extended lag, but very poorly as compared to the fast-switchers. It is therefore not surprising that introduction of the iclR<sup>IS1</sup> in the ancestor bares no consequences on the growth pattern.
A final note; micromolar concentrations of glucose were used for growth in the presence of both glucose and acetate (as indicated in M&M under Growth curve assay). The increased concentration of acetate in the medium from the slow-switchers (Figure 1C, red circles) should not occur at glucose concentrations used in Figure 1A as dissimilation of acetate occurs under aerobiosis in excess glucose.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Feb 23, Farrel Buchinsky commented:
An interesting prospective cohort. Would like to find studies where there is randomization and a control group.
Looks as if my answer is in Guilleminault C, 2008
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Mar 29, Jonathan Wren commented:
URL relocated to: http://bioinf.gen.tcd.ie/casbah/
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Nov 23, Harri Hemila commented:
A manuscript version is available at handle
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Aug 30, Ben Goldacre commented:
This trial has the wrong trial registry ID associated with it on PubMed: both in the XML on PubMed, and in the originating journal article. The ID given is NCT0014491. We believe the correct ID, which we have found in the text in an embedded link, is NCT00114491.
This comment is being posted as part of the OpenTrials.net project<sup>[1]</sup> , an open database threading together all publicly accessible documents and data on each trial, globally. In the course of creating the database, and matching documents and data sources about trials from different locations, we have identified various anomalies in datasets such as PubMed, and in published papers. Alongside documenting the prevalence of problems, we are also attempting to correct these errors and anomalies wherever possible, by feeding back to the originators. We have corrected this data in the OpenTrials.net database; we hope that this trial’s text and metadata can also be corrected at source, in PubMed and in the accompanying paper.
Many thanks,
Jessica Fleminger, Ben Goldacre*
[1] Goldacre, B., Gray, J., 2016. OpenTrials: towards a collaborative open database of all available information on all clinical trials. Trials 17. doi:10.1186/s13063-016-1290-8 PMID: 27056367
* Dr Ben Goldacre BA MA MSc MBBS MRCPsych<br> Senior Clinical Research Fellow<br> ben.goldacre@phc.ox.ac.uk<br> www.ebmDataLab.net<br> Centre for Evidence Based Medicine<br> Department of Primary Care Health Sciences<br> University of Oxford<br> Radcliffe Observatory Quarter<br> Woodstock Road<br> Oxford OX2 6GG
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY. -
On 2016 Sep 07, Nalini M Rajamannan commented:
Thank you to open trials.net, we are grateful for your stringent checking of clinical trials.gov registration number. You are correct the exact registration number is NCT0014491.
The number 1 in the middle of the registration was missing in the final publication,
The typo was brought to our attention, and we did request for the final number 1 to be added to the publication, but the time to correct the typo was not possible due to the date of the original publication in 2007, according to the editorial office.
We agree that the discovery of the published NCT number: "The ID given is NCT0014491. We believe the correct ID, which we have found in the text in an embedded link, is NCT00114491."
On behalf of the RAAVE investigators, we are grateful for your diligent work and the correction of our registration in Clinicaltrials.gov as listed in PubMed Commons.
Sincerely
Nalini M. Rajamannan, MD
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2015 Mar 08, Jacob H. Hanna commented:
This Kaji et al. 2007 paper claims that it is absolutely impossible to derive Mbd3-/- ESCs from Mbd3 null E3.5 embryos. This result is surprising considering that the authors show in the same paper presence of Oct4+/Nanog+ cells in Mbd3-/- ICMs. Further, Mbd3-/- ESCs are "hyper-naive" and resist differentiation even in the absence of LIF (Kaji et al. Nature Cell Biology 2006, Reynolds et al. Cell Stem Cell 2012). Our group has revisited this result in Rais et al. Nature 2013, and was able to efficiently derive Mbd3-/- ESCs in serum free enriched 2i/LIF conditions. This suggests that the main conclusion of Kaji et al. 2007 Development paper titled "Mbd3 is required for development of pluripotent cells", is invalid and constitutes an artifact likely due to using a low quality fetal bovine serum (FBS) batch.
Jacob (Yaqub) Hanna M.D. Ph.D.
Department of Molecular Genetics
Weizmann Institute of Science | 234 Herzl St, Rehovot 7610001, Israel
Email: jacob.hanna@weizmann.ac.il
Lab website: http://hannalabweb.weizmann.ac.il/
Twitter: @Jacob_Hanna
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY. -
On 2016 Jan 23, Jacob H. Hanna commented:
After posting our comment and critiques below (http://1.usa.gov/1HeRjpX), we have noted a similar doubt was raised by Dr. Silva and Sr. Smith in 2007 in their Essay titled: "Capturing Pluripotency" Cell 2007 - http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0092867408002079 Silva J, 2008
"A critical test is whether repair of a gene defect is sufficient to restore differentiation capability. For example, ES cells in which the NuRD repressor complex is inactivated by deletion of the Mbd3 subunit are compromised in prosecuting differentiation, but this defect is eliminated upon re-expression of Mbd3, meaning that the pluripotent state has been preserved throughout ( Kaji et al., 2006). A specific requirement in maintaining the pluripotent state should only be claimed when a change in developmental potential is irreversible, as for example when Oct4 or Sox2 is deleted (Niwa, 2007). Interestingly, Mbd3 appears necessary for derivation of ES cells, but this is because in its absence the epiblast does not form properly (Kaji et al., 2007) and not because Mbd3 is required by ES cells."
Regarding the last sentence, we now know that Mbd3 is NOT necessary for deriving mouse Mbd3 null ESCs from KO mouse ICMs that retain Nanog and Oct4 expression in vivo (Rais et al. Nature 2013).
Jacob (Yaqub) Hanna M.D. Ph.D.
Department of Molecular Genetics
Weizmann Institute of Science | 234 Herzl St, Rehovot 7610001, Israel
Email: jacob.hanna@weizmann.ac.il
Lab website: http://hannalabweb.weizmann.ac.il/
Twitter: @Jacob_Hanna
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY. -
On 2016 Jan 25, Brian Hendrich commented:
Our paper describes the function of Mbd3/NuRD in pluripotent cells in the early mouse embryo. Whether one can derive ES cells from Mbd3 null blastocysts is not relevant to the message of this paper.
Figure 4 shows that Mbd3-/- ICMs taken from C57Bl/6 mouse embryos did not proliferate when cultured in Serum/LIF conditions. This paper predates the advent of 2i culture media, reported by Ying et al. 2008, and therefore the possibility that Mbd3-/- ES cells can be derived in 2i/LIF conditions is not inconsistent with what is reported in our paper. Furthermore, we previously showed that null ES cells generated by genetic manipulation in vitro are viable. Nowhere in this paper do we state “it is absolutely impossible to derive Mbd3-/- ESCs from Mbd3 null E3.5 embryos”.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2015 Jul 28, Christina Niederstadt commented:
The authors of this article are not very accurate regarding their citation of the available literature and their summaries of cited papers. E.g. the following paragraph: "Another controversial approach has involved the immunization of RCC patients with hybrids of DC fused to autologous RCC tumour cells. Early phase I trial results, reported by Kugler et al. [29], described tumour regressions in seven of 17 RCC patients using allogenic monocyte-derived mature DC fused in an electric field to autologous tumour cells. However, a follow-up phase II study failed to confirm these promising early results [30]."
Reference [29]: Kugler A, Stulher G, Walden P, et al. Regression of human metastatic renal cell carcinoma after vaccination with tumor cell-dendritic cell hybrids. Nat Med. 2000;6:332–6. [PMID: 10700237] Reference [30]: Kugler A, Stuhler G, Walden P, et al. Retraction: regression of human metastatic renal cell carcinoma after vaccination with tumor cell-dendritic cell hybrids. Nat Med. 2003;9:1221. [PMID: 12949529]
The denotation "a follow-up phase II study" seems to be a rather inappropriate description of a retraction notice!
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Dec 10, Kath Wright commented:
Other search filters are available from the InterTASC Information Specialists' Sub-Group Search Filter Resource at https://sites.google.com/a/york.ac.uk/issg-search-filters-resource/home
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Dec 10, David Keller commented:
This study was funded by the manufacturer of MiraLax brand polyethylene glycol laxative
Braintree Laboratories, the manufacturer of MiraLax, funded this year-long open-label study of the safety of their product. However, given the large number of consumers who ingest polyethylene glycol 3350 on a chronic basis, often for many years, it would be helpful to obtain longer-term follow-up of the participants in this study. In addition, a larger and longer-term study, not manufacturer-funded, is warranted, given the unrestricted over-the-counter sale and unmonitored use of PEG-3350 by consumers.
Ethylene glycol is known to be a toxic substance, as are its short multimers, di-ethylene glycol and tri-ethylene glycol. Even if polymerization of 54 ethylene glycol monomers, to an average molecular weight of 3350, renders the resulting long-chain polymers harmless, can we rely on this polymerization to hold up under all circumstances? Human digestion is largely the process of depolymerization of polysaccharides and polypeptides. To what extent does PEG-3350 de-polymerize to ethylene glycol or its toxic short multimers, during digestion or during preparation of the laxative? Are trace amounts of toxic monomers or short multimers produced under any circumstances - including mixture with various beverages and at various temperatures which consumers might use?
Even in the absence of de-polymerization, how much PEG-3350 is found in the serum and urine of humans during chronic daily use? What subtle long-term effects might be caused by the presence of trace amounts of these chemicals in the diets of diverse humans, for years on end? Only a larger, longer-term, independently-funded study can answer these questions.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2017 Aug 28, Peter Gøtzsche commented:
None
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY. -
On 2017 Sep 07, Peter Gøtzsche commented:
The primary outcome in the trial called TORCH (“Towards a Revolution in COPD Health”) was total mortality, which was not analysed correctly. GlaxoSmithKline randomised 6184 patients to four groups: placebo; salmeterol; fluticasone; and both drugs together. This factorial design is powerful, as it allows the investigators to study three research questions with a sample size that would usually only allow one question to be answered. Such a trial can tell us whether the two drugs are effective, and whether the combination is better than any of its components.
However, in violation with the published trial protocol (1), nowhere in the 15-page trial report is the correct factorial analysis to be found, and the abstract gives the readers the clear impression that the combination was better than any of its components. The misleading analysis in the TORCH trial included only half of the patients, thereby spoiling the advantage of the factorial design.
The authors of a letter to the editor showed the correct factorial analysis. The effect of the combination was entirely due to salmeterol (2). In contrast, the hazard ratio for fluticasone was 1.00 (0.87 to 1.15), p = 0.99. There was no good reason why a factorial analysis was not done and published (1). Both GlaxoSmithKline and AstraZeneca have not performed the appropriate analysis in other similar trials as well (3).
1 Gøtzsche PC. Questionable research and marketing of a combination drug for smoker’s lungs. J R Soc Med 2014;107:256-7.
2 La Vecchia C and Fabbri LM. Prevention of death in COPD. N Engl J Med 2007;356:2211–2.
3 Suissa S, Ernst P, Vandemheen KL, et al. Methodological issues in therapeutic trials of COPD. Eur Respir J 2008;31:927–33.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY. -
On 2017 Sep 25, Peter Gøtzsche commented:
The primary outcome in the trial called TORCH (“Towards a Revolution in COPD Health”) was total mortality. GlaxoSmithKline randomised 6184 patients to four groups: placebo; salmeterol; fluticasone; and both drugs together. This factorial design is powerful, as it allows the investigators to study three research questions with a sample size that would usually only allow one question to be answered. Such a trial can tell us whether the two drugs are effective, and whether the combination is better than any of its components.
However, nowhere in the 15-page trial report is the factorial analysis to be found, and the abstract gives the readers the impression that the combination was better than any of its components. The analysis in the TORCH trial included only half of the patients, thereby spoiling the advantage of the factorial design, although the published trial protocol stated that a factorial analysis was to be performed (1).
The authors of a letter to the editor used a factorial analysis and showed that the effect of the combination was entirely due to salmeterol (2); the hazard ratio for fluticasone was 1.00 (0.87 to 1.15), p = 0.99. In other similar trials, both GlaxoSmithKline and AstraZeneca did not perform a factorial analysis (3).
1 Gøtzsche PC. Questionable research and marketing of a combination drug for smoker’s lungs. J R Soc Med 2014;107:256-7.
2 La Vecchia C and Fabbri LM. Prevention of death in COPD. N Engl J Med 2007;356:2211–2.
3 Suissa S, Ernst P, Vandemheen KL, et al. Methodological issues in therapeutic trials of COPD. Eur Respir J 2008;31:927–33.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Aug 18, Paul Brookes commented:
I did not discover the following; these comments were sent to me by an anonymous correspondent, and I agree with their assessment of the data, so am posting here using my own name even though this is not a paper I have either read, or am familiar with the field of...
In Figure 2C, the anti-Flag blot (bottom panel) contains several bands which bear a striking resemblance (more than would be expected by pure coincidence) to bands in the anti-Myc blot of Figure 2B (middle panel). In fact, it's almost as if the latter is a longer exposure of the former.
Also in Figure 2C, the central panel (anti-GST blot), the bands in the top left appear strikingly similar to bands in the anti-tubulin blot (lower panel) in Figure 5A, with 180 degree rotation.
FYI, another paper from the same group Schwamborn JC, 2007 has also been flagged by the same person, and I have/will left a comment there too. I am reliably informed that the DFG (German equivalent of the NIH) is aware of these data problems, as are the journals involved. However more than 6 months has now passed with no actions taken.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2015 Jul 30, Keith Bradnam commented:
As of 2015, development of CEGMA v2 has ceased completely and we can no longer respond to support requests. While the latest version still runs, it should be pointed out that the underlying set of orthologs that CEGMA uses is based on the KOGs database that was published back in 2003.
We would advise potential users of CEGMA to consider newer tools like BUSCO which perform a similar role, are easier to install, take less time to run, and which are based on more modern sets of orthologous genes.
We don't rule out making a completely new version of CEGMA some day, but for now we consider CEGMA v2 an end-of-life product.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Nov 20, Morten Oksvold commented:
An investigation committee at Wayne State University (WSU) recommends that 42 articles from Fazlul Sarkar to be retracted (report finished August 31, 2015). This article represents one of them.
This information was published by Retraction Watch (November 17, 2016) and you can find a link to the full report here:
http://retractionwatch.com/2016/11/17/details-of-investigative-report-into-sarkar-released-by-aclu/
This article should therefore no longer be cited.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2015 Jul 06, Bill Cayley commented:
A good example of when "Less" is actually "More": https://lessismoreebm.wordpress.com/2015/07/06/comparing-hospice-and-nonhospice-patient-survival-among-patients-who-die-within-a-three-year-window/
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Mar 27, Eduardo Fox commented:
The present paper was retracted as of August 2011. Details on detected issues and retraction notice can be found online.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Dec 05, Alberto Halabe Bucay commented:
My work about treatment of cancer with citric acid (citrate) based on my hypothesis about biological competition between the cell and the mitochondria.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 May 25, Margaret Sampson commented:
For an update, please see: Page MJ, Shamseer L, Altman DG, Tetzlaff J, Sampson M, Tricco AC, Catalá-López F, Li L, Reid EK, Sarkis-Onofre R, Moher D. Epidemiology and Reporting Characteristics of Systematic Reviews of Biomedical Research: A Cross-Sectional Study. PLoS Med. 2016 May 24;13(5):e1002028. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1002028. eCollection 2016 May. PubMed PMID: 27218655.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Jan 03, Tom Kindlon commented:
Would it not be preferable if objective outcome measures were used?
This was originally posted at: http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-2377/7/6/comments but the line gaps have been removed so don't think many will fancy reading it there (also may click on the PubMed Central link for the paper and not see it).
I find it strange that, particularly in such a costly trial (over UK£3m* [2013: final estimate is around UK£5m]), only subjective outcome measures are being used.
The participants are assessed using actigraphy watches at the baseline assessment stage. Why are not these being used either during and particularly at the end of the trial? This is important given previous studies in the area.
One study (on a single patient)[1] found "using a 26-session graded activity intervention involved gradual increases in physical activity" that "from baseline to treatment termination, the patient’s self-reported increase in walk time from 0 to 155 min a week contrasted with a surprising 10.6% decrease in mean weekly step counts."
Another study [2], investigating CBT this time, found that (of 278 eligible) "241 patients had complete data (83 CBT, 80 support groups, 78 natural course) at 8 months. At 14 months CBT was significantly more effective than both control conditions for fatigue severity (CBT vs support groups 5.8 [2.2-9.4]; CBT vs natural course 5.6 [2.1-9.0]) and for functional impairment (CBT vs support groups 263 [38-488]; CBT vs natural course 222 [3-441]). Support groups were not more effective for CFS patients than the natural course. Among the CBT group, clinically significant improvement was seen in fatigue severity for 20 of 58 (35%), in Karnofsky performance status for 28 of 57 (49%), and self-rated improvement for 29 of 58 (50%)." Yet if one examines the actometer data from this study from the group given CBT, the increases in activity were minimal[3]. For instance, the baseline average was 67.9, which increased to 68.8 after treatment and to 72.2 at follow-up. About 4 points. Not unlike the medical care controls, who went from 64.9 to 68.7 in the same period. [3]
One of the aims of CBT (for CFS) has been said to be "increased confidence in exercise and physical activity"[4]
Thus it may be the case that when asked questions about one's ability to do things, such as in the physical functioning subscale of SF-36 (one of the two primary outcome measures), the patients might say that they are "Limited A Little" or "Not Limited At All" but may be just as limited as patients in other arms of study who say "limited a lot".
Ideally more than one more objective measure would be used. White himself has found immunological changes after both exercise and activity in Chronic Fatigue Syndrome [5]. It would have been interesting to see whether any of these treatments had an effect on such cytokines.
References:
[1]. Friedberg, F. Does graded activity increase activity? A case study of chronic fatigue syndrome. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 2002, 33, 3-4, 203-215
[2]. Prins JB, Bleijenberg G, Bazelmans E, et al. Cognitive behaviour therapy for chronic fatigue syndrome: a multicentre randomised controlled trial. Lancet 2001; 357: 841-47.
[3]. Van Essen, M and de Winter, LJM. Cognitieve gedragstherapie by het vermoeidheidssyndroom (cognitive behaviour therapy for chronic fatigue syndrome). Report from the College voor Zorgverzekeringen. Amstelveen: Holland. June 27th, 2002. Bijlage B. Table 2.
[4]. O'Dowd, H., Gladwell, P., Rogers, CA., Hollinghurst, S and Gregory, A. Cognitive behavioural therapy in chronic fatigue syndrome: a randomised controlled trial of an outpatient group programme. Health Technology Assessment, 2006, 10, 37, 1-140.
[5]. White, PD., Nye, KE., Pinching, AJ., Yap, TM., Power, N., Vleck, V., Bentley, DJ., Thomas, JM., Buckland, M and Parkin, JM. Immunological changes after both exercise and activity in chronic fatigue syndrome: a pilot study. Journal of Chronic Fatigue Syndrome, 2004, 12, 2, 51-66.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY. -
On 2014 Jan 03, Tom Kindlon commented:
Another example as to why objective measures would be useful
(I'm posting this e-letter/comment from 2007 here for the same reason as the first e-letter below)
Another example of why objective measurements are important in studies involving CBT was shown in a recently published study by Knoop et al [1]. Their results state that "the level of self-reported cognitive impairment decreased significantly more after CBT than in the control conditions. Neuropsychological test performance did not improve."
References:
[1] Knoop H, Prins JB, Stulemeijer M, van der Meer JW, Bleijenberg G:The effect of cognitive behaviour therapy for chronic fatigue syndrome on self-reported cognitive impairments and neuropsychological test performance. Journal of Neurology and Neurosurgery Psychiatry. 2007 Apr;78(4):434-6.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY. -
On 2014 Jan 03, Tom Kindlon commented:
CFS intervention studies - lessons can be learned from a previous review
(I'm posting this e-letter/comment from 2007 here for the same reason as the first e-letter below)
I have just been reminded that many of the points I have been made, such as suggesting the use of the actometer as an outcome measure, were covered in the Whiting et al review (2001) [1].
The review recommended the use of more objective outcome measures e.g. "Outcomes such as "improvement," in which participants were asked to rate themselves as better or worse than they were before the intervention began, were frequently reported. However, the person may feel better able to cope with daily activities because they have reduced their expectations of what they should achieve, rather than because they have made any recovery as a result of the intervention. A more objective measure of the effect of any intervention would be whether participants have increased their working hours, returned to work or school, or increased their physical activities."
This review also recommended long-term follow-up: "The relapsing nature of CFS suggests that follow-up should continue for at least an additional 6 to 12 months after the intervention period has ended, to confirm that any improvement observed was due to the intervention itself and not just to a naturally occurring fluctuation in the course of the illness."
It is slightly disappointing that the follow-up period in this study is scheduled at 52 weeks, only 16 weeks after the session at 36 weeks.
It is particularly disappointing that this current trial didn't learn from the Whiting et al review [1] as both White and Sharpe have shown awareness of the review, having made reference to it in articles [2-5].
Perhaps it is not too late to collect data from even some subjects, using the actometers?
References:
[1] Penny Whiting, Anne-Marie Bagnall, Amanda J. Sowden, John E. Cornell, Cynthia D. Mulrow, Gilbert Ramírez: Interventions for the Treatment and Management of Chronic Fatigue Syndrome - A Systematic Review JAMA. 2001;286:1360-1368 http://jama.ama-assn.org/cgi/content/full/286/11/1360
[2] Peter White: Commentary. Adv. Psychiatr. Treat. 2002;8:363-365. (September 2002)http://apt.rcpsych.org/cgi/content/full/8/5/363
[3] Peter White: Chronic unexplained fatigue.Postgrad. Med. J. 2002;78:445-446. (August 2002) http://pmj.bmj.com/cgi/content/full/78/922/445
[4] Peter White: What causes chronic fatigue syndrome?BMJ 2004;329:928-929.http://www.bmj.com/cgi/content/full/329/7472/928
[5] Michael Sharpe: The symptom of generalised fatigue.PN 2006;6:72-77.http://pn.bmj.com/cgi/content/full/6/2/72
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY. -
On 2014 Jan 03, Tom Kindlon commented:
If CBT for CFS is "to help patients improve activity levels and quality of life", would not actometers be useful for measuring outcomes?
(I'm posting this e-letter/comment from 2007 here for the same reason as the first e-letter below)
Just following up on a previous point: I have just read an oft-quoted paper on CBT for CFS[1], which was co-written by one of the co-authors and principal investigators in the current study, Prof Chalder. [CBT was provided, to the best of my knowledge, in one of the places where CBT will be offered in the PACE Trial].
The current paper says "Two independent systematic reviews have found that rehabilitative cognitive behaviour therapy (CBT) and GET were the most promising treatments for CFS/ME in secondary care". The two reviews reference this paper[1]. The paper[1] states that "cognitive behavior therapy was to help patients improve activity levels and quality of life". Unfortunately there is no mention of pedometers or actometers being used. So it seems disappointing that, according to the design presented, the researchers in the current trial (which many are hoping might be the definitive paper on the subject) will use actometers before the patients start the intervention but will not assess whether the treatment improves activity levels using the sort of measurements actometers can provide.
References:
[1] Deale A, Husain K, Chalder T, Wessely S. Long term outcome of cognitive behavior therapy versus relaxation therapy for chronic fatigue syndrome: a 5-year follow-up study. Am J Psychiatry 2001;158: 2038-42.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY. -
On 2014 Jan 03, Tom Kindlon commented:
Further information on existing data on management strategies for CFS
(I'm posting this e-letter/comment from 2008 here for the same reason as the first e-letter below)
As well as giving the protocol for the PACE Trial, this paper also gives information and data from some previous studies in the area.
Depending on which pieces of data are presented for any treatment in medicine, a treatment can often look more or less useful or effective. Readers of this paper may be interested to know about a recent meta-analysis of the efficacy of CBT for CFS[1]. The studies involved a total of 1371 patients. This involved calculating the size of an effect measure, the Cohen's d value.They calculated d using the following method: "Separate mean effect sizes were calculated for each category of outcome variable (e.g., fatigue self- rating) and for each type of outcome variable (mental, physical, and mixed mental and physical). Studies generally included multiple outcome measures. For all analyses except those that compared different categories or types of outcome variables, we used the mean effect size of all the relevant outcome variables of the study."d was calculated to be 0.48.
For anyone unfamiliar with Cohen's d values, they are not bounded by 1; also, the higher the score, the bigger the "effect size" i.e. the more "effective" a treatment was found to be. Cohen's d values are considered to be a small effect size at 0.2, a moderate effect size at 0.5, and a large effect size at 0.8[2]. CBT had a more general definition in this paper and included some papers on GET. For example, the current paper gives some information on a study by Fulcher and White[3]. Malouff et al[1] calculated the d value for this study as 0.46 (95% CI: 0.03-0.95).
References:
[1] Malouff, J. M., et al., Efficacy of cognitive behavioral therapy for chronic fatigue syndrome: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review (2007), doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.004
[2] Cohen J: Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences. Edited by: 2. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1988.
[3] Fulcher KY, White PD: Randomised controlled trial of graded exercise in patients with the chronic fatigue syndrome. BMJ 1997, 314:1647-1652.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY. -
On 2014 Jan 04, Tom Kindlon commented:
The dropping of actometers as an outcome measure and other points relating to the outcome measures being used
(I'm posting this e-letter/comment from 2008 here for the same reason as the first e-letter below)
In their reply to my comments, Peter White and colleagues say they are using [i]"several objective outcome measures"[/i] [1]. If they think these tests are useful as objective outcome measures, why is at least one of them not being used as a primary outcome measure rather than the current situation where there are only two subjective outcome measures being used.
I have already made some points on the outcome measures but another one is that the bimodal Chalder Fatigue Scale hardly seems a very good outcome measure for a "CFS/ME" trial where there is likely going to be so many maximum or near maximum scoring initially[2]
Also, there are so many (14) secondary outcome measures in this study, along with so many (18) predictor variables, that it seems unlikely all the different methods of looking at the secondary outcome measures can be explored in the final published paper, given authors are encouraged not to make papers too long (especially journals that have paper editions). The protocol itself is 20 pages long when all the different aspects of it are listed! At least some of the information will need to be re-iterated in the final paper.
It is of course important to take the burden on participants into account when deciding what outcome measures to use. However I find the following point very strange: "Although we originally planned to use actigraphy as an outcome measure, as well as a baseline measure, we decided that a test that required participants to wear an actometer around their ankle for a week was too great a burden at the end of the trial." Firstly they clearly don't find it that great a burden that they drop it altogether as it is being used on patients before the start. If they feel it was that big of a burden, it should probably have been dropped altogether.
Of course, other studies in the area have used measuring over a similar or longer period. For example, Bazelmans [3] used an actometer over 14 days, Black [4] used actigraphy over 14 days, Sisto[5] used actigraphy over 7 days, Vercoulen[6] used an actometer over 12 days and Van der Werf [7] used an actometer for 12 days.
Also if one wants to reduce the burden on patients, why not take out one or both of the exercise tests instead. As the clinicians in the study would know, post-exertional symptoms are part of the condition.
For example, Nijs[8] performed a gentle walking exercise on patients where they walked on average 558m(+/-340) (range: 120-1620) at a speed of 0.9m/s (+/-0.2) (range: 0.6-1.1). This resulted in a statistically significant (p<0.05) worsening of scores in the following areas when comparing pre-exercise, post-exercise and 24 hour post-exercise scores using ANOVA: VAS fatigue, VAS musculoskeletal pain, VAS sore throat, SF-36 bodily pain and SF-36 general health percention. 14 out of 24 subjects experienced a clinically meaningful change (worsening) in bodily pain (i.e. a minimum change of the SF-36 bodily pain subscale score of at least 10).
Those results are similar to another study[9] which involved the acute effects of 10 discontinuous 3-minute exercise bouts on a treadmill in 10 CFS patients. In between exercise bouts, there was a 3-minute recovery period between exercise bouts. The participants walked at a comfortable walking pace self-selected by the subjects. On average, the subjects walked at a speed of 0.71+/-0.20 m/s. Some patients reported experiencing headaches, leg pain, fatigue or sore throats.
In another study, Lapp [10] (not to be confused with Clapp[9]) reported on the effects of 31 patients to his practice who were asked to monitor their symptoms three weeks before to 12 days after a maximal exercise test. 74% of the patients experienced worsening fatigue and 26% stayed the same. None improved. The average relapse lasted 8.82 days although 22% were still in relapse when the study ended at 12 days. There were similar changes with exercise in lymph pain, depression, abdominal pain, sleep quality, joint and muscle pain and sore throat.
These are just a small selection of the studies which show patients experience an exacerbation of their symptoms following exercise testing. So these are the sorts of symptoms the patients may expect following the exercise. This reminds me that there seems to be a lot of concentration on measuring fatigue in this study - there are many other symptoms that are part of "CFS/ME".If they had used actometers instead of, say, doing one of the exercise tests, the response to the exercise could have been followed to see how long and how severe an effect the exercise had on the patient. Or they could have dropped both the exercise tests altogether.
As well as "subjective" findings following exercise testing, there have also been objective findings. Arnold et al[11] found excessive intracellular acidoss of skeletal muscles with exercise. Jammes[12] found an increase of damaging oxidative stress following exercise testing. So patients could not just endure temporary sysptom but possibly also longer-term harm from exercise testing. There are numerous other exercise abnormalities.As the clinicians involved in the study probably hear from patients, one of the frustrating things about ME or CFS is that people don't realise the payback that they can have from doing things. This would have been an opportunity to investigate this as part of the study. But now the effort patients will put in and the payback they will feel in some ways is being wasted as the effects won't be measured.
Anyway, to repeat again, given the authors familiarity with the literature, I find it strange that they would decide using an actometer would be worse than putting patients through two exercise tests.
I also find it surprising that in a study part-funded by the Department of Work and Pensions (DWP) that the objective outcome measures (not involving questionnaires) are all once-off exercise tests. It has been established that patients need to be able to do things on several days during a week before they can be passed fit for work. I have mentioned using actometers following exercise tests after an exercise test above; of course, actometers wouldn't have to be used at that time but also during a "normal week".
Proponents of pacing methods including APT would say that there is a "ceiling of activity" that patients can't go above without experiencing a worsening of symptoms. Black[13] has found evidence of this. Proponents of CBT or GET for "CFS/ME" would suggest that patients can gradually just increase how much activity they can do. Actometers would also have tested the hypothesis. As it stands, the study will not give us information on this as just because patients answer questionnaires saying they're improved (which could simply be because they think they're better) or improve their exercise results (which might simply be because they're willing to push themselves more) doesn't prove that they don't have an activity ceiling above which they experience disabling symptoms (esp. when, as in this study, there is no follow-up period following the exercise testing). This is the real "heart" of the issue but given the current design, the question won't be answered.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY. -
On 2014 Jan 04, Tom Kindlon commented:
Does housework count as exercise for somebody in the GET leg of the trial?
I wonder could the authors answer a question which is relevant to the real world application of Graded Exercise Therapy (GET). I have heard a participant in the Graded Exercise Therapy (GET) leg of the trial say that they are counting 10 minutes housework in lieu of a 10-minute walk. I think it would be very useful for the authors to clarify whether they think x minutes housework can be used in lieu of x minutes of walking or whether this is not in compliance with the protocol? I think attention to detail in this matter is very important if one is to apply the findings in the real world. With a drug it is easy to check whether the dosage is the same as published trials. With Graded Exercise Therapy (GET), we need clarification from the authors about what is meant by exercise, so that the protocol can be applied as in the trial, so that people either don't get too low a "dose" or too high a "dose" at the wrong stage.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY. -
On 2014 Feb 20, Tom Kindlon commented:
Further evidence showing why objective measures are preferable in CFS trials particularly where cognitions could be changed following the intervention
(This was originally posted as an e-letter here in 2009: http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-2377/7/6/comments#333618. However, many people will not read the paper on PubMed Central and so not see it there)
Since writing my previous posts, further data on the subject has come to my attention.
Friedberg and Sohl [1] have just published the results of a study on an intervention involving Cognitive Behavior Therapy (CBT) which included encouraging patients for going for longer walks. It found that on the SF-36 Physical Functioning (PF) scale, patients improved from a pre-treatment mean (SD) of 49.44 (25.19) to 58.18 (26.48) post-treatment, equivalent to a Cohen's d value of 0.35. On the Fatigue Severity Scale (FSS), the improvement as measured by the cohen's d value was even great (0.78) from an initial pre-treatment mean (SD) of 5.93 (0.93) to a 5.20 (0.95) post-treatment.
However on actigraphy there was actually a numerical decrease from a pre-treatment mean (SD) of 224696.90 (158389.64) to 203916.67 (122585.92) post-treatment (cohen's d: -0.13). So just because patients report lower fatigue and better scores on the SF-36 PF scale, doesn't mean they're doing more, which is what GET and CBT based on GET claim to bring about. These results seem particularly pertinent for this study given the primary outcome measures are the SF-36 PF scale and a fatigue scale.
Further reading show that another study[2], published over a decade ago, showed the problem of using self-report data in CFS patients. The authors' rationale for the study was: "It is not clear whether subjective accounts of physical activity level adequately reflect the actual level of physical activity. Therefore the primary aims of the present study were to assess actual activity level in patients with CFS to validate claims of lower levels of physical activity and to validate the reported relationship between fatigue and activity level that was found on self-report questionnaires. In addition, we evaluated whether physical activity level adequately can be assessed by self-report measures. An Accelerometer was used as a reference for actual level of physical activity.". The authors reported on the correlations on 7 outcome measures in relation to the actometer readings: "none of the self-report questionnaires had strong correlations with the Actometer. Thus, self-report questionnaires are no perfect parallel tests for the Actometer."
Prof. White seems to be aware of the findings of this study as he has been co-authored at least two papers [3,4] which quoted the findings. One of the times this paper was referenced even shows the problem I'm highlighting e.g. "support for this explanation comes from investigations that have described discrepancies between subjectively reported impairments and objective measures of activity" [4].
The authors of the 1997 study[2] pointed out that "The subjective instruments do not measure actual behaviour. Responses on these instruments appear to be an expression of the patients' views about activity and may be biased by cognitions concerning illness and disability." This was re-iterated in another paper[5]: "In earlier studies of our research group, actual motor activity has been recorded with an ankle-worn motion-sensing device (actometer) in conjunction with self-report measures of physical activity. The data of these studies suggest that self-report measures of activity reflect the patients' view about their physical activity and may have been biased by cognitions concerning illness and disability."
A corollary of the last statement is that reports of improvement in self-report measures in interventions which change "cognitions concerning illness and disability" may not be reliable. "Improvements" in self-report measures may simply show that patients have changed their cognitions with regard to how they view their illness, disability, symptoms, etc rather than actually representing improvements in activity levels and functional capacity.
Thus, I would suggest that actometers should be used whenever possible in CFS trials where one is investigating whether an intervention has brought about increased activity.
It is also interesting to note that in the large Van der Werf (2000) study[5], which involved 277 CFS patients (and 47 healthy controls), the authors divided the patients up "pervasively passive" (representing 24% of the patients), "moderately active" and "pervasively active". They found that "levels of daily experienced fatigue and psychological distress were equal for the three types of activity patterns". So one can't necessarily tell how active a patient is from the fatigue levels they report.
Incidentally they also "there were no significant group, gender or interaction effects for the number of absolute large or relatively large day-to-day fluctuations (Table 2 and Table 3)." "The day-to-day fluctuation measures were based on somewhat arbitrary criteria (1 S.D. and 33% activity change). However, when we post hoc tested alternative criteria (50% or 66% activity change), again no significant group differences between controls and CFS patients emerged." Part of the rationale of many behavioural interventions in CFS patients is said to be to reduce "boom and bust" (sample reference,[6]). However, it may be the case that the frequency of this activity pattern in CFS has been exaggerated.
References:
[1] Friedberg F, Sohl S. Cognitive-behavior therapy in chronic fatigue syndrome: is improvement related to increased physical activity? J Clin Psychol. 2009 Feb 11.
[2] Vercoulen JH, Bazelmans E, Swanink CM, Fennis JF, Galama JM, Jongen PJ, Hommes O, Van der Meer JW, Bleijenberg G. Physical activity in chronic fatigue syndrome: assessment and its role in fatigue. J Psychiatr Res. 1997 Nov-Dec;31(6):661-73.
[3] Fulcher KY, White PD. Strength and physiological response to exercise in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2000 September; 69(3): 302–307.
[4] Smith WR, White PD, Buchwald D. A case control study of premorbid and currently reported physical activity levels in chronic fatigue syndrome. BMC Psychiatry. 2006 Nov 13;6:53.
[5] van der Werf SP, Prins JB, Vercoulen JH, van der Meer JW, Bleijenberg G. Identifying physical activity patterns in chronic fatigue syndrome using actigraphic assessment. J Psychosom Res. 2000 Nov;49(5):373-9.
[6] Deary V, Chalder T: Chapter 11, "Conceptualisation in Chronic Fatigue Syndrome" in Formulation and Treatment in Clinical Health Psychology Edited by Ana V. Nikcevic, Andrzej R. Kuczmierczyk, Michael Bruch Competing interests
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY. -
On 2014 Feb 20, Tom Kindlon commented:
Discrepancies between momentary fatigue and recalled fatigue can exist
(This was originally posted as an e-letter here in 2009: http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-2377/7/6/comments#338630. However, many people will not read the paper on PubMed Central and so not see it there)
One of the primary outcome measures in this study is the bimodal Chalder Fatigue Scale [1] (possible individual scores 0 and 1, total scores can range from 0-11). One of the secondary outcome measures is the Chalder Fatigue Scale[1] using a different method of scoring (possible individual scores: 0-3, total scores can range from 0-33). This asks questions about the last month: "For the next few questions, we would like to know whether or not you have had any problems with feeling tired, weak, or lacking in energy in the last month".
But is it recalled fatigue, or the memory for fatigue, exactly equal to the fatigue people actually felt during a period?
Friedberg and Sohl have investigated this using electronic diaries[2]. Here is a description of what was involved: "In order to obtain a representative diurnal sample of symptoms without undue subject burden (Stone & Shiffman, 2002), the palm pilots prompted subjects for 21 days, 6 times a day, every 2 hr plus or minus a randomly programmed 1-20 min interval (Stone & Shiffman, 1994). The first daily prompt occurred within 1 hr of the subject's wakening and the last daily prompt approximately 12 hr later. No prompt signals occurred during the subject's reported sleep time. After each prompt, a screen was displayed with a numerical rating scale (0-10) that was labeled "Fatigue Now." The end point anchors on the numerical scales were None (0) and Highest (10). Subjects were instructed to record intensity ratings on the numerical scale for their current levels of fatigue."
What they found was "average weekly recall of fatigue intensity was significantly higher than average momentary ratings" (8.5% higher on average). If one used a scale like the Chalder Fatigue Scale[1] which asked for a period of over the preceding month, one could speculate that the discrepancy would be even higher.
This suggests that self-report fatigue questionnaires may not be ideal for intervention studies particularly in expensive trials like this one where one might be willing to pay a bit extra for greater accuracy; just as it has been shown that self-report questionnaires may not correlate strongly with more objective measures of activity (such as actometers)[3,4] (of course, in this study, the reason actometers are not being used does not seem to be due to the cost of obtaining them or the need to train participants to use them, as baseline measurements from actometers are being used).
References:
[1] Chalder T, Berelowitz G, Hirsch S, Pawlikowska T, Wallace P, Wessely S, Wright D: Development of a fatigue scale. J Psychosom Res 1993, 37:147-153.
[2] Friedberg F, Sohl SJ. Memory for fatigue in chronic fatigue syndrome: the relation between weekly recall and momentary ratings. Int J Behav Med. 2008 Jan-Mar;15(1):29-33.
[3] Friedberg F, Sohl S. Cognitive-behavior therapy in chronic fatigue syndrome: is improvement related to increased physical activity? J Clin Psychol. 2009 Feb 11.
[4] Vercoulen JH, Bazelmans E, Swanink CM, Fennis JF, Galama JM, Jongen PJ, Hommes O, Van der Meer JW, Bleijenberg G. Physical activity in chronic fatigue syndrome: assessment and its role in fatigue. J Psychiatr Res. 1997 Nov-Dec;31(6):661-73.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY. -
On 2014 Feb 20, Tom Kindlon commented:
New paper lists 3 CFS studies where there was no improvement in the actometer readings but an improvement was reported in subjective outcome measures
(This was originally posted as an e-letter here in 2010: http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-2377/7/6/comments#387679. However, many people will not read the paper on PubMed Central and so not see it there)
I know it might perhaps have seemed to some who have read these posts that I might be concerned about something that was not important (when I was calling for actometers to be used if possible for at least some of the patients at the end of the trial). So I feel "vindicated" in a way by a review[1] that has just been published. It found that in the three Dutch CFS trials looked at, studies which all found improvements in fatigue[2-4], there was no statistically significant increase in physical activity levels as measured by actometers.
The review also found that "changes in physical activity were not related to changes in fatigue."
The authors of the review (who include people who were involved in all the studies) say that, in the three studies, "treatment was based on the manual of CBT for CFS described in detail by Bleijenberg et al. (2003)" [5]. This form of CBT is comparable to the form of CBT being assessed in the PACE Trial [6].
It is useful to point out that fatigue wasn't the only subjective outcome measure that was said to have improved (in these trials where there was no increase in physical activity).
In all of the three studies [2-4], improvements were reported in functional impairment (as measured by questionnaires). In two of the studies [2,3], improvements in physical functioning as measured by the SF-36 physical functioning subscale were reported (this questionnaire does was not used in the third study[4]). So the patients reported improvements in "physical functioning" (as measured by the SF-36 physical functioning subscale) but there was no improvement in physical activity as measured by the actometers. The SF-36 physical functioning subscale is one of the primary outcome measures in the PACE Trial[6].
This discrepancy between objective measures of activity and questionnaire is similar to data I have previously drawn attention to[7] in a study by Friedberg and Sohl[8].
References:
[1] Wiborg JF, Knoop H, Stulemeijer M, Prins JB, Bleijenberg G. How does cognitive behaviour therapy reduce fatigue in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome? The role of physical activity. Psychol Med. 2010 Jan 5:1 -7. [Epub ahead of print]
[2] Stulemeijer M, de Jong LW, Fiselier TJ, Hoogveld SW, Bleijenberg G (2005). Cognitive behaviour therapy for adolescents with chronic fatigue syndrome: randomised controlled trial. British Medical Journal 330. Published online : 7 December 2004. doi:10.1136/bmj.38301.587106.63.
[3] Knoop H, van der Meer JW, Bleijenberg G (2008). Guided self-instructions for people with chronic fatigue syndrome: randomised controlled trial. British Journal of Psychiatry 193, 340–341.
[4] Prins JB, Bleijenberg G, Bazelmans E, Elving LD, de Boo TM, Severens JL, van der Wilt GJ, Spinhoven P, van der Meer JW (2001). Cognitive behaviour therapy for chronic fatigue syndrome: a multicentre randomised controlled trial. Lancet 357, 841–847.
[5] Bleijenberg G, Prins JB, Bazelmans E (2003). Cognitive-behavioral therapies. In Handbook of Chronic Fatigue Syndrome (ed. L. A. Jason, P. A. Fennell and R. R. Taylor), pp. 493–526. Wiley: New York.
[6] White PD, Sharpe MC,Chalder T, DeCesare JC and Walwyn R for the PACE trial group. Protocol for the PACE trial: A randomised controlled trial of adaptive pacing, cognitive behaviour therapy, and graded exercise as supplements to standardised specialist medical care versus standardised specialist medical care alone for patients with the chronic fatigue syndrome/myalgic encephalomyelitis or encephalopathy. BMC Neurology 2007, 7:6
[7] Kindlon T. Further evidence showing why objective measures are preferable in CFS trials particularly where cognitions could be changed following the intervention http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-2377/7/6/comments#333618
[8] Friedberg F, Sohl S. Cognitive-behavior therapy in chronic fatigue syndrome: is improvement related to increased physical activity? J Clin Psychol. 2009 Feb 11.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY. -
On 2014 Feb 20, Tom Kindlon commented:
CONSORT statement recommends sufficient details to allow replication (for nonpharmacologic treatments, the publishing of manuals is recommended)
(This was originally posted as an e-letter here in 2010: http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-2377/7/6/comments#415670. However, many people will not read the paper on PubMed Central and so not see it there)
The publishing of this trial protocol [1] is to be welcomed – it is something that the CONSORT statement on Randomized Control Trials (RCTs) recommends[2].
Item 5 of the checklist in the CONSORT guidelines [2] states the following information should be given: "The interventions for each group with sufficient details to allow replication, including how and when they were actually administered."
The explanation for this is given as [3]: "Explanation—Authors should describe each intervention thoroughly, including control interventions. The description should allow a clinician wanting to use the intervention to know exactly how to administer the intervention that was evaluated in the trial.102 For a drug intervention, information would include the drug name, dose, method of administration (such as oral, intravenous), timing and duration of administration, conditions under which interventions are withheld, and titration regimen if applicable. If the control group is to receive “usual care” it is important to describe thoroughly what that constitutes. If the control group or intervention group is to receive a combination of interventions the authors should provide a thorough description of each intervention, an explanation of the order in which the combination of interventions are introduced or withdrawn, and the triggers for their introduction if applicable.
"Specific extensions of the CONSORT statement address the reporting of non-pharmacologic and herbal interventions and their particular reporting requirements (such as expertise, details of how the interventions were standardised).43 44 We recommend readers consult the statements for non-pharma-cologic and herbal interventions as appropriate."
The equivalent item in the Extension for Trials Assessing Nonpharmacologic Treatments is:
"Precise details of both the experimental treatment and comparator
4A Description of the different components of the interventions and, when applicable, descriptions of the procedure for tailoring the interventions to individual participants
4B Details of how the interventions were standardized
4C Details of how adherence of care providers with the protocol was assessed or enhanced"
Here is an extract from the explanation for part a[5]: “It is important to provide a detailed description of nonpharmacologic treatments, which are usually complex interventions involving several components (75), each of which may influence the estimated treatment effect (27–32).”
[..]
“The description of any standardization methods is essential to allow adequate replication of the nonpharmacologic treatment. We recommend that authors allow interested readers to access the materials they used to standardize the interventions, either by including a Web appendix with their article or a link to a stable Web site. Such materials include written manuals, specific guidelines, and materials used to train care providers to uniformly deliver the intervention.”
[..]
“In a review of behavioral medicine interventions, insufficient intervention detail was a barrier to assessment of evidence and development of guidelines (80–82).”
[..]
“For rehabilitation, behavioral treatment, education, and psychotherapy, authors should report qualitative and quantitative data. Qualitative data describe the content of each session, how it is delivered (individual or group), whether the treatment is supervised, the content of the information exchanged with participants, and the instruments used to give information. Quantitative data describe the number of sessions, timing of each session, duration of each session, duration of each main component of each session, and overall duration of the intervention. It is also essential to report how the intervention was tailored to patients’ comorbid conditions, tolerance, and clinical course.”
Here is an extract from the explanation for part b which seems particularly relevant as treatment manuals are referred to in the protocol (but they have not been published):
“The description of any standardization methods is essential to allow adequate replication of the nonpharmacologic treatment. We recommend that authors allow interested readers to access the materials they used to standardize the interventions, either by including a Web appendix with their article or a link to a stable Web site. Such materials include written manuals, specific guidelines, and materials used to train care providers to uniformly deliver the intervention.”
The descriptions of Cognitive Behaviour Therapy (CBT) and Graded Exercise Therapy (GET) are 73 and 68 words respectively. These are not sufficient for replication. For example, what is the advice a therapist should give if a patient experiences an exacerbation of symptoms, which is common with this condition e.g. should they maintain their level of activity or exercise or reduce? And if they maintain their level of activity, how long should this continue for? Also what constitutes treatment adherence? In a previous comment, I pointed out a patient who is counting minutes spent doing housework as minutes for her exercise program.
This estimated cost for this trial is now 5m UK pounds of taxpayers' money[6]. It is important that it is reported as well as possible.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2018 Feb 01, Misha Koksharov commented:
Use of metallochromic dyes and potentiometric pH-meter titration to detect binding of divalent cations to “Good’s” buffers: 4-morpholinepropanesulfonic acid (Mops) does not bind Mg2+
This is a very useful report and an educative methodological note. I also use MOPS buffer with Mg<sup>2+</sup> in my work and I was a bit concerned if MOPS could affect the amount of free Mg<sup>2+</sup> given some uncertainty in the literature.
In a recent review Ferreira et al (RSC Adv., 2015) also summarize that "there is no evidence of complex formation for MES, MOPSO and MOPS with the main metals present in environmental and biological studies. [...] Despite these reports of complexation, most of the authors agree that there is no evidence of significant bonding to metals and several studies specifically chose MES or MOPSO due to their inability to interfere with the most important metals in biological and environmental applications." Though, unfortunately they have missed the current paper.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Nov 18, Morten Oksvold commented:
An investigation committee at Wayne State University (WSU) recommends that 42 articles from Fazlul Sarkar to be retracted (report finished August 31, 2015). This article represents one of them.
This information was published by Retraction Watch (November 17, 2016) and you can find a link to the full report here:
http://retractionwatch.com/2016/11/17/details-of-investigative-report-into-sarkar-released-by-aclu/
This articles should therefore no longer be cited.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov
-
On 2014 Feb 05, Michael Kraus commented:
We read this paper in the journal club I sponsor at the University of Illinois (each week we read an interesting social psychology article!). In general I find this topic to be fascinating--that one's mindset could change physiology and behavior in ways that lead to real gains in health is pretty neat!
I do have a question about the mean differences observed in the analysis: The groups have a standard deviation in weight that is about 22lbs (both at time 1 and time 2), and yet the standard error of the means used for the analysis is less than 1 lb. The repeated measures analysis must have accounted for a sizable portion of variability in weight, but I'm estimating, given how standard errors are calculated, that it wouldn't reduce the standard errors this drastically. My question: were there any data transformations or statistical techniques used to analyze the data that are not currently reported in the paper?
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Nov 23, Harri Hemila commented:
This letter was commented on Marmor M, 2008 and a reply to the comment is available at at DOI
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Sep 29, Alem Matthees commented:
This study did not have a control group, therefore reliable conclusions cannot be drawn about whether CBT helped CFS patients to recover from their illness. The recovery rates in the PACE trial, based on the published trial protocol, were not significantly higher in the CBT or GET groups when compared with specialist medical care alone.
Matthees A, Kindlon T, Maryhew C, Stark P, Levin B. A preliminary analysis of ‘recovery’ from chronic fatigue syndrome in the PACE trial using individual participant data. Virology Blog. 21 September 2016. http://www.virology.ws/wp-content/uploads/2016/09/preliminary-analysis.pdf
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Nov 23, Harri Hemila commented:
A manuscript version is available at handle
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2013 Nov 04, Steven Salzberg commented:
This is the first in a series of reports, all coming from the same lab, claiming to have found extant protein sequences in 68-million-year-old fossils. The technical comments published subsequently in Science, and comments published elsewhere, showed convincingly that these results were likely due to contamination or simply misinterpretation. Buckley et al (http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18174420) show that the amino acids in the mass spec data appear to be modern. Pevzner et al. (http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18719266) explain that they may represent statistical artifacts. Others have also published results showing that the "soft tissue" reported by Schweitzer and colleagues here and elsewhere is consistent with a bacterial biofilm.
These results have never been replicated by anyone other than the original authors. The prior likelihood of extant proteins or soft tissue in T. rex fossils is near zero, and the evidence supplied here is consistent with several other, far more likely interpretations. Nonetheless, these authors (but no others) have continued to publish papers asserting that they've found dinosaur peptides.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2015 May 13, Hilda Bastian commented:
This review contributed analysis that has been critical to the debate about editorial peer review. However, the date of the last search for studies was in June 2004. As the eligible literature was sparse, even a few good studies shifts the picture on some questions.
It seems to me likely that the number of relevant studies published in the last decade is now substantive. I include below studies that would be relevant to an update, particularly on the question of blinding/masking of authors' and/or peer reviewers' identities and affiliations, and open publication of peer review reports. A recent systematic on training is also relevant (Galipeau J, 2015).
There are some issues that I believe would be helpful for a new/updated review on editorial peer review to address:
(1) The scope of this review does not include potential sources of editorial/reviewer bias, in particular that related to racial background, gender, country of residence, and institutional prestige. The objective of the review is "to estimate the effect of processes in editorial peer review" and its key focus is the quality of published articles. However, the degree to which these types of biases are minimized in the scientific editorial process has important bearing on the fairness of the processes, as well as the overall quality of literature that may get the most attention in a field.
"Soundness of ethics" is one of the outcome measures of concern, including the avoidance of harm to research subjects. I believe avoidance of harm to authors, who are in a subordinate power relationship in the editorial process, is also a matter of ethics. Publishing mediocre papers from some groups preferentially over higher quality submissions from others, would patently undermine both the fairness and the value of the peer review process at a journal. That may have the power to influence career progress.
Systematic reviews should also point to key areas for further research. The lack of studies into methods to reduce editors’ biases is an important gap to point out, as so much of the literature is concerned primarily with peer reviewers’ bias.
(2) This review did not report on the methods used a priori to systematically assess the risk of bias of included studies, a critical omission in reporting the results of a systematic review (see Oxman AD, 1991, Moher D, 1999, and Liberati A, 2009). A wide variety of study types are eligible for inclusion, raising particular issues specific to them. And studies in this field have a range of specific potential biases. It would be helpful if the experience gained in this review led to an explicit set of criteria for assessing the risk of bias of included studies.
(3) Given the similarities in editorial processes and challenges across scientific disciplines, I believe a systematic review without this restriction would be more valuable, even if the search strategy may have more limitations.
Jefferson T, 2007 included studies with designs that were experimental and other comparative studies that included an attempt to control for confounding. I identified the following additional studies of blinding authors/peer reviewers or publishing peer review reports, that I think need to be considered by reviewers on these questions:
Biomedical science
- Laband DN, 1994
- Nylenna M, 1994 (English abstract, Swedish article Karlsson Y, 1995 - excluded by Jefferson et al)
- Isenberg SJ, 2009
- van Rooyen S, 2010
- Alam M, 2011
- Vinther S, 2012
- Kowalczuk, 2013 (Abstract only)
In addition, Hopewell S, 2014, while addressing another objective in relation to the impact of peer review, was conducted on published pre-publication peer reviews and subsequent manuscript versions.
Non-biomedical sciences
I have written more about the evidence base on anonymity and openness in peer review in this blog post.
Finally, a trial of blinding critical appraisers of clinical trials in the context of systematic reviewing was included in this systematic review (Jadad AR, 1996). That is not the context of peer reviewing for publication of those trials. (As it only involves 7 reviewers, including it or not has little effect on overall conclusions on this body of evidence.)
(Disclosure: Part of my job includes working on PubMed Commons, which does not allow anonymous commenting.)
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Sep 29, Ivan Oransky commented:
This paper has been retracted, one of three studies by the late first author to have been retracted: http://retractionwatch.com/2014/09/26/deceased-researcher-has-two-more-papers-retracted/
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Jun 17, Andrew Foote commented:
Caution: Two out of the five mtDNA haplotypes generated from the archeological samples (GenBank accession nos: EF540867 and EF540868) are a 100% match for common dolphin (Delphinus delphis) and only a 98% match for any other bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) sequences when using the BLAST search implemented in GenBank. The NCBI have been made aware of this taxonomic misidentification and have contacted the sequence submitter (Dr A. Rus Hoelzel), but at present any change requires permission from the sequence submitter. Unfortunately in this case the data submitter does not appear willing to correct the taxonomic classification.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY. -
On 2014 Jun 23, A Rus Hoelzel commented:
This is an odd comment, in that we have had no contact from Genbank, only PubMed about the existence of a comment, and only just today. Also there are only two haplotypes from the archaeological samples not 5 (see Table 2), and both match 100% to Tursiops sequences from various sources found on Genbank using BLAST. However, the two haplotypes mentioned do match mostly Delphinus, but also retrieve Stenella and Tursiops sequences. They are up to 3 bp different from Delphinus, and 2 bp different from Tursiops and Stenella haplotypes. The delphinid radiation is shallow based on mtDNA control region data and polytomous even for multi-locus trees for these three genera. This sequence is only 171 bp long, and generates only a complete polytomy in phylogenetic reconstructions. These two haplotypes that match Delphinus for this short sequence could conceivably represent misidentified samples (from modern stranded animals on the UK coast), but I wouldn’t have much confidence in using 171bp of control region as a barcode to identify species from this group of genera even from a phylogeny, and BLAST is really a very imperfect tool for this. Note that from this same study samples with these two haplotypes assigned with other UK Tursiops samples based on microsatellite DNA genotypes.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2017 Dec 08, Jesper M Kivelä commented:
Vivarelli and colleagues stated that late hepatic artery thrombosis (HAT) was diagnosed 0.4% (1/236) of patients with antiplatelet prophylaxis (AP) and 2.2% (13/592) of patients without AP. P-value for comparison between these groups was 0.049. The authors used Fisher’s exact test with SPSS version 10, and significance threshold for P-value was 0.05.
Based on my calculations, 2-tailed P-value is 0.130 (1-tailed 0.059) with Fisher’s exact test. I used three different statistical software (i.e. SPSS 22, Stata 12.1 and R 3.1.1).
Research colleague of mine tried to independently replicate (SPSS 22) the aforementioned result presented by Vivarelli and colleagues but without success. Our results, however, were identical.
Naturally, the proportion of late HAT cases is lower in patients with AP (0.4%) compared to patients without AP (2.2%) no matter what the P-value but the results should align with the methods used.
Of course, our calculations can also be wrong. However, we hope that Vivarelli and colleagues could re-examine their calculations.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2013 Nov 24, John Sotos commented:
The history and physical examination of the patient with ischemic cardiomyopathy that was presented in the May 2 Clinical Crossroads column (*) fell short of JAMA’s standards.
First, reporting patterns of “chest pain” in a patient with ischemic heart disease invites errors in history taking because ischemic chest discomfort is often not painful, but “squeezing,” “heavy,” etc.
Second, reporting jugular venous pressure on the basis of venous distention is crude and, unless the patient’s posture is provided, useless.
Third, reporting that a patient’s “pulses were intact,” leaving the arteries anonymous and the pulse-amplitude unspecified, communicates little.
Finally, a typographical error describing the patient’s “normal S1 and S1″ [sic] reinforces the inattention given to the case description.
All professions have their slang and verbal short-cuts, but these temptations to substitute reflex for reflection should be resisted.
(*) Zimetbaum PJ. A 59-year-old man considering implantation of a cardiac defibrillator. JAMA. 2007; 297: 1909-1916.
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2014 Dec 10, Kath Wright commented:
Other search filters are available from the InterTASC Information Specialists' Sub-Group Search Filter Resource at https://sites.google.com/a/york.ac.uk/issg-search-filters-resource/home
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2013 Dec 28, Tsuyoshi Miyakawa commented:
We failed to replicate most of the behavioral phenotype reported here in these mutant mice. The possible reasons for this discrepancy are discussed in our paper(Shoji H, 2012).
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-
-
europepmc.org europepmc.org
-
On 2016 Apr 30, Ivan Oransky commented:
Ben Ashby, who reviewed a later paper by Hanna Kokko, one of the authors, in which she corrected an error in this paper, says it should not have been retracted: http://retractionwatch.com/2016/04/29/why-that-evolution-paper-should-never-have-been-retracted-a-reviewer-speaks-out/
This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.
-