10,000 Matching Annotations
  1. Jul 2025
    1. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      This work by Pal et al. studied the relationship between protein expression noise and translational efficiency. They proposed a model based on ribosome demand to explain the positive correlation between them, which is new as far as I realize. Nevertheless, I found the evidence of the main idea that it is the ribosome demand generating this correlation is weak. Below are my major and minor comments.

      Major comments:

      (1) Besides a hypothetical numerical model, I did not find any direct experimental evidence supporting the ribosome demand model. Therefore, I think the main conclusions of this work are a bit overstated.

      (2) I found that the enhancement of protein noise due to high translational efficiency is quite mild, as shown in Figure 6A-B, which makes the biological significance of this effect unclear.

      (3) The captions for most of the figures are short and do not provide much explanation, making the figures difficult to read.

      (4) It would be helpful if the authors could define the meanings of noise (e.g., coefficient of variation?) and translational efficiency in the very beginning to avoid any confusion. It is also unclear to me whether the noise from the experimental data is defined according to protein numbers or concentrations, which is presumably important since budding yeasts are growing cells.

      (5) The conclusions from Figure 1D and 1E are not new. For example, the constant protein noise as a function of mean protein expression is a known result of the two-state model of gene expression, e.g., see Eq. (4) in Paulsson, Physics of Life Reviews 2005.

      (6) In Figure 4C-D, it is unclear to me how the authors changed the mean protein expression if the translation initiation rate is a function of variation in mRNA number and other random variables.

      (7) If I understand correctly, the authors somehow changed the translation initiation rate to change the mean protein expression in Figure 4C-D. However, the authors changed the protein sequences in the experimental data of Figure 6. I am not sure if the comparison between simulations and experimental data is appropriate.

      Comments on revisions:

      Updated Review: The authors have satisfactorily answered all of my questions and comments. The current manuscript is much clearer and stronger than the previous one. I do not have any other questions.

    2. Author Response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review): 

      Summary:

      The authors use analysis of existing data, mathematical modelling, and new experiments, to explore the relationship between protein expression noise, translation efficiency, and transcriptional bursting.

      Strengths:

      The analysis of the old data and the new data presented is interesting and mostly convincing.

      Thank you for the constructive suggestions and comments. We address the individual comments below. 

      Weaknesses:

      (1) My main concern is the analysis presented in Figure 4. This is the core of mechanistic analysis that suggests ribosomal demand can explain the observed phenomenon. I am both confused by the assumptions used here and the details of the mathematical modelling used in this section. Firstly, the authors' assumption that the fluctuations of a single gene mRNA levels will significantly affect ribosome demand is puzzling. On average the total level of mRNA across all genes would stay very constant and therefore there are no big fluctuations in the ribosome demand due to the burstiness of transcription of individual genes. Secondly, the analysis uses 19 mathematical functions that are in Table S1, but there are not really enough details for me to understand how this is used, are these included in a TASEP simulation? In what way are mRNA-prev and mRNA-curr used? What is the mechanistic meaning of different terms and exponents? As the authors use this analysis to argue ribosomal demand is at play, I would like this section to be very much clarified.

      Thank you for raising two important points. Regarding the first point, we agree that the overall ribosome demand in a cell will remain mostly the same even with fluctuations in mRNA levels of a few genes. However, what we refer to in the manuscript is the demand for ribosomes for translating mRNA molecules of a single gene. This demand will vary with the changes in the number of mRNA molecules of that gene. When the mRNA copy number of the gene is low, the number of ribosomes required for translation is low. At a subsequent timepoint when the mRNA number of the same gene goes up rapidly due to transcriptional bursting, the number of ribosomes required would also increase rapidly. This would increase ribosome demand. The process of allocation of ribosomes for translation of these mRNA molecules will vary between cells, and this process can lead to increased expression variation of that gene among cells. We have now rephrased the section between the lines 321 and 331 to clarify this point.

      Regarding the second point, each of the 19 mathematical functions was individually tested in the TASEP model and stochastic simulation. The parameters ‘mRNA-curr’ and ‘mRNA-prev’ are the mRNA copy numbers at the present time point and the previous time point in the stochastic simulations, respectively. These numbers were calculated from the rate of production of mRNA, which is influenced by the transcriptional burst frequency and the burst size, as well as the rate of mRNA removal. We have now incorporated more details about the modelling part along with explanation for parameters and terms in the revised manuscript (lines 390 to 411; lines 795 to lines 807). 

      (2) Overall, the paper is very long and as there are analytical expressions for protein noise (e.g. see Paulsson Nature 2004), some of these results do not need to rely on Gillespie simulations. Protein CV (noise) can be written as three terms representing protein noise contribution, mRNA expression contribution, and bursty transcription contribution. For example, the results in panel 1 are fully consistent with the parameter regime, protein noise is negligible compared to transcriptional noise. 

      Thank you for referring to the paper on analytical expressions for protein noise. We introduced translational bursting and ribosome demand in our model, and these are linked to stochastic fluctuations in mRNA and ribosome numbers. In addition, our model couples transcriptional bursting with translational bursting and ribosome demand. Since these processes are all stochastic in nature, we felt that the stochastic simulation would be able to better capture the fluctuations in mRNA and protein expression levels originating from these processes. For consistency, we used stochastic simulations throughout even when the coupling between transcription and translation were not considered. 

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):  

      (1) Figure 1B shows noise as Distance to Median (DM) that can be positive or negative. It is therefore misleading that the authors say there is a 10-fold increase in noise (this would be relevant if the quantity was strictly positive). How is the 10-fold estimated? Similar comments apply to Figure 1F and the estimated 37-fold. I also wonder if the datasets combined from different studies are necessarily compatible.

      We have now changed the statements and mentioned the actual noise values for different classes of genes rather than the fold-changes (lines 111-113 and 143-145). We agree that the measurements for mRNA expression levels, protein synthesis rates and protein noise were obtained from experiments done by different research labs, and this could introduce more variation in the data. However, it is unlikely the experimental variations are likely to be random and do not bias any specific class of genes (in Fig. 1B and Fig. 1F) more than others.  

      (2)   How Figure 1D has been generated seems confusing, the authors state this is based on the Gillespie algorithm, but in panel 1C and also in the methods, they are writing ODEs and Equations 3 and 4 stating the Euler method for the solution of ODEs. Also, I am concerned if this has been done at steady-state. The protein noise for the two-state model can be analytically obtained, and instead of simulations, the authors could have just used the expression. Also, Figure 1D shows CV while the corresponding data Figure 1B is showing mean adjusted DM. So, I am not sure if the comparison is valid. I am also very confused about the fact that the authors show CV does not depend on the mean expression of proteins and mRNA. Analytical solutions suggested there is always an inverse relationship exists between CV and mean and this has also been experimentally observed (see for example Newman et al 2006).

      We used Gillespie algorithm for stochastic simulations and identified the time points when an event (for example, switching to ON or OFF states during transcriptional bursting) occurred. If an event occurred at a time point, the rates of the reactions were guided by the equations 3 and 4, as the rates of reactions were dependent on the number of mRNA (or protein) molecules present, production rates and removal rates. 

      For all published datasets where we had measurements from many genes/promoters, we used the measures of adjusted noise (for mRNA noise) and Distance-to-median (DM, for protein noise). These measures of noise are corrected mean-dependence of expression noise (Newman et al., 2006). For simulations, which we performed for a single gene, and for experiments that we performed on a limited number of promoters, we used the measure of coefficient of variation (CV) to quantify noise, as calculation of adjusted noise or DM was not possible for a single gene. 

      The work of Newman et al. (2006) measures noise values of different genes with different transcriptional burst characteristics and different mRNA and protein removal rates. We also see similar results in our simulations (Fig. 1E), where as we increase the mean expression by changing the transcriptional burst frequency, the protein noise goes down.     

      (3) Estimating parameters of gene expression using reference 44 ignores the effect of variability in capture efficiency and cell size. In a recent paper, Tang et al Bioinformatics 39 (7), btad395 2023 addressed this issue.

      Thank you for referring to the work of Tang et al. (2023). We note that the cell size and capture efficiency have a small effect on the burst frequency (Kon) but has a more pronounced effect on burst size (Tang et al., 2023). In our analysis, we considered only burst frequency and even with likely small inaccuracies in our estimation of Kon, we can capture interesting association of burst frequency with noise trends. 

      (4) In the methods "αp = 0.007 per mRNA molecule per unit time", I believe it should be per protein molecule per unit time.

      Corrected.

      (5)  Figure 3 uses TASEP modelling but the details of this modelling are not described well.

      We have now expanded the description of the modelling approach in the revised manuscript (lines 391-412; lines 693-776 and lines 797-809). In addition, we have also added more details in the figure captions. 

      (6) Another overall issue is that when the authors talk about changes in burst frequency or changes in translation efficiency, it is not always clear, is this done while keeping all the other parameters constant therefore changing mean expressions, or is this done by keeping the mean expressions constant?

      To test for the association between mean protein expression and protein noise, we have varied the mean expression by changing the translation initiation rate (TLinit) for the most part of the manuscript while keeping other parameters constant. In figure 5, where we decoupled TLinit from ribosome traversal rate (V), we changed the mean protein expression by changing the ribosome traversal rate while keeping other parameters constant. We have now mentioned this in the manuscript. 

      (7)   I believe Figures 5 and 6 present the same data in different ways, I wonder if these can be combined or if some aspect of the data in Figure 5 could go to supplementary. Also, the statistical tests in Figure 5E and F are not clear what they are testing.

      We have now moved figures 5E and 5F to the supplement (Fig. S20). We have also added details of the statistical test in the figure caption. 

      Reviewer #2 (Public review): 

      This work by Pal et al. studied the relationship between protein expression noise and translational efficiency. They proposed a model based on ribosome demand to explain the positive correlation between them, which is new as far as I realize. Nevertheless, I found the evidence of the main idea that it is the ribosome demand generating this correlation is weak. Below are my major and minor comments.

      Thank you for your helpful suggestions and comments. We note that the direct experimental support required for the ribosome demand model would need experimental setups that are beyond the currently available methodologies. We address the individual comments below. 

      Major comments: 

      (1) Besides a hypothetical numerical model, I did not find any direct experimental evidence supporting the ribosome demand model. Therefore, I think the main conclusions of this work are a bit overstated.

      Direct experimental evidence of the hypothesis would require generation of ribosome occupancy maps of mRNA molecules of specific genes at the level of single cells and at time intervals that closely match the burst frequency of the genes. This is beyond the currently available methodologies. However, there are other evidences that support our model. For example, earlier work in cell-free systems have showed that constraining cellular resources required for transcription or translation can increase expression heterogeneity (Caveney et al., 2017). In addition, the ribosome demand model had two predictions both of which could be validated through modelling as well as from our experiments. 

      To further investigate whether removing ribosome demand from our model could eliminate the positive mean-noise correlation for a gene, we have now tested two additional sets of models where we decoupled the translation initiation rate (TLinit) from the ribosome traversal speed (V). In the first model, we changed the mean protein expression by changing the translation initiation rate but keeping the ribosome traversal speed constant. Thus, in this scenario, ribosome demand varied according to the variation in the translation initiation rate. As expected, the positive correlation between mean expression and protein noise was maintained in this condition (Fig. 5B). In the second model, we changed the mean expression by changing the ribosome traversal speed but keeping the translation initiation rate (and therefore, the ribosome demand) constant. In this situation, the relationship between mean expression and protein noise turned negative (Fig. 5B and fig. S16). These results further pointed that the ribosome demand was indeed driving the positive relationship between mean expression and protein noise. 

      (2) I found that the enhancement of protein noise due to high translational efficiency is quite mild, as shown in Figure 6A-B, which makes the biological significance of this effect unclear.

      We agree with the reviewer’s comment that the effect of translational efficiency on protein noise may not be as substantial as the effect of transcriptional bursting, but it has been observed in studies across bacteria, yeast, and Arabidopsis (Ozbudak et al., 2003; Blake et al., 2003; Wu et al., 2022). In addition, the relationship between translational efficiency and protein noise is in contrast with the inverse relationship observed between mean expression and noise (Newman et al., 2006; Silander et al., 2012). We also note that the goal of the manuscript was not to evaluate the relative strength of these associations, but to understand the molecular basis of the influence of translational efficiency on protein noise. 

      (3) The captions for most of the figures are short and do not provide much explanation, making the figures difficult to read.

      We have revised the figure captions to include more details as per the reviewer’s suggestion. 

      (4)  It would be helpful if the authors could define the meanings of noise (e.g., coefficient of variation?) and translational efficiency in the very beginning to avoid any confusion. It is also unclear to me whether the noise from the experimental data is defined according to protein numbers or concentrations, which is presumably important since budding yeasts are growing cells. 

      For all published datasets where we had measurements from many genes/promoters, we used the measures of adjusted noise (for mRNA noise) and Distance-tomedian (DM, for protein noise). These measures of noise are corrected mean-dependence of expression noise. For simulations, which we performed for a single gene, and for experiments that we performed on a limited number of promoters, we used the measure of coefficient of variation (CV) to quantify noise, as calculation of adjusted noise or DM was not possible for a single gene. We now mention this in line 123-124. We used the measure of protein synthesis rate per mRNA as the measure of translational efficiency (Riba et al., 2019; line 100). Alternatively, we also used tRNA adaptation index (tAI) as a measure of translational efficiency, as codon choice could also influence the translation rate per mRNA molecule (Tuller et al., 2010) (line 193). 

      The protein noise was quantified from the signal intensity of GFP tagged proteins (Newman et al., 2006; and our data), which was proportional to protein numbers without considering cell volume. For quantification of noise at the mRNA level, single-cell RNA-seq data was used, which provided mRNA numbers in individual cells.  

      (5) The conclusions from Figures 1D and 1E are not new. For example, the constant protein noise as a function of mean protein expression is a known result of the two-state model of gene expression, e.g., see Equation (4) in Paulsson, Physics of Life Reviews 2005.

      Yes, they may not be new, but we included these results for setting the baseline for comparison with simulation results that appear in the later part of the manuscript where we included translational bursting and ribosome demand in our models. 

      (6) In Figure 4C-D, it is unclear to me how the authors changed the mean protein expression if the translation initiation rate is a function of variation in mRNA number and other random variables.

      The translation initiation rate varied from a basal translation initiation rate depending on the mRNA numbers and other variables. We changed the basal translation initiation rate to alter the mean protein expression levels. We have now elaborated the modelling section to incorporate these details in the revised manuscript (lines 404 to 412). 

      (7) If I understand correctly, the authors somehow changed the translation initiation rate to change the mean protein expression in Figures 4C-D. However, the authors changed the protein sequences in the experimental data of Figure 6. I am not sure if the comparison between simulations and experimental data is appropriate.

      It is an important observation. Even though we changed the basal translation initiation rate to change the mean expression (Fig. 4C-D), we noted in the description of the model that the changes in the translation initiation rate were also linked to changes in the translation elongation rate (Fig. 3D). Thus, an increase in the translation initiation rate was associated with faster ribosome traversal through an mRNA molecule. This has also been observed in an experimental study by Barrington et al. (2023). Therefore, the models can also be expressed in terms of the translation elongation rate or ribosome traversal speed, instead of the translation initiation rate, and this modification will not change the results of the simulations due to interconnectedness of the initiation rate and the elongation rate.  

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Minor comments:

      (1)  The discussion from lines 180 to 182 appears consistent with Figure 1E. It seems that the twostate model can already explain why the genes with high burst frequency and high protein synthesis rate showed a small protein noise. It is unclear to me the purpose of this discussion.

      Yes, the results from Fig. 1E were from stochastic simulations, whereas the results discussed in the lines 191 to 193 (in the revised manuscript) were based on our analysis of experimental data that is shown in Fig. 2D.

      (2)  If I understand correctly, "translational efficiency" is the same as "protein synthesis rate" in this work. It would be helpful if the authors could keep the same notation throughout the paper to avoid confusion.

      The protein synthesis rate per mRNA molecule is the best measure of translational efficiency, and we used the experimental data from Riba et al. (2019) for this purpose (line 99-100). Alternatively, we also used tRNA Adaptation Index (tAI) as a measure of translational efficiency, as the codon choice also influences the rate at which an mRNA molecule is translated (Tuller et al., 2010) (line 192). 

      (3) On line 227, does "higher translation rate" mean "higher translation initiation rate"? The same issues happen in a few places in this paper.

      Corrected now (line 243 in the revised manuscript and throughout the manuscript). 

      (4) The discussion from lines 296 to 301 is unclear. It is not obvious to me how the authors obtained the conclusion that lowering translational efficiency would decrease the protein expression noise.

      High translational efficiency will require more ribosomes and hence, will increase ribosome demand. If ribosome demand is the molecular basis of high expression noise for genes with bursty transcription and high translational efficiency, then we can expect a reduction in ribosome demand and a reduction in noise if we lower the translational efficiency. We have rephrased this section for clarity between the lines 334 and 339 in the revised manuscript.   

      (5)  On line 324, should slower translation mean a shorter distance between neighboring ribosomes? One can imagine the extreme limit in which ribosomes move very slowly so that the mRNA is fully packed with ribosomes. 

      Slower translation or ribosome traversal rate would also lower the translation initiation rate (Barrington et al., 2023). Slower traversal of ribosomes reduces the chances of collision in case of transient slow-down of ribosomes due to occurrence of one or more non-preferred codons. We have now clarified this part in the lines 360 to 369 in the revised manuscript.

      (6) The text from lines 423 to 433 can be put in Methods.

      We have already added this part to the methods section (lines 900 to 910) and now minimize this discussion in the results section. 

      (7)  The discussion from lines 128 to 130 is unclear, and the statement appears to be consistent with the two-state model (see Figure 1E). The meaning of "initial mRNA numbers" is also unclear.

      An earlier study has proposed that essential genes in yeast employs high transcription and low translation strategy for expression, likely to maintain low expression noise in these genes and to prevent detrimental effects of high expression noise (Fraser et al., 2004). However, there has been no direct supportive evidence. Therefore, we were testing whether the differences in mRNA levels and translational efficiency of genes can lead to differences in protein noise through stochastic simulations. The discussion between the lines 130 and 132 in the revised manuscript summarises the results of the simulations. 

      Initial mRNA numbers - mRNA copy numbers that are present in the cell at the start of stochastic simulations. However, we have now changed it to ‘mRNA levels’ in the revised manuscript for clarity (line 131 in the revised manuscript).

      (8)  On line 212, is the translation initiation rate TL_init the same thing as beta_p in Figure 3A?

      βp refers to the rate of protein synthesis, which is influenced by the translational burst kinetics as well as the translation initiation rate, whereas TLinit refers to the translation initiation rate. So, these parameters are related, but are not the same.

    1. eLife Assessment

      Floeder and colleagues provide an important investigation that describes the experimental conditions that systematically produce "ramps" in dopamine signaling in the striatum. This somewhat nebulous feature of dopamine has been a significant part of recent theoretical and computational debates attempting to formally describe the different timescales on which dopamine functions. The current results are convincing and add context to that ongoing work.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this study, Floedder et al report that dopamine ramps in both Pavlovian and Instrumental conditions are shaped by reward interval statistics. Dopamine ramps are an interesting phenomenon because at first glance they do not represent the classical reward prediction errors associated with dopamine signaling. Instead, they seem somewhat to bridge the gap between tonic and phasic dopamine, with an intense discussion still being held in the field about what is their actual behavioral role. Here, in tests with head-fixed mice, and dopamine being recorded with a genetically encoded fluorescent sensor in the nucleus accumbens, the authors find that dopamine ramps were only present when intertrial intervals were relatively short and the structure of the task (Pavlovian cue or progression in a VR corridor) contained elements that indicated progression towards the reward (e.g., a dynamic cue). The authors propose that although these findings can be explained by classical theories of dopamine function, they are better explained by their model of Adjusted Net Contingency of Causal Relation (ANCCR). The results of this study provide constraints on future models of dopamine function, and are of high interest to the field.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      In this manuscript by Floeder et al., the authors report a correlation between ITI duration and the strength of a dopamine ramp occurring in the time between a predictive conditioned stimulus and a subsequent reward. They found this relationship occurring within two different tasks with mice, during both a Pavlovian task as well as an instrumental virtual visual navigation task. Additionally, they observed this relationship only in conditions when using a dynamic predictive stimulus. The authors relate this finding to their previously published model ANCCR in which the time constant of the eligibility trace is proportionate to the reward rate within the task.

      The relationship between ITI duration and the extent of a dopamine ramp which the authors have reported is very intriguing and certainly provides an important constraint for models for dopamine function. As such, these findings are potentially highly impactful to the field.

    4. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Floeder and colleagues measure dopamine signaling in the nucleus accumbens core using fiber photometry of the dLight sensor, in Pavlovian and instrumental tasks in mice. They test some predictions from a recently proposed model (ANCCR) regarding the existence of "ramps" in dopamine that have been seen in some previous research, the characteristics of which remain poorly understood.

      They find that cues signaling a progression toward rewards (akin to a countdown) specifically promote ramping dopamine signaling in the nucleus accumbens core, but only when the intertrial interval just experienced was short. This work is discussed in the context of ongoing theoretical conceptions of dopamine's role in learning.

      This work is the clearest demonstration to date of concrete training factors that seem to directly impact whether or not dopamine ramps occur. The existence of ramping signals has long been a feature of debates in the dopamine literature and this work adds important context to that. Further, as a practical assessment of the impact of a relatively simple trial structure manipulation on dopamine patterns, this work will be important for guiding future studies. These studies are well done and thoughtfully presented. The additional data, analyses, and discussion in the revised version of the paper add strength and clarity to the conclusions.

      The current results raise interesting questions regarding what, if any potential function cue-reward interval dopamine ramps serve. In the current data, licking behavior was similar on different trial types and was not related to ramping activity.

    5. Author Response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      In this study, Floedder et al report that dopamine ramps in both Pavlovian and Instrumental conditions are shaped by reward interval statistics. Dopamine ramps are an interesting phenomenon because at first glance they do not represent the classical reward prediction errors associated with dopamine signaling. Instead, they seem somewhat to bridge the gap between tonic and phasic dopamine, with an intense discussion still being held in the field about what is their actual behavioral role. Here, in tests with head-fixed mice, and dopamine being recorded with a genetically encoded fluorescent sensor in the nucleus accumbens, the authors find that dopamine ramps were only present when intertrial intervals were relatively short and the structure of the task (Pavlovian cue or progression in a VR corridor) contained elements that indicated progression towards the reward (e.g., a dynamic cue). The authors show that these findings are well explained by their previously published model of Adjusted Net Contingency of Causal Relation (ANCCR).

      Strengths:

      This descriptive study delineates some fundamental parameters that define dopamine ramps in the studied conditions. The short, objective, and to-the-point format of the manuscript is great and really does a service to potential readers. The authors are very careful with the scope of their conclusions, which is appreciated by this reviewer.

      We thank the reviewer for their overall support of the formatting and scope of the manuscript. 

      Weaknesses:

      The discussion of the results is very limited to the conceptual framework of the authors' preferred model (which the authors do recognize, but it still is a limitation). The correlation analysis presented in panel l of Figure 3 seems unnecessary at best and could be misleading, as it is really driven by the categorical differences between the two conditions that were grouped for this analysis. There are some key aspects of the data and their relationship with each other, the previous literature, and the methods used to collect them, that could have been better discussed and explored.

      We agree with the reviewer that a weakness of the discussion was the limited framing of the results within the ANCCR model. To address this, we have expanded our introduction and discussion sections to provide a more thorough explanation of our model and possible leading alternatives.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out that Figure 3l may be misleading for readers; we removed this panel from the revised Figure 4.

      We have further addressed the specific concerns raised by the reviewer in their comments to the authors. Indeed, we agree with the reviewer that the original manuscript was narrow in its focus regarding relationships between different aspects of the data. To more thoroughly explore how key variables – including dopamine ramp slope and onset response as well as licking behavior slope – could relate to each other, we have added Extended Data Figure 8. In this figure, we show that no correlations exist between any of these key variables in either dynamic tone condition; it is our hope that this additional analysis highlights the significance of the clear relationship between dopamine ramp slope and ITI duration. 

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      In this manuscript by Floeder et al., the authors report a correlation between ITI duration and the strength of a dopamine ramp occurring in the time between a predictive conditioned stimulus and a subsequent reward. They found this relationship occurring within two different tasks with mice, during both a Pavlovian task as well as an instrumental virtual visual navigation task. Additionally, they observed this relationship only in conditions when using a dynamic predictive stimulus. The authors relate this finding to their previously published model ANCCR in which the time constant of the eligibility trace is proportionate to the reward rate within the task.

      The relationship between ITI duration and the extent of a dopamine ramp which the authors have reported is very intriguing and certainly provides an important constraint for models for dopamine function. As such, these findings are potentially highly impactful to the field. I do have a few questions for the authors which are written below.

      We thank the reviewer for their interest in our findings and belief in their potential to be impactful in the field. 

      (1) I was surprised to see a lack of counterbalance within the Pavlovian design for the order of the long vs short ITI. Ramping of the lick rate does increase from the long-duration ITIs to the short-duration ITI sessions. Although of course, this increase in ramping of the licking across the two conditions is not necessarily a function of learning, it doesn't lend support to the opposite possibility that the timing of the dynamic CS hasn't reached asymptotic learning by the end of the long-duration ITI. The authors do reference papers in which overtraining tends to result in a reduction of ramping, which would argue against this possibility, yet differential learning of the dynamic CS would presumably be required to observe this effect. Do the authors have any evidence that the effect is not due to heightened learning of the timing of the dynamic CS across the experiment?

      We appreciate the reviewer expressing their surprise regarding the lack of counterbalance in our Pavlovian experimental design. We previously did not explicitly do this because the ramps disappeared in the short ITI/fixed tone condition, indicating that their presence is not just a matter of total experience in the task. However, we agree that this is incidental, but not direct evidence. To address this drawback, we repeated the Pavlovian experiment in a new cohort of animals with a revised training order, switching conditions such that the short ITI/dynamic tone (SD) condition preceded the long ITI/dynamic tone (LD) condition (see revised Figure 2a). Despite this change in the training order, the main findings remain consistent: positive dLight slopes (i.e., dopamine ramps) are only observed in the SD condition (Figure 2b-d). 

      We thank the reviewer for raising these questions regarding licking behavior and learning and their relationship with dopamine ramps. Indeed, a closer look at the average licking behavior reveals subtle differences across conditions (Figure 1f and Extended Data Figure 5a). While the average lick rate during the ramp window does not differ across conditions (Extended Data Figure 5c), the ramping of the lick rate during this window is higher for dynamic tone conditions compared to fixed tone conditions (Extended Data Figure 5d). Despite these differences, we still believe that the main comparison between the dopamine slope in the SD vs LD condition remains valid given their similar lick ramping slopes. Furthermore, our primary measure of learning is not lick slope, but anticipatory lick rate during the 1 s trace preceding reward delivery, which is robustly nonzero across cohorts and conditions (Figure 1g and Extended Data Figure 5b). 

      Taken together, we hope that the results from our counterbalanced Pavlovian training and more rigorous analysis of lick behavior across conditions provide sufficient evidence to assuage concerns that the differences in ramping dopamine simply reflect differences in learning. 

      (2) The dopamine response, as measured by dLight, seems to drop after the reward is delivered. This reduction in responding also tends to be observed with electrophysiological recordings of dopamine neurons. It seems possible that during the short ITI sessions, particularly on the shorter ITI duration trials, that dopamine levels may still be reduced from the previous trial at the onset of the CS on the subsequent trial. Perhaps the authors can observe the dynamics of the recovery of the dopamine response following a reward delivery on longer-duration ITIs in order to determine how quickly dopamine is recovering following a reward delivery. Are the trials with very short ITIs occurring within this period that dopamine is recovering from the previous trial? If so, how much of the effect may be due to this effect? It should be noted that the lack of observance of a ramp on the condition of shortduration ITIs with fixed CSs provides a potential control for this effect, yet the extent to which a natural ramp might occur following sucrose deliveries should be investigated.

      We thank the reviewer for highlighting the possibility that ramps may be due to the dopamine response recovery following reward delivery. Given that peak reward dopamine responses tend to be larger in long ITI conditions, however, we felt that it was inappropriate to compare post-reward dopamine recovery times across conditions. Instead, we decided to directly compare the dLight slope 2s before cue onset (“pre-cue window,” a proxy for recovery from previous trial) with the dLight slope during our ramp window from 3 to 8s after cue onset (Extended Data Figure 6a). There were no significant differences in pre-cue dLight slope across conditions (Extended Data Figure 6b); this suggests that the ramping slopes seen in the SD condition, but not other conditions, is not simply due to the natural dopamine recovery response following reward delivery. Furthermore, if the dopamine ramps observed in the SD condition were a continuation of the post-reward dopamine recovery from the previous trial, we would expect to see a positive correlation between the dLight slope before and during the cue. However, there is no such correlation between the dLight slopes in the ramp window vs. pre-cue window in the SD condition (Extended Data Figure 6c-d). We believe that this observation, along with the builtin control of the SF condition mentioned by the reviewer, serves as evidence against the possibility of our ramp results being due to a natural ramp after reward delivery.

      (3) The authors primarily relate the finding of the correlation between the ITI and the slope of the ramp to their ANCCR model by suggesting that shorter time constants of the eligibility trace will result in more precisely timed predictors of reward across discrete periods of the dynamic cue. Based on this prediction, would the change in slope be more gradual, and perhaps be more correlated with a broader cumulative estimate of reward rate than just a single trial?

      To clarify, we do not propose that a smaller eligibility trace time constant results in more precise timing per se. Instead, we believe that the rapid eligibility trace decay from smaller time constants gives greater causal predictive power for later periods in the dynamic cue (see Extended Data Figure 1) since the memory of the earlier periods of the cue is weaker. 

      We appreciate the reviewer’s curiosity regarding the influence of a broader cumulative estimate of reward vs. only the immediately preceding ITI on dopamine ramp slopes. Indeed, in several instrumental tasks (e.g., Krausz et al., Neuron, 2023), recent reward rate modulates the magnitude of dopamine ramps, making this an important variable to investigate. We chose to use linear regression for each mouse separately to analyze the relationship between the trial dopamine slope and the average previous ITI for the past 1 through 10 most recent trials. In the SD condition, as reported in our earlier manuscript, there was a significantly negative dependence of trial dopamine slope with the single previous ITI (i.e., if the previous ITI was long, the next trial tends to have a weaker ramp). This negative dependence, however, only held for a single previous trial; there was no clear relationship between the per-trial dopamine slope and the average of the past 2 through 10 ITIs (Extended Data Figure 7a). For the LD condition, on the other hand, there is no clear relationship between the per-trial dopamine slope and the average previous ITI for any of the past 1 through 10 trials, with one exception: there is a significantly negative dependence of trial dopamine slope with the average ITI of the previous 2 trials (Extended Data Figure 7b). This longer timescale relationship in the LD condition suggests that the adaptation of the eligibility trace time constant is nuanced and depends on the general ITI length. 

      In general, though we reason that the eligibility trace time constant should depend on overall event rates, we do not currently propose a real-time update rule for the eligibility trace time constant depending on recent event rates. Accordingly, we are currently agnostic about the actual time scale of history of recent event rate calculation that mediates the eligibility trace time constant. Our experimental results suggest that when the ITI is generally short for Pavlovian conditioning, the eligibility trace time constant adapts to ITI on a rapid timescale. However, only a small fraction of the variability of this rapid fluctuation is captured by recent ITI history. A more thorough investigation of this real-time update rule would need to be done in the future.

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      Floeder and colleagues measure dopamine signaling in the nucleus accumbens core using fiber photometry of the dLight sensor, in Pavlovian and instrumental tasks in mice. They test some predictions from a recently proposed model (ANCCR) regarding the existence of "ramps" in dopamine that have been seen in some previous research, the characteristics of which remain poorly understood.

      They find that cues signaling a progression toward rewards (akin to a countdown) specifically promote ramping dopamine signaling in the nucleus accumbens core, but only when the intertrial interval just experienced was short. This work is discussed in the context of ongoing theoretical conceptions of dopamine's role in learning.

      Strengths:

      This work is the clearest demonstration to date of concrete training factors that seem to directly impact whether or not dopamine ramps occur. The existence of ramping signals has long been a feature of debates in the dopamine literature and this work adds important context to that. Further, as a practical assessment of the impact of a relatively simple trial structure manipulation on dopamine patterns, this work will be important for guiding future studies. These studies are well done and thoughtfully presented.

      We thank the reviewer for recognizing the context that our study adds to the dopamine literature and the potential for our experiments to guide future work. 

      Weaknesses:

      It remains somewhat unclear what limits are in place on the extent to which an eligibility trace is reflected in dopamine signals. In the current study, a specific set of ITIs was used, and one wonders if the relative comparison of ITI/history variables ("shorter" or "longer") is a factor in how the dopamine signal emerges, in addition to the explicit length ("short" or "long") of the ITI. Another experimental condition, where variable ITIs were intermingled, could perhaps help clarify some remaining questions.

      Though we used ITIs of fixed means, due to the exponential nature of their distribution, we did intermingle ITIs of various durations in both our long and short ITI conditions. The distribution of ITI durations is visualized in Figure 1c for Pavlovian conditioning and Extended Data Figure 9b for VR navigation. 

      The relative comparison between consecutive ITIs was not something we originally explored, so we thank the reviewer for wondering how it impacts the dopamine signal. To investigate this, we quantified both the change in ITI (+ or - Δ ITI for relatively longer or shorter, respectively) and the change in dopamine ramp slope between consecutive trials in the SD condition (Figure 3d). Across each mouse separately, we found a significantly negative relationship between Δ slope and Δ ITI (Figure 3e-f). Also, the average Δ slope was significantly greater for consecutive trials with a Δ ITI below -1 s compared to trials with a Δ ITI above +1 s (Figure 3g). Altogether, these findings suggest that relative comparison of ITIs does correlate with changes in the dopamine signal; a relatively longer ITI tends to have a weaker ramp, which fits in nicely with the expected inverse relationship between ITI and dopamine ramp slope from our ANCCR model.

      In both tasks, cue onset responses are larger, and longer on long ITI trials. One concern is that this larger signal makes seeing a ramp during the cue-reward interval harder, especially with a fluorescence method like photometry. Examining the traces in Figure 1i - in the long, dynamic cue condition the dopamine trace has not returned to baseline at the time of the "ramp" window onset, but the short dynamic trace has. So one wonders if it's possible the overall return to baseline trend in the long dynamic conditions might wash out a ramp.

      This is a good point, and we thank the reviewer for raising it. Certainly, the cue onset response is significantly larger in long ITI conditions (see Figure 1i-j and Figure 4h-j). To avoid any bleed over effect, we intentionally chose ramp window periods during later portions of the trial (in line with work from others e.g., Kim et al., Cell, 2020). While the cue onset dopamine pulse seems to have flatlined by the start of the ramp window period, the dopamine levels clearly remain elevated relative to pre-cue baseline. This type of signal has been observed with fiber photometry in other Pavlovian conditioning paradigms with long cue durations (e.g., Jeong et al., Science, 2022). Because of the persistently elevated dopamine levels, it is certainly possible that a ramping signal during the cue is getting washed out; with the bulk fluorescence photometry technique we employed in this study, this possibility is unfortunately difficult to completely rule out. However, the long ITI/fixed tone (LF) condition could serve as a potential control given the overall similarity in the dopamine signal between the LF and LD conditions: both conditions have large cue onset responses with elevated dopamine throughout the duration of the cue (see Extended Data Figures 2c and 3c). Critically, the LD condition lacks a noticeable ramp despite the dynamic tone providing information on temporal proximity to reward, which is thought to be necessary for dopamine ramps to occur. Importantly, regardless of whether a ramp is masked in the long ITI dynamic condition, most studies investigate such a condition in isolation and would report the absence of dopamine ramps. Thus, at a descriptive level, we believe it remains true that observable dopamine ramps are only present when the ITI is short. 

      Not a weakness of this study, but the current results certainly make one ponder the potential function of cue-reward interval ramps in dopamine (assuming there is a determinable function). In the current data, licking behavior was similar on different trial types, and that is described as specifically not explaining ramp activity.

      We agree that this work naturally raises the question of the function of dopamine ramps. However, selective and precise manipulation of only the dopamine ramps without altering other features such as phasic responses, or inducing dopamine dips, is highly technically challenging at this moment; due to this challenge, we intentionally focused on the conditions that determine the presence or absence of dopamine ramps rather than their function. We agree with the reviewer that studying the specific function of dopamine ramps is an interesting future question. 

      Reviewing Editor:

      The reviewers felt the results are of considerable and broad interest to the neuroscience community, but that the framing in terms of ANCCR undermined the scope of the findings as did the brief nature of the formatting of the manuscript. In addition, the reviewers felt that the relationship between ramp dynamics, behavior, and ITI conditions requires more in-depth analyses. Relatedly, the lack of counterbalancing of the ITI durations was considered to be a drawback and needs to be addressed as it may affect the baseline. Addressing these issues in a satisfactory manner would improve the assessment of the manuscript to important/convincing.

      We truly appreciate the valuable feedback provided on this manuscript by all three reviewers and the reviewing editor. Based on this input, we have significantly revised the manuscript to address the issues brought up by the reviewers. Firstly, we have conducted additional experiments to counterbalance the ITI conditions for Pavlovian conditioning; this strengthened our results by confirming our original findings that ITI duration, rather than training order, is the key variable controlling the presence or absence of dopamine ramps. Secondly, we completed more rigorous analyses to further explore the relationship between dopamine dynamics, animal behavior, and ITI duration; we generally found no significant correlations between these variables, with a notable exception being our main finding between ITI duration and dopamine ramp slope. Finally, we revised and expanded our writing to both explain predictions from our ANCCR model in less technical language and explore how alternative theoretical frameworks could potentially explain our findings. In doing so, we hope that our manuscript is now more accessible and of interest to a broad audience of neuroscience readers.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      The study could be improved if the authors performed a more detailed comparison of how other theoretical frameworks, beyond ANCCR could account for the observed findings. Also, the correlation analysis presented in the panel I of Figure 3 seems unnecessary and potentially spurious, as the slope of the correlation is clearly mostly driven by the categorical differences between the two ITI conditions, which were combined for the analysis - it's not clear what is the value of this analysis beyond the group comparison presented in the following panel.

      Again, we thank the reviewer for elaborating on their concern regarding Figure 3l – we have removed it from the revised Figure 4. 

      The relationship between ramp dynamics with the behavior and the large differences in cue onset responses between short and long ITI conditions could have been better explored. If I understand correctly the overarching proposal of this and other publications by this group, then the differences in cue responses is determined by the spacing of rewards in a somewhat similar way that the ramps are. So, is there a trial-by-trial correlation between the amplitude of the cue responses and the slope of the ramps? Is there a correlation between any of these two measures with the licking behavior, and if so, does it change with the ITI condition? A more thorough exploration of these relationships would help support the proposal of the primacy of inter-event spacing in determining the different types of dopamine responses in learning.

      There are certainly interesting relationships between dopamine dynamics, behavior, and ITI that we failed to explore in our original manuscript – we appreciate the reviewer bringing them up. We found no correlation between dopamine ramp slope and cue onset response in either the SD or LD condition (Extended Data Fig 8a-b). Moreover, we found no correlation between either of these variables and the trial-by-trial licking behavior (Extended Data Fig 8c-f). Finally, there is no relationship between licking behavior and previous ITI duration (Extended Data Fig 8g-h), suggesting that behavioral differences do not account for differences in the dopamine ramp slope. Together, the lack of significant relationships between these other variables highlights the specific, clear relationship between ITI duration and dopamine ramp slope. 

      Finally, another issue I feel could have been better discussed is how the particular settings of both tasks might be biasing the results. For example, there is an issue to be considered about how the dopamine ramp dynamics reported here, especially the requirement of a dynamic cue for ramps to be present, square with the previous published results by one of the authors - Mohebi et al, Nature, 2019. In that manuscript, rats were executing a bandit task where, to this reviewer's understanding, there was no explicit dynamic cue aside from the standard sensory feedback of the rats moving around in the behavior boxes to approach a nose poke port. Is the idea that this sensory feedback could function as a dynamic cue? If that's the case, then this short-scale, movement-related feedback should also function as a dynamic cue in a freely moving Pavlovian condition, when the animals must also move towards a reward delivery port, right? Therefore, could it be that the experimental "requirement" of a dynamic cue is only present in a head-fixed condition? One could phrase this in a different way to Steelman and potentially further the authors' proposal: perhaps in any slightly more naturalistic setting, the interaction of the animals with their environment always functions as a dynamic cue indicating proximity to reward, and this relationship was experimentally isolated by the use of head fixation (but not explicitly compared with a freely moving condition) in the present study. I think that would be an interesting alternative to consider and discuss, and perhaps explore experimentally at some point.

      We thank the reviewer for raising this important point regarding the influence of our experimental settings on our results. At first glance, it could appear that our results demonstrating the necessity of a dynamic cue for ramps in a head-fixed setting do not fit neatly with other results in a freely moving setup (e.g., Collins et al., Scientific Reports, 2016; Mohebi et al., Nature, 2019). Exactly as the reviewer states though, we believe that sensory feedback from the environment in freely moving preparations serves the same function as a dynamic progression of cues. We have considered the implications of methodological differences between head-fixed and freely moving preparations in the discussion section. 

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      This comment relates indirectly to comment 3, in that the authors intermix theory throughout the manuscript. I think this would be fine if the experiment was framed directly in terms of ANCCR, but the authors specifically mention that this experiment wasn't developed to distinguish between different theories. As such, it seems difficult to assess the scope of the comments regarding theory within the paper because they tend to be specifically related to ANCCR. For instance, the last comment has broad implications of how the ramp might be related to the overall reward rate, an interesting finding that constrains classes of dopamine models rather than evidence just for ANCCR. Perhaps adding a discussion section that allows the authors to focus more on theory would be beneficial for this manuscript.

      We appreciate this suggestion by the reviewer. We have updated both our introduction and discussion sections to elaborate more thoroughly on theory.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      The paper could potentially benefit from the use of more accessible language to describe the conceptual basis of the work, and the predictions, and a bit of reformatting away from the brief structure with lots of supplemental discussion.

      For example, in the introduction, the line - "Varying the ITI was critical because our theory predicts that the ITI is a variable controlling the eligibility trace time constant, such that a short ITI would produce a small time constant relative to the cue-reward interval (Supplementary Note 1)". As far as I can tell, this is meant to get across the notion that dopamine represents some aspect of the time between rewards - dopamine signals will differ for cues following short vs long intervals between rewards.

      As written, the language of the paper takes a fair bit of parsing, but the notions are actually pretty simple. This is partly due to the brief format the paper is written in, where familiarity with the previous papers describing ANCCR is assumed.

      From a readability standpoint, and the potential impact of the paper on a broad audience, perhaps this could be considered as a point for revision.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out the drawbacks of our technical language and brief formatting. To address this, we have removed the majority of the supplementary notes and expanded our introduction and discussion sections. In doing so, we hope that the conceptual foundations of this work, and potential alternative theoretical explanations, are accessible and impactful for a broad audience of readers.

    1. eLife Assessment

      This valuable study by Wu and Zhou combines neurophysiological recordings and computational modelling to address an interesting question regarding the sequence of events from sensing to action. Neurophysiological evidence remains incomplete: explicit mapping of saccade-related activity in the same neurons and a better understanding of the influence of the spatial configuration of stimulus and targets would be required to pinpoint whether such activity might contribute, even partially, to the observed results and interpretations. These results are of interest for neuroscientists investigating decision-making.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, the authors recorded activity in the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) of monkeys performing a perceptual decision-making task. The monkeys were first shown two choice dots of two different colors. Then, they saw a random dot motion stimulus. They had to learn to categorize the direction of motion as referring to either the right or left dot. However, the rule was based on the color of the dot and not its location. So, the red dot could either be to the right or left, but the rule itself remained the same. It is known from past work that PPC neurons would code the learned categorization. Here, the authors showed that the categorization signal depended on whether the executed saccade was in the same hemifield as the recorded PPC neuron or in the opposite one. That is, if a neuron categorized the two motion directions such that it responded stronger for one than the other, then this differential motion direction coding effect was amplified if the subsequent choice saccade was in the same hemifield. The authors then built a computational RNN to replicate the results and make further tests by simulated "lesions".

      Strengths:

      Linking the results to RNN simulations and simulated lesions.

      Weaknesses:

      Potential interpretational issues due to a lack of explicit evidence on the sizes and locations of the response fields of the neurons. For example, is the contra/ipsi effect explained by the fact that in the contra condition, the response target and the saccade might have infringed on the outer edges of the response fields?

    3. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      This valuable study by Wu and Zhou combined neurophysiological recordings and computational modelling to investigate the neural mechanisms that underpin the interaction between sensory evaluation and action selection. The neurophysiological results suggest non-linear modulation of decision-related LIP activity by action selection, but some further analysis would be helpful in order to understand whether these results can be generalised to LIP circuitry or might be dependent on specific spatial task configurations. The authors present solid computational evidence that this might be due to projections from choice target representations. These results are of interest for neuroscientists investigating decision-making.

      Strengths:

      Wu and Zhou combine awake behaving neurophysiology for a sophisticated, flexible visual-motion discrimination task and a recurrent network model to disentangle the contribution of sensory evaluation and action selection to LIP firing patterns. The correct saccade response direction for preferred motion direction choices is randomly interleaved between contralateral and ipsilateral response targets, which allows the dissociation of perceptual choice from saccade direction.

      The neurophysiological recordings from area LIP indicate non-linear interaction between motion categorisation decisions and saccade choice direction.

      The careful investigation of a recurrent network model suggests that feedback from choice target representations to an earlier sensory evaluation stage might be the source for this non-linear modulation and that it is an important circuit component for behavioural performance.

      The paper presents a possible solution to a central controversy about the role of LIP in perceptual decision-making, but see below.

      Weaknesses:

      The paper presents a possible solution to a central controversy about the role of LIP in perceptual decision-making. However, the authors could be more clear and upfront about their interpretational framework and potential alternative interpretations.

      Centrally, the authors' model and experimental data appears to test only that LIP carries out sensory evaluation in its RFs. The model explicitly parks the representation of choice targets outside the "LIP" module receiving sensory input. The feedback from this separate target representation provides then the non-linear modulation that matches the neurophysiology. However, they ignore the neurophysiological results that LIP neurons can also represent motor planning to a saccade target.

      The neurophysiological results with a modulation of the direction tuning by choice direction (contralateral vs ipsilateral) are intriguing. However, the evaluation of the neurophysiological results are difficult, because some of the necessary information is missing to exclude alternative explanations. It would be good to see the actual distributions and sizes of the RF, which were determined based on visual responses not with a delayed saccade task. There might be for example a simple spatial configuration, for example, RF and preferred choice target in the same (contralateral) hemifield, for which there is an increase in firing. It is a shame that we do not see what these neurons would do if only a choice target would be put in the RF, as has been done in so many previous LIP experiments. The authors exclude also some spatial task configurations (vertical direction decisions), which makes it difficult to judge whether these data and models can be generalised. The whole section is difficult to follow, partly also because it appears to mix reporting results with interpretation (e.g. "feedback").

      The model and its investigation is very interesting and thorough, but given the neurophysiological literature on LIP, it is not clear that the target module would need to be in a separate brain area, but could be local circuitry within LIP between different neuron types.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, the authors recorded activity in the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) of monkeys performing a perceptual decision-making task. The monkeys were first shown two choice dots of two different colors. Then, they saw a random dot motion stimulus. They had to learn to categorize the direction of motion as referring to either the right or left dot. However, the rule was based on the color of the dot and not its location. So, the red dot could either be to the right or left, but the rule itself remained the same. It is known from past work that PPC neurons would code the learned categorization. Here, the authors showed that the categorization signal depended on whether the executed saccade was in the same hemifield as the recorded PPC neuron or in the opposite one. That is, if a neuron categorized the two motion directions such that it responded stronger for one than the other, then this differential motion direction coding effect was amplified if the subsequent choice saccade was in the same hemifield. The authors then built a computational RNN to replicate the results and make further tests by simulated "lesions".

      Strengths:

      Linking the results to RNN simulations and simulated lesions.

      Weaknesses:

      Potential interpretational issues due to a lack of evidence on what happens at the time of the saccades.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      (1) The neurophysiological results with a modulation of the direction tuning by choice direction are intriguing. However, the evaluation of the neurophysiological results are difficult because some of the necessary information is missing to exclude alternative explanations.

      We thank the reviewer for the helpful comments. We have addressed this point in detail in the following response.

      (a) Clearly state in the results how the response field "RF", where the stimulus was placed, was mapped. The methods give as "MGS"" (i.e., spatial selectivity during stimulus presentation and delay)" task rather than the standard delayed saccade. And also "while for those neurons which did not show a clear RF during the MGS task, we presented motion stimuli in the positions (always in the visual field contralateral to the recorded hemisphere) in which neurons exhibited the strongest response to the motion stimuli." All this sounds more like a sensory receptive field not an eye movement response filed". What was the exact task and criterion?

      We agree with the reviewer that the original description of how we mapped the response fields (RFs) of LIP neurons lacked sufficient detail. In this study, we used the memory-guided saccade (MGS) task to map the RFs of all isolated LIP neurons. Both MGS and delayed saccade tasks are commonly used to map a neuron's response field in previous decision-making studies.

      In the MGS task, monkeys initially fixate on the center of the screen. Subsequently, a dot randomly flashes at one of the eight possible locations surrounding the fixation dot with an eccentricity of 8 degree, requiring the monkeys to memorize the location of the flashed dot. After a delay of 1000 ms, the monkeys are instructed to saccade to the remembered location once the fixation dot disappears. The MGS task is a standard behavior task for mapping visual, memory, and motor RFs, particularly in brain regions involved in eye movement planning and control, such as LIP, FEF, and the superior colliculus.

      We believe the reviewer's confusion may stem from whether we mapped the visual, memory, or motor RFs of LIP neurons in the current study, as these "RFs" are not always consistent across individual neurons. In our study, we primarily mapped the visual and memory RFs of each LIP neuron by analyzing their activity during both the target presentation and delay periods. To focus on sensory evaluation-related activity, we presented the visual motion stimulus within the visual-memory RF of each neuron. For neurons that did not show a significant visual-memory RF, we used a different approach: we tested the neurons with the main task by altering the spatial configuration of the task stimuli to identify the visual field that elicited the strongest response when the motion stimulus was presented within it. This approach was used to guide the placement of the stimulus during the recording sessions.

      Following the reviewer’s suggestion, we have added the following clarification to the results section to better describe how we mapped the RF of LIP neurons:

      ‘We used the memory-guided saccade (MGS) task, which is commonly employed in LIP studies, to map the receptive fields (RFs) of all isolated LIP neurons. Specifically, we mapped both the visual and memory RFs of each neuron by analyzing their activity during the target presentation and delay periods of the MGS task (see Methods).’.

      (b) l.85 / l126: What do you mean by "orthogonal to the axis of the neural RF" - was the RF shape asymmetric, if so how did you determine this? OR do you mean the motion direction axis? Please explain.

      We realized that the original description of this point may have been unclear and could lead to confusion. The axis of the neural RF refers to the line connecting the center of the RF (which coincides with the center of the motion stimulus) to the fixation dot. We have revised this sentence in the revised manuscript as follows:

      ‘To examine the neural activity related to the evaluation of stimulus motion, we presented the motion stimuli within the RF of each neuron, while positioning the saccade targets at locations orthogonal to the line connecting the center of the RF (which also marks the center of the motion stimulus) and the fixation dot.’

      (c) Behavioural task. Figure 1 - are these example session? Please state this clearly. Can you show the examples (psychometric function and reaction times) separated for trials where correct choice direction aligning with the motion preference (within 90 degrees) and those that did not?

      Figure 1 shows the averaged behavioral results from all recording sessions. We have added this detail in the revised legend of Figure 1.

      We are uncertain about the reviewer’s reference to the “correct choice direction aligning with the motion preference,” as the term “motion preference” is specific to the neuron response, which are different for different neurons recorded simultaneously using multichannel recording probe.

      Nonetheless, following the reviewer’s suggestion, we grouped the trials in each recording session into two groups based on the relationship between the saccade direction and the preferred motion direction of the identified LIP neuron during one example single-channel recording. Both the RT and the performance accuracy during one example session were shown in the following figure.

      Author response image 1.

      Give also the performance averaged across all sites included in this study and range.<br /> If performance does differ for different configuration, please, show that the main modulatory effect does not align with this distinction.

      To clarify this point, we have plotted performance accuracy and RTs for horizontal, oblique, and vertical target position configurations separately, which are shown for both monkeys in the following figures. We did not observe any systematic influences of task configurations on the monkeys' performance accuracy. While the RTs did differ across different configurations, we believe these differences are likely attributable to several factors, such as varying levels of familiarity introduced by our training process and the intrinsic RT difference between different saccade directions.

      Author response image 2.

      (d) Show the distribution of RF positions and the direction preferences for the recording sites included in the quantitative analysis of this study. (And if available, separately those excluded).

      Following the reviewer’s suggestion, we have plotted the centers of the RFs for all neurons with identifiable RFs, categorizing them by their preferred motion directions. To determine each neuron’s RF, we analyzed the average firing rates from both the target presentation and delay periods during each trial of the memory-guided saccade (MGS) task. The RF centers of neurons with significant RFs were determined through a two-step process. First, we selected neurons that exhibited significant RFs in the MGS based on the following criteria: 1) there must be a significant activity difference between the eight target locations, and 2) the mean activity during the selected periods should be significantly greater than the baseline activity during the fixation period. Second, we fitted the activity data from the eight conditions to a Gaussian distribution, using the center of the fitted distribution as the RF center. A significant proportion of neurons from both monkeys that exhibited significant response to motion stimuli did not exhibited notable RFs based our current method. The following figures show the distributions of RFs and motion direction preference for all LIP neurons with identifiable RFs separately for each monkey. Since this is not the focus of the current study, we are not planning to include this result in the revised manuscript.

      Author response image 3.

      (e) Following on from d), was there a systematic relationship between RF position or direction preference and modulation by choice direction? For instance could the responses be simply explained by an increase in modulation for choices into the same (contralateral) hemifield as where the stimulus was placed?

      The reviewer raised a good point. To address whether there was a systematic relationship between RF position or direction preference and modulation by choice direction, we calculated a modulation index for each neuron to quantify the influence of saccade direction on neuronal responses to motion stimuli. We then plotted the modulation index against the RF position for each LIP neuron, shown as following:

      Author response image 4.

      As shown in the figures above, neurons with RFs farther from the horizontal meridian were more likely to exhibit stronger modulation by the saccade direction, while neurons with RFs closer to the horizontal meridian showed inconsistent and weaker modulation. This is because when the RFs was on the horizontal meridian, saccade directions were aligned with the vertical axis (with no contralateral or ipsilateral directions). This is consistent with the finding in Figure S3—no significant differences in direction selectivity between the CT and IT conditions in the data sessions where the saccade targets were aligned close to the vertical direction. Since fewer than half of the identified neurons showed clear receptive fields using our method, the figure above did not include all the neurons used in the analysis in the manuscript. Therefore, we chose not to include this figure in the revised manuscript.

      Additionally, we quantified the relationship between the modulation index and direction preference for neurons in sessions where the monkeys’ saccades were aligned to either horizontal or oblique directions. As shown in the following figure, no systematic relationship was found between direction preference and modulation by the choice direction for LIP neurons at the population level.

      Author response image 5.

      We have added this result as Figure S 2 in the revised manuscript.

      Notably, the observed modulation of saccade direction on LIP neurons’ response to motion stimuli cannot be simply explained by saccade direction selectivity. We presented two more evidence to rule out such possibility in the original manuscript. First, the modulation effect we observed was nonlinear; specifically, the firing rate of neurons increased for the preferred motion direction but decreased for the non-preferred motion direction (Figure 2i and Figure S1A-D). This phenomenon is unlikely to be attributed to a linear gain modulation driven by saccade directions. Second, we plotted the averaged neural activity for contralateral and ipsilateral saccade directions separately, and found that LIP neurons showed similar levels of activity between two saccade directions (revised Figure 2L).

      Additionally, we added a paragraph in the Methods section to describe the way we calculated modulation index as follows:

      “We have calculated a modulation index for each neuron to reflect the influence of saccade direction on neuron’s response to visual stimuli. The modulation index is calculated as:

      where represents the average firing rate from 50ms to 250ms after sample onset for all contralateral saccade trails with a neuron’s preferred moving direction of visual stimuli. The naming conventions are the same for , , and . An MI value between 0 and 1 indicate higher modulation in contralateral saccade trials, and an MI value between -1 and 0 indicates higher modulation in ipsilateral saccade trials.”

      Please split Figures 2G,H,I J,K, by whether the RF was located contralaterally or ipsilaterally. If there are only a small number of ipsilateral RFs, please show these examples, perhaps in an appendix.

      This is a reasonable suggestion; however, it is not applicable to our study. Among all the neurons included in our analysis, only one neuron from each monkey exhibited ipsilateral receptive fields (RFs). Therefore, we believe it may not be necessary to plot the result for this outlier.

      (f) Were the choice targets always equi-distant from the stimulus and at what distance was this? Please give quantitative details in methods.

      The review was correct that the choice targets were always equidistant form the stimulus. The distance between the motion stimulus and the target was typically 12-15 degree. We have added the details in the revised Methods section as follows:

      ‘Therefore, the two saccade targets were equidistant from the stimulus, with the distance typically ranging from 12 to 15 degrees.

      (2) For Figure 3E, how do you explain that there is an up regulation of for contralateral choices before the stimulus onset, i.e. before the animal can make a decision? Is this difference larger for error trials?

      This is a good question, which we have attempted to clarify in the revised manuscript. We believe that the observed upregulation in neural activity for contralateral choices may reflect the monkeys’ internal choice bias or expectation (choice between two motion directions) prior to stimulus presentation, which could influence their subsequent decisions. In Figure 3E, we calculated the r-choice to assess the correlation between the neuron’s direction selectivity and the monkeys’ decisions on motion stimuli, separately for contralateral and ipsilateral choice conditions. The increased r-decision during the pre-stimulus period indicates stronger neural activity for trials in which the monkeys later reported that the upcoming stimulus was in the preferred direction, and weaker activity for trials where the stimulus was judged to be in the non-preferred direction. This correlation was more pronounced for contralateral choices than for ipsilateral ones. It is important to note that while the monkeys cannot predict the upcoming stimulus direction with greater-than-chance accuracy, these results suggest that pre-stimulus neural activity in LIP is correlated with the monkeys’ eventual decision for that trial. Furthermore, LIP neural activity was more strongly correlated with the monkeys’ decisions in the contralateral choice condition compared to the ipsilateral one.

      Additionally, we clarify that the r-decision was calculated using both correct and error trials. When comparing Figure 2J with Figure 2K, the correlation between neural activity and the monkeys’ upcoming decision during the pre-stimulus period was most prominent in low- and zero-coherence trials, where the monkeys either made more errors or based decisions on guesswork. We infer that the monkeys' confidence in these decisions was likely lower compared to high-coherence trials. Thus, the decision process appears to be influenced by pre-stimulus neural activity, particularly in low-coherence and zero-coherence trials.

      Although it is unclear precisely what covert process this pre-stimulus activity reflects, similar patterns of choice-predictive pre-stimulus activity have been observed in LIP and other brain areas (Shadlen, M.N. and Newsome,T.W., 2001; Coe, B., at al. 2002; Baso, M.A. and Wurtz, R.H., 1998; Z. M. Williams at al. 2003). We have clarified this point in the revised manuscript, including a revision of the relevant sentence in the Results section for clarity, shown as follows:

      “Furthermore, we used partial correlation analysis to examine decision- and stimulus-related components of DS (i.e., r-decision and r-stimulus, Figure 3E and 3F) using all four coherence levels. The decision-related component of LIP DS was significantly greater in the CT condition than in the IT condition (Figure 3E; nested ANOVA: P = 1.07e-6, F= 25.72), and this difference emerged even before motion stimulus onset. This suggests that the LIP DS was more closely correlated with monkeys’ decisions in the CT condition than in the IT condition. The upregulation in r-decision for contralateral choices may reflect the monkeys’ internal choice bias or expectation (choice between two motion directions) prior to stimulus presentation, which could influence their subsequent decisions more in the CT condition”

      (3) Figure 2K: what is the very large condition-independent contribution? It almost seems as most of what these neurons code for is neither saccade or motion related.

      The condition-independent contribution is the time-dependent component that is unrelated to saccade, motion, or their interaction. Our findings are consistent with previous methodological studies, where this time-dependent component was shown to account for a significant portion of the variance in population activity (Kobak, D. et al., 2016)

      (4) Abstract:

      a) "We found that the PPC activity related to monkeys' abstract decisions about visual stimuli was nonlinearly modulated by monkeys' following saccade choices directing outside each neuron's response field."

      This sentence is not clear/precise in two regards:

      Should "directing" be "directed"?

      Also, it is not just saccades directed outside the RF, but towards the contralateral hemifield.

      We thank the reviewer for the suggestion. We agree that ‘directing’ should be ‘directed’ and revised it accordingly. However, we do not believe that ‘directed outside each neuron's response field’ should be replaced with “towards the contralateral hemifield”. There are two major reasons. First, the modulation effect was identified as the difference between contralateral and ipsilateral saccade directions. We cannot conclude that the modulation mainly happened in the contralateral saccade direction. Second, we used ‘directed outside each neuron's response field’ to emphasize that this modulation cannot be simply explained by saccade direction selectivity, whereas ‘towards the contralateral hemifield’ cannot fulfill this purpose.

      (b) " Recurrent neural network modeling indicated that the feedback connections, matching the learned stimuli-response associations during the task, mediated such feedback modulation."

      - should be "that feedback connection .... might mediate". A model can only ever give a possible explanation.

      Thanks for the help on the writing again! We have revised this sentence as following: “Recurrent neural network modeling indicated that the feedback connections, matching the learned stimuli-response associations during the task, might mediate such feedback modulation.”

      (c) "thereby increasing the consistency of flexible decisions." I am not sure what is really meant by increasing the consistency of flexible decisions? More correct or more the same?

      We apologize for the confusion. In the manuscript, "decision consistency" refers to the degree of agreement in the model's decisions under specific conditions. A higher decision consistency indicates that the model is more likely to produce the same choice when encountering encounters a stimulus in that condition. We have incorporated your suggestion and revise this sentence as “thereby increasing the reliability of flexible decisions”. We also clarified the definition of consistency in the main text as follows:

      “These disrupted patterns of saccade DS observed in the target module following projection-specific inactivation aligned with the decreased decision consistency of RNNs, where decision consistency reflects the degree of agreement in the model's choices under specific task conditions. This suggests a diminished reliance on sensory input and an increased dependence on internal noise in the decision-making process.”.

      (5) Results: headers should be changed to reflect the actual results, not the interpretation:

      "Nonlinear feedback modulation of saccade choice on visual motion selectivity in LIP"

      "Feedback modulation specifically impacted the decision-correlated activity in LIP"

      These first parts of the results describe neurophysiological modulations of LIP activity, the source cannot be known from the presented data alone. I thought that this feedback is suggested by the modelling results in the last part of the results. It is confusing to the reader that the titles already refer to the source of the modulation as "feedback". The titles should more accurelty describe what is found, not pre-judge the interpretation.

      We thank the reviewer for those valuable suggestions. We have updated the subtitles to: “Nonlinear modulation of saccade choice on visual motion selectivity in LIP” and “Decision-correlated but not stimulus-correlated activity was modulated in LIP.”

      (6) page 8, l366-380. Can you link the statements more directly to panels in Figure 6. For Figure 6H-K, it needs to be clarified that the headers for 6D-G also apply to H-K.

      ­We have added headers for Figure 6H-K in the revised version, and revised the corresponding results section as follows.

      ‘We further examined how the energy landscape in the 1-D subspace changed in relation to task difficulty (motion coherence). Consistent with prior findings, trials with lower decision consistency (trials using lower motion coherence) exhibited shallower attractor basins at the time of decision for all types of RNNs (Fig. 6H-K). However, both the depth and the positional separation of attractor basins in the network dynamics significantly decreased for all non-zero motion coherence levels after the ablation of all feedback connections (comparing Figure 6I with Figure 6H; P(depth) = 5.20e-25, F = 122.80; P(position) = 1.82e-27, F = 137.75; two-way ANOVA). Notably, this reduction in basin depth and separation was more pronounced in the specific group compared to the nonspecific groups after ablating the feedback connections (comparing Figure 6J with Figure 6K; P(depth) = 2.65e-13, F =57.35; P(position) = 3.73e-14, F = 61.79; two-way ANOVA). These results might underlie the computational mechanisms that explain the observed reduction in the decision consistency of RNNs following projection-specific inactivation: the shallower and closer attractor basins after ablating feedback connections resulted in less consistent decisions. This happened because the variability in neural activity made it more likely for population activity to stochastically shift out of the shallower basins and into nearby alternative ones.’

      (7) line 556-557: Please provide a reference or data for the assertion that nearby recording sites in LIP (100 microns apart) have similar RFs.

      The reviewer raised an interesting question that we are unable to address in depth with the current data, as we lack information on the specific cortical location for each recording session. In the original manuscript, we suggested that nearby recording sites in LIP have similar receptive fields (RFs), based on both our own experience with LIP recordings and previous studies. Specifically, we observed that neurons recorded within a single penetration using a single-channel electrode typically exhibited similar RFs. Similarly, the majority of neurons recorded from the same multichannel linear probe within a single session also showed comparable RFs. Additionally, several studies (both electrophysiological and fMRI) have reported topographic organization of RFs in LIP (Gaurav H. Patel et al., 2010; S. Ben Hamed et al., 2001; Gene J. Blatt et al., 1990).

      (8) Line 568, Methods: a response criterion of a maximum firing rate of 2 spikes/s seems very low, especially for LIP. How do the results change if this lifted to something more realistic like 5 spikes/s or 10 spikes/s?

      We chose this criterion to ensure we included as many neurons as possible in our analysis. To further clarify, we have plotted the distribution of maximum firing rates across all neurons. Based on our findings, relaxing this criterion is unlikely to affect the results, as the majority of neurons exhibit maximum firing rates well above 5 spikes/s, and many exceed 10 spikes/s. We hope this explanation addresses the concern.

      Author response image 6.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      In this manuscript, the authors recorded activity in the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) of monkeys performing a perceptual decision-making task. The monkeys were first shown two choice dots of two different colors. Then, they saw a random dot motion stimulus. They had to learn to categorize the direction of motion as referring to either the right or left dot. However, the rule was based on the color of the dot and not its location. So, the red dot could either be to the right or left, but the rule itself remained the same. It is known from past work that PPC neurons would code the learned categorization. Here, the authors showed that the categorization signal depended on whether the executed saccade was in the same hemifield as the recorded PPC neuron or in the opposite one. That is, if a neuron categorized the two motion directions such that it responded stronger for one than the other, then this differential motion direction coding effect was amplified if the subsequent choice saccade was in the same hemifield. The authors then built a computational RNN to replicate the results and make further tests by simulated "lesions".

      The data are generally interesting, and the manuscript is generally well written (but see some specific comments below on where I was confused). However, I'm still not sure about the conclusions. The way the experiment is setup, the "contra" saccade target is essentially in the same hemifield as the motion patch stimulus. Given that the RF's can be quite large, isn't it important to try to check whether the saccade itself contributed to the effects? i.e. if the RF is on the left side, and the "contra" saccade is to the left, then even if it is orthogonal to the location of the stimulus motion patch itself, couldn't the saccade still be part of a residual edge of the RF? This could potentially contribute to elevating the firing rate on the preferred motion direction trials. I think it would help to align the data on saccade onset to see what happens. It would also help to have fully mapped the neurons' movement fields by asking the monkeys to generate saccades to all screen locations in the monitor. The authors mention briefly that they used a memory-guided saccade task to map RF's, but it is also important to map with a visual target. And, in any case, it would be important to show the mapping results aligned on saccade onset.

      Another comment is that the authors might want to mention this other recent related paper by the Pack group: https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/2023.08.03.551852v2.full.pdf

      We thank the reviewer for the comments and realized that we did not explain our results clearly in the original manuscript. We agree with the reviewer that saccade direction selectivity might be a confounding factor for the modulation of the saccade choice direction onto LIP neurons’ activity responded to visual motion stimuli. Because the RFs of LIP neurons might be large and the saccade target might be presented within the edge of the RFs. However, we believe that the observed modulation of saccade direction on LIP neurons’ response to motion stimuli cannot be simply explained by saccade direction selectivity. We presented several pieces of evidence to rule out such possibility. First, the modulation effect we observed was not linear; specifically, the firing rate of neurons increased for the preferred motion direction but decreased for the non-preferred motion direction (Figure 2i and Figure S1A-D). This phenomenon is unlikely to be attributed to a linear gain modulation driven by saccade directions. Second, we plotted the averaged neural activity for contralateral and ipsilateral saccade directions separately, aligned the activity to either motion stimulus onset or saccade onset, and found that LIP neurons showed similar levels of activity between the contralateral and ipsilateral directions (revised Figure 2L), which is not consistent with obvious saccade direction selectivity.

      To better control for this confound, we have added figures plotting the mean neural activity aligned to saccade onset for both contralateral and ipsilateral saccades, which are now included in the revised main Figure 2. These figures are presented in the detailed response below. Additionally, we have revised the corresponding results section to clarify our points, as outlined below:

      “Figure 2A-2F shows three example LIP neurons that exhibited significant motion coherence correlated DS. Surprisingly, LIP neurons showed greater DS in the CT condition than in the IT condition, even though the same motion stimuli were used in the same spatial location for both conditions. The averaged population activity showed this DS difference between CT and IT conditions for all four coherence levels (Figure 2G, 2H). During presentation of their preferred motion direction, LIP neurons showed significantly elevated activity in the CT relative to the IT at all coherence levels (Figure S1A, S1B, nested ANOVA: P(high) = 0.0326, F = 4.65; P(medium) = 0.0088, 142 F = 7.03; P(low) = 0.0076, F = 7.32; P(zero) = 0.0124, F = 6.4), and a trend toward lower activity to the nonpreferred direction for CT vs. IT (Figure S1C, S1D, nested ANOVA: P(high) = 0.0994, F = 2.75; P(medium) = 0.0649, F = 3.12; P(low) = 0.0311, F = 4.73; P(zero) = 0.0273, F = 4.96). Most of the LIP neurons (48 of 83) showed such opposing trends in activity modulation between the preferred and nonpreferred directions (Figure 2I). These results indicated a nonlinear modulation of saccade choice on motion DS in LIP, aligned precisely with the response property of each neuron. This is unlikely to be driven by a linear gain modulation of saccade direction selectivity. Receiver operating characteristic (ROC) analysis further confirmed significantly greater motion DS in the CT condition than in the IT condition (Figure 2J 148 and 2K; nested ANOVA: P(high) = 5.0e-4, F= 12.44; P(medium) = 9.53e-6, F = 20.91; P(low) = 9.33e-7, F 149 = 26.03; P(zero) = 2.56e-8, F= 34.3). Such DS differences were observed even before stimulus onset. Moreover, LIP neurons exhibited similar levels of mean activity between different saccade directions (CT vs. IT) before monkeys’ saccade choice (Figure 2L), further supporting that saccade direction selectivity did not significantly contribute to the observed modulation of LIP neurons’ responses to motion stimuli.

      We also thank the reviewer for pointing out the missing of this relevant study, we have added the suggested refence in the revised discussion section as follows:

      ‘A recent study demonstrated that neurons in the middle temporal area responded more strongly to motion stimuli when monkeys saccaded toward their RFs in a standard decision task with a fixed mapping between motion stimuli and saccade directions. This modulation emerged through the training process and contributed causally to the monkeys' following saccade choices. Consistently, we found that the response of LIP neurons to motion stimuli was more strongly correlated with the monkeys' decisions in the CT condition (saccades toward RFs) than in the IT condition, in a more flexible decision task. Together, these results suggest that the modulation of action selection on sensory processing may be a general process in perceptual decision-making. However, the observed modulation of saccade direction on LIP neurons' responses to motion stimuli cannot be simply explained by saccade direction selectivity. Several lines of evidence argue against this possibility. First, the modulation effect was nonlinear; specifically, neuronal firing rates increased for preferred motion directions but decreased for non-preferred directions (Figure 2I and Figure S1). This pattern is unlikely to be driven by a linear gain modulation based on saccade directions. Second, we found that LIP neurons exhibited similar levels of activity in both the CT and IT conditions (Figure 2L), which is inconsistent with the presence of clear saccade direction selectivity.

      Some more specific comments are below:

      - I had a bit of a hard time with the abstract. It does not appear to be crystal clear to me, and it is the first thing that I am reading after the title. For example, if there is a claim about both perceptual decision-making and later target selection, then I feel that the task should be explained a bit more clearly than saying "flexible decision" task. Also, "..modulated by monkeys' following saccade choices directing outside each neuron's response field" was hard to read. It needs to be rewritten. Maybe just say "...modulated by the subsequent eye movement choices, even when these eye movement choices always directed the eyes away from the recorded neuron's response field". Also, I don't fully understand what "selectivity-specific feedback" means. Then, the concept of "consistency" in flexible decisions is brought up, again without much context. The above are examples of why I had a hard time with the abstract.

      We realize that our original statement may have been unclear and potentially caused confusion for the readers. Following the reviewer’s suggestions, we have revised the abstract as follows:

      ‘Neural activity in the primate brain correlates with both sensory evaluation and action selection aspects of decision-making. However, the intricate interaction between these distinct neural processes and their impact on decision behaviors remains unexplored. Here, we examined the interplay of these decision processes in posterior parietal cortex (PPC) when monkeys performed a flexible decision task, in which they chose between two color targets based on a visual motion stimulus. We found that the PPC activity related to monkeys’ abstract decisions about visual stimuli was nonlinearly modulated by their subsequent saccade choices, which were directed outside each neuron’s response field. Recurrent neural network modeling indicated that the feedback connections, matching the learned stimuli-response associations during the task, might mediate such feedback modulation. Further analysis on network dynamics revealed that selectivity-specific feedback connectivity intensified the attractor basins of population activity underlying saccade choices, thereby increasing the reliability of flexible decisions. These results highlight an iterative computation between different decision processes, mediated primarily by precise feedback connectivity, contributing to the optimization of flexible decision-making.’

      Specifically, selectivity-specific feedback refers to the feedback connections with positive or negative weights between selectivity-matched and selectivity-nonmatched unit pairs, respectively.

      Regarding "decision consistency," we define it as the degree to which the model’s decisions remain congruent under specific conditions. A higher level of decision consistency indicates that the model is more likely to produce the same choice each time it is presented with a stimulus under those conditions, in another words, decision reliability. We have revised the corresponding results section to make these concepts clearer.

      - Line 69: I'm not fully sure, but I think that some people might suggest that superior colliculus is also involved in the sensory aspect of the evaluation. But, I guess the sentence itself is correct as you write it. So, I don't think anyone should argue with it. However, if someone does argue with it, then they would flag the next sentence, since if the colliculus does both, then do the sensory and motor parts really employ distinct neural processes? Anyway, I think this is very minor.

      This is an interesting point. We have also noticed a recent study that demonstrates that the superior colliculus is causally involved in the sensory aspect of decision-making, specifically in visual categorization. However, the study also distinguishes between neural activity related to categorical decisions and that related to saccade planning. This suggests that the sensory and motor aspects of decision-making likely involve distinct neural processing, even within the same brain region—potentially reflecting separate populations of neurons. Therefore, we stand by our statement in the ‘next sentence’.

      - Line 79-80: you might want to look at this work because I feel that it is relevant to cite here: https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/2023.08.03.551852v2

      We have discussed this reference in the revised discussion section of the manuscript, please refer to the above response.

      - For a result like that shown in Fig. 2, I feel that it is important to show RF mapping with a saccade task alone. i.e. for the same neurons, have a monkey make a delayed visually guided saccade task to all possible locations on the display, and demonstrate that there is no modulation by saccades to the targets. Otherwise, the result in Fig. 2 could reflect first an onset response by a motion, and then the saccade-related response that would happen anyway, even without the decision task. So, I feel that now, it is not entirely clear whether the result reflects this so-called feedback modulation, or whether simply planning the saccade to the target itself activates the neurons. With large RF's, this is a distinct possibility in my opinion.

      - Line 174: this would also be predicted if the neuron's were responding based on the saccade target plan independent of the motion stimulus

      - On a related note, I would recommend plotting all data also aligned on saccade onset. This can help establish what the cause of the effects described is

      We understand the reviewer’s concern that the modulation might be related to saccade planning, and we acknowledge that the original manuscript might not adequately address this potential confound. Unfortunately, we did not map the LIP neurons' receptive fields (RFs) using a saccade-only task. However, as mentioned earlier, we believe that the modulation of LIP neurons' responses to motion stimuli based on saccade choice direction cannot be simply attributed to saccade direction selectivity. Several lines of evidence support this conclusion. First, the modulation we observed was nonlinear: the firing rate of neurons increased for the preferred motion direction but decreased for the non-preferred motion direction (Figure 2i and Figure S1A-D). This pattern is inconsistent with a simple linear gain modulation driven by saccade direction selectivity. Second, we directly compared LIP neuronal activity for contralateral and ipsilateral target conditions, and found no significant differences between the two. This suggests that saccade direction selectivity is unlikely to be the primary contributor to the observed modulation. In the revised figure, we added a plot (Figure 2L) that aligns neural activity to saccade onset, in addition to the original alignment to motion stimulus onset (Figure S1E). This new analysis further supports our interpretation.

      Author response image 7.

      - Even when reading the simulation results, I'm still not 100% sure I understand what is meant by this idea of "consistency" of flexible decision-making

      We have addressed this issue in a previous comment and please refer to the response above.

    1. eLife Assessment

      The manuscript by Russell et al. investigates an important problem: the current lack of methods for early and accurate N. fowleri diagnosis, which is >95% fatal. The authors provide solid evidence that a small RNA secreted by N. fowleri is detectable in biological fluids like blood and urine in a mouse model, and is present in cerebrospinal fluid and blood for a limited number of patient samples. This could potentially help with earlier diagnosis, which could save lives.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Early and accurate diagnosis is critical to treating N. fowleri infections, which often lead to death within 2 weeks of exposure. Current methods are based on sampling cerebrospinal fluid, and are invasive, slow, and sometimes unreliable. Therefore, there is a need for a new diagnostic method. Russell et al. address this need by identifying small RNAs secreted by Naegleria fowleri (Fig. 1) that are detectable by RT-qPCR in multiple biological fluids including blood and urine. SmallRNA-1 and smallRNA-2 were detectable in plasma samples of mice experimentally infected with 6 different N. fowleri strains, and were not detected in uninfected mouse or human samples (Fig. 4). Further, smallRNA-1 is detectable in the urine of experimentally infected mice as early as 24 hours post infection (Fig. 5). The study culminates with testing human samples (obtained from the CDC) from patients with confirmed N. fowleri infections; smallRNA-1 was detectable in cerebrospinal fluid in 6 out of 6 samples (Fig. 6B), and in whole blood from 2 out of 2 samples (Fig. 6C). These results suggest that smallRNA-1 could be a valuable diagnostic marker for N. fowleri infection, detectable in cerebrospinal fluid, blood, or potentially urine.

      Strengths:

      This study investigates an important problem, and comes to a potential solution with a new diagnostic test for N. fowleri infection that is fast, less invasive than current methods, and seems robust to multiple N. fowleri strains. The work in mice is convincing that smallRNA1 is detectable in blood and urine early in infection. Analysis of patient blood samples shows that whole blood could be tested for smallRNA-1 to diagnose N. fowleri infections. The potential for human blood or urine to be tested for N. fowleri could lead to critical early interventions.

      Weaknesses:

      There are not many N. fowleri cases, so the authors were limited in the human samples available for testing. It is difficult to know how robust this biomarker is in whole blood, serum, or human urine due to little to no sample material being available for testing. This limitation is examined thoroughly in the discussion section, and additional tests are beyond the scope of this work.

    3. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      Early and accurate diagnosis is critical to treating N. fowleri infections, which often lead to death within 2 weeks of exposure. Current methods-sampling cerebrospinal fluid are invasive, slow, and sometimes unreliable. Therefore, there is a need for a new diagnostic method. Russell et al. address this need by identifying small RNAs secreted by Naegleria fowleri (Figure 1) that are detectable by RT-qPCR in multiple biological fluids including blood and urine. SmallRNA-1 and smallRNA-2 were detectable in plasma samples of mice experimentally infected with 6 different N. fowleri strains, and were not detected in uninfected mouse or human samples (Figure 4). Further, smallRNA-1 is detectable in the urine of experimentally infected mice as early as 24 hours post-infection (Figure 5). The study culminates with testing human samples (obtained from the CDC) from patients with confirmed N. fowleri infections; smallRNA-1 was detectable in cerebrospinal fluid in 6 out of 6 samples (Figure 6B), and in whole blood from 2 out of 2 samples (Figure 6C). These results suggest that smallRNA-1 could be a valuable diagnostic marker for N. fowleri infection, detectable in cerebrospinal fluid, blood, or potentially urine. 

      Strengths: 

      This study investigates an important problem, and comes to a potential solution with a new diagnostic test for N. fowleri infection that is fast, less invasive than current methods, and seems robust to multiple N. fowleri strains. The work in mice is convincing that smallRNA1 is detectable in blood and urine early in infection. Analysis of patient blood samples suggest that whole blood (but not plasma) could be tested for smallRNA-1 to diagnose N. fowleri infections. 

      Thank you for comments regarding the strengths of this study. We agree that our data for detecting the biomarker in biofluids from mice is convincing. In addition, our spike-in studies with human cerebrospinal fluid, plasma, and urine (Figure 6) suggest these biofluids from humans could be used for diagnosis.

      We appreciate the comment regarding plasma and recognize this was not fully explained in the manuscript. We do believe that plasma can be used to assess the biomarker. Firstly, we demonstrated equivalent sensitivity of the method to detect smallRNA-1 in plasma and urine in mice with end-stage PAM (Figure 5). In addition, spike in samples of human plasma, cerebrospinal fluid, and urine demonstrated equivalent sensitivity of detecting the biomarker (Figure 6). 

      The negative result for human plasma in Figure 6C requires clarification; this sample was convalescent plasma from a survivor. The patient presented to the hospital on August 7, 2016, was treated, made a remarkable recovery, and was released from the hospital later that month. The plasma sample in Figure 6C was collected September 7, 2016, which is a month after treatment was initiated and weeks after the patient was symptom free. Our interpretation of the convalescent plasma result is the patient had cleared the active amoeba infection and that is why we did not detect the biomarker. We have added text in the discussion and in the legend for Figure 6 to clarify the convalescent plasma result. 

      One additional caveat for consideration is that many of the samples we received from amoebaeinfected humans were stored at room temperatures for undefined periods of time before being moved to <-20°C (see details in Table S9). We can’t rule out possible sample degradation, but this is an unfortunate reality of obtaining human samples from individuals later confirmed to be infected with pathogenic free-living amoebae.

      Weaknesses: 

      (1) There are not many N. fowleri cases, so the authors were limited in the human samples available for testing. It is difficult to know how robust this biomarker is in whole blood (only 2 samples were tested, both had detectable smallRNA-1), serum (1 out of 1 sample tested negative), or human urine (presumably there is no material available for testing). This limitation is openly discussed in the last paragraph of the discussion section. 

      We agree the extremely limited availability of human samples is a limitation of this study. Given the rarity of these infections in the United States, even prospective studies to systematically collect samples would be very challenging. We hope that by publishing the details of this biomarker detection is that the method can be used by diagnostic reference centers, especially in areas where outbreaks of multiple cases per year have been reported.

      (2) There seems to be some noise in the data for uninfected samples (Figures 4B-C, 5B, and 6C), especially for those with serum (2E). While this is often orders of magnitude lower than the positive results, it does raise questions about false positives, especially early in infection when diagnosis would be the most useful. A few additional uninfected human samples may be helpful. 

      We agree; however, we would like to point out the progression of disease in humans and mice are similar. Typically, patients survive between 10-14 days after presumed exposure and mice have similar survival times following instillation of N. fowleri amoebae into a nare of the mouse. Therefore, detection of this biomarker as early as 72 h in mice is seemingly equivalent to the onset of initial symptoms in humans.  

      Reviewer #2 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      The authors sought to develop a rapid and non-invasive diagnostic method for primary amoebic meningoencephalitis (PAM), a highly fatal disease caused by Naegleria fowleri. Due to the challenges of early diagnosis, they investigated extracellular vesicles (EVs) from N. fowleri, identifying small RNA biomarkers. They developed an RT-qPCR assay to detect these biomarkers in various biofluids. 

      Strengths: 

      (1)  This study has a clear methodological approach, which allows for the reproducibility of the experiments. 

      (2) Early and Non-Invasive Diagnosis - The identification of a small RNA biomarker that can be detected in urine, plasma, and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) provides a non-invasive diagnostic approach, which is crucial for improving early detection of PAM. 

      (3) High Sensitivity and Rapid Detection - The RT-qPCR assay developed in the study is highly sensitive, detecting the biomarker in 100% of CSF samples from human PAM cases and in mouse urine as early as 24 hours post-infection. Additionally, the test can be completed in ~3 hours, making it feasible for clinical use. 

      (4)  Potential for Disease Monitoring - Since the biomarker is detectable throughout the course of infection, it could be used not only for early diagnosis but also for tracking disease progression and monitoring treatment efficacy. 

      (5)  Strong Experimental Validation - The study demonstrates biomarker detection across multiple sample types (CSF, urine, whole blood, plasma) in both animal models and human cases, providing robust evidence for its clinical relevance. 

      (6) Addresses a Critical Unmet Need - With a >97% case fatality rate, PAM urgently requires improved diagnostics. This study provides one of the first viable liquid biopsy-based diagnostic approaches, potentially transforming how PAM is detected and managed. 

      Thank you for summarizing the strengths of the study.

      Weaknesses: 

      (1) Limited Human Sample Size - While the biomarker was detected in 100% of CSF samples from human PAM cases, the number of human samples analyzed (n=6 for CSF) is relatively small. A larger cohort is needed to validate its diagnostic reliability across diverse populations. 

      As noted in response to Reviewer #1 above, we agree this is a limitation of the study; however, we were fortunate to obtain even 15 µL samples of cerebrospinal fluid, plasma, serum, or whole blood from as many patients as we did. There is an urgent need for more systematic collection and storage of samples for rare diseases like primary amoebic meningoencephalitis so that advancements in diagnostics and biomarker discovery can be conducted. It is our sincere hope that by publishing our detailed methods and experimental results in this manuscript, that additional hospitals and research centers can replicate our studies and help advance this or other techniques for early diagnosis of PAM.

      (2) Lack of Pre-Symptomatic or Early-Stage Human Data - Although the biomarker was detected in mouse urine as early as 24 hours post-infection, there is no data on whether it can be reliably detected before symptoms appear in humans, which is crucial for early diagnosis and treatment initiation. 

      It is difficult to envision a method to obtain these biofluids from infected humans prior to onset of symptoms. More likely the best we can hope for is that physicians include primary amoebic meningoencephalitis in their assessment of patients that present with prodromal symptoms of meningitis.

      (3)  Plasma Detection Challenges - While the biomarker was detected in whole blood, it was not detected in human plasma, which could limit the ease of clinical implementation since plasma-based diagnostics are more common. Further investigation is needed to understand why it is absent in plasma and whether alternative blood-based approaches (e.g., whole blood assays) could be optimized. 

      See response to Reviewer #1 above.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      (1) What is the evidence that these small RNAs are secreted specifically in EVs? I believe that they are, and ultimately it doesn't impact the conclusions, but I think the evidence here could be either stronger or presented in a more obvious way. 

      Our data demonstrates that smallRNA-1 is present in N. fowleri-derived EVs (Figures 2 and Supplemental Figure 7) and in the intact amoebae (Figure 3B).  Initial sequencing data to identify these smallRNA biomarkers came from PEG-precipitated EVs (Figure S1), by using methods we previously published (22). The PEG-precipitated EVs were extracted specifically for spike in studies. Finally, the smallRNAs in EVs were confirmed after extraction of EVs from 7 N. fowleri strains (Figure 2). We do not have evidence that they are secreted outside of EVs.

      (2) The figure legends would be more useful with some additional information. For example: why are there two points for Nf69 in Fig 2B? In Figure 3A-B, please add more detail as to what the graphs are showing (are they histograms binned by a number of amoebae? This does not seem obvious to me). 

      We agree the Figure legends should be edited for clarity and to add additional information. Both Figure legends have been updated.

      In Figure 2B, each point represents the mean of three technical replicates of EV preps for each N. fowleri strain.

      In Figure 3 the points indicate the Copy#/µL of a well from a 96-well plate. The histograms show the mean of these observations for each condition. 

      (3)  In Figure 2E, the FBS seems like it has near detectable levels of smallRNA-1 compared to Ac and Bm (albeit N. fowleri has 4 orders of magnitude higher levels than the FBS). Because cows are likely exposed to N. fowleri and have documented infections (e.g. doi: 10.1016/j.rvsc.2012.01.002), is it possible this signal is real? 

      Thank you for making this interesting observation. We agree that cows are likely to have significant exposure to N. fowleri, yet documented infections are rare. In this case we do not believe the near detectable levels of smallRNA-1 in FBS was due to an infected donor animal. This noise was likely due to extracting RNA from concentrated FBS rather than FBS diluted in cell culture media. In addition, as shown in Supplemental Figure 4, the qPCR product from EVs extracted from FBS were not the same as that from the N. fowleri-derived EVs. Please note we used a PEG extraction reagent that separates lipid particles, so this is additional evidence the smallRNAs are present in EVs.

      (4)  In Figure 6A, why was the sample size greater for water and unspiked urine? Similarly, why is the number of infected mice so variable in Figure 4B? 

      In Figure 6A we assayed de-identified biofluids provided by Advent Hospital in Orlando, Florida. The plasma and serum samples were pooled from multiple individuals; whereas, individual urine samples (n=8) were provided for this experiment. We have updated the legend for Figure 6A to include these details.

      For Figure 4B we used plasma collected at the end-stage of disease following infections with five different strains of N. fowleri. The sample sizes varied for two reasons. First, Nf69 was the strain used most by our lab and we had plasma from several in vivo experiments. The lower sample sizes for the other strains came from an experiment with 8 mice per group. Some of these strains were less virulent and did not succumb to disease with the number of amoebae inoculated in this experiment. Thus, plasma was only collected from animals that were euthanized due to severe N.

      fowleri infections. In follow up studies (e.g., Figure 5B), plasma was collected every 24 hr for analysis.

      Very minor points: 

      (1)  The number of acronyms (FLA, PAM, EVs, CNS, CSF, LOD) could be reduced to make this paper more reader-friendly. 

      Acronyms that were used infrequently in the manuscript (FLA, CNS, LOD, mNGS, UC) have been edited to spell out the complete names. We kept the acronyms EVs and CSF because they are each used more than twenty times in the manuscript.

      (2)  The decimal point in the Cq values is formatted strangely. 

      The decimal points have been edited to normal format in both the manuscript and supplementary material.

      (3)  Figure 3C is not intuitive. I do not understand the logic for the placement of the different samples (was row A only amoebae, B only Veros, C blank, D a mix, and F more Veros?). 

      Thank you for this comment; we agree the microtiter plate schematic (Fig 3C) was misleading. We have revised Figure 3C to make the point that we tested amoebae alone, Vero cells alone, and we combined supernatants from Vero cells (alone) plus amoebae (alone) to confirm that 1) smallRNA-1 was only detected in amoeba-conditioned media, and 2) that Vero-conditioned media does not affect detection of smallRNA-1.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      Minor corrections: 

      The abbreviation 'Nf' for Naegleria fowleri is not appropriate in a scientific publication. According to taxonomic conventions, the correct way to abbreviate a scientific name is as follows: 

      The first mention should be written in full: Naegleria fowleri. 

      In subsequent mentions, the genus name should be abbreviated to its initial in uppercase, followed by a period, while the species name remains in lowercase: N. fowleri. 

      The same rule applies to Balamuthia mandrillaris and Acanthamoeba species, which should be abbreviated as B. mandrillaris and Acanthamoeba spp. after their first mention. 

      We agree and each of the scientific names have been updated to the proper format. Please note Nf69 is the accepted nomenclature for this N. fowleri strain, so no changes were made when referring to this specific strain.

      Temperatures should be expressed in international units (°C). Please update the temperatures reported in Fahrenheit (°F) in the 'Materials and Methods' section, specifically in the 'Animal Studies' subsection. 

      These changes were made in the revised manuscript.

    1. eLife Assessment

      This convincing study, which is based on a survey of researchers, finds that women are less likely than men to submit articles to elite journals. It also finds that there is no relation between gender and reported desk rejection. The study is an important contribution to work on gender bias in the scientific literature.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary

      This paper summarises responses from a survey completed by around 5,000 academics on their manuscript submission behaviours. The authors find several interesting stylised facts, including (but not limited to):

      - Women are less likely to submit their papers to highly influential journals (e.g., Nature, Science and PNAS).<br /> - Women are more likely to cite the demands of co-authors as a reason why they didn't submit to highly influential journals.<br /> - Women are also more likely to say that they were advised not to submit to highly influential journals.

      The paper highlights an important point, namely that the submission behaviours of men and women scientists may not be the same (either due to preferences that vary by gender, selection effects that arise earlier in scientists' careers or social factors that affect men and women differently and also influence submission patterns). As a result, simply observing gender differences in acceptance rates - or a lack thereof - should not be automatically interpreted as as evidence for or against discrimination (broadly defined) in the peer review process.

      Major comments

      What do you mean by bias?

      In the second paragraph of the introduction, it is claimed that "if no biases were present in the case of peer review, then we should expect the rate with which members of less powerful social groups enjoy successful peer review outcomes to be proportionate to their representation in submission rates." There are a couple of issues with this statement.

      First, the authors are implicitly making a normative assumption that manuscript submission and acceptance rates *should* be equalised across groups. This may very well be the case, but there can also be valid reasons - even when women are not intrinsically better at research than men - why a greater fraction of female-authored submissions are accepted relative to male-authored submissions (or vice versa). For example, if men are more likely to submit their less ground-breaking work, then one might reasonably expect that they experience higher rejection rates compared to women, conditional on submission.

      Second, I assume by "bias", the authors are taking a broad definition, i.e., they are not only including factors that specifically relate to gender but also factors that are themselves independent of gender but nevertheless disproportionately are associated with one gender or another (e.g., perhaps women are more likely to write on certain topics and those topics are rated more poorly by (more prevalent) male referees; alternatively, referees may be more likely to accept articles by authors they've met before, most referees are men and men are more likely to have met a given author if he's male instead of female). If that is the case, I would define more clearly what you mean by bias. (And if that isn't the case, then I would encourage the authors to consider a broader definition of "bias"!)

      Identifying policy interventions is not a major contribution of this paper

      I would take out the final sentence in the abstract. In my opinion, your survey evidence isn't really strong enough to support definitive policy interventions to address the issue and, indeed, providing policy advice is not a major - or even minor - contribution of your paper. (Basically, I would hope that someone interested in policy interventions would consult another paper that much more thoughtfully and comprehensively discusses the costs and benefits of various interventions!) While it's fine to briefly discuss them at the end of your paper - as you currently do - I wouldn't highlight that in the abstract as being an important contribution of your paper.

      Minor comments

      - What is the rationale for conditioning on academic rank and does this have explanatory power on its own - i.e., does it at least superficially potentially explain part of the gender gap in intention to submit?

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Basson et al. present compelling evidence supporting a gender disparity in article submission to "elite" journals. Most notably, they found that women were more likely to avoid submitting to one of these journals based on advice from a colleague/mentor. Overall, this work is an important addition to the study of gender disparities in the publishing process.

      I thank the authors for addressing my concerns.

    4. Reviewer #4 (Public review):

      Main strengths

      The topic of the MS is very relevant given that across the sciences/academia, genders are unevenly represented, which has a range of potential negative consequences. To change this, we need to have the evidence on what mechanisms cause this pattern. Given that promotion and merit in academia are still largely based on the number of publications and the impact factor, one part of the gap likely originates from differences in publication rates of women compared to men.

      Women are underrepresented compared to men in journals with a high impact factor. While previous work has detected this gap and identified some potential mechanisms, the current MS provides strong evidence that this gap might be due to a lower submission rate of women compared to men, rather than the rejection rates. These results are based on a survey of close to 5000 authors. The survey seems to be conducted well (though I am not an expert in surveys), and data analysis is appropriate to address the main research aims. It was impossible to check the original data because of the privacy concerns.

      Interestingly, the results show no gender bias in rejection rates (desk rejection or overall) in three high-impact journals (Science, Nature, PNAS). However, submission rates are lower for women compared to men, indicating that gender biases might act through this pathway. The survey also showed that women are more likely to rate their work as not groundbreaking and are advised not to submit to prestigious journals, indicating that both intrinsic and extrinsic factors shape women's submission behaviour.

      With these results, the MS has the potential to inform actions to reduce gender bias in publishing, but also to inform assessment reform at a larger scale.

      I do not find any major weaknesses in the revised manuscript.

    5. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary

      This paper summarises responses from a survey completed by around 5,000 academics on their manuscript submission behaviours. The authors find several interesting stylised facts, including (but not limited to):

      - Women are less likely to submit their papers to highly influential journals (*e.g.*, Nature, Science and PNAS).

      - Women are more likely to cite the demands of co-authors as a reason why they didn't submit to highly influential journals.

      - Women are also more likely to say that they were advised not to submit to highly influential journals.

      Recommendation

      This paper highlights an important point, namely that the submissions' behaviours of men and women scientists may not be the same (either due to preferences that vary by gender, selection effects that arise earlier in scientists' careers or social factors that affect men and women differently and also influence submission patterns). As a result, simply observing gender differences in acceptance rates---or a lack thereof---should not be automatically interpreted as as evidence of for or against discrimination (broadly defined) in the peer review process. I do, however, make a few suggestions below that the authors may (or may not) wish to address.

      We thank the author for this comment and for the following suggestions, which we take into account in our revision of the manuscript.

      Major comments

      What do you mean by bias?

      In the second paragraph of the introduction, it is claimed that "if no biases were present in the case of peer review, then 'we should expect the rate with which members of less powerful social groups enjoy successful peer review outcomes to be proportionate to their representation in submission rates." There are a couple of issues with this statement.

      - First, the authors are implicitly making a normative assumption that manuscript submission and acceptance rates *should* be equalised across groups. This may very well be the case, but there can also be important reasons why not -- e.g., if men are more likely to submit their less ground-breaking work, then one might reasonably expect that they experience higher rejection rates compared to women, conditional on submission.

      We do assume that normative statement: unless we believe that men’s papers are intrinsically better than women’s papers, the acceptance rate should be the same. But the referee is right: we have no way of controlling for the intrinsic quality of the work of men and women. That said, our manuscript does not show that there is a different acceptance rate for men and women; it shows that women are less likely to submit papers to a subset of journals that are of a lower Journal Impact Factor, controlling for their most cited paper, in an attempt to control for intrinsic quality of the manuscripts.

      - Second, I assume by "bias", the authors are taking a broad definition, i.e., they are not only including factors that specifically relate to gender but also factors that are themselves independent of gender but nevertheless disproportionately are associated with one gender or another (e.g., perhaps women are more likely to write on certain topics and those topics are rated more poorly by (more prevalent) male referees; alternatively, referees may be more likely to accept articles by authors they've met before, most referees are men and men are more likely to have met a given author if he's male instead of female). If that is the case, I would define more clearly what you mean by bias. (And if that isn't the case, then I would encourage the authors to consider a broader definition of "bias"!)

      Yes, the referee is right that we are taking a broad definition of bias. We provide a definition of bias on page 3, line 92. This definition is focused on differential evaluation which leads to differential outcomes. We also hedge our conversation (e.g., page 3, line 104) to acknowledge that observations of disparities may only be an indicator of potential bias, as many other things could explain the disparity. In short, disparities are a necessary but insufficient indicator of bias. We add a line in the introduction to reinforce this. The only other reference to the term bias comes on page 10, line 276. We add a reference to Lee here to contextualize.

      Identifying policy interventions is not a major contribution of this paper

      In my opinion, the survey evidence reported here isn't really strong enough to support definitive policy interventions to address the issue and, indeed, providing policy advice is not a major -- or even minor -- contribution of your paper, so I would not mention policy interventions in the abstract. (Basically, I would hope that someone interested in policy interventions would consult another paper that much more thoughtfully and comprehensively discusses the costs and benefits of various interventions!)

      We thank the referee for this comment. While we agree that our results do not lead to definitive policy interventions, we believe that our findings point to a phenomenon that should be addressed through policy interventions. Given that some interventions are proposed in our conclusion, we feel like stating this in the abstract is coherent.

      Minor comments

      - What is the rationale for conditioning on academic rank and does this have explanatory power on its own---i.e., does it at least superficially potentially explain part of the gender gap in intention to submit?

      The referee is right: academic rank was added to control for career age of researchers, with the assumption that this variable would influence submission behavior. However, the rank information we collected was for the time that the individual respondent took the survey, which could be different from the rank they held concerning their submission behaviors mentioned in the survey. That is why we didn't consider rank as an independent variable of interest. But I do also agree with the reviewer that it could be related to their submission behaviors in some cases. Our initial analysis shows that academic rank is not a significant predictor of whether researchers submitted to SNP, but does contribute significantly to the SNP acceptance rates and desk rejection rates of individuals in Medical Sciences.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, Basson et al. study the representation of women in "high-impact" journals through the lens of gendered submission behavior. This work is clear and thorough, and it provides new insights into gender disparities in submissions, such as that women were more likely to avoid submitting to one of these journals based on advice from a colleague/mentor. The results have broad implications for all academic communities and may help toward reducing gender disparities in "high-impact" journal submissions. I enjoyed reading this article, and I have several recommendations regarding the methodology/reporting details that could help to enhance this work.

      We thank the referee for their comments.

      Strengths:

      This is an important area of investigation that is often overlooked in the study of gender bias in publishing. Several strengths of the paper include:

      (1) A comprehensive survey of thousands of academics. It is admirable that the authors retroactively reached out to other researchers and collected an extensive amount of data.

      (2) Overall, the modeling procedures appear thorough, and many different questions are modeled.

      (3) There are interesting new results, as well as a thoughtful discussion. This work will likely spark further investigation into gender bias in submission behavior, particularly regarding the possible gendered effect of mentorship on article submission.

      Thank you for those comments.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The GitHub page should be further clarified. A detailed description of how to run the analysis and the location of the data would be helpful. For example, although the paper says that "Aggregated and de-identified data by gender, discipline, and rank for analyses are available on GitHub," I was unable to find such data.

      We added the link to the Github page, as well as more details on the how to run the statistical analysis. Unfortunately, our IRB approval does not allow for the sharing of the raw data.

      (2) Why is desk rejection rate defined as "the number of manuscripts that did not go out for peer review divided by the number of manuscripts rejected for each survey respondent"? For example, in your Grossman 2020 reference, it appears that manuscripts are categorized as "reviewed" or "desk-rejected" (Grossman Figure 2). If there are gender differences in the denominator, then this could affect the results.

      We thank the referee for pointing this out. Actually, what the referee is proposing is how we calculated it in the manuscript; the calculation mentioned in the manuscript was a mistake. We corrected the manuscript.

      (3) Have you considered correcting for multiple comparisons? Alternatively, you could consider reporting P-values and effect sizes in the main text. Otherwise, sometimes the conclusions can be misleading. For example, in Figure 3 (and Table S28), the effect is described as significant in Social Sciences (p=0.04) but not in Medical Sciences (p=0.07).

      We highly appreciate the suggestion. We’ve added Odds Ratio values and p-values to the main manuscript.

      (4) More detail about the models could be included. It may be helpful to include this in each table caption so that it is clear what all the terms of the model were. For instance, I was wondering if journal or discipline are included in the models.

      We appreciate the suggestion. We’ve added model details to the figure and table captions in the manuscript and the supplemental materials.

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      This is a strong manuscript by Basson and colleagues which contributes to our understanding of gender disparities in scientific publishing. The authors examine attitudes and behaviors related to manuscript submission in influential journals (specifically, Science, Nature and PNAS). The authors rightly note that much attention has been paid to gender disparities in work that is already published, but this fails to capture the unseen hurdles that occur prior to publication (which include decisions about where to publish, desk rejections, revisions and resubmissions, etc.). They conducted a survey study to address some of these components and their results are interesting:

      They find that women are less likely to submit their manuscript to Science, Nature or PNAS. While both men and women feel their work would be better suited for more specialized journals, women were more likely to think their work was 'less novel or groundbreaking.'

      A smaller proportion of respondents indicated that they were actively discouraged from submitting their manuscripts to these journals. In this instance, women were more likely to receive this advice than men.

      Lastly, the authors also looked at self-reported acceptance and rejection rates and found that there were no gender differences in acceptance or rejection rates.

      These data are helpful in developing strategies to mitigate gender disparities in influential journals.

      We thank the referee for their comments

      Comments:

      The methods the authors used are appropriate for this study. The low response rate is common for this type of recruitment strategy. The authors provide a thoughtful interpretation of their data in the Discussion.

      We thank the referee for their comments

      Reviewer #4 (Public Review):

      This manuscript covers an important topic of gender biases in the authorship of scientific publications. Specifically, it investigates potential mechanisms behind these biases, using a solid approach, based on a survey of researchers.

      Main strengths

      The topic of the MS is very relevant given that across sciences/academia representation of genders is uneven, and identified as concerning. To change this, we need to have evidence on what mechanisms cause this pattern. Given that promotion and merit in academia are still largely based on the number of publications and impact factor, one part of the gap likely originates from differences in publication rates of women compared to men.

      Women are underrepresented compared to men in journals with high impact factor. While previous work has detected this gap, as well as some potential mechanisms, the current MS provides strong evidence, based on a survey of close to 5000 authors, that this gap might be due to lower submission rates of women compared to men, rather than the rejection rates. The data analysis is appropriate to address the main research aims. The results interestingly show that there is no gender bias in rejection rates (desk rejection or overall) in three high-impact journals (Science, Nature, PNAS). However, submission rates are lower for women compared to men, indicating that gender biases might act through this pathway. The survey also showed that women are more likely to rate their work as not groundbreaking, and be advised not to submit to prestigious journals

      With these results, the MS has the potential to inform actions to reduce gender bias in publishing, and actions to include other forms of measuring scientific impact and merit.

      We thank the referee for their comments.

      Main weakness and suggestions for improvement

      (1) The main message/further actions: I feel that the MS fails to sufficiently emphasise the need for a different evaluation system for researchers (and their research). While we might act to support women to submit more to high-impact journals, we could also (and several initiatives do this) consider a broader spectrum of merits (e.g. see https://coara.eu/ ). Thus, I suggest more space to discuss this route in the Discussion. Also, I would suggest changing the terms that imply that prestigious journals have a better quality of research or the highest scientific impact (line 40: journals of the highest scientific impact) with terms that actually state what we definitely know (i.e. that they have the highest impact factor). And think this could broaden the impact of the MS

      We agree with the referee. We changed the wording on impact, and added a few lines were added on this in the discussion.

      (2) Methods: while methods are all sound, in places it is difficult to understand what has been done or measured. For example, only quite late (as far as I can find, it's in the supplement) we learn the type of authorship considered in the MS is the corresponding authorship. This information should be clear from the very start (including the Abstract).

      We performed the suggested edits.

      Second, I am unclear about the question on the perceived quality of research work. Was this quality defined for researchers, as quality can mean different things (e.g. how robust their set-up was, how important their research question was)? If researchers have different definitions of what quality means, this can cause additional heterogeneity in responses. Given that the survey cannot be repeated now, maybe this can be discussed as a limitation.

      We agree that this can mean something different for researchers—probably varies by discipline, but also by gender. But that was precisely the point: whether men/women considered their “best work” to be published in higher impact venue. While there may be heterogeneity in those perceptions, the fact that 1) men and women rate their research at the same level and 2) we control for disciplinary differences should mitigate some of that.

      I was surprised to see that discipline was considered as a moderator for some of the analyses but not for the main analysis on the acceptance and rejection rates.

      We appreciate the attention to detail. In our analysis of acceptance and rejection rates, we conducted separate regression analyses for each discipline to capture any field-specific patterns that might otherwise be obscured.

      We added more details on this to clarify.

      I was also suppressed not to see publication charges as one of the reasons asked for not submitting to selected journals. Low and middle-income countries often have more women in science but are also less likely to support high publication charges.

      That is a good point. However, both Science and Nature have subscription options, which do not require any APCs.

      Finally, academic rank was asked of respondents but was not taken as a moderator.

      Academic rank is included in the regression as a control variable (Figure 1).

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      In addition to the points in the "Weaknesses" section of the my Public Review above, I have several suggestions to improve this work.

      (1) Can you please indicate what the error bars mean in each plot? I am assuming that they are 95% confidence intervals.

      We appreciate the attention to detail. Yes, they are 95% confidence intervals. We’ve clarified this in the captions of the corresponding figures. 

      (2) Can you provide a more detailed explanation for why the 7 journals were separated? I see that on page 3 of the supporting information you write that "Due to limited responses, analysis per journal was not always viable. The results pertaining to the journals were aggregated, with new categories based on the shared similarities in disciplinary foci of the journals and their prestige." Specifically, why did you divide the data into (somewhat arbitrary) categories as opposed to using all the data and including a journal term in your model?

      The survey covered 7 journals:

      • Science, Nature, and PNAS (S.N.P.)

      • Nature Communications and Science Advances (NC.SA.)

      • NEJM and Cell (NEJM.C.)

      We believe that the first three are a class of their own: they cover all fields (while NEJM and Cell are limited to (bio)medical sciences), and have a much higher symbolic capital than both Nature Comms and Science Advances (which are receiving cascading papers from Nature and Science, respectively). We believe that factors leading to submission to S.N.P. are much different than those leading to submission to the other groups of journals, which is why we separated the analysis in that manner.

      (3) You included random effects for linear regression but not for logistic regression. Please justify this choice or include additional logistic regression models with random effects.

      We used mixed-effect models for linear regressions (where number of submissions, acceptance rate, or rejection rate is the dependent variable). As mentioned in the previous comment, we tested using rank as the control variable and found it had a potential impact on the variables we analyzed using linear regressions in some disciplines. Therefore, we introduced it as a random effect for all the linear regression models.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      The limitations of this work are currently described in the Supplement. It may be helpful to bring several of these items into the Discussion so that they can be addressed more prominently.

      Added content

      Reviewer #4 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      (1) Line 40: add 'as leading authors of papers published in' before ' 'journals'

      Done

      (2) Explain what the direction in the ' relationship between' line 62 is

      Added

      (3) Lines 101-102 - this is a bit unclear. Please, provide some more info, also including what did these studies find.

      Added

      (4) Is 'sociodemographic' the best term in line 120

      Yes, we believe so.

      (5) Results would benefit from a short intro with the info on the number of respondents, also by gender.

      Those are present at the end of the intro (and in the methods, at the end). We nonetheless added gender.

      (6) Line 134 add how many woman and man did submit to Science, Nature, and PNAS

      Added. In all disciplines combined, 552 women and 1,583 men ever submitted to these three elite journals. More details can be found in SI Table 9

      (7) Add 'Self-' before reported, line 141

      Added

      (8) Add sample sizes to Figs 1 and 2

      Those are in the appendix

      (9) Line 168 - unclear if this is ever or as their first choice

      We do not discriminate – it is whether the considered it at all.

      (10) Add sample size in line 177

      Added. 480 women and 1404 men across all disciplines reported desk rejections by S.N.P. journals.

      (11) I would like to see some discussion on the fact that the highest citation paper will also be a paper that the authors have submitted earlier in their careers given that citations will pile up over time.

      Those are actually quite evenly distributed. We modified the supplementary materials.

      (12) Data availability - be clear that supporting info contains only summary data. Also, while the Data availability statement refers to de-identified data on Github, the Github page only contains the code, and the note that 'The STAT code used for our analyses is shared.

      We are unable to share the survey response details publicly per IRB protocols.' Why were de-identified data shared? This is extremely important to allow for the reproducibility of MS results. I would also suggest sharing data in a trusted repository (e.g. Dryad, ZENODO...) rather than on Github, as per current recommendations on the best practices for data sharing.

      Thank you for your careful reading and for highlighting the importance of clear data availability. We will revise our Data Availability Statement to explicitly state that the supporting information contains only summary data and that the complete analysis code is available on GitHub.

      We understand the importance of sharing de-identified data for reproducibility. However, our IRB strictly prohibits the sharing of any individual-level data, including de-identified files, to protect participant confidentiality. Consequently, the summary data included in the supporting information, together with the provided code, is intended to facilitate the verification of our core findings. Our previous statement regarding “de-identified” data sharing was inaccurate and thus has been removed. We apologize for the confusion.

      In light of your suggestion, we are also exploring depositing the summary data and code in a trusted repository (e.g., Dryad or Zenodo) to further align with current best practices for data sharing.

    1. eLife Assessment

      In this useful study, the authors perform voltage imaging of CA1 pyramidal cells in head-fixed mice running on a track while local field potentials (LFPs) were recorded in the contralateral hemisphere. The authors conclude that synchronous ensembles of neurons are associated with theta rhythms but not with contralateral sharp wave-ripples. However, evidence for some of the paper's primary claims remains incomplete, due to limitations of the experimental approach.

    2. Joint Public Review:

      Summary:

      For many years, there has been extensive electrophysiological research investigating the relationship between local field potential patterns and individual cell spike patterns in the hippocampus. In this study, using innovative imaging techniques, they examined spike synchrony of hippocampal cells during locomotion and immobility states. The authors demonstrated that hippocampal place cells exhibit prominent synchronous spikes locked to theta oscillations.

      Strengths:

      The single cell voltage imaging used in this study is a highly novel method that may allow recordings that were not previously possible using existing methods.

      Weaknesses:

      The strength of evidence remains incomplete because of the main claim that synchronous events are not associated with ripples. As was mentioned in previous rounds of review, ripples emerge locally and independently in the two hemispheres. Thus, obtaining ripple recordings from the contralateral hemisphere does not provide solid evidence for this claim. The papers the authors are citing to make the claim that "Additionally, we implanted electrodes in the contralateral CA1 region to monitor theta and ripple oscillations, which are known to co-occur across hemispheres (29-31)" do not support this claim. For example, reference 29 contains the following statement: "These findings suggest that ripples emerge locally and independently in the two hemispheres".

    3. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the current reviews.

      We thank the editor and reviewers for their thoughtful evaluations. We would like to clarify that the revised manuscript does not make a general claim about the absence of ripple-associated synchronous population activity. Rather, we report only that the synchronous ensembles observed in our data were not associated with contralateral ripple oscillations. This distinction is clearly reflected in the revised Title, Abstract, Introduction, Results, and Discussion. We also explicitly acknowledged the methodological limitation of recording LFP from the contralateral side of the hippocampus.

      To further improve clarity and prevent potential misinterpretation, we are submitting a revised version (R4) in which we:

      (1) Replace the word "surprisingly" with the more neutral "Moreover";

      (2) Refer to ripple events consistently as "contralateral ripples (c-ripples)";

      (3)Expand the discussion of limitations inherent to contralateral LFP recordings.

      Additionally, while Buzsaki et al. (2003) wrote that "These findings suggest ripples emerge locally and independently in the two hemispheres", the same study also presents data and reports that "Ripple episodes occurred simultaneously in the left and right CA1 regions" (p. 206). Our original citation was intended to reflect this nuance. Nevertheless, to avoid any potential misinterpretation, we have removed the co-occurrence statement with its associated citations in the revised (R4) manuscript.


      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      For many years, there has been extensive electrophysiological research investigating the relationship between local field potential patterns and individual cell spike patterns in the hippocampus. In this study, using state-ofthe-art imaging techniques, they examined spike synchrony of hippocampal cells during locomotion and immobility states. In contrast to conventional understanding of the hippocampus, the authors demonstrated that hippocampal place cells exhibit prominent synchronous spikes locked to theta oscillations.

      Strengths:

      The voltage imaging used in this study is a highly novel method that allows recording not only suprathreshold-level spikes but also subthreshold-level activity. With its high frame rate, it offers time resolution comparable to electrophysiological recordings.

      Comments on revisions: I have no further comments.

      We thank the reviewer for constructive reviews and for recognizing the strength of our study.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study employed voltage imaging in the CA1 region of the mouse hippocampus during the exploration of a novel environment. The authors report synchronous activity, involving almost half of the imaged neurons, occurred during periods of immobility. These events did not correlate with SWRs, but instead, occurred during theta oscillations and were phased locked to the trough of theta. Moreover, pairs of neurons with high synchronization tended to display non-overlapping place fields, leading the authors to suggest these events may play a role in binding a distributed representation of the context.

      Strengths:

      Technically this is an impressive study, using an emerging approach that allows single cell resolution voltage imaging in animals, that while head-fixed, can move through a real environment. The paper is written clearly and suggests novel observations about population level activity in CA1.

      Comments on revisions:

      I have no further major requests and thank the authors for the additional data and analyses.

      We thank the reviewer for recognizing the strength of our study and for appreciating the additional data and analyses we provided during the revision process.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In the present manuscript, the authors use a few minutes of voltage imaging of CA1 pyramidal cells in head fixed mice running on a track while local field potential (LFPs) are recorded. The authors suggest that synchronous ensembles of neurons are differentially associated with different types of LFP patterns, theta and ripples. The experiments are flawed in that the LFP is not "local" but rather collected the other side of the brain.

      Strengths:

      The authors use a cutting-edge technique.

      Weaknesses:

      Although the authors have toned down their claims, the statement in the title ("Synchronous Ensembles of Hippocampal CA1 Pyramidal Neurons Associated with Theta but not Ripple Oscillations During Novel Exploration") is still unsupported.

      One could write the same title while voltage imaging one mouse and recording LFP from another mouse.

      To properly convey the results, the title should be modified to read

      "Synchronous Ensembles of Hippocampal CA1 Pyramidal Neurons Associated with Contralateral Theta but not with Contralateral Ripple Oscillations During Novel Exploration"

      Without making this change, the title - and therefore the entire work - is misleading at best.

      We thank the reviewer for the thoughtful and constructive suggestion regarding the title. We fully understand the concern that our original title may have overstated the specificity of the contralateral LFP recordings, potentially allowing for misinterpretation.

      In our results, synchronous ensembles are associated with intracellular theta oscillations recorded from the ipsilateral hippocampus and with extracellular theta but not ripples oscillations recorded from the contralateral hippocampus. To clarify this distinction and minimize the potential for misinterpretation, we have revised the abstract accordingly. 

      Abstract (line18):

      “… Notably, these synchronous ensembles were not associated with contralateral ripple oscillations but were instead phase-locked to theta waves recorded in the contralateral CA1 region. Moreover, the subthreshold membrane potentials of neurons exhibited coherent intracellular theta oscillations with a depolarizing peak at the moment of synchrony.”

      Based on this, we propose the following revised title, which we believe more effectively communicates the central finding of our study: 

      “Synchronous Ensembles of Hippocampal CA1 Pyramidal Neurons During Novel Exploration”. 

      Compared to the reviewer’s suggested title, this version offers a clearer and more concise summary of our findings while allowing important methodological details to be fully conveyed in the abstract and main text. While the suggested title accurately reflects the source of the LFP signals, it does not mention the intracellular theta oscillations recorded from the ipsilateral hippocampus, which are a critical part of our results. Including both the intracellular and extracellular recording contexts in the title would make it overly long and potentially less accessible to readers. In contrast, the revised title succinctly captures the core phenomenon, and the updated abstract now explicitly clarifies the relationship between the synchronous ensembles and both types of oscillatory signals. 

      We sincerely appreciate the reviewer’s input, which helped us refine both the language and the presentation of our findings. We hope these changes address the concern and clarify the scope of our work. 

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Change the title. Although the authors have toned down their claims, the statement in the title ("Synchronous Ensembles of Hippocampal CA1 Pyramidal Neurons Associated with Theta but not Ripple Oscillations During Novel Exploration") is still unsupported. One could write the same title while voltage imaging one mouse and recording LFP from another mouse. To properly convey the results, the title should be modified to read

      "Synchronous Ensembles of Hippocampal CA1 Pyramidal Neurons Associated with Contralateral Theta but not with Contralateral Ripple Oscillations During Novel Exploration"

      Without making this change, the title - and therefore the entire work - is misleading at best. But if you can manage that (and attend to comment #2 below), then the manuscript would not be making any false statements.

      Please see our reply in the public review above.

      (2) Report the exact locations of the contralateral recording electrodes. In their rebuttal, the authors supplies a figure ("Author response image 1") in which they show damage to the neocortex and fluorescence signal in the CA1 pyramidal cell layer. This is useful, but it is unclear from which animal this histology was generated.

      Please include this (or another similar) photograph in Figure 1B, right next to the voltage imaging photograph. Indicate from which animal each photograph was obtained - ideally, provide the two photographs from the same animal. Second, please include such paired photographs - along with paired signals - for every animal that you are able to.

      If you can manage that, it will add credibility to the statement that the recordings are indeed from the contralateral CA1 pyramidal cell layer (as opposed to from the contralateral hemisphere).

      We thank the reviewer for this important point. We have followed the suggestion and now provide paired photographs showing LFP electrode tracks and voltage images from the same animal (see revised Figure 1B)

      In addition, we have included similar paired photographs for additional animals used in this study (see Figure 1-figure supplement 1).

      These updates directly support the claim that LFP recordings were obtained from the contralateral CA1 pyramidal layer, rather than from the contralateral hemisphere. We sincerely thank the reviewer for the valuable suggestion, which has substantially strengthened our manuscript.

    1. eLife Assessment

      This valuable study reveals surprising morphological diversity of Drosophila sensory neurons. Using serial block-face electron microscopy, the authors created detailed 3D reconstructions of large neuronal populations, convincingly finding significant structural variation both within and across distinct classes. These results form the basis for testable hypotheses on how neuronal arborization is optimized for particular sensory functions. This research will be highly relevant to biologists in the fields of physiology, insect chemosensation, and neuroscience.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      The authors of this study use electron microscopy and 3D reconstruction techniques to study the morphology of distinct classes of Drosophila sensory neurons *across many neurons of the same class.* This is a comprehensive study attempting to look at nearly all the sensory neurons across multiple sensilla in the same animal to determine a) how much morphological variability exists between and within neurons of different and similar sensory classes and b) identify dendritic features that may have evolved to support particular sensory functions. This study builds upon the authors' previous work which allowed them to identify and distinguish sensory neuron subtypes in the EM volumes without additional staining so that reconstructed neurons could reliably be placed in the appropriate class. This work is unique in looking at a large number of individual neurons of the same class to determine what is consistent and what is variable about their class-specific morphologies.

      This means that in addition to providing specific structural information about these particular cells, the authors explore broader questions of how much morphological diversity exists between sensory neurons of the same class. This then informs our conceptualization about how different dendritic morphologies might affect specific sensory and physiological properties of neurons.

      The authors found that CO2 sensing neurons have an unusual, sheet-like morphology in contrast to the thin branches of odor-sensing neurons. They show that this morphology greatly increases the surface area to volume ratio above what could be achieved by modest branching of thin dendrites, and posit that this might be important for their sensory function, though this was not directly tested in their study due to technical limitations. The study is mainly descriptive in nature, but thorough, and provides a nice jumping off point for future functional studies. One interesting future analysis could be to examine all four cell types within a single sensilla together to see if there are any general correlations that could reveal insights about how morphology is determined and relative contributions of intrinsic mechanisms vs interactions with neighboring cells. For example, if higher-than-average branching in one cell type correlated with higher-than-average branching in another type when within the same sensilla, it might suggest differential amounts of extracellular growth or branching cues within a given sensillum drive any heterogeneity observed within a class across sensilla. Conversely, if higher branching in one cell type consistently leads to reduced length or branching of the other neurons within its sensillum, this might point to dendrite-dendrite interactions between cells undergoing competitive or repulsive interactions to define territories within each sensillum as a major determinant of the variability.

      Strengths:

      This work provides a thorough morphometric analysis of the neurons of the *majority of all ab1 sensilla* across a single antenna. The authors use this analysis to 1) characterize the unique dendritic architecture of ab1C neurons relative to other ORNs including ab1D and 2) provide evidence of substantial morphological diversity even within a single subclass of neuron.

      Weaknesses:

      This is primarily a descriptive paper due to technical limitations since it is not currently technically feasible to determine individual ORN response properties and tie them to identified neurons with detailed EM-based ultrastructural analyses, nor to predictably alter dendritic morphology of these cells to directly test how different morphologies affect sensory function. However, the quantitative descriptive findings presented here will shape these future questions and are necessary for any such future work.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript employs serial block‐face electron microscopy (SBEM) and cryofixation to obtain high‐resolution, three‐dimensional reconstructions of Drosophila antennal sensilla containing olfactory receptor neurons (ORNs) that detect CO2. This method has been used previously by the same lab in Gonzales et. al, 2021. (https://elifesciences.org/articles/69896), and Zhang et. al, 2019 Nature Communications. The previous study by Zhang also correlated morphometric measurements from SBEM with asymmetric ephaptic activity for paired neurons using electrophysiology across multiple olfactory sensilla. This manuscript applies the same SBEM method to now characterize the ab1 sensillum which houses the ab1C, CO2 detecting neuron, but stops short of integration neuronal activity with structural variability.

      The SBEM-based morphometric studies do however significantly advance preliminary observations from older two-dimensional TEM-based reports. Previous images of the putative CO2 neuron in Drosophila (Shanbhag et al., 1999) and in mosquitoes (McIver and Siemicki, 1975; Lu et al, 2007) reported that the dendritic architecture of the CO2 neuron was somewhat different (circular and flattened, lamellated) from other olfactory neurons in the antenna of insects. In this study, the authors confirm this different morphology but also classify it into distinct subtypes (loosely curled, fully curled, split, and mixed).

      Strengths:

      The study makes a convincing case that ab1C neurons exhibit a unique, dendritic morphology unlike the canonical cylindrical dendrites found in ab1D neurons. This observation extends previous qualitative TEM findings by not only confirming the presence of flattened lamellae in CO₂ neurons but also quantifying key morphometrics such as dendritic length, surface area, and volume, and calculating surface area-to-volume ratios. The enhanced ratios observed in the flattened segments are speculated to be linked to potential advantages in receptor distribution (e.g., Gr21a/Gr63a) and efficient signal propagation.

      Weaknesses:

      Although this quantitative approach is very robust compared to earlier reports, interpretations are somewhat limited by the absence of direct electrophysiological data to confirm whether ultrastructural differences translate into altered neuronal function. The biggest question remains unanswered: whether structural variation observed in the ab1C dendrites by SBEM have an electrophysiological functional relevance?

      Surveys of ab1 sensillum with single-sensillum recordings (even a few from multiple Drosophila antenna) as they have done for ab2s and others in the past, would have measured spontaneous activity, spike amplitude, and response to CO2. This could have allowed for comparison of frequency of functional variation, if any, to structural variation and a discussion would therefore have strengthened the overall characterization. In the case of ab2 sensilla the authors find very little variance, could the ab1 also be the same? In the absence of this data, it becomes hard to speculate whether structural variation observed in the ab1C dendrites by SBEM have any functional relevance or whether they are simply random variations in dendrite development.

      Additionally, artifacts could be a consideration, even though Cryofixation is superior to chemical fixation. Although this is hard to address, all types of fixations in TEMs cause some artifacts, as does serial sectioning. An understanding of the error rates for the SBEM method would have increased the confidence in the conclusions drawn. For example, what is the structural variation of SBEMs in the ab2 population, which shows very little electrophysiological variation? Can a comparison be done?

    4. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In the current manuscript entitled "Population-level morphological analysis of paired CO2- and odor-sensing olfactory neurons in D. melanogaster via volume electron microscopy", Choy, Charara et al. use volume electron microscopy and neuron reconstruction to compare the dendritic morphology of ab1C and ab1D neurons of the Drosophila basiconic ab1 sensillum. They aim to investigate the degree of dendritic heterogenity within a functional class of neurons using ab1C and ab1D, which they can identify due to the unique feature of ab1 sensilla to house four neurons and the stereotypic location on the third antennal segment. This is a great use of volumetric electron imaging and neuron reconstruction to sample a population of neurons of the same type. Their data convincingly shows that there is dendritic heterogenity in both investigated populations and their sample size is sufficient to strongly support this observation. This data proposes that the phenomenon of dendritic heterogenity is common in the Drosophila olfactory system and will stimulate future investigations into the developmental origin, functional implications and potential adaptive advantage of this feature.

      Moreover, the authors discovered that there is a difference between CO2- and odour sensing neurons of which the first show a characteristic flattened and sheet-like structure not observed in other sensory neurons sampled in this and previous studies. They hypothesize that this unique dendritic organization which increases the surface area to volume ratio, might allow more efficient Co2 sensing by housing higher numbers of Co2 receptors. This is supported by previous attempts to express Co2 sensors in olfactory sensory neurons which lack this dendritic morphology, resulting in lower Co2 sensitivity compared to endogenous neurons.

      Overall, this detailed morphological description of olfactory sensory neurons' dendrites convincingly shows heterogeneity in two neuron classes with potential functional impacts for odour sensing.

      Strength:

      The volumetric EM imaging and reconstruction approach offers unpreceeded details in single cell morphology and compares dendrite heterogenity across a great fraction of ab1 sensilla.<br /> The authors identify specific shapes for ab1C sensilla potentially linked to their unique function in CO2 sensing.

      Weaknesses:

      While the morphological description is highly detailed, current methods prevent linking morphology to odour sensitivity or other properties of the neurons. Therefore, this study remains mainly descriptive and will require future work to link neuron structure and function.

    5. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      The authors of this study use electron microscopy and 3D reconstruction techniques to study the morphology of distinct classes of Drosophila sensory neurons *across many neurons of the same class.* This is a comprehensive study attempting to look at nearly all the sensory neurons across multiple sensilla to determine a) how much morphological variability exists between and within neurons of different and similar sensory classes, and 2) identify dendritic features that may have evolved to support particular sensory functions. This study builds upon the authors' previous work, which allowed them to identify and distinguish sensory neuron subtypes in the EM volumes without additional staining so that reconstructed neurons could reliably be placed in the appropriate class. This work is unique in looking at a large number of individual neurons of the same class to determine what is consistent and what is variable about their class-specific morphologies.

      This means that in addition to providing specific structural information about these particular cells, the authors explore broader questions of how much morphological diversity exists between sensory neurons of the same class and how different dendritic morphologies might affect sensory and physiological properties of neurons.

      The authors found that CO2-sensing neurons have an unusual, sheet-like morphology in contrast to the thin branches of odor-sensing neurons. They show that this morphology greatly increases the surface area to volume ratio above what could be achieved by modest branching of thin dendrites, and posit that this might be important for their sensory function, though this was not directly tested in their study. The study is mainly descriptive in nature, but thorough, and provides a nice jumping-off point for future functional studies. One interesting future analysis could be to examine all four cell types within a single sensilla together to see if there are any general correlations that could reveal insights about how morphology is determined and the relative contributions of intrinsic mechanisms vs interactions with neighboring cells. For example, if higher than average branching in one cell type correlated with higher than average branching in another type, if in the same sensilla. This might suggest higher extracellular growth or branching cues within a sensilla. Conversely, if higher branching in one cell type consistently leads to reduced length or branching in another, this might point to dendrite-dendrite interactions between cells undergoing competitive or repulsive interactions to define territories within each sensilla as a major determinant of the variability.

      We thank the reviewer for the insightful comments and appreciation for our study.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      The manuscript employs serial block‐face electron microscopy (SBEM) and cryofixation to obtain high‐resolution, three‐dimensional reconstructions of Drosophila antennal sensilla containing olfactory receptor neurons (ORNs) that detectCO2. This method has been used previously by the same lab in Gonzales et. al, 2021. (https://elifesciences.org/articles/69896), which had provided an exemplary model by integrating high-resolution EM with electrophysiology and cell-type-specific labeling.

      We thank the reviewer for expressing appreciation for our published study.

      The previous study ended up correlating morphology with activity for multiple olfactory sensillar types. Compared to the 2021 study, this current manuscript appears somewhat incomplete and lacks integration with activity.

      We thank the reviewer for their feedback. However, we would like to clarify that our previous study did not correlate morphology with activity to a greater extent than the current study. Both employed the same cryofixation, SBEM-based approach without recording odor-induced activity, but the focus of the current work is fundamentally different. While the previous study examined multiple sensillum types, the current study concentrates on a single sensillum type to address a distinct biological question regarding morphological heterogeneity. We appreciate the opportunity to clarify this distinction, and we hope that the revised manuscript more clearly conveys the unique scope and contributions of this study.

      In fact older studies have also reported two-dimensional TEM images of the putative CO2 neuron in Drosophila (Shanbhag et al., 1999) and in mosquitoes (McIver and Siemicki, 1975; Lu et al, 2007), and in these instances reported that the dendritic architecture of the CO2 neuron was somewhat different (circular and flattened, lamellated) from other olfactory neurons.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing this out. As noted in both the Introduction and Discussion sections, previous studies—including those cited by the reviewer—suggested that CO2-sensing neurons may have a distinct dendritic morphology. However, those earlier studies lacked the means to definitively link the observed morphology to CO2 neuron identity.

      In contrast, our study assigns neuronal identity based on quantitative morphometric measurements, allowing us to confidently associate the unique dendritic architecture with CO2 neurons. Furthermore, we extend previous observations by providing full 3D reconstructions and nanoscale morphometric analyses, offering a much more comprehensive and definitive characterization of these neurons. We believe this represents a significant advancement over earlier work.

      The authors claim that this approach offers an artifact‐minimized ultrastructural dataset compared to earlier. In this study, not only do they confirm this different morphology but also classify it into distinct subtypes (loosely curled, fully curled, split, and mixed). This detailed morphological categorization was not provided in prior studies (e.g., Shanbhag et al., 1999).

      We thank the reviewer for acknowledging the significance of our study.

      The authors would benefit from providing quantitative thresholds or objective metrics to improve reproducibility and to clarify whether these structural distinctions correlate with distinct functional roles.

      We thank the reviewer for raising this point. However, we would like to clarify that assigning neurons to strict morphological subtypes was not the primary aim of our study. In practice, dendritic architectures can be highly complex, with individual neurons often displaying features characteristic of multiple subtypes. This is precisely why we included a “mixed” subtype category—to acknowledge and capture this morphological heterogeneity rather than impose rigid classification boundaries.

      Our intent in defining subtypes was not to imply discrete functional classes, but rather to highlight the range of morphological variation observed across ab1C neurons. While we agree that exploring potential correlations between structure and function is an important future direction, the current study focuses on characterizing this diversity using 3D reconstruction and morphometric analysis. We hope this clarifies the purpose and scope of our morphological categorization.

      Strengths:

      The study makes a convincing case that ab1C neurons exhibit a unique, flattened dendritic morphology unlike the cylindrical dendrites found in ab1D neurons. This observation extends previous qualitative TEM findings by not only confirming the presence of flattened lamellae in CO₂ neurons but also quantifying key morphometrics such as dendritic length, surface area, and volume, and calculating surface area-to-volume ratios. The enhanced ratios observed in the flattened segments are speculated to be linked to potential advantages in receptor distribution (e.g., Gr21a/Gr63a) and efficient signal propagation.

      We thank the reviewer for appreciating the significance our current study.

      Weaknesses:

      While the manuscript offers valuable ultrastructural insights and reveals previously unappreciated heterogeneity among CO₂-sensing neurons, several issues warrant further investigation in addition to the points made above.

      (1) Although this quantitative approach is robust compared to earlier descriptive reports, its impact is somewhat limited by the absence of direct electrophysiological data to confirm that ultrastructural differences translate into altered neuronal function. A direct comparison or discussion of how the present findings align with the functional data obtained from electrophysiology would strengthen the overall argument.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. We would like to clarify, however, that our study does not claim that the observed morphological heterogeneity necessarily leads to functional diversity. Rather, we consider this as a possible implication and discuss it as a potential question for future research. This idea is raised only in the Discussion section, and we are carefully not to present functional diversity as a conclusion of our study. Nonetheless, we have reviewed the relevant paragraph to ensure the language remains cautious and does not overstate our interpretation.

      We also acknowledge the significance of directly linking ultrastructural features to neuronal function through electrophysiological recordings. However, at present, it is technically challenging to correlate the nanoscale morphology of individual ORNs with their functional activity, as this would require volume EM imaging of the very same neurons that were recorded via electrophysiology. Currently, there is no dye-labeling method compatible with single-sensillum recording and SBEM sample preparation that allows for unambiguous identification and segmentation of recorded ORNs at the necessary ultrastructural resolution.

      To acknowledge this important limitation, we have added a paragraph in the Discussion section, as suggested, to clarify the current technical barriers and to highlight this as a promising direction for future methodological advances.

      (2) Clarifying the criteria for dendritic subtype classification with quantitative parameters would enhance reproducibility and interpretability. Moreover, incorporating electrophysiological recordings from ab1C neurons would provide compelling evidence linking structure and function, and mapping key receptor proteins through immunolabeling could directly correlate receptor distribution with the observed morphological diversity.

      Please see our response to the comment regarding the technical limitations of directly correlating ultrastructure with electrophysiological data.

      In addition, we would like to address the suggestion of using immunolabeling to map receptor distribution in relation to the 3D EM models. Currently, antibodies against Gr21a or Gr63a (the receptors expressed in ab1C neurons) are not available. Even if such antibodies were available, immunogold labeling for electron microscopy requires harsh detergent treatment to increase antibody permeability, damaging morphological integrity. These treatments would compromise the very morphological detail that our study aims to capture and quantify.

      (3) Even though Cryofixation is claimed to be superior to chemical fixation for generating fewer artifacts, authors need to confirm independently the variation observed in the CO2 neuron morphologies across populations. All types of fixation in TEMs cause some artifacts, as does serial sectioning. Without understanding the error rates or without independent validation with another method, it is hard to have confidence in the conclusions drawn by the authors of the paper.

      We thank the reviewer for raising concerns regarding potential artifacts in morphological analyses. However, we would like to clarify that cryofixation is widely regarded as a gold standard for ultrastructural preservation and minimizing fixation-induced artifacts, as supported by extensive literature. This is why we adopted high-pressure freezing and freeze substitution in our study.

      We have also published a separate methods paper (Tsang et al., eLife, 2018) directly comparing our cryofixation-based protocol with conventional chemical fixation, demonstrating substantial improvements in morphological preservation. This provides strong empirical support for the reliability of our approach.

      Regarding the suggestion to validate observed morphological variation across populations: we note that determining the presence of artifacts requires a known ground truth, which is inherently unavailable as we could not measure the morphometrics of fly olfactory receptor neurons in their native state. In the absence of such a benchmark, we have instead prioritized using the best-available preparation methods and high-resolution imaging to ensure structural integrity.

      Addressing these concerns and integrating additional experiments would significantly bolster the manuscript's completeness and advancement.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s feedback. As discussed in our responses to the specific comments above, certain suggested experiments are currently limited by technical constraints, particularly in the context of high-resolution volume EM for insect tissues enclosed in cuticles.

      Nevertheless, we have carefully addressed the reviewer’s concerns to the fullest extent possible within the scope of this study. We have revised the manuscript to clarify methodological limitations, added new explanatory content where appropriate, and ensured that our interpretations remain well grounded in the data. We hope these revisions strengthen the clarity and completeness of the manuscript.

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      In the current manuscript entitled "Population-level morphological analysis of paired CO2- and odor-sensing olfactory neurons in D. melanogaster via volume electron microscopy", Choy, Charara et al. use volume electron microscopy and sensillum. They aim to investigate the degree of dendritic heterogeneity within a functional class of neurons using ab1Cand ab1D, which they can identify due to the unique feature of ab1 sensilla to house four neurons and the stereotypic location on the third antennal segment. This is a great use of volumetric electron imaging and neuron reconstruction to sample a population of neurons of the same type. Their data convincingly shows that there is dendritic heterogeneity in both investigated populations, and their sample size is sufficient to strongly support this observation. This data proposes that the phenomenon of dendritic heterogeneity is common in the Drosophila olfactory system and will stimulate future investigations into the developmental origin, functional implications, and potential adaptive advantage of this feature.

      Moreover, the authors discovered that there is a difference between CO2- and odour-sensing neurons of which the first show a characteristic flattened and sheet-like structure not observed in other sensory neurons sampled in this and previous studies. They hypothesize that this unique dendritic organization, which increases the surface area to volume ratio, might allow more efficient CO2 sensing by housing higher numbers of CO2 receptors. This is supported by previous attempts to express CO2 sensors in olfactory sensory neurons, which lack this dendritic morphology, resulting in lower CO2 sensitivity compared to endogenous neurons.

      Overall, this detailed morphological description of olfactory sensory neurons' dendrites convincingly shows heterogeneity in two neuron classes with potential functional impacts for odour sensing.

      Strength:

      The volumetric EM imaging and reconstruction approach offers unprecedented details in single cell morphology and compares dendrite heterogeneity across a great fraction of ab1 sensilla. The authors identify specific shapes for ab1C sensilla potentially linked to their unique function in CO2 sensing.

      We thank the reviewer for the insightful comments and appreciation for our study.

      Weaknesses:

      While the morphological description is highly detailed, no attempts are made to link this to odour sensitivity or other properties of the neurons. It would have been exciting to see how altered morphology impacts physiology in these olfactory sensory cells.

      We agree that linking morphological variation to physiological properties, such as odor sensitivity, would be a highly valuable direction for future research. However, the aim of the current study is to provide an in-depth nanoscale characterization based on a substantial proportion of ab1 sensilla, highlighting morphological heterogeneity among homotypic ORNs.

      At present, it is technically challenging to correlate the nanoscale morphology of individual ORNs with their physiological responses, as this would require volume EM imaging of the exact neurons recorded via single-sensillum electrophysiology. Currently, no dye-labeling method exists that is compatible with both single-sensillum recording and the stringent requirements of SBEM sample preparation to allow for unambiguous identification and segmentation of recorded ORNs.

      To acknowledge this important limitation, we have added a paragraph in the Discussion section clarifying the current technical barriers and highlighting this as a promising area for future methodological development. Please also see our responses to the reviewer’s 4th comment below, where we present preliminary experiments examining whether odor sensitivity varies among homotypic ORNs.

      (Please see the following pages for additional responses to the reviewers’ specific comments. These responses are not intended for publication.)

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      As this is mainly a descriptive paper I have no suggestions for additional experiments. Minor Text Suggestions:

      (1) The authors might want to include a better description/definition of the fly antennae, olfactory sensilla and their basic structure/makeup, position of the sensory neurons and dendrites within, etc, in the introduction perhaps in cartoon form to help readers that are not familiar (i.e. non-Drosophila readers) with the terminology and basic organization can follow the paper more easily from the start.

      We thank the reviewer for the helpful suggestion to broaden the appeal of our study to a wider readership. In response, we added a new introductory paragraph at the beginning of the Results section, along with illustrations in a new supplementary figure (Figure 1—figure supplement 1). The new paragraph reads as follows.

      “The primary olfactory organ in Drosophila is the antenna, which contains hundreds olfactory sensilla on the surface of its third segment (Figure 1—figure supplement 1A) . Each sensillum typically encapsulates the outer dendrites of two to four ORNs. The outer dendrites are the sites where odorant receptors are expressed, enabling the detection of volatile chemicals. A small portion of the outer dendrites lies beneath the base of the sensillum cuticle. At the ciliary constriction, the outer dendrites connect to the inner dendritic segment, which then links to the soma of each ORN (Figure 1—figure supplement 1B).”

      (2) In Figure 4D, the letter annotations above the graphs are not clearly defined anywhere that I could easily find. Please clarify with different symbols and/or in the figure legend so readers can easily comprehend the stats that are presented.

      We thank the reviewer for raising this point. As suggested, in the revised Figure 4D legend, following the original sentence “Statistical significance is determined by Kruskal-Wallis one-way ANOVA on ranks and denoted by different letters”, we added “For example, labels “a” and “b” indicate a significant difference between groups (P < 0.05), whereas labels with identical or shared letters (e.g., “a” and “a”, “a,b” and “a”, or “a,b” and “b”) indicate no significant difference.”

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      There are several aspects that I would like the authors to consider to improve the current manuscript:

      (1) Line 331: "Our analysis highlights how structural scaling in ab1D neurons achieves enhanced sensory capacity while maintaining the biophysical properties of dendrites". This is a strong statement, and not shown by the authors. They speculate about this in the discussion, but I would like them to soften the language here.

      We thank the reviewer for raising this point. As suggested, we have softened the language in the sentence in question. The revised version is as follows.

      “Our analysis suggests that structural scaling in ab1D neurons may enhance sensory capacity while preserving the biophysical properties of dendrites.”

      (2) The Supplementary material is not well presented and is not cited in the manuscript. It is not clear what the individual data files show, where they refer to, etc. Please provide clear labels of all data, cite them at the appropriate location in the manuscript, and make them more accessible to the reader. Also, there are two Videos mentioned in the manuscript that are not included in the submission.

      We thank the reviewer for bringing this to our attention and apologize for the oversight. We appreciate the reviewer’s careful attention to the supplementary materials. We have addressed these issues accordingly: 1) all source data have been consolidated in to a single, clearly labeled Excel file to improve accessibility for readers; this file is now cited at the appropriate locations in the manuscript. 2) The supplementary videos mentioned in the manuscript have also been included in the re-submission.

      (3) In Figure 1B, it is hard to recapitulate the increase in dendritic density in the presented pictures. Could the authors please highlight dendrites in the raw imaging files (e.g. by colour coding as done later in the manuscript). Also, it might be helpful to indicate the measured parameters visually in this Figure (e.g. volume, length, etc.).

      We thank the reviewer for the helpful suggestion. As suggested, we have pseudocolored the dendrites in Figure 1B to enhance visual clarity.

      As noted, the original legend stated that “the sensilla were arranged from left to right in order of increasing dendritic branch counts”. To improve clarity, we have now added the number of dendritic branches above each sensillum to make this information more explicit.

      We hope these changes make the figure more accessible and informative for readers.

      (4) Given the strength of the authors in in vivo physiology and single sensilla recordings, I would be very curious about how the described morphological heterogeneity is reflected in the response properties of ab1Cs and ab1Ds. Can the authors provide data (already existing from their lab) of these two neurons on response heterogeneity? I acknowledge that spike sorting can be very challenging in ab1s, but maybe it is possible to show the range of response sensitivities upon CO2 stimulation in ab1Cs? The authors speculate in the discussion and presented data will only be correlative - however I think it would strengthen the manuscript to have some link to physiology included.

      We thank the reviewer for this insightful comment. We share the same curiosity about response variability among homotypic ORNs, including ab1C and ab1D. Ideally, this question could be addressed by recording from a large proportion of neurons of a given ORN type to assess the response variability within a single antenna. However, due to technical limitations, we are only able to reliably record from 3–4 ab1 sensilla per antennal preparation, representing approximately 8% of the total ab1 population.

      Moreover, our recordings are typically limited to ab1 sensilla located on the posterior-medial side of the antenna, as this region provides the best accessibility for our recording electrode. This spatial constraint may limit our ability to sample the full morphological diversity of ab1C and ab1D neurons.

      Given these limitations, it is technically challenging to rigorously assess physiological variability in ab1C and ab1D responses across the entire ab1 population. Nonetheless, we attempted to address this question using a different sensillum type where a larger proportion of the population is accessible to single-sensillum recording per antennal preparation. Specifically, we focused on ab2 sensilla in the following analysis because we can reliably record from 6 sensilla per antenna, representing approximately 25% of the total ab2 population.

      In the preliminary data presented below, we recorded from 6 ab2A ORNs per antenna across a total of 6 flies. Spike analysis revealed that odor-evoked responses were consistent across individual ab2A neurons (Author response image 1A). When analyzing the dose-response curve for each ORN, we found no statistically significant differences in odor sensitivity, either among ORNs within the same antenna or across different flies (Author response image 1B; two-way ANOVA: P > 0.99 within antennae, P > 0.99 across flies). This is further supported by the closely clustered EC50 values (Author response image 1C). This result suggests that odor sensitivity is largely uniform among homotypic ab2A ORNs.

      Author response image 1.

      Homotypic ab2A ORNs display similar odorant sensitivity. (A) Single-sensillum recording. Raster plots of ab2A/Or59b ORN spike responses. Six ab2A ORNs from the same antenna were recorded per fly. Odor stimulus: methyl acetate (10-6). (B) Dose-response relationships of peak spike responses, normalized to the maximum response of the ORN to facilitate comparison of odor sensitivity. Each curve represents responses from a single ab2A ORN fitted with the Hill equation (n=36 ab2 sensilla from 6 flies). Responses recorded from the same antenna are indicated by the same color. Statistical comparisons between different ab2A ORNs from the same antenna (P > 0.99) or across flies (P > 0.99) were performed by two-way ANOVA. (C) Quantification of individual pEC50 values from (B), defined as -logEC50.

      However, we are hesitant to include this result in the main manuscript for several reasons. First, it does not directly relate to the morphometric analysis of ab1C and ab1D neurons, which is the primary focus of our study. Second, while we were able to record from approximately 25% of the ab2 population, this level of coverage is still limited and potentially subject to sampling bias due to the spatial constraints of the antennal region accessible to the recording electrode.

      At best, our data suggest limited variability in odor sensitivity among the recorded ab2A ORNs. However, we are cautious about generalizing this finding to the entire ab2 population. In light of these considerations, we hope the reviewer can appreciate the technical challenges inherent in addressing what may appear to be a straightforward question.

      For these reasons, we have chosen to include this preliminary result in the response only, rather than in the main manuscript.

    1. eLife Assessment

      This important work develops C. elegans as a model organism for studying effort-based discounting by asking the worms to choose between easy and hard to digest bacteria. The authors provide convincing evidence that the nematodes are effort-discounting. However, evidence regarding the role of dopamine is incomplete and this weakens the authors connection of the behavior in C. elegans with mammals.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      Here, Millet et al. consider whether the nematode C. elegans 'discounts' the value of reward due to effort in a manner similar to that shown in other species, including rodents and humans. They designed a T-maze effort choice paradigm inspired by previous literature, but manipulated how effortful the food is to consume. C. elegans worms were sensitive to this novel manipulation, exhibiting effort-discounting-like behaviour that could be shaped by varying the density of food at each alternative in order to calculate an indifference point. This discounting-like behaviour was related to worms' rates of patch leaving, which differed between the low and high effort patches in isolation. The authors also found a potential relationship to dopamine signalling, and also that this discounting behaviour was not specific to lab-based strains of C. elegans.

      Strengths:

      The question is well-motivated, and the approach taken here is novel. The authors are careful in their approach to altering and testing the properties of the effortful, elongated bacteria. Similarly, they go to some effort to understand what exactly is driving behavioural choices in this context, both through the application of simple standard models of effort discounting and a kinetic analysis of patch leaving. The comparisons to various dopamine mutants further extend the translational potential of their findings. I also appreciate the comparison to natural isolate strains, as the question of whether this behaviour may be driven by some sort of strain-specific adaptation to the environment is not regularly addressed in mammalian counterparts. The manuscript is well-written, and the figures are clear and comprehensible.

      Weaknesses:

      Discounting is typically defined as the alteration of a subjective value by effort (or time, risk, etc.), which is then used to guide future decision-making. By adapting the standard t-maze task for C. elegans as a patch-leaving paradigm, the authors observe behaviour strongly consistent with discounting models, but that is likely driven by a different process, in particular by an online estimate of the type of food in the current patch, which then influences patch-leaving dynamics (Figure 3). This is fundamentally different from decision-making strategies relating to effort that have been described in the rodent and human literatures. Similarly, the calculation of indifference points at the group instead of at the individual level also suggests a different underlying process and limits the translational potential of their findings. The authors do not discuss the implications of these differences or why they chose not to attempt a more analogous trial-based experiment.

      In the case of both the dopamine and natural isolate experiments, the data are very noisy despite large (relative to other C. elegans experiments) sample sizes. In the dopamine experiment, disruption of dop-1, dop-2, and cat-2 had no statistically significant effect. There do not appear to be any corrections for multiple comparisons, and the single significant comparison, for dop-3, had a small effect size. More detailed behavioural analyses on both these and the wild isolate strains, for example by applying their kinetic analysis, would likely give greater insight as to what is driving these inconsistent effects.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      Millet et al. show that C. elegans systematically prefers easy-to-eat bacteria but will switch its choice when harder-to-eat bacteria are offered at higher densities, producing indifference points that fit standard economic discounting models. Detailed kinetic analysis reveals that this bias arises from unchanged patch-entry rates but significantly elevated exit rates on effortful food, and dop-3 mutants lose the preference altogether, implicating dopamine in effort sensitivity. These findings extend effort-discounting behavior to a simple nematode, pushing the phylogenetic boundary of economic cost-benefit decision-making.

      Strengths:

      (1) Extends the well-characterized concept of effort discounting into _C. elegans_, setting a new phylogenetic boundary and opening invertebrate genetics to economic-behavior studies.

      (2) Elegant use of cephalexin-elongated bacteria to manipulate "effort" without altering nutritional or olfactory cues, yielding clear preference reversals and reproducible indifference points.

      (3) Application of standard discounting models to predict novel indifference points is both rigorous and quantitatively satisfying, reinforcing the interpretation of worm behavior in economic terms.

      (4) The three-state patch-model cleanly separates entry and exit dynamics, showing that increased leaving rates-rather than altered re-entry-drive choice biases.

      (5) Investigates the role of dopamine in this behavior to try to establish shared mechanisms with vertebrates.

      (6) Demonstration of discounting in wild strain (solid evidence).

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The kinetic model omits rich trajectory details-such as turning angles or hazard functions-that could distinguish a bona fide roaming transition from other exit behaviors.

      (2) Only _dop-3_ shows an effect, and the statistical validity of this result is questionable. It is not clear if the authors corrected for multiple comparisons, and the effect size is quite small and noisy, given the large number of worms tested. Other mutants do not show effects. Given these two concerns, the role of dopamine in c. elegans effort discounting was unconvincing.

      (3) With only five wild isolates tested (and variable data quality), it's hard to conclude that effort discounting isn't a lab-strain artifact or how broadly it varies in natural populations.

      (4) Detailed analysis of behavior beyond preference indices would strengthen the dopamine link and the claim of effort discounting in wild strains.

      (5) A few mechanistic statements (e.g., tying satiety exclusively to nutrient signals) would benefit from explicit citations or brief clarifications for non-worm specialists.

    4. Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The authors establish a behavioral task to explore effort discounting in C. elegans. By using bacterial food that takes longer to consume, the authors show that, for equivalent effort, as measured by pumping rate, they obtain less food, as measured by fat deposition.

      The authors formalize the task by applying a formal neuroeconomic decision-making model that includes value, effort, and discounting. They use this to estimate the discounting that C. elegans applies based on ingestion effort by using a population-level 2-choice T-maze.

      They then analyze the behavioral dynamics of individual animals transitioning between on-food and off-food states. Harder to ingest bacteria led to increased food patch leaving.

      Finally, they examined a set of mutants defective in different aspects of dopamine signaling, as dopamine plays a key role in discounting in vertebrates and regulates certain aspects of C. elegans foraging.

      Strengths:

      The behavioral experiments and neuroeconomic analysis framework are compelling and interesting, and make a significant contribution to the field. While these foraging behaviors have been extensively studied, few include clearly articulated theoretical models to be tested.

      Demonstrating that C. elegans effort discounting fits model predictions and has stable indifference points is important for establishing these tasks as a model for decision making.

      Weaknesses:

      The dopamine experiments are harder to interpret. The authors point out the perplexing lack of an effect of dat-1 and cat-2. dop-3 leads to general indifference. I am not sure this is the expected result if the argument is a parallel functional role to discounting in vertebrates. dop-3 causes a range of locomotor phenotypes and may affect feeding (reduced fat storage), and thus, there may be a general defect in the ability to perform the task rather than anything specific to discounting.

      That said, some of the other DA mutants also have locomotor defects and do not differ from N2. But there is no clear result here - my concern is that global mutants in such a critical pathway exhibit such pleiotropy that it's difficult to conclude there is a clear and specific role for DA in effort discounting. This would require more targeted or cell-specific approaches.

      Meanwhile, there are other pathways known to affect responses to food and patch leaving decisions: serotonin, pigment-dispersing factor, tyramine, etc. The paper would have benefited from a clarification about why these were not considered as promising candidates to test (in addition to or instead of dopamine).

    1. eLife Assessment

      This study provides compelling evidence that action potential (AP) broadening is not a universal feature of homeostatic plasticity in response to chronic activity deprivation. By leveraging state-of-the-art methods across multiple brain regions and laboratories, the authors demonstrate that AP half-width remains largely stable, challenging previous assumptions in the field. These important findings help resolve longstanding inconsistencies in the literature and significantly advance our understanding of neuronal network homeostasis.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Ritzau-Jost et al. investigate the potential contribution of AP broadening in homeostatic upregulation of neuronal network activity with a specific focus on dissociated neuronal cultures. In cultures obtained from a few brain regions from mice or rats using different culture conditions and examined by different laboratories, AP half-width remained stable despite chronic activity block with TTX. The finding suggests that AP width is not significantly modulated by changes in sodium channel activity.

      Strengths:

      The collaborative nature of the study amongst the neuronal culture experts and the rigorous electrophysiological assessments provides for a compelling support of the main conclusion.

      Weaknesses:

      Given the negative nature of the results, a couple of remaining issues (such as the cell density of cultures and the presentation of imaging experiments with a voltage sensor) warrant further consideration. In addition, a discussion of the reasons for the seeming stability of AP half-width to sodium channel modulation might help extend the scope of the study beyond the presentation of a negative conclusion.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study reexamined the idea that action potential broadening serves as a homeostatic mechanism to compensate for changes in network activity. The key finding was that, while action potential broadening does occur in certain neurons - such as CA3 pyramidal cells-it is far from a universal response. This is important because it helps resolve longstanding discrepancies in the field, thereby contributing to a better understanding of network dynamics. The replication of these findings across multiple laboratories further strengthened the study's rigor.

      Strengths:

      Mechanisms of network homeostasis are essential to understand network dynamics.

      Weaknesses:

      No weaknesses were noted by this reviewer.

    4. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript "Unreliable homeostatic action potential broadening in cultured dissociated neurons" by Ritzau-Jost et al. investigates action potential (AP) broadening as a mechanism underlying homeostatic synaptic plasticity. Given the existing variability in the literature concerning AP broadening, the authors address an important and timely research question of considerable interest to the field.

      The study systematically demonstrates cell-type- and model-specific AP broadening in hippocampal neurons after chronic treatment with either tetrodotoxin (TTX) or glutamatergic transmission blockers. The findings indicate AP broadening in CA3 pyramidal neurons in organotypic cultures after TTX treatment, but notably not in dissociated hippocampal neurons under identical conditions. However, blocking glutamatergic neurotransmission caused AP broadening in dissociated hippocampal neurons. Moreover, extensive evaluations in neocortical dissociated cultures robustly challenge previous findings by revealing a lack of AP broadening following TTX treatment. Additionally, the proposed role of BK-type potassium channels in mediating AP broadening is convincingly questioned through complementary electrophysiological and voltage-imaging experiments.

      Strengths:

      The manuscript exhibits an outstanding experimental design, employing state-of-the-art techniques and a rigorous multi-lab validation approach that greatly enhances scientific reliability. The experimental results are meticulously illustrated, and the conclusions drawn are justified and supported by the presented data. Furthermore, the manuscript is comprehensively and clearly written.

      Weaknesses:

      Concerning the statistical analyses employed, it is advisable to consider the Kruskal-Wallis test with corrections for multiple comparisons when evaluating more than two experimental groups.

    1. eLife Assessment

      This valuable work investigates cooperative behaviors in adolescents using a repeated Prisoner's Dilemma game. The computational modeling approach used in the study is solid and well established, yet evidence supporting certain claims remains incomplete. The work could be strengthened with the consideration of additional experimental contexts, non-linear relationships between age and observed behavior, and modeling details. If these concerns are addressed, the results will be of interest to developmental psychologists, economists, and social psychologists.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Wu and colleagues aimed to explain previous findings that adolescents, compared to adults, show reduced cooperation following cooperative behaviour from a partner in several social scenarios. The authors analysed behavioural data from adolescents and adults performing a zero-sum Prisoner's Dilemma task and compared a range of social and non-social reinforcement learning models to identify potential algorithmic differences. Their findings suggest that adolescents' lower cooperation is best explained by a reduced learning rate for cooperative outcomes, rather than differences in prior expectations about the cooperativeness of a partner. The authors situate their results within the broader literature, proposing that adolescents' behaviour reflects a stronger preference for self-interest rather than a deficit in mentalising.

      Strengths:

      The work as a whole suggests that, in line with past work, adolescents prioritise value accumulation, and this can be, in part, explained by algorithmic differences in weighted value learning. The authors situate their work very clearly in past literature, and make it obvious the gap they are testing and trying to explain. The work also includes social contexts that move the field beyond non-social value accumulation in adolescents. The authors compare a series of formal approaches that might explain the results and establish generative and model-comparison procedures to demonstrate the validity of their winning model and individual parameters. The writing was clear, and the presentation of the results was logical and well-structured.

      Weaknesses:

      I also have some concerns about the methods used to fit and approximate parameters of interest. Namely, the use of maximum likelihood versus hierarchical methods to fit models on an individual level, which may reduce some of the outliers noted in the supplement, and also may improve model identifiability.

      There was also little discussion given the structure of the Prisoner's Dilemma, and the strategy of the game (that defection is always dominant), meaning that the preferences of the adolescents cannot necessarily be distinguished from the incentives of the game, i.e. they may seem less cooperative simply because they want to play the dominant strategy, rather than a lower preferences for cooperation if all else was the same.

      Appraisal & Discussion:

      The authors have partially achieved their aims, but I believe the manuscript would benefit from additional methodological clarification, specifically regarding the use of hierarchical model fitting and the inclusion of Bayes Factors, to more robustly support their conclusions. It would also be important to investigate the source of the model confusion observed in two of their models.

      I am unconvinced by the claim that failures in mentalising have been empirically ruled out, even though I am theoretically inclined to believe that adolescents can mentalise using the same procedures as adults. While reinforcement learning models are useful for identifying biases in learning weights, they do not directly capture formal representations of others' mental states. Greater clarity on this point is needed in the discussion, or a toning down of this language.

      Additionally, a more detailed discussion of the incentives embedded in the Prisoner's Dilemma task would be valuable. In particular, the authors' interpretation of reduced adolescent cooperativeness might be reconsidered in light of the zero-sum nature of the game, which differs from broader conceptualisations of cooperation in contexts where defection is not structurally incentivised.

      Overall, I believe this work has the potential to make a meaningful contribution to the field. Its impact would be strengthened by more rigorous modelling checks and fitting procedures, as well as by framing the findings in terms of the specific game-theoretic context, rather than general cooperation.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript investigates age-related differences in cooperative behavior by comparing adolescents and adults in a repeated Prisoner's Dilemma Game (rPDG). The authors find that adolescents exhibit lower levels of cooperation than adults. Specifically, adolescents reciprocate partners' cooperation to a lesser degree than adults do. Through computational modeling, they show that this relatively low cooperation rate is not due to impaired expectations or mentalizing deficits, but rather a diminished intrinsic reward for reciprocity. A social reinforcement learning model with asymmetric learning rate best captured these dynamics, revealing age-related differences in how positive and negative outcomes drive behavioral updates. These findings contribute to understanding the developmental trajectory of cooperation and highlight adolescence as a period marked by heightened sensitivity to immediate rewards at the expense of long-term prosocial gains.

      Strengths:

      (1) Rigid model comparison and parameter recovery procedure.

      (2) Conceptually comprehensive model space.

      (3) Well-powered samples.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) A key conceptual distinction between learning from non-human agents (e.g., bandit machines) and human partners is that the latter are typically assumed to possess stable behavioral dispositions or moral traits. When a non-human source abruptly shifts behavior (e.g., from 80% to 20% reward), learners may simply update their expectations. In contrast, a sudden behavioral shift by a previously cooperative human partner can prompt higher-order inferences about the partner's trustworthiness or the integrity of the experimental setup (e.g., whether the partner is truly interactive or human). The authors may consider whether their modeling framework captures such higher-order social inferences. Specifically, trait-based models-such as those explored in Hackel et al. (2015, Nature Neuroscience)-suggest that learners form enduring beliefs about others' moral dispositions, which then modulate trial-by-trial learning. A learner who believes their partner is inherently cooperative may update less in response to a surprising defection, effectively showing a trait-based dampening of learning rate.

      (2) This asymmetry in belief updating has been observed in prior work (e.g., Siegel et al., 2018, Nature Human Behaviour) and could be captured using a dynamic or belief-weighted learning rate. Models incorporating such mechanisms (e.g., dynamic learning rate models as in Jian Li et al., 2011, Nature Neuroscience) could better account for flexible adjustments in response to surprising behavior, particularly in the social domain.

      (3) Second, the developmental interpretation of the observed effects would be strengthened by considering possible non-linear relationships between age and model parameters. For instance, certain cognitive or affective traits relevant to social learning-such as sensitivity to reciprocity or reward updating-may follow non-monotonic trajectories, peaking in late adolescence or early adulthood. Fitting age as a continuous variable, possibly with quadratic or spline terms, may yield more nuanced developmental insights.

      (4) Finally, the two age groups compared - adolescents (high school students) and adults (university students) - differ not only in age but also in sociocultural and economic backgrounds. High school students are likely more homogenous in regional background (e.g., Beijing locals), while university students may be drawn from a broader geographic and socioeconomic pool. Additionally, differences in financial independence, family structure (e.g., single-child status), and social network complexity may systematically affect cooperative behavior and valuation of rewards. Although these factors are difficult to control fully, the authors should more explicitly address the extent to which their findings reflect biological development versus social and contextual influences.

    4. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Wu and colleagues find that in a repeated Prisoner's Dilemma, adolescents, compared to adults, are less likely to increase their cooperation behavior in response to repeated cooperation from a simulated partner. In contrast, after repeated defection by the partner, both age groups show comparable behavior.

      To uncover the mechanisms underlying these patterns, the authors compare eight different models. They report that a social reward learning model, which includes separate learning rates for positive and negative prediction errors, best fits the behavior of both groups. Key parameters in this winning model vary with age: notably, the intrinsic value of cooperating is lower in adolescents. Adults and adolescents also differ in learning rates for positive and negative prediction errors, as well as in the inverse temperature parameter.

      Strengths:

      The modeling results are compelling in their ability to distinguish between learned expectations and the intrinsic value of cooperation. The authors skillfully compare relevant models to demonstrate which mechanisms drive cooperation behavior in the two age groups.

      Weaknesses:

      Some of the claims made are not fully supported by the data:

      The central parameter reflecting preference for cooperation is positive in both groups. Thus, framing the results as self-interest versus other-interest may be misleading.

      It is unclear why the authors assume adolescents and adults have the same expectations about the partner's cooperation, yet simultaneously demonstrate age-related differences in learning about the partner. To support their claim mechanistically, simulations showing that differences in cooperation preference (i.e., the w parameter), rather than differences in learning, drive behavioral differences would be helpful.

      Two different schedules of 120 trials were used: one with stable partner behavior and one with behavior changing after 20 trials. While results for order effects are reported, the results for the stable vs. changing phases within each schedule are not. Since learning is influenced by reward structure, it is important to test whether key findings hold across both phases.

      The division of participants at the legal threshold of 18 years should be more explicitly justified. The age distribution appears continuous rather than clearly split. Providing rationale and including continuous analyses would clarify how groupings were determined.

      Claims of null effects (e.g., in the abstract: "adults increased their intrinsic reward for reciprocating... a pattern absent in adolescents") should be supported with appropriate statistics, such as Bayesian regression.

      Once claims are more closely aligned with the data, the study will offer a valuable contribution to the field, given its use of relevant models and a well-established paradigm.

    1. eLife Assessment

      The authors investigated the potential role of IgG N-glycosylation in Haemorrhagic Fever with Renal Syndrome (HFRS), which may offer significant insights for understanding molecular mechanisms and for the development of therapeutic strategies for this infectious disease. The findings are useful to the field, although the strength of evidence to support the findings is incomplete. Several issues need to be addressed, including more detail on the background, methods, and results. Additional statistical tests should be performed, and the conclusions should reflect the correlational findings of the paper.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors investigated the potential role of IgG N-glycosylation in Haemorrhagic Fever with Renal Syndrome (HFRS), which may offer significant insights for understanding molecular mechanisms and for the development of therapeutic strategies for this infectious disease. However, several issues need to be addressed.

      Major Points:

      (1) The authors should provide a detailed description of the pathogenesis of Haemorrhagic Fever with Renal Syndrome (HFRS) and elaborate on the crucial role of IgG proteins in the disease's progression (line 65).

      (2) An additional discussion on the significance of glycosylation, particularly IgG N-glycosylation, in viral infections should be included in the Introduction section.

      (3) In the Abstract section, the authors state that HTNV-specific IgG antibody titers were detected and IgG N-glycosylation was analyzed. However, the analysis of plasma IgG N-glycans is described in the Methods section. Therefore, the authors should clarify the glycome analysis process. Was the specific IgG glycome profile similar to the total IgG N-glycome? Given the biological relevance of specific IgG in immunological diseases, characterizing the specific IgG N-glycome profile would be more significant than analyzing the total plasma IgG.

      (4) Further details regarding the N-glycome analysis should be provided, including the quantity of IgG protein used and the methodology employed for analyzing IgG N-glycans (lines 286-287).

      (5) Additional statistical analyses should be performed, including multiple comparisons with p-value adjustment, false discovery rate (FDR) control, and Pearson correlation (line 291).

      (6) Quality control should be conducted prior to the IgG N-glycome analysis. Additionally, both biological and technical replicates are essential to assess the reproducibility and robustness of the methods.

      (7) Multiple regression analysis should be conducted to evaluate the influence of genetic and environmental factors on the IgG N-glycome.

      (8) Line 196. Additional discussions should be included, focusing on the underlying correlation between the differential expression of B-cell glycogenes and the dysregulated IgG N-glycome profile, as well as the potential molecular mechanisms of IgG N-glycosylation in the development of HFRS.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This work sought to explore antibody responses in the context of hemorrhagic fever with renal syndrome (HFRS) - a severe disease caused by Hantaan virus infection. Little is known about the characteristics or functional relevance of IgG Fc glycosylation in HFRS. To address this gap, the authors analyzed samples from 65 patients with HFRS spanning the acute and convalescent phases of disease via IgG Fc glycan analysis, scRNAseq, and flow cytometry. The authors observed changes in Fc glycosylation (increased fucosylation and decreased bisection) coinciding with a 4-fold or greater increase in Haantan virus-specific antibody titer. They suggest that these shifts contribute to disease recovery. The study also includes exploratory analyses linking IgG glycan profiles to glycosylation-related gene expression in distinct B cell subsets, using single-cell transcriptomics. Overall, this is an interesting study that combines serological profiling with transcriptomic data to shed light on humoral immune responses in an underexplored infectious disease. The integration of Fc glycosylation data with single-cell transcriptomic data is a strength. However, some improvements could be made in the clarity of both the Results and Materials and Methods sections, and some conclusions would benefit from greater caution, particularly in avoiding overinterpretation of correlative findings.

      Comments:

      (1) While it is great to reference prior publications in the Materials and Methods section, the current level of detail is insufficient to clearly understand the study design and experimental procedures performed. Readers should not be expected to consult multiple previous papers to grasp the core methodological aspects of the present paper. For instance, the categorization of HFRS patients into different clinical subtypes/courses, and the methods for measuring Fc glycosylation should be explicitly described in the Materials and Methods section of this manuscript.

      (2) The authors should explain the nature of their cohort in a bit more detail. While it appears that HFRS cases were identified based on IgM ELISA and/or PCR, these are indicators of the Haantan virus infection. My understanding is that not all Haantan virus infections progress to HFRS. Thus, it is unclear whether all patients in the HFRS group actually had hemorrhagic fever. This distinction is critical for interpreting how the results observed relate to disease severity.

      (3) The authors state that: "A 4-fold or greater increase in HTNV-NP-specific antibody titers usually indicates a protective humoral immune response during the acute phase", but they do not cite any references or provide any context that supports this claim. Given that in their own words, one of the most significant findings in the study is changes in glycosylation coinciding with this 4-fold increase, it is important to ground this claim in evidence. Without this, the use of a 4-fold threshold appears arbitrary and weakens the rationale for using this immune state as a proxy for protective immunity.

      (4) The authors also claim that changes in Fc glycosylation influence recovery from HFRS - a point even emphasized in the manuscript title. However, this conclusion is not well supported by the data for two main reasons. First, the authors appear to measure bulk IgG Fc glycans, not Fc glycans of Hantaan virus-specific antibodies. While reasonable, this is something that should be communicated in the manuscript. Hantaan virus-specific antibodies are likely a very small fraction of total circulating IgG antibodies (perhaps ~1%), even during acute infection. As a result, changes in bulk Fc glycosylation may (or may not) accurately reflect the glycosylation state of Hantaan virus-specific antibodies. Second, even if the bulk Fc glycan shifts do mirror those of Hantaan virus-specific antibodies, it remains unclear whether these changes causally drive recovery or are merely a consequence of the infection being resolved. Thus, while the differences in Fc glycosylation observed are interesting - and it is tempting to speculate on their functional significance - the manuscript treats the observed correlations as causal mechanistic insight without sufficient data or justification.

      (5) Fc glycosylation is known to be influenced by covariates such as age and sex. While it is helpful that the authors stratified the patients by age group and looked for significant differences in glycosylation across them, a more robust approach would be to directly control for these covariates in the statistical analysis - such as by using a linear mixed effects model, in which disease state (e.g., acute vs. convalescent), age, and sex are treated as fixed effects, and subject ID is included as a random effect to account for repeated measures. This would allow the authors to assess whether observed differences in Fc glycosylation remain significant after accounting for potential confounders. This could be important given that some of the reported differences are quite small, for example, 94.29% vs. 94.89% fucosylation.

      (6) The manuscript states that there are limited studies on antibody glycosylation in the context of HFRS, but does not cite any relevant literature. If prior work exists, it should be cited to contextualize the current study. If no prior studies have been conducted/reported, to the author's knowledge, that should be stated explicitly to show the novelty of the work.

    1. eLife Assessment

      This study presents a valuable technical advance in the long-term live imaging of limb regeneration at cellular resolution in Parhyale hawaiensis. The authors develop and carefully validate a method to continuously image entire regenerating legs over several days while minimizing photodamage and optimizing conditions for robust cell tracking, together with post-hoc in situ identification of cell types. The data are convincing, the methodology is rigorous and clearly documented, and the results will be of interest to researchers in regeneration biology, developmental biology, and advanced live imaging and cell tracking software development.

      [Editors' note: this paper was reviewed by Review Commons.]

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Building upon their previous work, the authors present an enhanced method for confocal live imaging of leg regeneration in the crustacean Parhyale hawaiensis. Parhyale is an emerging and tractable model system that offers insights into the evolution and mechanisms of development and regeneration. Çevrim et al. demonstrate the ability to image the complete leg regeneration process, spanning several days, with 10-20 minute time intervals and cellular resolution. They have concurrently optimized imaging conditions to enable cell tracking while minimizing phototoxicity. Additionally, they report successfully implementing HCR in situ hybridization in Parhyale, allowing for specific gene transcript staining at the endpoint of live imaging. This opens the possibility of assigning molecular identities to tracked cells.

      A key challenge in many regeneration models is achieving continuous imaging throughout the entire regenerative process, as many organisms are difficult to immobilize or cannot tolerate extended imaging without stress. This manuscript's major strength lies in providing practical solutions to these challenges in Parhyale, a compelling and accessible arthropod model for limb regeneration. The authors also employ complementary tools to analyze time-lapse movies and correlate them with endpoint staining. Together, these advances will serve as a useful resource for researchers studying regeneration in Parhyale or in other systems where parts of this workflow can be adapted.

      While the data demonstrating the methodological advancement and technical feasibility are solid, much of the benchmarking and regeneration characterization remains qualitative. This does not undermine the validity of the proof-of-principle, but limits the study's broader appeal.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      The manuscript by Çevrim et al. presents a live-imaging workflow that captures the complete leg regeneration process in the crustacean Parhyale hawaiensis, at a resolution suitable for cell tracking and gene expression analysis. Building on earlier work describing selective stages of leg regeneration (Alwes et al., 2016), the authors recorded 22 confocal time-lapse movies, starting from amputation to full regeneration. They defined three distinct phases of regeneration (wound closure, cell proliferation and morphogenesis, and differentiation) based on cellular and morphological features.

      One movie was used to assess how imaging parameters (z-spacing, time intervals, and image quality) influence tracking reliability and the time required for manual proofreading, with an effort to minimize phototoxicity. Tracking was performed in the upper tissue layers using an improved version of the Mastodon plugin Elephant in Fiji. The same sample was fixed post-imaging for in situ hybridization using an HCR protocol adapted for adult legs, targeting the gene spineless. This enabled the alignment of gene expression with specific cell lineages and the identification of progenitor cells present at the time of amputation.

      In summary, the study provides a proof-of-principle for combining long-term live imaging, cell tracking, and gene expression analysis during regeneration. Given the labor-intensive nature of tracking over a 5-10 day time-lapse movie, the use of a single movie for this study is well justified. The workflow, from imaging to lineage reconstruction and molecular annotation, is successfully demonstrated and well documented with this dataset.

      Although the biological insights from the cell lineage and molecular mapping are still limited, the methodology offers significant potential in regenerative biology to uncover the cellular and molecular contributions to tissue and cell type re-formation.

      Confocal microscopy was used for live imaging, which restricted imaging to the upper 30 µm tissue layer. Light-sheet microscopy could have provided gentler imaging and enabled imaging from multiple angles to image the whole leg. While the authors acknowledge this possibility in the manuscript, they discarded it due to incompatibility between their mounting strategy and available light-sheet microscopes. As a future direction, optimizing the mounting approach for compatibility with light-sheet microscopes could enable more comprehensive tissue imaging.

    4. Author response: 

      We thank the reviewers for their feedback on our paper. We have taken all their comments into account in revising the manuscript. We provide a point-by-point response to their comments, below.

      Reviewer #1:

      Major comments:

      The manuscript is clearly written with a level of detail that allows others to reproduce the imaging and cell-tracking pipeline. Of the 22 movies recorded one was used for cell tracking. One movie seems sufficient for the second part of the manuscript, as this manuscript presents a proof-of-principle pipeline for an imaging experiment followed by cell tracking and molecular characterisation of the cells by HCR. In addition, cell tracking in a 5-10 day time-lapse movie is an enormous time commitment.

      My only major comment is regarding "Suppl_data_5_spineless_tracking". The image file does not load.

      It looks like the wrong file is linked to the mastodon dataset. The "Current BDV dataset path" is set to "Beryl_data_files/BLB mosaic cut movie-02.xml", but this file does not exist in the folder. Please link it to the correct file.

      We have corrected the file path in the updated version of Suppl. Data 5.

      Minor comments:

      The authors state that their imaging settings aim to reduce photo damage. Do they see cell death in the regenerating legs? Is the cell death induced by the light exposure or can they tell if the same cells die between the movies? That is, do they observe cell death in the same phases of regeneration and/or in the same regions of the regenerating legs?

      Yes, we observe cell death during Parhyale leg regeneration. We have added the following sentence to explain this in the revised manuscript: "During the course of regeneration some cells undergo apoptosis (reported in Alwes et al., 2016). Using the H2B-mRFPruby marker, apoptotic cells appear as bright pyknotic nuclei that break up and become engulfed by circulating phagocytes (see bright specks in Figure 2F)."

      We now also document apoptosis in regenerated legs that have not been subjected to live imaging in a new supplementary figure (Suppl. Figure 3),  and we refer to these observations as follows: "While some cell death might be caused by photodamage, apoptosis can also be observed in similar numbers in regenerating legs that have not been subjected to live imaging (Suppl. Figure 3)."

      Based on 22 movies, the authors divide the regeneration process into three phases and they describe that the timing of leg regeneration varies between individuals. Are the phases proportionally the same length between regenerating legs or do the authors find differences between fast/slow regenerating legs? If there is a difference in the proportions, why might this be?

      Both early and late phases contribute to variation in the speed of regeneration, but there is no clear relationship between the relative duration of each phase and the speed of regeneration. We now present graphs supporting these points in a new supplementary figure (Suppl. Figure 2).  

      To clarify this point, we have added the following sentence in the manuscript: "We find that the overall speed of leg regeneration is determined largely by variation in the speed of the early (wound closure) phase of regeneration, and to a lesser extent by variation in later phases when leg morphogenesis takes place (Suppl. Figure 2 A,B). There is no clear relationship between the relative duration of each phase and the speed of regeneration (Suppl. Figure 2 A',B')."

      Based on their initial cell tracing experiment, could the authors elaborate more on what kind of biological information can be extracted from the cell lineages, apart from determining which is the progenitor of a cell? What does it tell us about the cell population in the tissue? Is there indication of multi- or pluripotent stem cells? What does it say about the type of regeneration that is taking place in terms of epimorphosis and morphallaxis, the old concepts of regeneration?

      In the first paragraph of Future Directions we describe briefly the kind of biological information that could be gained by applying our live imaging approach with appropriate cell-type markers (see below). We do not comment further, as we do not currently have this information at hand. Regarding the concepts of epimorphosis and morphallaxis, as we explain in Alwes et al. 2016, these terms describe two extreme conditions that do not capture what we observe during Parhyale leg regeneration. Our current work does not bring new insights on this topic.

      Page 5. The authors mention the possibility of identifying the cell ID based on transcriptomic profiling data. Can they suggest how many and which cell types they expect to find in the last stage based on their transcriptomic data?

      We have added this sentence: "Using single-nucleus transcriptional profiling, we have identified approximately 15 transcriptionally-distinct cell types in adult Parhyale legs (Almazán et al., 2022), including epidermis, muscle, neurons, hemocytes, and a number of still unidentified cell types."

      Page 6. Correction: "..molecular and other makers.." should be "..molecular and other markers.."

      Corrected

      Page 8. The HCR in situ protocol probably has another important advantage over the conventional in situ protocol, which is not mentioned in this study. The hybridisation step in HCR is performed at a lower temperature (37˚C) than in conventional in situ hybridisation (65˚C, Rehm et al., 2009). In other organisms, a high hybridisation temperature affects the overall tissue morphology and cell location (tissue shrinkage). A lower hybridisation temperature has less impact on the tissue and makes manual cell alignment between the live imaging movie and the fixed HCR in situ stained specimen easier and more reliable. If this is also the case in Parhyale, the authors must mention it.

      This may be correct, but all our specimens were treated at 37˚C, so we cannot assess whether hybridisation temperature affects morphological preservation in our specimens.

      Page 9. The authors should include more information on the spineless study. What been is spineless? What do the cell lineages tell about the spineless progenitors, apart from them being spread in the tissue at the time of amputation? Do spineless progenitors proliferate during regeneration? Do any spineless expressing cells share a common progenitor cell?

      We now point out that spineless encodes a transcription factor. We provide a summary of the lineages generating spineless-expressing cells in Suppl. Figure 6, and we explain that "These epidermal progenitors undergo 0, 1 or 2 cell divisions, and generate mostly spineless-expressing cells (Suppl. Figure 5)."

      Page 10. Regarding the imaging temperature, the Materials and Methods state "... a temperature control chamber set to 26 or 27˚C..."; however, in Suppl. Data 1, 26˚C and 29˚C are indicated as imaging temperatures. Which is correct?

      We corrected the Methods by adding "with the exception of dataset li51, imaged at 29°C"

      Page 10. Regarding the imaging step size, the Materials and Methods state "...step size of 1-2.46 µm..."; however, Suppl. Data 1 indicate a step size between 1.24 - 2.48 µm. Which is correct?

      We corrected the Methods.

      Page 11. Correct "...as the highest resolution data..." to "...at the highest resolution data..."

      The original text is correct ("standardised to the same dimensions as the highest resolution data").

      Page 11. Indicate which supplementary data set is referred to: "Using Mastodon, we generated ground truth annotations on the original image dataset, consisting of 278 cell tracks, including 13,888 spots and 13,610 links across 55 time points (see Supplementary Data)."

      Corrected

      p. 15. Indicate which supplementary data set is referred to: "In this study we used HCR probes for the Parhyale orthologues of futsch (MSTRG.441), nompA (MSTRG.6903) and spineless (MSTRG.197), ordered from Molecular Instruments (20 oligonucleotides per probe set). The transcript sequences targeted by each probe set are given in the Supplementary Data."

      Corrected

      Figure 3. Suggestion to the overview schematics: The authors might consider adding "molting" as the end point of the red bar (representing differentiation).

      The time of molting is not known in the majority of these datasets, because the specimens were fixed and stained prior to molting. We added the relevant information in the figure legend: "Datasets li-13 and li-16 were recorded until the molt; the other recordings were stopped before molting."

      Figure 4B': Please indicate that the nuclei signal is DAPI.

      Corrected

      Supplementary figure 1A. Word is missing in the figure legend: ...the image also shows weak…

      Corrected

      Supplementary Figure 2: Please indicate the autofluorescence in the granular cells. Does it correspond to the yellow cells?

      Corrected

      Video legend for video 1 and 2. Please correct "H2B-mREFruby" to "H2B-mRFPruby".

      Corrected

      Reviewer #2:

      Major comments:

      MC 1. Given that most of the technical advances necessary to achieve the work described in this manuscript have been published previously, it would be helpful for the authors to more clearly identify the primary novelty of this manuscript. The abstract and introduction to the manuscript focus heavily on the technical details of imaging and analysis optimization and some additional summary of the implications of these advances should be included here to aid the reader.

      This paper describes a technical advance. While previous work (Alwes et al. 2016) established some key elements of our live imaging approach, we were not at that time able to record the entire time course of leg regeneration (the longest recordings were 3.5 days long). Here we present a method for imaging the entire course of leg regeneration (up to 10 days of imaging), optimised to reduce photodamage and to improve cell tracking. We also develop a method of in situ staining in cuticularised adult legs (an important technical breakthrough in this experimental system), which we combine with live imaging to determine the fate of tracked cells. We have revised the abstract and introduction of the paper to point out these novelties, in relation to our previous publications.

      In the abstract we explain: "Building on previous work that allowed us to image different parts of the process of leg regeneration in the crustacean Parhyale hawaiensis, we present here a method for live imaging that captures the entire process of leg regeneration, spanning up to 10 days, at cellular resolution. Our method includes (1) mounting and long-term live imaging of regenerating legs under conditions that yield high spatial and temporal resolution but minimise photodamage, (2) fixing and in situ staining of the regenerated legs that were imaged, to identify cell fates, and (3) computer-assisted cell tracking to determine the cell lineages and progenitors of identified cells. The method is optimised to limit light exposure while maximising tracking efficiency."

      The introduction includes the following text: "Our first systematic study using this approach presented continuous live imaging over periods of 2-3 days, capturing key events of leg regeneration such as wound closure, cell proliferation and morphogenesis of regenerating legs with single-cell resolution (Alwes et al., 2016). Here, we extend this work by developing a method for imaging the entire course of leg regeneration, optimised to reduce photodamage and to improve cell tracking. We also develop a method of in situ staining of gene expression in cuticularised adult legs, which we combine with live imaging to determine the fate of tracked cells."

      MC 2. The description of the regeneration time course is nicely detailed but also very qualitative. A major advantage of continuous recording and automated cell tracking in the manner presented in this manuscript would be to enable deeper quantitative characterization of cellular and tissue dynamics during regeneration. Rather than providing movies and manually annotated timelines, some characterization of the dynamics of the regeneration process (the heterogeneity in this is very very interesting, but not analyzed at all) and correlating them against cellular behaviors would dramatically increase the impact of the work and leverage the advances presented here. For example, do migration rates differ between replicates? Division rates? Division synchrony? Migration orientation? This seems to be an incredibly rich dataset that would be fascinating to explore in greater detail, which seems to me to be the primary advance presented in this manuscript. I can appreciate that the authors may want to segregate some biological findings from the method, but I believe some nominal effort highlighting the quantitative nature of what this method enables would strengthen the impact of the paper and be useful for the reader. Selecting a small number of simple metrics (eg. Division frequency, average cell migration speed) and plotting them alongside the qualitative phases of the regeneration timeline that have already been generated would be a fairly modest investment of effort using tools that already exist in the Mastodon interface, I would roughly estimate on the order of an hour or two per dataset. I believe that this effort would be well worth it and better highlight a major strength of the approach.

      The primary goal of this work was to establish a robust method for continuous long-term live imaging of regeneration, but we do appreciate that a more quantitative analysis would add value to the data we are presenting. We tried to address this request in three steps:

      First, we examined whether clear temporal patterns in cell division, cell movements or other cellular features can be observed in an accurately tracked dataset (li13-t4, tracked in Sugawara et al. 2022). To test this we used the feature extraction functions now available on the Mastodon platform (see link). We could discern a meaningful temporal pattern for cell divisions (see below); the other features showed no interpretable pattern of variation.

      Second, we asked whether we could use automated cell tracking to analyse the patterns of cell division in all our datasets. Using an Elephant deep learning model trained on the tracks of the li13-t4 dataset, we performed automated cell tracking in the same dataset, and compared the pattern of cell divisions from the automated cell track predictions with those coming from manually validated cell tracks. We observed that the automated tracks gave very imprecise results, with a high background of false positives obscuring the real temporal pattern (see images below, with validated data on the left, automated tracking on the right). These results show that the automated cell tracking is not accurate enough to provide a meaningful picture on the pattern of cell divisions.

      Third, we tried to improve the accuracy of detection of dividing cells by additional training of Elephant models on each dataset (to lower the rate of false positives), followed by manual proofreading. Given how labour intensive this is, we could only apply this approach to 4 additional datasets. The results of this analysis are presented in Figure 4.

      Author response image 1.

      MC 3. The authors describe the challenges faced by their described approach:

      Using this mode of semi-automated and manual cell tracking, we find that most cells in the upper slices of our image stacks (top 30 microns) can be tracked with a high degree of confidence. A smaller proportion of cell lineages are trackable in the deeper layers.

      Given that the authors quantify this in Table 1, it would aid the reader to provide metrics in the manuscript text at this point. Furthermore, the metrics provided in Table 1 appear to be for overall performance, but the text describes that performance appears to be heavily depth dependent. Segregating the performance metrics further, for example providing DET, TRA, precision and recall for superficial layers only and for the overall dataset, would help support these arguments and better highlight performance a potential adopter of the method might expect.

      In the revised manuscript we have added data on the tracking performance of Elephant in relation to imaging depth in Suppl. Figure 3. These data confirm our original statement (which was based on manual tracking) that nuclei are more challenging to track in deeper layers.

      We point to these new results in two parts of the paper, as follows: "A smaller proportion of cells are trackable in the deeper layers (see Suppl. Figure 3)", and "Our results, summarised in Table 1A, show that the detection of nuclei can be enhanced by doubling the z resolution at the expense of xy resolution and image quality. This improvement is particularly evident in the deeper layers of the imaging stacks, which are usually the most challenging to track (Suppl. Figure 3)."

      MC 4. Performance characterization in Table 1 appears to derive from a single dataset that is then subsampled and processed in different ways to assess the impact of these changes on cell tracking and detection performance. While this is a suitable strategy for this type of optimization it leaves open the question of performance consistency across datasets. I fully recognize that this type of quantification can be onerous and time consuming, but some attempt to assess performance variability across datasets would be valuable. Manual curation over a short time window over a random sampling of the acquired data would be sufficient to assess this.

      We think that similar trade-offs will apply to all our datasets because tracking performance is constrained by the same features, which are intrinsic to our system; e.g. by the crowding of nuclei in relation to axial resolution, or the speed of mitosis in relation to the temporal resolution of imaging. We therefore do not see a clear rationale for repeating this analysis. On a practical level, our existing image datasets could not be subsampled to generate the various conditions tested in Table 1, so proving this point experimentally would require generating new recordings, and tracking these to generate ground truth data. This would require months of additional work.

      A second, related question is whether Elephant would perform equally well in detecting and tracking nuclei across different datasets. This point has been addressed in the Sugawara et al. 2022 paper, where the performance of Elephant was tested on diverse fluorescence datasets.

      Reviewer #3:

      Major comments:

      • The authors should clearly specify what are the key technical improvements compared to their previous studies (Alwes et al. 2016, Elife; Konstantinides & Averof 2014, Science). There, the approaches for mounting, imaging, and cell tracking are already introduced, and the imaging is reported to run for up to 7 days in some cases.

      In Konstantinides and Averof (2014) we did not present any live imaging at cellular resolution. In Alwes et al. (2016) we described key elements of our live imaging approach, but we were never able to record the entire time course of leg regeneration. The longest recordings in that work were 3.5 days long.

      We have revised the abstract and introduction to clarify the novelty of this work, in relation to our previous publications. Please see our response to comment MC1 of reviewer 2.

      • While the authors mention testing the effect of imaging parameters (such as scanning speed and line averaging) on the imaging/tracking outcome, very little or no information is provided on how this was done beyond the parameters that they finally arrived to.

      Scan speed and averaging parameters were determined by measuring contrast and signal-to-noise ratios in images captured over a range of settings. We have now added these data in Supplementary Figure 1.

      • The authors claim that, using the acquired live imaging data across entire regeneration time course, they are now able to confirm and extend their description of leg regeneration. However, many claims about the order and timing of various cellular events during regeneration are supported only by references to individual snapshots in figures or supplementary movies. Presenting a more quantitative description of cellular processes during regeneration from the acquired data would significantly enhance the manuscript and showcase the usefulness of the improved workflow.

      The events we describe can be easily observed in the maximum projections, available in Suppl. Data 2. Regarding the quantitative analysis, please see our response to comment MC2 of reviewer 2.  

      • Table 1 summarizes the performance of cell tracking using simulated datasets of different quality. However only averages and/or maxima are given for the different metrics, which makes it difficult to evaluate the associated conclusions. In some cases, only 1 or 2 test runs were performed.

      The metrics extracted from each of the three replicates, per dataset, are now included in Suppl. Data 4.

      We consistently used 3 replicates to measure tracking performance with each of the datasets. The "replicates" column label in Table 1 referred to the number of scans that were averaged to generate the image, not to the replicates used for estimating the tracking performance. To avoid confusion, we changed that label to "averaging".

      • OPTIONAL: An imaging approach that allows using the current mounting strategy but could help with some of the tradeoffs is using a spinning-disk confocal microscope instead of a laser scanning one. If the authors have such a system available, it could be interesting to compare it with their current scanning confocal setup.

      Preliminary experiments that we carried out several years ago on a spinning disk confocal (with a 20x objective and the CSU-W1 spinning disk) were not very encouraging, and we therefore did not pursue this approach further. The main problem was bad image quality in deeper tissue layers.

      Minor comments:

      • The presented imaging protocol was optimized for one laser wavelength only (561 nm) - this should be mentioned when discussing the technical limitations since animals tend to react differently to different wavelengths. Same settings might thus not be applicable for imaging a different fluorescent protein.

      In the second paragraph of the Results section, we explain that we perform the imaging at long wavelengths in order to minimise photodamage. It should be clear to the readers that changing the excitation wavelength will have an impact for long-term live imaging.

      • For transferability, it would be useful if the intensity of laser illumination was measured and given in the Methods, instead of just a relative intensity setting from the imaging software. Similarly,more details of the imaging system should be provided where appropriate (e.g., detector specifications).

      We have now measured the intensity of the laser illumination and added this information in the

      Methods: "Laser power was typically set to 0.3% to 0.8%, which yields 0.51 to 1.37 µW at 561 nm (measured with a ThorLabs Microscope Slide Power Sensor, #S170C)."

      Regarding the imaging system and the detector, we provide all the information that is available to us on the microscope's technical sheets.

      • The versions of analysis scripts associated with the manuscript should be uploaded to an online repository that permanently preserves the respective version.

      The scripts are now available on gitbub and online repositories. The relevant links are included in the revised manuscript.

    1. eLife Assessment

      This fundamental study demonstrates how a left-right bias in the relationship between numerical magnitude and space depends on brain lateralization. The evidence is compelling and will be of interest to researchers studying numerical cognition, brain lateralization, and cognitive brain development more broadly.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Functional lateralization between the right and left hemispheres is reported widely in animal taxa, including humans. However, it remains largely speculative as to whether the lateralized brains have a cognitive gain or a sort of fitness advantage. In the present study, by making use of the advantages of domestic chicks as a model, the authors are successful in revealing that the lateralized brain is advantageous in the number sense, in which numerosity is associated with spatial arrangements of items. Behavioral evidence is strong enough to support their arguments. Brain lateralization was manipulated by light exposure during the terminal phase of incubation, and the left-to-right numerical representation appeared when the distance between items gave a reliable spatial cue. The light-exposure induced lateralization, though quite unique in avian species, together with the lack of intense inter-hemispheric direct connections (such as the corpus callosum in the mammalian cerebrum), was critical for the successful analysis in this study. Specification of the responsible neural substrates in the presumed right hemisphere is expected in future research. Comparable experimental manipulation in the mammalian brain must be developed to address this general question (functional significance of brain laterality) is also expected.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This is the first study to show how a L-R bias in the relationship between numerical magnitude and space depends on brain lateralisation, and moreover, how this is modulated by in ovo conditions.

      Strengths:

      Novel methodology for investigating the innateness and neural basis of a L-R bias in the relationship between number and space.

      Weaknesses:

      I would query the way the experiment was contextualised. They ask whether culture or innate pre-wiring determines the 'left-to-right orientation of the MNL [mental number line]'.<br /> The term, 'Mental Number Line' is an inference from experimental tasks. One of the first experimental demonstrations of a preference or bias for small numbers in the left of space and larger numbers in the right of space, was more carefully described as the spatial-numerical association of response codes - the SNARC effect (Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and numerical magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 371-396).<br /> This has meant that the background to the study is confusing. First, they note correctly that many other creatures, including insects can show this bias, though in none of these has neural lateralisation been shown to be a cause. Second, their clever experiment shows that an experimental manipulation creates the bias. If it were innate and common to other species, the experimental manipulation shouldn't matter. There would always be a L-R bias. Third, they seem to be asserting that humans have a left-to-right (L-R) MNL. This is highly contentious, and in some studies, reading direction affects it, as the original study by Dehaene et al showed; and in others, task affects direction (e.g. Bachtold, D., Baumüller, M., & Brugger, P. (1998). Stimulus-response compatibility in representational space. Neuropsychologia, 36, 731-735, not cited). Moreover, a very careful study of adult humans, found no L-R bias (Karolis, V., Iuculano, T., & Butterworth, B. (2011), not cited). Mapping numerical magnitudes along the right lines: Differentiating between scale and bias. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140(4), 693-706). Indeed, Rugani et al claim, incorrectly, that the L-R bias was first reported by Galton in 1880. There are two errors here: first, Galton was reporting what he called 'visualised numerals' and are typically referred to now as 'number forms' - spontaneous and habitual conscious visual representations - not an inference from a number line task. Second, Galton reported right-to-left, circular, and vertical visualised numerals, and no simple left-to-right examples (Galton, F. (1880). Visualised numerals. Nature, 21, 252-256.). So in fact did Bertillon, J. (1880). De la vision des nombres. La Nature, 378, 196-198, and more recently Seron, X., Pesenti, M., Noël, M.-P., Deloche, G., & Cornet, J.-A. (1992). Images of numbers, or "When 98 is upper left and 6 sky blue". Cognition, 44, 159-196, and Tang, J., Ward, J., & Butterworth, B. (2008). Number forms in the brain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(9), 1547-1556.

      If the authors are committed to chicks' MN Line they should test a series of numbers showing that the bias to left is greater for 2 and 3 than for 4 etc.

      What does all this mean? I think that the experiment should absolutely be published in eLife, but the paper should be shorn of its misleading contextualisation, including the term 'Mental Number Line'. The authors also speculate, usefully, on why chicks and other species might have a L-R bias. I don't think the speculations are convincing, but at least if there is an evolutionary basis for the bias, it should at least be discussed.

      In fact, I think it would make a very interesting special issue to bring up to date how and why the L-R bias exists, and where and why it does not.

      Karolis, V., Iuculano, T., & Butterworth, B. (2011). Mapping numerical magnitudes along the right lines: Differentiating between scale and bias. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140(4), 693-706. doi:10.1037/a0024255

      Review of the revised version:

      The background and terminology in the text have been significantly altered and clarified: Spatial Numerical Association (SNA) instead of Mental Number Line (MNL) in the text, but with a discussion about how SNA might be the basis of MNL. This entails a link from SNA - a bias - to mental representation of a sequence of numerical magnitudes, which will need to be spelt out in subsequent work with a sequence of numbers rather than a single number, in this case 4. Could the effect be generalised to much larger numbers?

      Although the relationship between number and space seems fundamental, the key question is why the L-R SNA bias should exist at all. The authors take on this challenge and make important arguments for the evolutionary advantage of the bias is (see lines 138ff, 375ff, 444ff), though this is likely still to be controversial.

      Subsequent work may clarify its interaction of brain lateralisation with culture, notably reading and writing direction (e.g. Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and numerical magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 371-396), though this relationship has exceptions and challenges (e.g. Karolis, V., Iuculano, T., & Butterworth, B. (2011). Mapping numerical magnitudes along the right lines: Differentiating between scale and bias. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140(4), 693-706).

      For example, would humans with more lateralised brains show a stronger bias? Would humans with reverse lateralisation show a R-L SNA?

    4. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Functional lateralization between the right and left hemispheres is reported widely in animal taxa, including humans. However, it remains largely speculative as to whether the lateralized brains have a cognitive gain or a sort of fitness advantage. In the present study, by making use of the advantages of domestic chicks as a model, the authors are successful in revealing that the lateralized brain is advantageous in the number sense, in which numerosity is associated with spatial arrangements of items. Behavioral evidence is strong enough to support their arguments. Brain lateralization was manipulated by light exposure during the terminal phase of incubation, and the left-to-right numerical representation appeared when the distance between items gave a reliable spatial cue. The light-exposure induced lateralization, though quite unique in avian species, together with the lack of intense inter-hemispheric direct connections (such as the corpus callosum in the mammalian cerebrum), was critical for the successful analysis in this study. Specification of the responsible neural substrates in the presumed right hemisphere is expected in future research. Comparable experimental manipulation in the mammalian brain must be developed to address this general question (functional significance of brain laterality) is also expected.

      We sincerely appreciate the Reviewer's insightful feedback and his/her recognition of the key contributions of our study.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This is the first study to show how a L-R bias in the relationship between numerical magnitude and space depends on brain lateralisation, and moreover, how is modulated by in ovo conditions.

      Strengths:

      Novel methodology for investigating the innateness and neural basis of an L-R bias in the relationship between number and space.

      We would like to thank the Reviewer for their valuable feedback and for highlighting the key contributions of our study.

      Weaknesses:

      I would query the way the experiment was contextualised. They ask whether culture or innate pre-wiring determines the 'left-to-right orientation of the MNL [mental number line]'.

      We thank the Reviewer for raising this point, which has allowed us to provide a more detailed explanation of this aspect. Rather than framing the left-to-right orientation of the mental number line (MNL) as exclusively determined by either cultural influences or innate pre-wiring, our study highlights the role of environmental stimulation. Specifically, prenatal light exposure can shape hemispheric specialization, which in turn contributes to spatial biases in numerical processing. Please see lines 115-118.

      The term, 'Mental Number Line' is an inference from experimental tasks. One of the first experimental demonstrations of a preference or bias for small numbers in the left of space and larger numbers in the right of space, was more carefully described as the spatial-numerical association of response codes - the SNARC effect (Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and numerical magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 371-396).

      We have refined our description of the MNL and SNARC effect to ensure conceptual accuracy in the revised manuscript; please see lines 53-59.

      This has meant that the background to the study is confusing. First, the authors note, correctly, that many other creatures, including insects, can show this bias, though in none of these has neural lateralisation been shown to be a cause. Second, their clever experiment shows that an experimental manipulation creates the bias. If it were innate and common to other species, the experimental manipulation shouldn't matter. There would always be an L-R bias. Third, they seem to be asserting that humans have a left-to-right (L-R) MNL. This is highly contentious, and in some studies, reading direction affects it, as the original study by Dehaene et al showed; and in others, task affects direction (e.g. Bachtold, D., Baumüller, M., & Brugger, P. (1998). Stimulus-response compatibility in representational space. Neuropsychologia, 36, 731-735, not cited). Moreover, a very careful study of adult humans, found no L-R bias (Karolis, V., Iuculano, T., & Butterworth, B. (2011), not cited, Mapping numerical magnitudes along the right lines: Differentiating between scale and bias. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140(4), 693-706). Indeed, Rugani et al claim, incorrectly, that the L-R bias was first reported by Galton in 1880. There are two errors here: first, Galton was reporting what he called 'visualised numerals', which are typically referred to now as 'number forms' - spontaneous and habitual conscious visual representations - not an inference from a number line task. Second, Galton reported right-to-left, circular, and vertical visualised numerals, and no simple left-to-right examples (Galton, F. (1880). Visualised numerals. Nature, 21, 252-256.). So in fact did Bertillon, J. (1880). De la vision des nombres. La Nature, 378, 196-198, and more recently Seron, X., Pesenti, M., Noël, M.-P., Deloche, G., & Cornet, J.-A. (1992). Images of numbers, or "When 98 is upper left and 6 sky blue". Cognition, 44, 159-196, and Tang, J., Ward, J., & Butterworth, B. (2008). Number forms in the brain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(9), 1547-1556.

      We sincerely appreciate the opportunity to discuss numerical spatialization in greater detail. We have clarified that an innate predisposition to spatialize numerosity does not necessarily exclude the influence of environmental stimulation and experience. We have proposed an integrative perspective, incorporating both cultural and innate factors, suggesting that numerical spatialization originates from neural foundations while remaining flexible and modifiable by experience and contextual influences. Please see lines 69–75.

      We have incorporated the Reviewer’s suggestions and cited all the recommended papers; please see lines 47–75.

      If the authors are committed to chicks' MN Line they should test a series of numbers showing that the bias to the left is greater for 2 and 3 than for 4, etc.

      What does all this mean? I think that the paper should be shorn of its misleading contextualisation, including the term 'Mental Number Line'. The authors also speculate, usefully, on why chicks and other species might have a L-R bias. I don't think the speculations are convincing, but at least if there is an evolutionary basis for the bias, it should at least be discussed.

      In the revised version of the manuscript, we have resorted to adopt the Spatial Numerical Association (SNA). We thank the Reviewer for this valuable comment.

      We appreciated the Reviewer’s suggestion regarding the evolutionary basis of lateralization and have included considerations of its relevance in chicks and other species; please see lines 143-151 and 381-386.

      This paper is very interesting with its focus on why the L-R bias exists, and where and why it does not.

      We wish to thank the Reviewer again for his/her work.

      Reviewer #1(Public review)

      (1) Introduction needs to be edited to make it much more concise and shorter. Hypotheses (from line 67 to 81) and predictions (from line 107 to 124) must be thoroughly rephrased, because (a) general readers are not familiar with the hypotheses (emotional valence and BAFT), (b) the hypotheses may or may not be mutually exclusive, and therefore (c) the logical linkage between the hypotheses and the predicted results are not necessarily clear. Most general readers may be embarrassed by the apparently complicated logical constructs of this study. Instead, it is recommended that focal spotlight should be given to the issue of functional contributions of brain lateralization to the cognitive development of number sense.

      We thank the Reviewer for these comments, which allowed us to improve the clarity of our hypotheses and predictions. We thoroughly rephrased them to ensure they are accessible to general readers and specified that the models may or may not be mutually exclusive. Additionally, we highlighted the functional contributions of brain lateralization to the cognitive development of number sense, addressing the suggested focal point. While we have shortened the introduction, we opted to retain essential background information to ensure readers are well-informed about the relevant scientific literature. Please review the entire introduction, particularly lines 84–118 and 218.

      (2) In relation to the above (a), abbreviations need to be reexamined. MNL (mental number line) appears early on lines 27 and 49, whereas the possibly related conceptual term SNA appeared first on line 213, without specification to "spatial numerical association".

      We thank the Reviewer for bringing this to our attention. We have addressed the suggestions, and the term SNA has been used specifically to refer to numerical spatialization in non-human animals. Please see lines 27-30.

      (3) By the way, what difference is there between MNL and SNA? Please specify the difference if it is important. If not important, is it possible that one of these two is consistently used in this report, at least in the Introduction?

      We clarified the distinction between MNL and SNA and have consistently used SNA in this report; please see lines 47-75.

      (4) In relation to the above (a and b), clarification of the hypotheses and their abbreviations in the form of a table or a graphical representation will strongly reinforce the general readers' understanding. It is also possible that some of these hypotheses are discussed later in the Discussion, rather than in Introduction.

      We appreciated this suggestion and have now clarified the hypotheses, also providing a table/graphical representation, aiming to enhance accessibility for general readers; please see lines 110-118, and 218.

      (5) Figures 1 and 2 are transparent and easily understandable; however, the statistical details in the Results may bother the readers as the main points are doubly represented in Figures 1, 2, and Table 1. These (statistics and Table 1) may go to the supplementary file, if the editor agrees.

      We would prefer to keep Table 1 and the statistical details as part of the main article to provide readers with a comprehensive overview of the experimental results. However, if the editors also suggest to move them to the supplementary file, we are open to making this adjustment.

      (6) In Figure 1D and E, and text lines 139-140. Figure 1D shows that the chick is looking monocularly by the right eye, but the text (line 139) says "left eye in use. Is it correct?

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out this incongruity. We have corrected the text to align with Figure 1D and E; please see lines 180-181.

      (7) Methods. The behavioral experiment was initiated on Wednesday (8 a.m.; line 479), but at what age? At what post-hatch day was the experiment terminated? A simple graphical illustration of the schedule will be quite helpful.

      We have added the requested details, specifying that experiments began on the third post-hatch day and ended on the fifth day; please see lines 533-539.

      Additionally, we have included a graphical illustration of the schedule to enhance clarity; please see line 666.  

      (8) Methods. How many chicks were excluded from the study in the course of Pre-training (line 525) and Training (line 535-536)? Was the exclusion rate high, or just negligible?

      We appreciate the reviewer's suggestion. We have now included the number of subjects excluded during the training phase; please see lines 593-597.

      We wish to thank the Reviewer again for his/her work.

    1. eLife Assessment

      This is a fundamental study that provides a detailed single-cell transcriptomic and epigenomic map of the mouse trabecular meshwor, identifying three distinct trabecular meshwor subtypes with specific functional roles. It links the glaucoma-associated transcription factor LMX1B to mitochondrial regulation in TM3 cells and demonstrates that nicotinamide treatment prevents IOP elevation in Lmx1bV265D/+ mutant mice, highlighting a potential metabolic therapeutic strategy for glaucoma. This convincing work would be further supported by data that link the transcriptional data with mitochondrial functional assays.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study provides a comprehensive single-cell and multiomic characterization of trabecular meshwork (TM) cells in the mouse eye, a structure critical to intraocular pressure (IOP) regulation and glaucoma pathogenesis. Using scRNA-seq, snATAC-seq, immunofluorescence, and in situ hybridization, the authors identify three transcriptionally and spatially distinct TM cell subtypes. The study further demonstrates that mitochondrial dysfunction, specifically in one subtype (TM3), contributes to elevated IOP in a genetic mouse model of glaucoma carrying a mutation in the transcription factor Lmx1b. Importantly, treatment with nicotinamide (vitamin B3), known to support mitochondrial health, prevents IOP elevation in this model. The authors also link their findings to human datasets, suggesting the existence of analogous TM3-like cells with potential relevance to human glaucoma.

      Strengths:

      The study is methodologically rigorous, integrating single-cell transcriptomic and chromatin accessibility profiling with spatial validation and in vivo functional testing. The identification of TM subtypes is consistent across mouse strains and institutions, providing robust evidence of conserved TM cell heterogeneity. The use of a glaucoma model to show subtype-specific vulnerability, combined with a therapeutic intervention-gives the study strong mechanistic and translational significance. The inclusion of chromatin accessibility data adds further depth by implicating active transcription factors such as LMX1B, a gene known to be associated with glaucoma risk. The integration with human single-cell datasets enhances the potential relevance of the findings to human disease.

      Weaknesses:

      Although the LMX1B transcription factor is implicated as a key regulator in TM3 cells, its role in directly controlling mitochondrial gene expression is not fully explored. Additional analysis of motif accessibility or binding enrichment near relevant target genes could substantiate this mechanistic link. The therapeutic effect of vitamin B3 is clearly demonstrated phenotypically, but the underlying cellular and molecular mechanisms remain somewhat underdeveloped - for instance, changes in mitochondrial function, oxidative stress markers, or NAD+ levels are not directly measured. While the human relevance of TM3 cells is suggested through marker overlap, more quantitative approaches, such as cell identity mapping or gene signature scoring in human datasets, would strengthen the translational connection.

      Overall, this is a compelling and carefully executed study that offers significant advances in our understanding of TM cell biology and its role in glaucoma. The integration of multimodal data, disease modeling, and therapeutic testing represents a valuable contribution to the field. With additional mechanistic depth, the study has the potential to become a foundational resource for future research into IOP regulation and glaucoma treatment.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This elegant study by Tolman and colleagues provides fundamental findings that substantially advance our knowledge of the major cell types within the limbus of the mouse eye, focusing on the aqueous humor outflow pathway. The authors used single-cell and single-nuclei RNAseq to very clearly identify 3 subtypes of the trabecular meshwork (TM) cells in the mouse eye, with each subtype having unique markers and proposed functions. The U. Columbia results are strengthened by an independent replication in a different mouse strain at a separate laboratory (Duke). Bioinformatics analyses of these expression data were used to identify cellular compartments, molecular functions, and biological processes. Although there were some common pathways among the 3 subtypes of TM cells (e.g., ECM metabolism), there also were distinct functions. For example:

      • TM1 cell expression supports heavy engagement in ECM metabolism and structure, as well as TGF2 signaling.

      • TM2 cells were enriched in laminin and pathways involved in phagocytosis, lysosomal function, and antigen expression, as well as End3/VEGF/angiopoietin signaling.

      • TM3 cells were enriched in actin binding and mitochondrial metabolism.

      They used high-resolution immunostaining and in situ hybridization to show that these 3 TM subtypes express distinct markers and occupy distinct locations within the TM tissue. The authors compared their expression data with other published scRNAseq studies of the mouse as well as the human aqueous outflow pathway. They used ATAC-seq to map open chromatin regions in order to predict transcription factor binding sites. Their results were also evaluated in the context of human IOP and glaucoma risk alleles from published GWAS data, with interesting and meaningful correlations. Although not discussed in their manuscript, their expression data support other signaling pathways/ proteins/ genes that have been implicated in glaucoma, including: TGF2, BMP signaling (including involvement of ID proteins), MYOC, actin cytoskeleton (CLANs), WNT signaling, etc.

      In addition to these very impressive data, the authors used scRNAseq to examine changes in TM cell gene expression in the mouse glaucoma model of mutant Lmxb1-induced ocular hypertension. In man, LMX1B is associated with Nail-Patella syndrome, which can include the development of glaucoma, demonstrating the clinical relevance of this mouse model. Among the gene expression changes detected, TM3 cells had altered expression of genes associated with mitochondrial metabolism. The authors used their previous experience using nicotinamide to metabolically protect DBA2/J mice from glaucomatous damage, and they hypothesized that nicotinamide supplementation of mutant Lmx1b mice would help restore normal mitochondrial metabolism in the TM and prevent Lmx1b-mediated ocular hypertension. Adding nicotinamide to the drinking water significantly prevented Lmxb1 mutant mice from developing high intraocular pressure. This is a laudable example of dissecting the molecular pathogenic mechanisms responsible for a disease (glaucoma) and then discovering and testing a potential therapy that directly intervenes in the disease process and thereby protects from the disease.

      Strengths:<br /> There are numerous strengths in this comprehensive study including:<br /> • Deep scRNA sequencing that was confirmed by an independent dataset in another mouse strain at another university.<br /> • Identification and validation of molecular markers for each mouse TM cell subset along with localization of these subsets within the mouse aqueous outflow pathway.<br /> • Rigorous bioinformatics analysis of these data as well as comparison of the current data with previously published mouse and human scRNAseq data.<br /> • Correlating their current data with GWAS glaucoma and IOP "hits".<br /> • Discovering gene expression changes in the 3 TM subgroups in the mouse mutant Lmx1b model of glaucoma.<br /> • Further pursuing the indication of dysfunctional mitochondrial metabolism in TM3 cells from Lmx1b mutant mice to test the efficacy of dietary supplementation with nicotinamide. The authors nicely demonstrate the disease modifying efficacy of nicotinamide in preventing IOP elevation in these Lmx1b mutant mice, preventing the development of glaucoma. These results have clinical implications for new glaucoma therapies.

      Weaknesses:<br /> • Occasional over-interpretation of data. The authors have used changes in gene expression (RNAseq) to implicate functions and signaling pathways. For example: they have not directly measured "changes in metabolism", "mitochondrial dysfunction" or "activity of Lmx1b".<br /> • In their very thorough data set, there is enrichment of or changes in gene expression that support other pathways that have been previously reported to be associated with glaucoma (such as TGF2, BMP signaling, actin cytoskeletal organization (CLANs), WNT signaling, ossification, etc. that appears to be a lost opportunity to further enhance the significance of this work.

    4. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:In this study, the authors perform multimodal single-cell transcriptomic and epigenomic profiling of 9,394 mouse TM cells, identifying three transcriptionally distinct TM subtypes with validated molecular signatures. TM1 cells are enriched for extracellular matrix genes, TM2 for secreted ligands supporting Schlemm's canal, and TM3 for contractile and mitochondrial/metabolic functions. The transcription factor LMX1B, previously linked to glaucoma, shows the highest expression in TM3 cells and appears to regulate mitochondrial pathways. In Lmx1bV265D mutant mice, TM3 cells exhibit transcriptional signs of mitochondrial dysfunction associated with elevated IOP. Notably, vitamin B3 treatment significantly mitigates IOP elevation, suggesting a potential therapeutic avenue.

      This is an excellent and collaborative study involving investigators from two institutions, offering the most detailed single-cell transcriptomic and epigenetic profiling of the mouse limbal tissues-including both TM and Schlemm's canal (SC), from wild-type and Lmx1bV265D mutant mice. The study defines three TM subtypes and characterizes their distinct molecular signatures, associated pathways, and transcriptional regulators. The authors also compare their dataset with previously published murine and human studies, including those by Van Zyl et al., providing valuable cross-species insights.

      Strengths:

      (1) Comprehensive dataset with high single-cell resolution<br /> (2) Use of multiple bioinformatic and cross-comparative approaches<br /> (3) Integration of 3D imaging of TM and SC for anatomical context<br /> (4) Convincing identification and validation of three TM subtypes using molecular markers.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) Insufficient evidence linking mitochondrial dysfunction to TM3 cells in Lmx1bV265D mice: While the identification of TM3 cells as metabolically specialized and Lmx1b-enriched is compelling, the proposed link between Lmx1b mutation and mitochondrial dysfunction remains underdeveloped. It is unclear whether mitochondrial defects are a primary consequence of Lmx1b-mediated transcriptional dysregulation or a secondary response to elevated IOP. Additional evidence is needed to clarify whether Lmx1b directly regulates mitochondrial genes (e.g., via ChIP-seq, motif analysis, or ATAC-seq), or whether mitochondrial changes are downstream effects.<br /> Furthermore, the protective effects of nicotinamide (NAM) are interpreted as evidence of mitochondrial involvement, but no direct mitochondrial measurements (e.g., immunostaining, electron microscopy, OCR assays) are provided. It is essential to validate mitochondrial dysfunction in TM3 cells using in vivo functional assays to support the central conclusion of the paper. Without this, the claim that mitochondrial dysfunction drives IOP elevation in Lmx1bV265D mice remains speculative. Alternatively, authors should consider revising their claims that mitochondrial dysfunction in these mice is a central driver of TM dysfunction.

      (2) Mechanism of NAM-mediated protection is unclear: The manuscript states that NAM treatment prevents IOP elevation in Lmx1bV265D mice via metabolic support, yet no data are shown to confirm that NAM specifically rescues mitochondrial function. Do NAM-treated TM3 cells show improved mitochondrial integrity? Are reactive oxygen species (ROS) reduced? Does NAM also protect RGCs from glaucomatous damage? Addressing these points would clarify whether the therapeutic effects of NAM are indeed mitochondrial.

      (3) Lack of direct evidence that LMX1B regulates mitochondrial genes: While transcriptomic and motif accessibility analyses suggest that LMX1B is enriched in TM3 cells and may influence mitochondrial function, no mechanistic data are provided to demonstrate direct regulation of mitochondrial genes. Including ChIP-seq data, motif enrichment at mitochondrial gene loci, or perturbation studies (e.g., Lmx1b knockout or overexpression in TM3 cells) would greatly strengthen this central claim.

      (4)Focus on LMX1B in Fig. 5F lacks broader context: Figure 5F shows that several transcription factors (TFs)-including Tcf21, Foxs1, Arid3b, Myc, Gli2, Patz1, Plag1, Npas2, Nr1h4, and Nfatc2-exhibit stronger positive correlations or motif accessibility changes than LMX1B. Yet the manuscript focuses almost exclusively on LMX1B. The rationale for this focus should be clarified, especially given LMX1B's relatively lower ranking in the correlation analysis. Were the functions of these other highly ranked TFs examined or considered in the context of TM biology or glaucoma? Discussing their potential roles would enhance the interpretation of the transcriptional regulatory landscape and demonstrate the broader relevance of the findings.

      Other weaknesses:

      (1) In abstract, they say a number of 9,394 wild-type TM cell transcriptomes. The number of Lmx1bV265D/+ TM cell transcriptomes analyzed is not provided. This information is essential for evaluating the comparative analysis and should be clearly stated in the Abstract and again in the main text (e.g., lines 121-123). Including both wild-type and mutant cell counts will help readers assess the balance and robustness of the dataset.

      (2) Did the authors monitor mouse weight or other health parameters to assess potential systemic effects of treatment? It is known that the taste of compounds in drinking water can alter fluid or food intake, which may influence general health. Also, does Lmx1bV265D/+ have mice exhibit non-ocular phenotypes, and if so, does nicotinamide confer protection in those tissues as well? Additionally, starting the dose of the nicotinamide at postnatal day 2, how long the mice were treated with water containing nicotinamide, and after how many days or weeks IOP was reduced, and how long the decrease in the IOP was sustained.<br /> (3) While the IOP reduction observed in NAM-treated Lmx1bV265D/+ mice appears statistically significant, it is unclear whether this reflects meaningful biological protection. Several untreated mice exhibit very high IOP values, which may skew the analysis. The authors should report the mean values for IOP in both untreated and NAM-treated groups to clarify the magnitude and variability of the response.<br /> (4) Additionally, since NAM has been shown to protect RGCs in other glaucoma models directly, the authors should assess whether RGCs are preserved in NAM-treated Lmx1b V265D/+ mice. Demonstrating RGC protection would support a synergistic effect of NAM through both IOP reduction and direct neuroprotection, strengthening the translational relevance of the treatment.<br /> (5) Can the authors add any other functional validation studies to explore to understand the pathways enriched in all the subtypes of TM1, TM2, and TM3 cells, in addition to the ICH/IF/RNAscope validation?<br /> (6) The authors should include a representative image of the limbal dissection. While Figure S1 provides a schematic, mouse eyes are very small, and dissecting unfixed limbal tissue is technically challenging. It is also difficult to reconcile the claim that the majority of cells in the limbal region are TM and endothelium. As shown in Figure S6, DAPI staining suggests a much higher abundance of scleral cells compared to TM cells within the limbal strip. Additional clarification or visual evidence would help validate the dissection strategy and cellular composition of the captured region.

    1. Author response:

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The image analysis pipeline is tested in analysing microscopy imaging data of gastruloids of varying sizes, for which an optimised protocol for in toto image acquisition is established based on whole mount sample preparation using an optimal refractive index matched mounting media, opposing dual side imaging with two-photon microscopy for enhanced laser penetration, dual view registration, and weighted fusion for improved in toto sample data representation. For enhanced imaging speed in a two-photon microscope, parallel imaging was used, and the authors performed spectral unmixing analysis to avoid issues of signal cross-talk.

      In the image analysis pipeline, different pre-treatments are done depending on the analysis to be performed (for nuclear segmentation - contrast enhancement and normalisation; for quantitative analysis of gene expression - corrections for optical artifacts inducing signal intensity variations). Stardist3D was used for the nuclear segmentation. The study analyses into properties of gastruloid nuclear density, patterns of cell division, morphology, deformation, and gene expression.

      Strengths:

      The methods developed are sound, well described, and well-validated, using a sample challenging for microscopy, gastruloids. Many of the established methods are very useful (e.g. registration, corrections, signal normalisation, lazy loading bioimage visualisation, spectral decomposition analysis), facilitate the development of quantitative research, and would be of interest to the wider scientific community.

      We thank the reviewer for this positive feedback.

      Weaknesses:

      A recommendation should be added on when or under which conditions to use this pipeline.

      We thank the reviewer for this valuable feedback, which will be addressed in the revision. In general, the pipeline is applicable to any tissue, but it is particularly useful for large and dense 3D samples—such as organoids, embryos, explants, spheroids, or tumors—that are typically composed of multiple cell layers and have a thickness greater than 50 µm.

      The processing and analysis pipeline are compatible with any type of 3D imaging data (e.g. confocal, 2 photon, light-sheet, live or fixed).

      - Spectral unmixing to remove signal cross-talk of multiple fluorescent targets is typically more relevant in two-photon imaging due to the broader excitation spectra of fluorophores compared to single-photon imaging. In confocal or light-sheet microscopy, alternating excitation wavelengths often circumvents the need for unmixing. Spectral decomposition performs even better with true spectral detectors; however, these are usually not non-descanned detectors, which are more appropriate for deep tissue imaging. Our approach demonstrates that simultaneous cross-talk-free four-color two-photon imaging can be achieved in dense 3D specimen with four non-descanned detectors and co-excitation by just two laser lines. Depending on the dispersion in optically dense samples, depth-dependent apparent emission spectra need to be considered.

      - Nuclei segmentation using our trained StarDist3D model is applicable to any system under two conditions: (1) the nuclei exhibit a star-convex shape, as required by the StarDist architecture, and (2) the image resolution is sufficient in XYZ to allow resampling. The exact sampling required is object- and system-dependent, but the goal is to achieve nearly isotropic objects with diameters of approximately 15 pixels while maintaining image quality. In practice, images containing objects that are natively close to or larger than 15 pixels in diameter should segment well after resampling. Conversely, images with objects that are significantly smaller along one or more dimensions will require careful inspection of the segmentation results.

      - Normalization is broadly applicable to multicolor data when at least one channel is expected to be ubiquitously expressed within its domain. Wavelength-dependent correction requires experimental calibration using either an ubiquitous signal at each wavelength. Importantly, this calibration only needs to be performed once for a given set of experimental conditions (e.g., fluorophores, tissue type, mounting medium).

      - Multi-scale analysis of gene expression and morphometrics is applicable to any 3D multicolor image. This includes both the 3D visualization tools (Napari plugins) and the various analytical plots (e.g., correlation plots, radial analysis). Multi-scale analysis can be performed even with imperfect segmentation, as long as segmentation errors tend to cancel out when averaged locally at the relevant spatial scale. However, systematic errors—such as segmentation uncertainty along the Z-axis due to strong anisotropy—may accumulate and introduce bias in downstream analyses. Caution is advised when analyzing hollow structures (e.g., curved epithelial monolayers with large cavities), as the pipeline was developed primarily for 3D bulk tissues, and appropriate masking of cavities would be needed.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study presents an integrated experimental and computational pipeline for high-resolution, quantitative imaging and analysis of gastruloids. The experimental module employs dual-view two-photon spectral imaging combined with optimized clearing and mounting techniques to image whole-mount immunostained gastruloids. This approach enables the acquisition of comprehensive 3D images that capture both tissue-scale and single-cell level information.

      The computational module encompasses both pre-processing of acquired images and downstream analysis, providing quantitative insights into the structural and molecular characteristics of gastruloids. The pre-processing pipeline, tailored for dual-view two-photon microscopy, includes spectral unmixing of fluorescence signals using depth-dependent spectral profiles, as well as image fusion via rigid 3D transformation based on content-based block-matching algorithms. Nuclei segmentation was performed using a custom-trained StarDist3D model, validated against 2D manual annotations, and achieving an F1 score of 85+/-3% at a 50% intersection-over-union (IoU) threshold. Another custom-trained StarDist3D model enabled accurate detection of proliferating cells and the generation of 3D spatial maps of nuclear density and proliferation probability. Moreover, the pipeline facilitates detailed morphometric analysis of cell density and nuclear deformation, revealing pronounced spatial heterogeneities during early gastruloid morphogenesis.

      All computational tools developed in this study are released as open-source, Python-based software.

      Strengths:

      The authors applied two-photon microscopy to whole-mount deep imaging of gastruloids, achieving in toto visualization at single-cell resolution. By combining spectral imaging with an unmixing algorithm, they successfully separated four fluorescent signals, enabling spatial analysis of gene expression patterns.

      The entire computational workflow, from image pre-processing to segmentation with a custom-trained StarDist3D model and subsequent quantitative analysis, is made available as open-source software. In addition, user-friendly interfaces are provided through the open-source, community-driven Napari platform, facilitating interactive exploration and analysis.

      We thank the reviewer for this positive feedback.

      Weaknesses:

      The computational module appears promising. However, the analysis pipeline has not been validated on datasets beyond those generated by the authors, making it difficult to assess its general applicability.

      We agree that applying our analysis pipeline to published datasets—particularly those acquired with different imaging systems—would be valuable. However, only a few high-resolution datasets of large organoid samples are publicly available, and most of these either lack multiple fluorescence channels or represent 3D hollow structures. Our computational pipeline consists of several independent modules: spectral filtering, dual-view registration, local contrast enhancement, 3D nuclei segmentation, image normalization based on a ubiquitous marker, and multiscale analysis of gene expression and morphometrics.

      Spectral filtering has already been applied in other systems (e.g. [7] and [8]), but is here extended to account for imaging depth-dependent apparent emission spectra of the different fluorophores. In our pipeline, we provide code to run spectral filtering on multichannel images, integrated in Python. In order to apply the spectral filtering algorithm utilized here, spectral patterns of each fluorophore need to be calibrated as a function of imaging depth, which depend on the specific emission windows and detector settings of the microscope.

      Image normalization using a wavelength-dependent correction also requires calibration on a given imaging setup to measure the difference in signal decay among the different fluorophores species. To our knowledge, the calibration procedures for spectral-filtering and our image-normalization approach have not been performed previously in 3D samples, which is why validation on published datasets is not readily possible. Nevertheless, they are described in detail in the Methods section, and the code used—from the calibration measurements to the corrected images—is available open-source at the Zenodo link in the manuscript.

      Dual-view registration, local contrast enhancement, and multiscale analysis of gene expression and morphometrics are not limited to organoid data or our specific imaging modalities. If we identify suitable datasets to validate these modules, we will include them in the revised manuscript.

      To evaluate our 3D nuclei segmentation model, we plan to test it on diverse systems, including gastruloids stained with the nuclear marker Draq5 from Moos et al. [1]; breast cancer spheroids; primary ductal adenocarcinoma organoids; human colon organoids and HCT116 monolayers from Ong et al. [2]; and zebrafish tissues imaged by confocal microscopy from Li et al [3]. These datasets were acquired using either light-sheet or confocal microscopy, with varying imaging parameters (e.g., objective lens, pixel size, staining method).

      Preliminary results are promising (see Author response image 1). We will provide quantitative comparisons of our model’s performance on these datasets, using annotations or reference predictions provided by the original authors where available.

      Author response image 1.

      Qualitative comparison of our custom Stardist3D segmentation strategy on diverse published 3D nuclei datasets. We show one slice from the XY plane for simplicity. (a) Gastruloid stained with the nuclear marker DRAQ5 imaged with an open-top dual-view and dual-illumination LSM [1]. (b) Breast cancer spheroid [2]. (c) Primary pancreatic ductal adenocarcinoma organoids imaged with confocal microscopy[2]. (d) Human colon organoid imaged with LSM laser scanning confocal microscope [2]. (e) Monolayer HCT116 cells imaged with LSM laser scanning confocal microscope [2]. (f) Fixed zebrafish embryo stained for nuclei and imaged with a Zeiss LSM 880 confocal microscopy [3].

      Besides, the nuclei segmentation component lacks benchmarking against existing methods.

      We agree with the reviewer that a benchmark against existing segmentation methods would be very useful. We tried different pre-trained models:

      - CellPose, which we tested in a previous paper ([4]) and which showed poor performances compared to our trained StarDist3D model.

      - DeepStar3D ([2]) is only available in the software 3DCellScope. We could not benchmark the model on our data, because the free and accessible version of the software is limited to small datasets. An image of a single whole-mount gastruloid with one channel, having dimensions (347,467,477) was too large to be processed, see screenshot below. The segmentation model could not be extracted from the source code and tested externally because the trained DeepStar3D weights are encrypted.

      Author response image 2.

      Screenshot of the 3DCellScore software. We could not perform 3D nuclei segmentation of a whole-mount gastruloids because the image size was too large to be processed.

      - AnyStar ([5]), which is a model trained from the StarDist3D architecture, was not performing well on our data because of the heterogeneous stainings. Basic pre-processing such as median and gaussian filtering did not improve the results and led to wrong segmentation of touching nuclei. AnyStar was demonstrated to segment well colon organoids in Ong et al, 2025 ([2]), but the nuclei were more homogeneously stained. Our Hoechst staining displays bright chromatin spots that are incorrectly labeled as individual nuclei.

      - Cellos ([6]), another model trained from StarDist3D, was also not performing well. The objects used for training and to validate the results are sparse and not touching, so the predicted segmentation has a lot of false negatives even when lowering the probability threshold to detect more objects. Additionally, the network was trained with an anisotropy of (9,1,1), based on images with low z resolution, so it performed poorly on almost isotropic images. Adapting our images to the network’s anisotropy results in an imprecise segmentation that can not be used to measure 3D nuclei deformations.

      We tried both Cellos and AnyStar predictions on a gastruloid image from Fig. S2 of our main manuscript. Author response image 3 displays the results qualitatively compared to our trained model Stardist-tapenade. For the revision of the paper, we will perform a comprehensive benchmark of these state-of-the-art routines, including quantitative assessment of the performance.

      Author response image 3.

      Qualitative comparison of two published segmentation models versus our model. We show one slice from the XY plane for simplicity. Segmentations are displayed with their contours only. (Top left) Gastruloid stained with Hoechst, image extracted from Fig S2 of our manuscript. (Top right) Same image overlayed with the prediction from the Cellos model, showing many false negatives. (Bottom left) Same image overlayed with the prediction from our Stardist-tapenade model. (Bottom right) Same image overlayed with the prediction from the AnyStar model, false positives are indicated with a red arrow.

      Appraisal:

      The authors set out to establish a quantitative imaging and analysis pipeline for gastruloids using dual-view two-photon microscopy, spectral unmixing, and a custom computational framework for 3D segmentation and gene expression analysis. This aim is largely achieved. The integration of experimental and computational modules enables high-resolution in toto imaging and robust quantitative analysis at the single-cell level. The data presented support the authors' conclusions regarding the ability to capture spatial patterns of gene expression and cellular morphology across developmental stages.

      Impact and utility:

      This work presents a compelling and broadly applicable methodological advance. The approach is particularly impactful for the developmental biology community, as it allows researchers to extract quantitative information from high-resolution images to better understand morphogenetic processes. The data are publicly available on Zenodo, and the software is released on GitHub, making them highly valuable resources for the community.

      We thank the reviewer for these positive feedbacks.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary

      The paper presents an imaging and analysis pipeline for whole-mount gastruloid imaging with two-photon microscopy. The presented pipeline includes spectral unmixing, registration, segmentation, and a wavelength-dependent intensity normalization step, followed by quantitative analysis of spatial gene expression patterns and nuclear morphometry on a tissue level. The utility of the approach is demonstrated by several experimental findings, such as establishing spatial correlations between local nuclear deformation and tissue density changes, as well as the radial distribution pattern of mesoderm markers. The pipeline is distributed as a Python package, notebooks, and multiple napari plugins.

      Strengths

      The paper is well-written with detailed methodological descriptions, which I think would make it a valuable reference for researchers performing similar volumetric tissue imaging experiments (gastruloids/organoids). The pipeline itself addresses many practical challenges, including resolution loss within tissue, registration of large volumes, nuclear segmentation, and intensity normalization. Especially the intensity decay measurements and wavelength-dependent intensity normalization approach using nuclear (Hoechst) signal as reference are very interesting and should be applicable to other imaging contexts. The morphometric analysis is equally well done, with the correlation between nuclear shape deformation and tissue density changes being an interesting finding. The paper is quite thorough in its technical description of the methods (which are a lot), and their experimental validation is appropriate. Finally, the provided code and napari plugins seem to be well done (I installed a selected list of the plugins and they ran without issues) and should be very helpful for the community.

      We thank the reviewer for his positive feedback and appreciation of our work.

      Weaknesses

      I don't see any major weaknesses, and I would only have two issues that I think should be addressed in a revision:

      (1) The demonstration notebooks lack accompanying sample datasets, preventing users from running them immediately and limiting the pipeline's accessibility. I would suggest to include (selective) demo data set that can be used to run the notebooks (e.g. for spectral unmixing) and or provide easily accessible demo input sample data for the napari plugins (I saw that there is some sample data for the processing plugin, so this maybe could already be used for the notebooks?).

      We thank the reviewer for this relevant suggestion. The 7 notebooks were updated to automatically download sample tests. The different parts of the pipeline can now be run immediately: https://github.com/GuignardLab/tapenade/tree/chekcs_on_notebooks/src/tapenade/notebooks

      (2) The results for the morphometric analysis (Figure 4) seem to be only shown in lateral (xy) views without the corresponding axial (z) views. I would suggest adding this to the figure and showing the density/strain/angle distributions for those axial views as well.

      We agree with the reviewer that a morphometric analysis based on the axial views would be informative and plan to perform this analysis for the revision.

      (1) Moos, F., Suppinger, S., de Medeiros, G., Oost, K.C., Boni, A., Rémy, C., Weevers, S.L., Tsiairis, C., Strnad, P. and Liberali, P., 2024. Open-top multisample dual-view light-sheet microscope for live imaging of large multicellular systems. Nature Methods, 21(5), pp.798-803.

      (2) Ong, H.T., Karatas, E., Poquillon, T., Grenci, G., Furlan, A., Dilasser, F., Mohamad Raffi, S.B., Blanc, D., Drimaracci, E., Mikec, D. and Galisot, G., 2025. Digitalized organoids: integrated pipeline for high-speed 3D analysis of organoid structures using multilevel segmentation and cellular topology. Nature Methods, 22(6), pp.1343-1354.

      (3) Li, L., Wu, L., Chen, A., Delp, E.J. and Umulis, D.M., 2023. 3D nuclei segmentation for multi-cellular quantification of zebrafish embryos using NISNet3D. Electronic Imaging, 35, pp.1-9.

      (4) Vanaret, J., Dupuis, V., Lenne, P. F., Richard, F., Tlili, S., & Roudot, P. (2023). A detector-independent quality score for cell segmentation without ground truth in 3D live fluorescence microscopy. IEEE Journal of Selected Topics in Quantum Electronics, 29(4: Biophotonics), 1-12.

      (5) Dey, N., Abulnaga, M., Billot, B., Turk, E. A., Grant, E., Dalca, A. V., & Golland, P. (2024). AnyStar: Domain randomized universal star-convex 3D instance segmentation. In Proceedings of the IEEE/CVF Winter Conference on Applications of Computer Vision (pp. 7593-7603).

      (6) Mukashyaka, P., Kumar, P., Mellert, D. J., Nicholas, S., Noorbakhsh, J., Brugiolo, M., ... & Chuang, J. H. (2023). High-throughput deconvolution of 3D organoid dynamics at cellular resolution for cancer pharmacology with Cellos. Nature Communications, 14(1), 8406.

      (7) Rakhymzhan, A., Leben, R., Zimmermann, H., Günther, R., Mex, P., Reismann, D., ... & Niesner, R. A. (2017). Synergistic strategy for multicolor two-photon microscopy: application to the analysis of germinal center reactions in vivo. Scientific reports, 7(1), 7101.

      (8) Dunsing, V., Petrich, A., & Chiantia, S. (2021). Multicolor fluorescence fluctuation spectroscopy in living cells via spectral detection. Elife, 10, e69687.

    2. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary

      The paper presents an imaging and analysis pipeline for whole-mount gastruloid imaging with two-photon microscopy. The presented pipeline includes spectral unmixing, registration, segmentation, and a wavelength-dependent intensity normalization step, followed by quantitative analysis of spatial gene expression patterns and nuclear morphometry on a tissue level. The utility of the approach is demonstrated by several experimental findings, such as establishing spatial correlations between local nuclear deformation and tissue density changes, as well as the radial distribution pattern of mesoderm markers. The pipeline is distributed as a Python package, notebooks, and multiple napari plugins.

      Strengths

      The paper is well-written with detailed methodological descriptions, which I think would make it a valuable reference for researchers performing similar volumetric tissue imaging experiments (gastruloids/organoids). The pipeline itself addresses many practical challenges, including resolution loss within tissue, registration of large volumes, nuclear segmentation, and intensity normalization. Especially the intensity decay measurements and wavelength-dependent intensity normalization approach using nuclear (Hoechst) signal as reference are very interesting and should be applicable to other imaging contexts. The morphometric analysis is equally well done, with the correlation between nuclear shape deformation and tissue density changes being an interesting finding. The paper is quite thorough in its technical description of the methods (which are a lot), and their experimental validation is appropriate. Finally, the provided code and napari plugins seem to be well done (I installed a selected list of the plugins and they ran without issues) and should be very helpful for the community.

      Weaknesses

      I don't see any major weaknesses, and I would only have two issues that I think should be addressed in a revision:

      (1) The demonstration notebooks lack accompanying sample datasets, preventing users from running them immediately and limiting the pipeline's accessibility. I would suggest to include (selective) demo data set that can be used to run the notebooks (e.g. for spectral unmixing) and or provide easily accessible demo input sample data for the napari plugins (I saw that there is some sample data for the processing plugin, so this maybe could already be used for the notebooks?).

      (2) The results for the morphometric analysis (Figure 4) seem to be only shown in lateral (xy) views without the corresponding axial (z) views. I would suggest adding this to the figure and showing the density/strain/angle distributions for those axial views as well.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study presents an integrated experimental and computational pipeline for high-resolution, quantitative imaging and analysis of gastruloids. The experimental module employs dual-view two-photon spectral imaging combined with optimized clearing and mounting techniques to image whole-mount immunostained gastruloids. This approach enables the acquisition of comprehensive 3D images that capture both tissue-scale and single-cell level information.

      The computational module encompasses both pre-processing of acquired images and downstream analysis, providing quantitative insights into the structural and molecular characteristics of gastruloids. The pre-processing pipeline, tailored for dual-view two-photon microscopy, includes spectral unmixing of fluorescence signals using depth-dependent spectral profiles, as well as image fusion via rigid 3D transformation based on content-based block-matching algorithms. Nuclei segmentation was performed using a custom-trained StarDist3D model, validated against 2D manual annotations, and achieving an F1 score of 85+/-3% at a 50% intersection-over-union (IoU) threshold. Another custom-trained StarDist3D model enabled accurate detection of proliferating cells and the generation of 3D spatial maps of nuclear density and proliferation probability. Moreover, the pipeline facilitates detailed morphometric analysis of cell density and nuclear deformation, revealing pronounced spatial heterogeneities during early gastruloid morphogenesis.

      All computational tools developed in this study are released as open-source, Python-based software.

      Strengths:

      The authors applied two-photon microscopy to whole-mount deep imaging of gastruloids, achieving in toto visualization at single-cell resolution. By combining spectral imaging with an unmixing algorithm, they successfully separated four fluorescent signals, enabling spatial analysis of gene expression patterns.

      The entire computational workflow, from image pre-processing to segmentation with a custom-trained StarDist3D model and subsequent quantitative analysis, is made available as open-source software. In addition, user-friendly interfaces are provided through the open-source, community-driven Napari platform, facilitating interactive exploration and analysis.

      Weaknesses:

      The computational module appears promising. However, the analysis pipeline has not been validated on datasets beyond those generated by the authors, making it difficult to assess its general applicability.<br /> Besides, the nuclei segmentation component lacks benchmarking against existing methods.

      Appraisal:

      The authors set out to establish a quantitative imaging and analysis pipeline for gastruloids using dual-view two-photon microscopy, spectral unmixing, and a custom computational framework for 3D segmentation and gene expression analysis. This aim is largely achieved. The integration of experimental and computational modules enables high-resolution in toto imaging and robust quantitative analysis at the single-cell level. The data presented support the authors' conclusions regarding the ability to capture spatial patterns of gene expression and cellular morphology across developmental stages.

      Impact and utility:

      This work presents a compelling and broadly applicable methodological advance. The approach is particularly impactful for the developmental biology community, as it allows researchers to extract quantitative information from high-resolution images to better understand morphogenetic processes. The data are publicly available on Zenodo, and the software is released on GitHub, making them highly valuable resources for the community.

    4. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The image analysis pipeline is tested in analysing microscopy imaging data of gastruloids of varying sizes, for which an optimised protocol for in toto image acquisition is established based on whole mount sample preparation using an optimal refractive index matched mounting media, opposing dual side imaging with two-photon microscopy for enhanced laser penetration, dual view registration, and weighted fusion for improved in toto sample data representation. For enhanced imaging speed in a two-photon microscope, parallel imaging was used, and the authors performed spectral unmixing analysis to avoid issues of signal cross-talk.

      In the image analysis pipeline, different pre-treatments are done depending on the analysis to be performed (for nuclear segmentation - contrast enhancement and normalisation; for quantitative analysis of gene expression - corrections for optical artifacts inducing signal intensity variations). Stardist3D was used for the nuclear segmentation. The study analyses into properties of gastruloid nuclear density, patterns of cell division, morphology, deformation, and gene expression.

      Strengths:

      The methods developed are sound, well described, and well-validated, using a sample challenging for microscopy, gastruloids. Many of the established methods are very useful (e.g. registration, corrections, signal normalisation, lazy loading bioimage visualisation, spectral decomposition analysis), facilitate the development of quantitative research, and would be of interest to the wider scientific community.

      Weaknesses:

      A recommendation should be added on when or under which conditions to use this pipeline.

    5. eLife Assessment

      This important study introduces a powerful imaging approach that enables deep-tissue visualization in gastruloids using two-photon microscopy, combined with spectral imaging and unmixing to achieve four-color 3D image acquisition. The evidence is compelling that many of the established methods are very helpful (e.g., registration, corrections, signal normalisation, lazy loading bioimage visualisation, spectral decomposition analysis), facilitate the development of quantitative research, and would be of interest to the wider scientific community.

    1. eLife Assessment

      The findings of this important study substantially advance our understanding of the transcription factors that can induce hair cell-like cells from human pluripotent stem cells. The presented evidence supporting these findings is compelling, including rigorous characterization of the effects of hair cell induction using both single-cell RNA sequencing and electrophysiological assessments.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, Rainey et al investigated the effects of transcription factors, ATOH1, POU4F3, GFI1, and SIX1 on the induction of hair cells from human pluripotent stem cells. The authors used a doxycycline-inducible system to control transgene expression and demonstrated significant improvement in the efficiency of MYO7A+ hair cell differentiation compared to the retrovirus-mediated approach. Next, they characterized differentiated cells using single-cell RNA-seq and identified a population of hair cell-like cells with gene expression profiles similar to the fetal human vestibular hair cells. Finally, they revealed the electrophysiological properties of induced cells consistent with those of mechanosensitive hair cells.

      Strengths:

      A key finding in this study is the rapid induction of cells expressing multiple hair cell markers that takes place within 21 days after overexpression of the four transcription factors. Additionally, the authors demonstrate that doxycycline-mediated gene overexpression outperforms retroviral-mediated gene transfer in terms of both the efficiency and reproducibility of hair cell induction. Furthermore, the authors demonstrate that these induced hair cells can be used to study hair cell protection from cisplatin ototoxicity.

      Weaknesses:

      The authors conclude that the induced cells lack distinct hair cell subtypes. However, the characterization of generated hair cells in single-cell RNA-seq data is insufficient. Additional vestibular or cochlear hair cell-enriched marker gene and protein expression should be analyzed. Moreover, the morphological features and mechanotransduction channel activity of the induced hair cells have not been analyzed.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The study employs a specific set of transcription factors to promote lineage conversion of pluripotent stem cells into fetal hair cells. In pluripotent stem cells, an inducible expression system containing SIX1, ATOH1, POU4F3, and GFI1 (SAPG) was inserted into a safe harbor site. The stable cell line allows for doxycycline-inducible expression of transcription factors to generate induced hair cells (iHCs). These changes were observed in gene expression and electrophysiological properties. Comparing the transcriptome with iHCs derived from fibroblasts or primary human inner ear tissue suggested that it is similar to human hair cells. Although the iHCs did not have hair bundles - a key morphological feature of hair cells - the cellular system has immense potential for the field. The defined transcription factors allow for the dissection of gene regulatory networks and provide a molecular handle for the lineage conversion process. The results also suggest that the pluripotent stem cells were not directly converted into iHCs. Instead, there are several transitional cell states. These observations indicate that lineage conversion may still be hampered by yet undefined molecular obstacles and may help identify and overcome these in future work. The stable cell line allows for repeatable and large-scale screening studies, which is not feasible using primary human cells.

      Strengths:

      The cellular system is well-designed, with clearly described expression of the defined factors. Transient expression of the exogenous transcription factors SIX1, ATOH1, POU4F3, and GFI1 (SAPG) upon doxycycline induction is well-documented. Increased expression of endogenous SAPG factors suggests activation of self-regulatory feedback pathways during conversion. The stable iPS cell line provides a tool for the field to study lineage conversion or generate large numbers of iHCs.

      Single-nuclear RNA-seq distinguishes distinct cell clusters and cellular transition states, validating the system's utility. A comparison of previously published data from iHCs and human fetal hair cells also suggested that iHCs are similar to developing human hair cells at the transcriptome level. Whole-cell patch clamp recordings show the generation of excitable cells with heterogeneous ion channel properties, which suggests a change in the cell type.

      Weaknesses:

      The interpretation of the snRNA-seq results could be strengthened by explaining the three distinct clusters for uninduced cells and how they transition into the iHC trajectory.

      Although the analysis focuses on the cell cluster that represents iHCs (R5), a short discussion on what clusters R1-R4 (Figure 3B) represent would be useful. These cells do not express high levels of the SAPG factors even after 21 days of continuous doxycycline induction and may provide insight into hurdles that hamper lineage conversion.

      RNA velocity analysis on single-nuclear RNA-seq is impressive but requires clarification on inferring the pseudotime trajectory. Some rationale and explanation on how the ratio of unspliced to spliced mRNA in the nucleus can be used to infer the differentiation trajectory would strengthen the discussion.

    4. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, Robert N. Rainey et. al. reported a new approach to induce hair cell-like cells from a human induced pluripotent stem cell line. Based on the previously identified key transcription factors SIX1, ATOH1, POU4F3, and GFI1 (SAPG), which are essential for the conversion into induced hair cell-like cells in mice. The manuscript represents an advance over the authors' previous published work, which used the same transcription factors but viral gene delivery.

      Strengths:

      The manuscript is clear and well-written. The background is easy to follow for people outside of the field. The data are well-organized and well-described. The evidence is strong.

      Weaknesses:

      General comments:

      (1) The manuscript generated multiple valuable datasets for the field. However, the data are not deposited in the hearing field central resource for gene expression (umgear.org), and links are not provided in the figure legends to datasets or dataset collections in the gEAR. This is a major comment as it significantly decreases the utility of the datasets generated in the manuscript and decreases the ease of reuse of the data. This is a flaw that could be easily addressed by uploading the data and generating links to datasets in the body of the manuscript.

      (2) If a pulse of Dox induces the SAPG and starts the conversion process, it is not clear why the analyzed cells were treated for 21 days - a duration that can negatively affect the fate of converting hair cells.

      (3) Foxj1 is listed as a supporting cell-specific gene; however, it is expressed in the cochlear hair cells until the end of the first postnatal week.

      (4) It is not clear why cells were sorted for analysis of the retrovirally induced cells but not in the stable cell line, which also expressed tdTomato.

      (5) Figure 1D and Supplementary Figure 2: the authors state that the endogenous ATOH1 and POU4F3 expressions decrease after 7d. Should the authors have stats on the graphs?

      (6) Supplementary Figure 4: OCT4 should be replaced by POU5F1 (or vice versa) for consistency.

      (7) The authors show the induction or decrease of the exogenous transcription factor expressions by RT-qPCR. It would be nice, if possible, to also see either WB or immuno with antibodies directed against the tags.

      Bioinformatic comments:

      (1) In the previous study (Menendez et al. 2020), ATAC-seq and regulatory elements are employed in the analysis, while a similar analysis is missing in this study. It will be informative to show the motif enrichment analysis at promoter regions of differentially expressed genes (DEGs) in the most hair cell-like cluster 3 (RV-R3).

      (2) In the previous study (Menendez et al. 2020), it was stated that SAPG can convert supporting cells to hair cells, while in this study, the authors stated that "reprogramming with SAPG does not activate supporting cell networks in the stable cell line". Can the authors provide more analysis/comments on this difference?

      (3) The approach in this study tends to generate a very similar level of expression for the SAPG factors, while the real levels of expression might be different for actual transcriptional regulation, eg, Figure 1C. How will this very close expression level of SAPG affect the features of the induced hair cell?

      (4) Figure 5B, missing color bar to show the DEG strength in the heatmap. Why are Six1 and Gfi1 not shown in this heatmap?

    1. eLife Assessment

      This important study examines the relationship between cognition and mental health and investigates how brain, genetics, and environmental measures mediate that relationship. The methods and results are compelling and well-executed. Overall, this study will be of interest in the field of population neuroscience and in studies of mental health.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This work integrates two timepoints from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development Study to understand how neuroimaging, genetic and environmental data contribute to the predictive power of mental health variables in predicting cognition in a large early adolescent sample. Their multimodal and multivariate prediction framework involves a novel opportunistic stacking model to handle complex types of information to predict variables that are important in understanding mental health-cognitive performance associations.

      Strengths:

      The authors are commended for incorporating and directly comparing the contribution of multiple imaging modalities (task fMRI, resting state fMRI, diffusion MRI, structural MRI), neurodevelopmental markers, environmental factors and polygenic risk scores in a novel multivariate framework (via opportunistic stacking), as well as interpreting mental health-cognition associations with latent factors derived from Partial Least Squares. The authors also use a large well-characterized and diverse cohort of adolescents from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study. The paper is also strengthened by commonality analyses to understand the shared and unique contribution of different categories of factors (e.g., neuroimaging vs mental health vs polygenic scores vs sociodemographic and adverse developmental events) in explaining variance in cognitive performance

      Weaknesses:

      The paper is framed with an over-reliance on the RDoC framework in the introduction, despite deviations from the RDoC framework in the methods. The field is also learning more about RDoC's limitations when mapping cognitive performance to biology. The authors also focus on a single general factor of cognition as the core outcome of interest as opposed to different domains of cognition. The authors could consider predicting mental health rather than cognition. Using mental health as a predictor could be limited by the included 9-11 year age range at baseline (where mental health concerns are likely to be low or not well captured), as well as the nature of how the data was collected, i.e., either by self-report or from parent/caregiver report.

      Comments on revisions:

      The authors have done an excellent job of addressing my comments. I have no other suggestions to add. Great work!

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This paper by Wang et al. uses rich brain, behaviour, and genetics data from the ABCD cohort to ask how well cognitive abilities can be predicted from mental health related measures, and how brain and genetics influence that prediction. They obtain an out of sample correlation of 0.4, with neuroimaging (in particular task fMRI) proving the key mediator. Polygenic scores contributed less.

      Strengths:

      This paper is characterized by the intelligent use of a superb sample (ABCD) alongside strong statistical learning methods and a clear set of questions. The outcome - the moderate level of prediction between brain, cognition, genetics and mental health - is interesting, and particularly important is the dissection of which features best mediate that prediction and how developmental and lifestyle factors play a role.

      Weaknesses:

      There are relatively few weaknesses to this paper. It has already undergone review at a different journal, and the authors clearly took the original set of comments into account in revising their paper. Overall, while the ABCD sample is superb for the questions asked, it would have been highly informative to extend the analyses to datasets containing more participants with neurological/psychiatric diagnoses (e.g. HBN, POND) or extending it into adolescent/early adult onset psychopathology cohorts. But it is fair enough that the authors want to leave that for future work.

    4. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews: 

      Reviewer #1 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      This work integrates two timepoints from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study to understand how neuroimaging, genetic, and environmental data contribute to the predictive power of mental health variables in predicting cognition in a large early adolescent sample. Their multimodal and multivariate prediction framework involves a novel opportunistic stacking model to handle complex types of information to predict variables that are important in understanding mental health-cognitive performance associations. 

      Strengths: 

      The authors are commended for incorporating and directly comparing the contribution of multiple imaging modalities (task fMRI, resting state fMRI, diffusion MRI, structural MRI), neurodevelopmental markers, environmental factors, and polygenic risk scores in a novel multivariate framework (via opportunistic stacking), as well as interpreting mental health-cognition associations with latent factors derived from partial least squares. The authors also use a large well-characterized and diverse cohort of adolescents from the ABCD Study. The paper is also strengthened by commonality analyses to understand the shared and unique contribution of different categories of factors (e.g., neuroimaging vs mental health vs polygenic scores vs sociodemographic and adverse developmental events) in explaining variance in cognitive performance 

      Weaknesses: 

      The paper is framed with an over-reliance on the RDoC framework in the introduction, despite deviations from the RDoC framework in the methods. The field is also learning more about RDoC's limitations when mapping cognitive performance to biology. The authors also focus on a single general factor of cognition as the core outcome of interest as opposed to different domains of cognition. The authors could consider predicting mental health rather than cognition. Using mental health as a predictor could be limited by the included 9-11 year age range at baseline (where many mental health concerns are likely to be low or not well captured), as well as the nature of how the data was collected, i.e., either by self-report or from parent/caregiver report. 

      Thank you so much for your encouragement.

      We appreciate your comments on the strengths of our manuscript.

      Regarding the weaknesses, the reliance on the RDoC framework is by design. Even with its limitations, following RDoC allows us to investigate mental health holistically. In our case, RDoC enabled us to focus on a) a functional domain (i.e., cognitive ability), b) the biological units of analysis of this functional domain (i.e., neuroimaging and polygenic scores), c) potential contribution of environments, and d) the continuous individual deviation in this domain (as opposed to distinct categories). We are unaware of any framework with all these four features.

      Focusing on modelling biological units of analysis of a functional domain, as opposed to mental health per se, has some empirical support from the literature. For instance, in Marek and colleagues’ (2022) study, as mentioned by a previous reviewer, fMRI is shown to have a more robust prediction for cognitive ability than mental health. Accordingly, our reasons for predicting cognitive ability instead of mental health in this study are motivated theoretically (i.e., through RDoC) and empirically (i.e., through fMRI findings). We have clarified this reason in the introduction of the manuscript.

      We are aware of the debates surrounding the actual structure of functional domains where the originally proposed RDoC’s specific constructs might not fit the data as well as the data-driven approach (Beam et al., 2021; Quah et al., 2025). However, we consider this debate as an attempt to improve the characterisation of functional domains of RDoC, not an effort to invalidate its holistic, neurobiological and basicfunctioning approach. Our use of a latent-variable modelling approach through factor analyses moves towards a data-driven direction. We made the changes to the second-to-last paragraph in the introduction to make this point clear:

      “In this study, inspired by RDoC, we a) focused on cognitive abilities as a functional domain, b) created predictive models to capture the continuous individual variation (as opposed to distinct categories) in cognitive abilities, c) computed two neurobiological units of analysis of cognitive abilities: multimodal neuroimaging and PGS, and d) investigated the potential contributions of environmental factors. To operationalise cognitive abilities, we estimated a latent variable representing behavioural performance across various cognitive tasks, commonly referred to as general cognitive ability or the gfactor (Deary, 2012). The g-factor was computed from various cognitive tasks pertinent to RDoC constructs, including attention, working memory, declarative memory, language, and cognitive control. However, using the g-factor to operationalise cognitive abilities caused this study to diverge from the original conceptualisation of RDoC, which emphasises studying separate constructs within cognitive abilities (Morris et al., 2022; Morris & Cuthbert, 2012). Recent studies suggest an improvement to the structure of functional domains by including a general factor, such as the g-factor, in the model, rather than treating each construct separately (Beam et al., 2021; Quah et al., 2025). The g-factor in children is also longitudinally stable and can forecast future health outcomes (Calvin et al., 2017; Deary et al., 2013). Notably, our previous research found that neuroimaging predicts the g-factor more accurately than predicting performance from separate individual cognitive tasks (Pat et al., 2023). Accordingly, we decided to conduct predictive models on the g-factor while keeping the RDoC’s holistic, neurobiological, and basic-functioning characteristics.”

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary: 

      This paper by Wang et al. uses rich brain, behaviour, and genetics data from the ABCD cohort to ask how well cognitive abilities can be predicted from mental-health-related measures, and how brain and genetics influence that prediction. They obtain an out-ofsample correlation of 0.4, with neuroimaging (in particular task fMRI) proving the key mediator. Polygenic scores contributed less. 

      Strengths: 

      This paper is characterized by the intelligent use of a superb sample (ABCD) alongside strong statistical learning methods and a clear set of questions. The outcome - the moderate level of prediction between the brain, cognition, genetics, and mental health - is interesting. Particularly important is the dissection of which features best mediate that prediction and how developmental and lifestyle factors play a role. 

      Thank you so much for the encouragement. 

      Weaknesses: 

      There are relatively few weaknesses to this paper. It has already undergone review at a different journal, and the authors clearly took the original set of comments into account in revising their paper. Overall, while the ABCD sample is superb for the questions asked, it would have been highly informative to extend the analyses to datasets containing more participants with neurological/psychiatric diagnoses (e.g. HBN, POND) or extend it into adolescent/early adult onset psychopathology cohorts. But it is fair enough that the authors want to leave that for future work. 

      Thank you very much for providing this valuable comment and for your flexibility.

      For the current manuscript, we have drawn inspiration from the RDoC framework, which emphasises the variation from normal to abnormal in normative samples (Morris et al., 2022). The ABCD samples align well with this framework.

      We hope to extend this framework to include participants with neurological and psychiatric diagnoses in the future. We have begun applying neurobiological units of analysis for cognitive abilities, assessed through multimodal neuroimaging and polygenic scores (PGS), to other datasets containing more participants with neurological and psychiatric diagnoses. However, this is beyond the scope of the current manuscript. We have listed this as one of the limitations in the discussion section:

      “Similarly, our ABCD samples were young and community-based, likely limiting the severity of their psychopathological issues (Kessler et al., 2007). Future work needs to test if the results found here are generalisable to adults and participants with stronger severity.”

      In terms of more practical concerns, much of the paper relies on comparing r or R2 measures between different tests. These are always presented as point estimates without uncertainty. There would be some value, I think, in incorporating uncertainty from repeated sampling to better understand the improvements/differences between the reported correlations. 

      This is a good suggestion. We have now included bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals in all of our scatter plots, showing the uncertainty of predictive performance.

      The focus on mental health in a largely normative sample leads to the predictions being largely based on the normal range. It would be interesting to subsample the data and ask how well the extremes are predicted. 

      We appreciate this comment. Similar to our response to Reviewer 2’s Weakness #1, our approach has drawn inspiration from the RDoC framework, which emphasises the variation from normal to abnormal in normative samples (Morris et al., 2022). Subsampling the data would make us deviate from our original motivation. 

      Moreover, we used 17 mental healh variables in our predictive models: 8 CBCL subscales, 4 BIS/BAS subscales and 5 UPSS subscales. It is difficult to subsample them. Perhaps a better approach is to test the applicability of our neurobiological units of analysis for cognitive abilities (multimodal neuroimaging and PGS) in other datasets that include more extreme samples. We are working on this line of studies at the moment, and hope to show that in our future work. 

      Reviewer 2’s Weakness #4

      A minor query - why are only cortical features shown in Figure 3? 

      We presented both cortical and subcortical features in Figure 3. The cortical features are shown on the surface space, while the subcortical features are displayed on the coronal plane. Below is an example of these cortical and subcortical features from the ENBack contrast. The subcortical features are presented in the far-right coronal image.

      We separated the presentation of cortical and subcortical features because the ABCD uses the CIFTI format (https://www.humanconnectome.org/software/workbenchcommand/-cifti-help). CIFTI-format images combine cortical surface (in vertices) with subcortical volume (in voxels). For task fMRI, the ABCD parcellated cortical vertices using Freesurfer’s Destrieux atlas and subcortical voxels using Freesurfer’s automatically segmented brain volume (ASEG).

      Due to the size of the images in Figure 3, it may have been difficult for Reviewer 2 to see the subcortical features clearly. We have now added zoomed-in versions of this figure as Supplementary Figures 4–13.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the autors):

      (1) In the abstract, could the authors mention which imaging modalities contribute most to the prediction of cognitive abilities (e.g., working memory-related task fMRI)? 

      Thank you for the suggestion. Following this advice, we now mention which imaging modalities led to the highest predictive performance. Please see the abstract below.

      “Cognitive abilities are often linked to mental health across various disorders, a pattern observed even in childhood. However, the extent to which this relationship is represented by different neurobiological units of analysis, such as multimodal neuroimaging and polygenic scores (PGS), remains unclear. 

      Using large-scale data from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study, we first quantified the relationship between cognitive abilities and mental health by applying multivariate models to predict cognitive abilities from mental health in children aged 9-10, finding an out-of-sample r\=.36 . We then applied similar multivariate models to predict cognitive abilities from multimodal neuroimaging, polygenic scores (PGS) and environmental factors. Multimodal neuroimaging was based on 45 types of brain MRI (e.g., task fMRI contrasts, resting-state fMRI, structural MRI, and diffusion tensor imaging). Among these MRI types, the fMRI contrast, 2-Back vs. 0-Back, from the ENBack task provided the highest predictive performance (r\=.4). Combining information across all 45 types of brain MRI led to the predictive performance of r\=.54. The PGS, based on previous genome-wide association studies on cognitive abilities, achieved a predictive performance of r\=.25. Environmental factors, including socio-demographics (e.g., parent’s income and education), lifestyles (e.g., extracurricular activities, sleep) and developmental adverse events (e.g., parental use of alcohol/tobacco, pregnancy complications), led to a predictive performance of r\=.49. 

      In a series of separate commonality analyses, we found that the relationship between cognitive abilities and mental health was primarily represented by multimodal neuroimaging (66%) and, to a lesser extent, by PGS (21%). Additionally, environmental factors accounted for 63% of the variance in the relationship between cognitive abilities and mental health. The multimodal neuroimaging and PGS then explained 58% and 21% of the variance due to environmental factors, respectively. Notably, these patterns remained stable over two years. 

      Our findings underscore the significance of neurobiological units of analysis for cognitive abilities, as measured by multimodal neuroimaging and PGS, in understanding both a) the relationship between cognitive abilities and mental health and b) the variance in this relationship shared with environmental factors.”

      (2) Could the authors clarify what they mean by "completing the transdiagnostic aetiology of mental health" in the introduction? (Second paragraph). 

      Thank you. 

      We intended to convey that understanding the transdiagnostic aetiology of mental health would be enhanced by knowing how neurobiological units of cognitive abilities, from the brain to genes, capture variations due to environmental factors. We realise this sentence might be confusing. Removing it does not alter the intended meaning of the paragraph, as we clarified this point later. The paragraph now reads:

      “According to the National Institute of Mental Health’s Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) framework (Insel et al., 2010), cognitive abilities should be investigated not only behaviourally but also neurobiologically, from the brain to genes. It remains unclear to what extent the relationship between cognitive abilities and mental health is represented in part by different neurobiological units of analysis -- such as neural and genetic levels measured by multimodal neuroimaging and polygenic scores (PGS). To fully comprehend the role of neurobiology in the relationship between cognitive abilities and mental health, we must also consider how these neurobiological units capture variations due to environmental factors, such as sociodemographics, lifestyles, and childhood developmental adverse events (Morris et al., 2022). Our study investigated the extent to which a) environmental factors explain the relationship between cognitive abilities and mental health, and b) cognitive abilities at the neural and genetic levels capture these associations due to environmental factors. Specifically, we conducted these investigations in a large normative group of children from the ABCD study (Casey et al., 2018). We chose to examine children because, while their emotional and behavioural problems might not meet full diagnostic criteria (Kessler et al., 2007), issues at a young age often forecast adult psychopathology (Reef et al., 2010; Roza et al., 2003). Moreover, the associations among different emotional and behavioural problems in children reflect transdiagnostic dimensions of psychopathology (Michelini et al., 2019; Pat et al., 2022), making children an appropriate population to study the transdiagnostic aetiology of mental health, especially within a framework that emphasises normative variation from normal to abnormal, such as the RDoC (Morris et al., 2022).“

      (3) It is unclear to me what the authors mean by this statement in the introduction: "Note that using the word 'proxy measure' does not necessarily mean that the predictive model for a particular measure has a high predictive performance - some proxy measures have better predictive performance than others". 

      We added this sentence to address a previous reviewer’s comment: “The authors use the phrasing throughout 'proxy measures of cognitive abilities' when they discuss PRS, neuroimaging, sociodemographics/lifestyle, and developmental factors. Indeed, the authors are able to explain a large proportion of variance with different combinations of these measures, but I think it may be a leap to call all of these proxy measures of cognition. I would suggest keeping the language more objective and stating these measures are associated with cognition.” 

      Because of this comment, we assumed that the reviewers wanted us to avoid the misinterpretation that a proxy measure implies high predictive performance. This term is used in machine learning literature (for instance, Dadi et al., 2021). We added the aforementioned sentence to ensure readers that using the term 'proxy measure' does not necessarily mean that the predictive model for a particular measure has high predictive performance. However, it seems that our intention led to an even more confusing message. Therefore, we decided to delete that sentence but keep an earlier sentence that explains the meaning of a proxy measure (see below).

      “With opportunistic stacking, we created a ‘proxy’ measure of cognitive abilities (i.e., predicted value from the model) at the neural unit of analysis using multimodal neuroimaging.”

      (4) Overall, despite comments from reviewers at another journal, I think the authors still refer to RDoC more than needed in the intro given the restructuring of the manuscript. For instance, at the end of page 4 and top of page 5, it becomes a bit confusing when the authors mention how they deviated from the RDoC framework, but their choice of cognitive domains is still motivated by RDoC. I think the chosen cognitive constructs are consistent with what is in ABCD and what other studies have incorporated into the g factor and do not require the authors to further justify their choice through RDoC. Also, there is emerging work showing that RDoC is limited in its ability to parse apart meaningful neuroimaging-based patterns; see for instance, Quah et al., Nature 2025 (https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-025-55831-z). 

      Thank you very much for your comment. We have addressed it in our Response to Reviewer 1’s summary, strengths, and weaknesses above. We have rewritten the paragraph to clarify the relevance of our work to the RDoC framework and to recent studies aiming to improve RDoC constructs (including that from Quah and colleagues).

      (5) I am still on the fence about the use of 'proxy measures of cognitive abilities' given that it is defined as the predictive performance of mental health measures in predicting cognition - what about just calling these mental health predictors? Also, it would be easier to follow this train of thought throughout the manuscript. But I leave it to the authors if they decide to keep their current language of 'proxy measure of cognition'. 

      Thank you so much for your flexibility. As we explained previously, this ‘proxy measures’ term is used in machine learning literature (for instance, Dadi et al., 2021). We thought about other terms, such as “score”, which is used in genetics, i.e., polygenic scores (Choi et al., 2020). and has recently been used in neuroimaging, i.e., neuroscore (Rodrigue et al., 2024). However, using a ‘score’ is a bit awkward for mental health and socio-demographics, lifestyle and developmental adverse events. Accordingly, we decided to keep the term ‘proxy measures’.

      (6) It is unclear which cognitive abilities are being predicted in Figure 1, given the various domains that authors describe in their intro. Is it the g-factor from CFA? This should be clarified in all figure captions. 

      Yes, cognitive abilities are operationalised using a second-order latent variable, the g-factor from a CFA. We now added the following sentence to Figure 1, 2, 4 to make this point clearer. Thank you for the suggestion:

      “Cognitive abilities are based on the second-order latent variable, the g-factor, based on a confirmatory factor analysis of six cognitive tasks.”

      (7) I think it may also be worthwhile to showcase the explanatory power cognitive abilities have in predicting mental health or at least comment on this in the discussion. Certainly, there may be a bidirectional relationship here. The prediction direction from cognition to mental health may be an altogether different objective than what the paper currently presents, but many researchers working in psychiatry may take the stance (with support from the literature) that cognitive performance may serve as premorbid markers for later mental health concerns, particularly given the age range that the authors are working with in ABCD. 

      Thank you for this comment. 

      It is important to note that we do not make a directional claim in these cross-sectional analyses. The term "prediction" is used in a machine learning sense, implying only that we made an out-of-sample prediction (Yarkoni & Westfall, 2017). Specifically, we built predictive models on some samples (i.e., training participants) and applied our models to test participants who were not part of the model-building process. Accordingly, our predictive models cannot determine whether mental health “causes” cognitive abilities or vice versa, regardless of whether we treat mental health or cognitive abilities as feature/explanatory/independent variables or as target/response/outcome variables in the models. To demonstrate directionality, we would need to conduct a longitudinal analysis with many more repeated samples and use appropriate techniques, such as a cross-lagged panel model. It is beyond the scope of this manuscript and will need future releases of the ABCD data.

      We decided to use cognitive abilities as a target variable here, rather than a feature variable, mainly for theoretical reasons. This work was inspired by the RDoC framework, which emphasises functional domains. Cognitive abilities is the functional domain in the current study. We created predictive models to predict cognitive abilities based on a) mental health, b) multimodal neuroimaging, c) polygenic scores, and d) environmental factors. We could not treat cognitive abilities as a functional domain if we used them as a feature variable. For instance, if we predicted mental health (instead of cognitive abilities) from multimodal neuroimaging and polygenic scores, we would no longer capture the neurobiological units of analysis for cognitive abilities.

      We now made it clearer in the discussion that our use of predictive models cannot provide the directional of the effects

      “Our predictive modelling revealed a medium-sized predictive relationship between cognitive abilities and mental health. This finding aligns with recent meta-analyses of case-control studies that link cognitive abilities and mental disorders across various psychiatric conditions (Abramovitch et al., 2021; East-Richard et al., 2020). Unlike previous studies, we estimated the predictive, out-of-sample relationship between cognitive abilities and mental disorders in a large normative sample of children. Although our predictive models, like other cross-sectional models, cannot determine the directionality of the effects, the strength of the relationship between cognitive abilities and mental health estimated here should be more robust than when calculated using the same sample as the model itself, known as in-sample prediction/association (Marek et al., 2022; Yarkoni & Westfall, 2017). Examining the PLS loadings of our predictive models revealed that the relationship was driven by various aspects of mental health, including thought and externalising symptoms, as well as motivation. This suggests that there are multiple pathways—encompassing a broad range of emotional and behavioural problems and temperaments—through which cognitive abilities and mental health are linked.”

      (8) There is a lot of information packed into Figure 3 in the brain maps; I understand the authors wanted to fit this onto one page, and perhaps a higher resolution figure would resolve this, but the brain maps are very hard to read and/or compare, particularly the coronal sections. 

      Thank you for this suggestion. We agree with Reviewer 1 that we need to have a better visualisation of the feature-importance brain maps. To ensure that readers can clearly see the feature importance, we added a Zoom-in version of the feature-importance brain maps as Supplementary Figures 4 – 13.

      (9) It would be helpful for authors to cluster features in the resting state functional connectivity correlation matrices, and perhaps use shorter names/acronyms for the labels. 

      Thank you for this suggestion. 

      We have now added a zoomed-in version of the feature importance for rs-fmri as Supplementary Figure 7 (for baseline) and 12 (for follow-up).

      (10) Figures 4a) and 4b): please elaborate on "developmental adverse" in the title. I am assuming this is referring to childhood adverse events, or "developmental adversities". 

      Thank you so much for pointing this out. We meant ‘developmental adverse events’. We have made changes to this figure in the current manuscript.

      (11) For the "follow-up" analyses, I would recommend the authors present this using only the features that are indeed available at follow-up, even if the list of features is lower, otherwise it becomes a bit confusing with the mix of baseline and follow-up features. Or perhaps the authors could make this more clear in the figures by perhaps having a different color for baseline vs follow-up features along the y-axis labels. 

      Thank you for this advice. We have now added an indicator in the plot to show whether the features were collected in the baseline or follow-up. We also added colours to indicate which type of environmental factors they were. It is now clear that the majority of the features that were collected at baseline, but were used for the followup predictive model, were developmental adverse events.

      (12) Minor: Makowski et al 2023 reference can be updated to Makowski et al 2024, published in Cerebral Cortex. 

      Thank you for pointing this out. We have updated the citation accordingly. 

      References

      Abramovitch, A., Short, T., & Schweiger, A. (2021). The C Factor: Cognitive dysfunction as a transdiagnostic dimension in psychopathology. Clinical Psychology Review, 86, 102007. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2021.102007

      Beam, E., Potts, C., Poldrack, R. A., & Etkin, A. (2021). A data-driven framework for mapping domains of human neurobiology. Nature Neuroscience, 24(12), 1733–1744. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-021-00948-9

      Calvin, C. M., Batty, G. D., Der, G., Brett, C. E., Taylor, A., Pattie, A., Čukić, I., & Deary, I. J. (2017). Childhood intelligence in relation to major causes of death in 68 year follow-up: Prospective population study. BMJ, j2708. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.j2708

      Casey, B. J., Cannonier, T., Conley, M. I., Cohen, A. O., Barch, D. M., Heitzeg, M. M., Soules, M. E., Teslovich, T., Dellarco, D. V., Garavan, H., Orr, C. A., Wager, T. D., Banich, M. T., Speer, N. K., Sutherland, M. T., Riedel, M. C., Dick, A. S., Bjork, J. M., Thomas, K. M., … ABCD Imaging Acquisition Workgroup. (2018). The Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study: Imaging acquisition across 21 sites. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 32, 43–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2018.03.001

      Choi, S. W., Mak, T. S.-H., & O’Reilly, P. F. (2020). Tutorial: A guide to performing polygenic risk score analyses. Nature Protocols, 15(9), Article 9. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41596-020-0353-1

      Dadi, K., Varoquaux, G., Houenou, J., Bzdok, D., Thirion, B., & Engemann, D. (2021). Population modeling with machine learning can enhance measures of mental health. GigaScience, 10(10), giab071. https://doi.org/10.1093/gigascience/giab071

      Deary, I. J. (2012). Intelligence. Annual Review of Psychology, 63(1), 453–482. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100353

      Deary, I. J., Pattie, A., & Starr, J. M. (2013). The Stability of Intelligence From Age 11 to Age 90 Years: The Lothian Birth Cohort of 1921. Psychological Science, 24(12), 2361–2368. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613486487

      East-Richard, C., R. -Mercier, A., Nadeau, D., & Cellard, C. (2020). Transdiagnostic neurocognitive deficits in psychiatry: A review of meta-analyses. Canadian Psychology / Psychologie Canadienne, 61(3), 190–214. https://doi.org/10.1037/cap0000196

      Insel, T., Cuthbert, B., Garvey, M., Heinssen, R., Pine, D. S., Quinn, K., Sanislow, C., & Wang, P. (2010). Research Domain Criteria (RDoC): Toward a New Classification Framework for Research on Mental Disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(7), 748–751. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379

      Kessler, R. C., Amminger, G. P., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Alonso, J., Lee, S., & Üstün, T. B. (2007). Age of onset of mental disorders: A review of recent literature. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 20(4). https://journals.lww.com/co-psychiatry/fulltext/2007/07000/age_of_onset_of_mental_disorders_a_review_of .10.aspx

      Marek, S., Tervo-Clemmens, B., Calabro, F. J., Montez, D. F., Kay, B. P., Hatoum, A. S., Donohue, M. R., Foran, W., Miller, R. L., Hendrickson, T. J., Malone, S. M., Kandala, S., Feczko, E., Miranda-Dominguez, O., Graham, A. M., Earl, E. A., Perrone, A. J., Cordova, M., Doyle, O., … Dosenbach, N. U. F. (2022). eproducible brain-wide association studies require thousands of individuals. Nature, 603(7902), 654–660. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-022-04492-9

      Michelini, G., Barch, D. M., Tian, Y., Watson, D., Klein, D. N., & Kotov, R. (2019). Delineating and validating higher-order dimensions of psychopathology in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study. Translational Psychiatry, 9(1), 261. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-019-0593-4

      Morris, S. E., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2012). Research Domain Criteria: Cognitive systems, neural circuits, and dimensions of behavior. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 14(1), 29–37.

      Morris, S. E., Sanislow, C. A., Pacheco, J., Vaidyanathan, U., Gordon, J. A., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2022). Revisiting the seven pillars of RDoC. BMC Medicine, 20(1), 220. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12916-022-02414-0

      Pat, N., Riglin, L., Anney, R., Wang, Y., Barch, D. M., Thapar, A., & Stringaris, A. (2022). Motivation and Cognitive Abilities as Mediators Between Polygenic Scores and Psychopathology in Children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 61(6), 782-795.e3. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2021.08.019

      Pat, N., Wang, Y., Bartonicek, A., Candia, J., & Stringaris, A. (2023). Explainable machine learning approach to predict and explain the relationship between task-based fMRI and individual differences in cognition. Cerebral Cortex, 33(6), 2682–2703. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhac235

      Quah, S. K. L., Jo, B., Geniesse, C., Uddin, L. Q., Mumford, J. A., Barch, D. M., Fair, D. A., Gotlib, I. H., Poldrack, R. A., & Saggar, M. (2025). A data-driven latent variable approach to validating the research domain criteria framework. Nature Communications, 16(1), 830. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-025-55831-z

      Reef, J., Diamantopoulou, S., van Meurs, I., Verhulst, F., & van der Ende, J. (2010). Predicting adult emotional and behavioral problems from externalizing problem trajectories in a 24-year longitudinal study. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 19(7), 577–585. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-010-0088-6

      Rodrigue, A. L., Hayes, R. A., Waite, E., Corcoran, M., Glahn, D. C., & Jalbrzikowski, M. (2024). Multimodal Neuroimaging Summary Scores as Neurobiological Markers of Psychosis. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 50(4), 792–803. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbad149

      Roza, S. J., Hofstra, M. B., Van Der Ende, J., & Verhulst, F. C. (2003). Stable Prediction of Mood and Anxiety Disorders Based on Behavioral and Emotional Problems in Childhood: A 14-Year Follow-Up During Childhood, Adolescence, and Young Adulthood. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(12), 2116–2121. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.160.12.2116

      Yarkoni, T., & Westfall, J. (2017). Choosing Prediction Over Explanation in Psychology: Lessons From Machine Learning. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(6), 1100–1122. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617693393

    1. eLife Assessment

      This important manuscript introduces a genetic tool utilizing mutant mitfa-Cas9 expressing zebrafish to knockout genes to analyze melanocyte function in development and tumorigenesis. The data are convincing and the authors cover potential caveats from their model that might impact its utility for future work. This work significantly adds to the existing approaches in the field, as the mitfa:Cas9 strategy taken here provides a roadmap for generating similar platforms for using other tissue-specific regulators and Cas proteins in the future.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Perlee et al. sought to generate a zebrafish line where CRISPR-based gene editing is exclusively limited to the melanocyte lineage, allowing assessment of cell-type restricted gene knockouts. To achieve this, they knocked in Cas9 to the endogenous mitfa locus, as mitfa is a master regulator of melanocyte development. The authors use multiple candidate genes - albino, sox10, tuba1a, ptena/ptenb, tp53 - to demonstrate that their system induces lineage-restricted gene editing. This method allows researchers to bypass embryonic lethal and non-cell autonomous phenotypes emerging from whole body knockout (sox10, tuba1a), drive directed phenotypes, such as depigmentation (albino), and induce lineage-specific tumors, such as melanomas (ptena/ptenb, tp53, when accompanied with expression of BRAFV600E). The main weakness of the manuscript is that the mechanistic explanations proposed to underlie the presented phenotypes are minimally interrogated, but nonetheless interesting and motivating for future experimentation. Overall, there is a clear use for this genetic methodology, and its implementation will be of value to many in vivo researchers.

      Strengths:

      The strongest component of this manuscript is the genetic control offered by the mitfa:Cas9 system and the ability to make stable, lineage-specific knockouts in zebrafish. This is exemplified by the studies of tuba1a, where the authors nicely show non-cell autonomous mechanisms have obfuscated the role of this gene in melanocyte development. In addition, the mitfa:Cas9 system is elegantly straightforward and can be easily implemented in many labs. Mostly, the figures are clean, controls are appropriate, and phenotypes are reproducible. The invented method is a welcome addition to the arsenal of genetic tools used in zebrafish. The authors kindly and honestly responded to reviewer criticism, which has led to an improved manuscript and a pleasant review process.

      Weaknesses:

      The authors argue that the benefit of their system is the maintenance of endogenous regulatory elements. However, no direct comparison is made with other tools that offer similar genetic control, such as MAZERATI. This is a missed opportunity to provide researchers the ability to evaluate these two similar genetic approaches. There is a slight concern that tumor onset with this system is hindered by the heterozygous state it imparts to the lineage master regulator (here, mitfa). The authors do a good job at addressing these issues in the Discussion, but experimentation would have been appreciated. Additionally, the authors claim 86% of mitfa+ cells express Cas9. The image shown in Figure 1C does not do a convincing job at showing this percentage.

      Another weakness of the manuscript regards minimally investigated mechanistic explanations for each biological vignette. Detailed mechanistic information is indeed out-of-scope for this manuscript, which intends to prove the efficacy of a genetic tool. Readers are cautioned to use the mechanistic insights from these vignettes as inspiration rather than bona fide truth.

      The authors performed the necessary experiments to address each of the reviewers' concerns and thereby quell any substantial issues raised during the first review. They have additionally edited their language appropriately to make their claims more accurate. Their efforts during the review process are appreciated.

      Conclusion:

      The authors were highly receptive to reviewer comments and improved their manuscript from the first submission. The authors were successful in their goal of creating a rapid genetic approach to study cell-type specific genetic insults in vivo. They have presented multiple interesting and convincing stories to support the power of their invented methodology. The refined mechanisms underlying their observed phenotypes may be lacking but this does not take away from the methodological benefit this manuscript provides to the large field of in vivo researchers.

    3. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Perlee et al. present a method for generating cell-type restricted knockouts in zebrafish, focusing on melanocytes. For this method, the authors knock-in a Cas9 encoding sequence into the mitfa locus. This mitfaCas9 line has restricted Cas9 expression, allowing the authors to generate melanocyte-specific knockouts rapidly by follow-up injection of sgRNA expressing transposon vectors.

      The paper presents some interesting vignettes to illustrate the utility of their approach. These include 1) a derivation of albino mutant fish as a demonstration of the method's efficiency, 2) an interrogation and novel description of tuba1a/tuba1c as a potential non-autonomous contributor to melanosome dispersion, and 3) the generation of sox10 deficient melanoma tumors that show "escape" of sox10 loss through upregulation of sox9. The latter two examples highlight the usefulness of cell-type targeted knockouts (Body-wide sox10 and tuba1a loss elicit developmental defects). Additionally, the tumor models involve highly multiplexed sgRNAs for tumor initiation which is nicely facilitated by the stable Cas9.

      Strengths:

      The approach is clever and could prove very useful for studying melanocytes and other cell types. As the authors hint at in their discussion, this approach would become even more powerful with the generation of other Cas9-restricted lineages so a single sgRNA construct can be screened across many lineages rapidly (or many sgRNA and fish lines screened combinatorially).

      The biological findings used to demonstrate the power of the approach are interesting in their own right. The non-autonomous effect of tuba1a/tuba1c loss on melanosome dispersion are striking and demonstrates very nicely how one could use Perlee et al.'s approach to search for similar mechanisms systematically. The dual targeting nature of the tuba1a/tuba1c sgRNA also suggests similar approaches might be explored for knocking out paralogs. The observation of the sox9 escape mechanism with sox10 loss is a beautiful demonstration of the relevance of SOX10/SOX9's reciprocal regulation in vivo. This system would be a very nice model for further interrogating mechanisms/interventions surrounding Sox10 in melanoma.

      Finally, the figure presentation is very nice. This work involves complex genetic approaches, including multiple fish generations and multiplexed construct injections. The vector diagrams and breeding schemes in the paper make everything very clear/"grok-able," and the paper was enjoyable to read.

      Weaknesses:

      The authors' claims are grounded and tested rigorously. The major weaknesses that we raised in the first round of reviews were either addressed experimentally or are now detailed as limitations in the text. Congrats on the beautiful paper!

    4. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Perlee et al. sought to generate a zebrafish line where CRISPR-based gene editing is exclusively limited to the melanocyte lineage, allowing assessment of cell-type restricted gene knockouts. To achieve this, they knocked in Cas9 to the endogenous mitfa locus, as mitfa is a master regulator of melanocyte development. The authors use multiple candidate genes - albino, sox10, tuba1a, ptena/ptenb, tp53 - to demonstrate their system induces lineagerestricted gene editing. This method allows researchers to bypass embryonic lethal and non-cell autonomous phenotypes emerging from whole body knockout (sox10, tuba1a), drive directed phenotypes, such as depigmentation (albino), and induce lineage-specific tumors, such as melanomas (ptena/ptenb, tp53, when accompanied with expression of BRAFV600E). While the genetic approaches are solid, the argued increase in efficiency of this model compared to current tools was untested, and therefore unable to be assessed. Furthermore, the mechanistic explanations proposed to underlie their phenotypes are mostly unfounded, as discussed further in the Weaknesses section. Despite these concerns, there is still a clear use for this genetic methodology and its implementation will be of value to many in vivo researchers.

      Strengths:

      The strongest component of this manuscript is the genetic control offered by the mitfa:Cas9 system and the ability to make stable, lineage-specific knockouts in zebrafish. This is exemplified by the studies of tuba1a, where the authors nicely show non-cell autonomous mechanisms have obfuscated the role of this gene in melanocyte development. In addition, the mitfa:Cas9 system is elegantly straightforward and can be easily implemented in many labs. Mostly, the figures are clean, controls are appropriate, and phenotypes are reproducible. The invented method is a welcomed addition to the arsenal of genetic tools used in zebrafish.

      Weaknesses:

      The major weaknesses of the manuscript include the overly bold descriptions of the value of the model and the superficial mechanistic explanations for each biological vignette.

      The authors argue that a major advantage of this system is its high efficiency. However, no direct comparison is made with other tools that achieve the same genetic control, such as MAZERATI. This is a missed opportunity to provide researchers the ability to evaluate these two similar genetic approaches. In addition, Fig.1 shows that not all melanocytes express Cas9. This is a major caveat that goes unaddressed. It is of paramount importance to understand the percentage of mitfa+ cells that express Cas9. The histology shown is unclear and too zoomed out of a scale to make any insightful conclusions, especially in Fig.S1. It would also be beneficial to see data regarding Cas9 expression in adult melanocytes, which are distinct from embryonic melanocytes in zebrafish. Moreover, this system still requires the injection of a plasmid encoding gRNAs of interest, which will yield mosaicism. A prime example of this discrepancy is in Fig.6, where sox10 is clearly still present in "sox10 KO" tumors.

      We agree with these points. While our method has the advantage of endogenous knockin (thus keeping all regulatory elements), you are correct that we did not make a direct comparison with existing technologies like MAZERATI, and therefore we cannot make comparative claims about efficiency. Based on this, we have revised the manuscript to remove these points, reduce the strength/boldness of the claims, and make it more clear what our system achieves in comparison to existing systems. In reference to the other specific points you raise above about mosaicism and extent of Cas9 expression:

      - We have added a paragraph to address the advantages and disadvantages of mitfaCas9 compared to expression of Cas9 with lineage-specific promoters including MAZERATI in the discussion.  

      - Figure 1C has been revised to more clearly show the overlap of mitfa and Cas9 in melanocytes. 

      - We then quantified the percentage of mitfa+ cells expressing Cas9 from the in situ hybridizations (Supplemental Figure S1D). We did attempt to look at Cas9 protein expression in both embryonic and adult melanocytes by immunofluorescence. Unfortunately, the Cas9 antibodies commercially available did not work on the zebrafish embryos or adult tailfins, so we are limited in proper quantification to the in situs in the embryos.

      The authors argue that their model allows rapid manipulation of melanocyte gene expression. Enthusiasm for the speed of this model is diminished by minimal phenotypes in the F0, as exemplified in Fig.2. Although the authors say >90% of fish have loss of pigmentation, this is misleading as the phenotype is a very weak, partial loss. Only in the F1 generation do robust phenotypes emerge, which takes >6 months to generate. How this is more efficient than other tools that currently exist is unclear and should be discussed in more detail.

      This needed clarification, and we have now modified the Discussion to reflect this more accurately. What we were trying to show is that both F0 and F1 fish can be useful in screening for the effect of any given gene. In the F0, while you are correct that the phenotype is indeed weak/partial, it is also quantifiable and therefore can be used as a rapid screen for potential effects of knockout, so it can help with speed. The major advantage of the F1 generation is that we can generate fully penetrant phenotypes for recessive genes since the fish just needs to have 1 copy of the Cas9/sgRNA instead of 2. This means we do not have to go to F2 or F3 generations, which really does save time. But we agree this could be achieved using MAZERATI, and so we have added these considerations to the manuscript, as we feel these are important.

      In Figure 3, the authors find that melanocyte-specific knockout of sox10 leads to only a 25% reduction in melanocytes in the F1 generation. This is in contradiction to prior literature cited describing sox10 as indispensable for melanocyte development. In addition, the authors argue that sox10 is required for melanocyte regeneration. This claim is not accurate, as >50% of melanocytes killed upon neocuproine treatment can regenerate. This data would indicate that sox10 is required for only a subset of melanocytes to develop (Fig.3C) and for only a subset to regenerate (Fig.3G). This is an interesting finding that is not discussed or interrogated further.

      We too were initially very puzzled by this result. We do not completely understand it, but we have two thoughts about it. First could be timing. sox10 usually starts to be expressed around the 1-somite stage, and so in the original sox10/colourless mutant (which truly has no melanocytes), sox10 will be lost during those early stages. In contrast, mitf comes on later (around 18hpf) so this might indicate that there is a subset of melanocytes that are dependent upon this early expression of sox10. This may indicate that there could be different functions of sox10 early in melanocyte development versus later timepoints after melanocytes have already been specified. This might also help explain our findings during regeneration.  Second could be genetic compensation. Since in the other parts of the paper we seem to see a somewhat reciprocal relationship between sox10 and sox9, it is conceivable that loss of sox10 in the melanocytes could be compensated for by sox9 (or even other genes) in our CRISPR approach (as opposed to the ENU allele in colourless). Since we really do not fully understand this, we have added a section to the Discussion about this issue, mentioning these possibilities but leaving open other yet to be defined mechanisms.

      Tumor induction by this model is weak, as indicated by the tumor curves in Figs.5,6. This might be because these fish are mitfa heterozygous. Whereas the avoidance of mitfa overexpression driven by other models including MAZERATI is a benefit of this system, the effect of mitfa heterozygosity on tumor incidence was untested. This is an essential question unaddressed in the manuscript.

      We agree that in the BRAF;p53 group especially tumor incidence is very low, although PTEN loss does accelerate it. One possibility is exactly as you stated, and that mitfa heterozygosity is the etiology. The other possibility is that in the MAZERATI approach (https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/30385465/) the authors used the casper background as opposed to the wild-type T5D as we did in our study. In unpublished observations, we have found that casper (with miniCoopR rescue) is markedly more sensitive to melanoma induction compared to WT fish in this setting. In fact, in looking at our BRAF;p53 curves compared to the original Patton paper curves (https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/15694309/) which were also done in a WT background with no miniCoopR, they are fairly similar. This might indicate that casper + miniCoopR particularly sensitizes the fish to melanoma. However, because we do not fully know the reasons for this, we have now included both of these possible reasons in the Discussion.

      In Fig.6, the authors recapitulate previous findings with their model, showing sox10 KO inhibits tumor onset. The tumors that do develop are argued to be highly invasive, have mesenchymal morphology, and undergo phenotypic switching from sox10 to sox9 expression. The data presented do not sufficiently support these claims. The histology is not readily suggestive of invasive, mesenchymal melanomas. Sox10 is still present in many cells and sox9 expression is only found in a small subset (<20%). Whether sox10-null cells are the ones expressing sox9 is untested. If sox9-mediated phenotypic switching is the major driver of these tumors, the authors would need to knockout sox9 and sox10 simultaneously and test whether these "rare" types of tumors still emerge. Additional histological and genetic evaluation is required to make the conclusions presented in Fig.6. It feels like a missed opportunity that the authors did not attempt to study genes of unknown contribution to melanoma with their system.

      We did not mean to overstate the admittedly early observations from these fish. Invasiveness in the fish models can be difficult to precisely quantify, and therefore is somewhat qualitative. While we did not mean to imply that every cell that loses sox10 will become sox9 positive (which is clearly not the case), the human single-cell RNA-seq data does suggest these are somewhat mutually exclusive populations (https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/32753671/). This phenomenon has also long been observed even prior to single-cell approaches (https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/25629959/). So while we agree our data is not definitive in this regard, it is consistent with the literature and was presented mainly to provide areas for future exploration with the model. 

      Overall, this manuscript introduces a solid method to the arsenal of zebrafish genetic tools but falls short of justifying itself as a more efficient and robust approach than what currently exists. The mechanisms provided to explain observed phenotypes are tenuous. Nonetheless, the mitfa:Cas9 approach will certainly be of value to many in vivo biologists and lays the foundation to generate similar methods using other tissue-specific regulators and other Cas proteins.

      We hope that by toning down the language around what we have observed, and providing as honest an assessment as possible as to what might be occurring, that the manuscript will be helpful for future studies aiming to knock out genes in the melanocyte lineage.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript describes a genetic tool utilizing mutant mitfa-Cas9 expressing zebrafish to knockout genes to analyze their function in melanocytes in a range of assays from developmental biology to tumorigenesis. Overall, the data are convincing and the authors cover potential caveats from their model that might impact its utility for future work.

      Strengths:

      The authors do an excellent job of characterizing several gene deletions that show the specificity and applicability of the genetic mitfa-Cas9 zebrafish to studying melanocytes.

      Weaknesses:

      Variability across animals not fully analyzed.

      To more clearly show variability across animals, we calculated the percentage of mitfa+ cells that express Cas9 across n=7 mitfaCas9 embryos. We also expanded Supplemental Figure 2 to show loss of pigmentation across n=7 individual adult MG-albino F2 fish instead of one representative image.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Perlee et al. present a method for generating cell-type restricted knockouts in zebrafish, focusing on melanocytes. For this method, the authors knock-in a Cas9 encoding sequence into the mitfa locus. This mitfaCas9 line has restricted Cas9 expression, allowing the authors to generate melanocyte-specific knockouts rapidly by follow-up injection of sgRNA expressing transposon vectors.

      The paper presents some interesting vignettes to illustrate the utility of their approach. These include 1) a derivation of albino mutant fish as a demonstration of the method's efficiency, 2) an interrogation and novel description of tuba1a as a potential non-autonomous contributor to melanocyte dispersion, and 3) the generation of sox10 deficient melanoma tumors that show "escape" of sox10 loss through upregulation of sox9. The latter two examples highlight the usefulness of cell-type targeted knockouts (Body-wide sox10 and tuba1a loss elicit developmental defects). Additionally, the tumor models involve highly multiplexed sgRNAs for tumor initiation which is nicely facilitated by the stable Cas9.

      Strengths:

      The approach is clever and could prove very useful for studying melanocytes and other cell types. As the authors hint at in their discussion, this approach would become even more powerful with the generation of other Cas9-restricted lineages so a single sgRNA construct can be screened across many lineages rapidly (or many sgRNA and fish lines screened combinatorially).

      The biological findings used to demonstrate the power of the approach are interesting in their own right. If it proves true, tuba1a's non-autonomous effects on melanosome dispersion are striking, and this example demonstrates very nicely how one could use Perlee et al.'s approach to search for other non-autonomous mechanisms systematically. Similarly, the observation of the sox9 escape mechanism with sox10 loss is a beautiful demonstration of the relevance of SOX10/SOX9's reciprocal regulation in vivo. This system would be a very nice model for further interrogating mechanisms/interventions surrounding Sox10 in melanoma.

      Finally, the figure presentation is very nice. This work involves complex genetic approaches including multiple fish generations and multiplexed construct injections. The vector diagrams and breeding schemes in the paper make everything very clear/"grok-able," and the paper was enjoyable to read.

      Weaknesses:

      The mitfa-driven GFP on their sgRNA-expressing cassette is elegant, but it makes one wonder why the endogenous knock-in is necessary. It would strengthen the motivation of the work if the authors could detail the potential advantages and disadvantages of their system compared to expressing Cas9 with a lineage-specific promoter from a transposon in their introduction or discussion.

      We agree this needed a better and more clear explanation. There are many excellent examples of promoter driven Cas9 approaches. Within melanocytes, Ablain and others have developed the MAZERATI system (https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/30385465/) which is very powerful, especially for melanoma development. In our minds, the major advantage of endogenous knockin is that we retain all of the natural regulatory elements (many of which are not known) and so small promoter fragments always run the risk of missing certain types of regulation. While these regulatory elements may not matter under homeostatic conditions, they may become very important under perturbation, stress or disease states. This is why it is common, for example, in the mouse field, to knock in things like Cre into endogenous loci. We have now added a clarification of this to the manuscript.

      Related to the above - is mitfa haplosufficient? If the mitfaCas9/+ fish have any notable phenotypes, it would be worth noting for others interested in using this approach to study melanoma and pigmentation.

      In normal melanocytes, mitfa is haplosufficient. There are no visible differences between mitfaCas9/+ and wild-type fish at any stages of development (Figure S1C). Although we did not directly compare tumor growth in mitfa-/+ and mitfa+/+ fish in this study, it is possible that the disruption of mitfa in mitfaCas9/+ fish affects melanoma development. Most zebrafish melanoma models involve the overexpression of mitfa with MiniCoopR vectors and it would be interesting in future studies to determine how mitfa heterozygosity affects melanoma initiation or progression. 

      A core weakness (and also potential strength) of the system is that introduced edits will always be non-clonal (Fig 2H/I). The activity of individual sgRNAs should always be validated in the absence of any noticeable phenotype to interpret a negative result. Additionally, caution should be taken when interpreting results from rare events involving positive outgrowth (like tumorogenesis) to account for the fact many cells in the population might not have biallelic null alleles (i.e., 100% of the gene product removed).

      Along those lines: in my opinion, the tuba1a results are the most provocative finding in the paper, but they lack key validation. With respect to cutting activity, the Alt-R and transgenic sgRNA expression approaches are not directly comparable. Since there is no phenotype in the melanocyte specific tuba1a knockouts, the authors must confirm high knockout efficiency with this set of reagents before making the claim there is a non-autonomous phenotype. This can be achieved with GFP+ sorting and NGS like they performed with their albino melanocytes.

      The whole-body tuba1a knockout phenotype is expected to be pleiotropic, and this expectation might mask off-target effects. Controls for knockout specificity should be included. For instance, confidence in the claims would greatly increase if the dispersed melanosome phenotype could be recovered with guide-resistant tuba1a re-expression and if melanocyte-restricted tuba1a reexpression failed to rescue. As a less definitive but adequate alternative, the authors could also test if another guide or a morpholino against tuba1a phenocopies the described Alt-R edited fish.

      Thank you for your thoughtful suggestions, which led us to an important discovery. While validating the original tuba1a guide RNA, we found that tuba1a sg1 also targets tuba1c, a gene that shares 99.78% homology with tuba1a in zebrafish. To determine which gene was responsible for the melanocyte phenotype, we designed multiple new guide RNAs specifically targeting either tuba1a or tuba1c and used Alt-R to globally knock them out in zebrafish embryos. However, none of these guides successfully replicated the phenotype (Sanger sequencing validation for the most efficient tuba1a and tuba1c guides is provided below).

      Ultimately, we identified a new guide RNA (5’-GGTCTACAAAGACAGCCCTA-3’) that successfully phenocopied the original tuba1a sg1 melanocyte phenotype. Tuba1c—but not tuba1a—was predicted to have a mismatch at the 3’ end of the guide sequence, which is typically expected to inhibit target cleavage. Surprisingly, despite this mismatch, we observed robust cleavage in both tuba1a and tuba1c. Since the melanocyte phenotype was only reproducible when both tuba1a and tuba1c were targeted, this suggests potential compensatory interactions between these highly similar genes. We have updated the text and figures to reflect this finding and have included validation of this second guide RNA (tuba1a/c sg2) in Supplemental Figure 3.

      As you suggested, we also conducted GFP+ sorting and NGS to confirm knockout of both tuba1a and tuba1c in melanocytes of mitfaCas9 fish (Figure S3G). The knockout percentages were comparable to those observed in our previous experiment with MG_-albino_ fish. This also confirms that this method can be used to sort and sequence GFP+ cells even when pigmentation is retained, which was not the case for albino fish. 

      I have similar questions about the sox10 escapers, but these suggestions are less critical for supporting the authors claims (especially given the nice staining). Are the sox10 tumors relatively clonal with respect to sox10 mutations? And are the sox10 tumor mutations mostly biallelic frameshifts or potential missense mutations/single mutations that might not completely remove activity? I am particularly curious as SOX10 doesn't seem to be completely absent (and is still very high in some nuclei) in the immunohistochemistry.

      We attempted to address this question by performing DNA sequencing on the FFPE blocks that we had retained from the original study. While our sequencing facility said this should be possible, we could not consistently generate high enough quality DNA to make a definitive statement either way. While we are very curious to know what the nature of the mutations are in these “escapers”, the student who performed these studies has now graduated, and it would take us several additional months to a year to fully address it. Given this, we would prefer to leave this open question to a future paper, but have addressed this limitation in the Discussion.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewing Editor:

      Overall, the reviewers felt and eLife concurs that your manuscript is insightful and appropriate for publication. Reviewers were impressed by your generating a zebrafish line where CRISPRbased gene editing is exclusively limited to the melanocyte lineage, allowing assessment of celltype restricted gene knockouts. Your use of multiple candidate genes to demonstrate that your system induces lineage-restricted gene editing is compelling and will be of interest to the broad readership of eLife. This method will allow researchers to bypass embryonic lethal and non-cell autonomous phenotypes emerging from whole body knockout, drive directed phenotypes, such as depigmentation, and induce lineage-specific tumors, such as melanomas. This said, the argued increase in efficiency of this model compared to current tools was untested, and therefore it remains difficult for a reader to assess the extent to which your new model represents a major advance over prior ones. Of additional concern are the mechanistic explanations proposed to underlie the phenotypes, as these are largely unfounded. Thus, in preparing your final publication version of the paper, eLife strongly encourages you to fully address the reviewers' thoughtful comments. In particular, the boldness of the claims made in the manuscript should be reduced. Terms like "highly efficient" and "rapid" are unsupported due to the lack of comparison with other well-established methods, like MAZERATI.

      As discussed above in each of the reviewer points above, we agree with both of these points. We have reduced the boldness of the claims, with a better discussion of the different approaches. We also address the potential mechanisms of our observations, and where and why we still lack an understanding of what gives rise to those phenotypes. 

      There are also some minor discrepancies that should be edited in the manuscript: Fig.2A plasmid description is written oppositely in text; Fig.3 labels G-H are swapped in the legend description; Fig.5A MTdT is unexplained. This is a non-exhaustive list, and the authors are encouraged to carefully read through their manuscript to revise other minor mistakes and formatting errors.

      Figure 2A was revised to show the correct orientation of mitfa:GFP and the guide RNA cassette as described in the text. Figure 3 legend was fixed. We have gone through the manuscript again to make sure we have not made any other errors, to the best of our knowledge.

      The biggest concern is the expression of cas9 and the weak histological support shown in Fig.1 and Fig.S1. It would be a benefit to all readers and potential future users to know how robust cas9 expression is in the melanocyte lineage. It would be helpful if there is a way to analyze the percentage of cells that are mutated in each animal to understand the variability that can exist across animals with the method.

      We have revised Figure 1C to show additional melanocytes and added a new quantification of Cas9 RNA expression in melanocytes (S1D). 

      The analysis of the scRNA sequencing could also be described more fully.

      More details have been added to the scRNA sequencing analysis including the functions that were used. 

      The final major concern is whether this model is genuinely more valuable than MAZERATI. A more elaborate discussion would benefit potential future users to guide their decisions regarding which tool best suits their experimental goals.

      As noted above, we agree with this statement. The reviewers are correct in that we did not directly compare our system to MAZERATI, and therefore cannot make any claims about efficiency in a comparative regard. Therefore, in our revised Discussion, we talk about the relative strengths and weaknesses of each approach, and emphasize that our approach mainly has the advantage of retaining endogenous regulatory elements for mitfa, but that each user should decide which is the best approach for their problem.

      There are also some minor concerns that should be addressed.

      Are the mitfaCas9 fish used as homozygotes before the first cross? If so, might be nice to include their nacre-like phenotype in diagrams like Fig 2A.

      For these studies, heterozygous mitfaCas9 fish were used for all breedings and progeny were sorted for BFP+ eyes. This enabled the comparison to sibling controls without Cas9 expression. 

      BFP+ eye screening for mitfaCas9 is elegant and included nicely in the diagrams. Are germline sgRNA integrants identified in F1 with melanocyte GFP? Or present at a high enough efficiency that this is not relevant? This would be good to include in the diagrams.

      Germline sgRNA integrants are identified with melanocyte GFP in embryos. Figure 2A has been edited to show GFP expression. 

      Most cells are GFP positive in S3C (the F0 "mosaic"). It might be nice to show a single GFP stripe like in the other panels for direct comparison of edited/non-edited in the same fish.

      This figure (now S3E) has been edited to show a clear comparison between GFP+ and GFP- cells in the same fish. 

      177 - CRISPR-Seq is basically amplicon sequencing. This would measure efficiency but not "specificity" as described. Off-target activity would have to be measured at other loci etc. Not necessary to do, but I don't think measured.

      In this case, “specificity” refers to cell type specificity, not genomic specificity. We are measuring cell type specificity by comparing on-target cutting in GFP+ cells (melanocytes) versus GFP- cells (non-mitfa expressing cells). We did not look at off-target activity of Cas9 in this study and have edited the text to make this clearer. 

      219 -"several gaps were visible"

      Fixed

      286 - TUBA1A should be italicized

      Fixed

      399 - SOX9's most enriched dependency in DepMap is cutaneous melanoma and its top coessential gene is SOX10. I'm not sure the SOX9/SOX10 interaction couldn't be parsed from DepMap alone.

      This is true, and the DepMap was actually somewhat of an inspiration for our own studies. We have modified the line to acknowledge this and explain the main advantage of our system is in vivo confirmation of what the DepMap had alluded to.

      433 - "fewer animals since all F1 animals (even those for recessive alleles) are informative."

      The fact that this is approach is faster and more efficient per animal is important to highlight (and very believable), but is this technically true given not all F1 fish will have Cas9 or a germline sgRNA integration?

      In considering this statement, we agree with you and decided to remove it from the text.

      We hope the comments in both the public and private reviews will help improve the manuscript.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Overall, the boldness of the claims made in the manuscript should be reduced. Terms like "highly efficient" and "rapid" are unsupported due to the lack of comparison with other wellestablished methods, like MAZERATI.

      As discussed above, we agree with this and have now modified the manuscript to better reflect what our system achieves in comparison to the well developed systems such as MAZERATI. Because we have not done a direct comparison, we are not able to make any claims about comparative efficiency, and instead focus on the potential benefits of a knockin approach, which is the maintenance of endogenous regulatory elements.

      There are some minor discrepancies that should be edited in the manuscript: Fig.2A plasmid description is written oppositely in text; Fig.3 labels G-H are swapped in the legend description; Fig.5A MTdT is unexplained. This is a non-exhaustive list, and the authors are encouraged to carefully read through their manuscript to revise other minor mistakes and formatting errors.

      Figure 2A was revised to show the correct orientation of mitfa:GFP and the guide RNA cassette as described in the text. Figure 3 legend was fixed. We have gone through the manuscript again to make sure we have not made any other errors, to the best of our knowledge.

      The biggest concern is the expression of cas9 and the weak histological support shown in Fig.1 and Fig.S1. It would be a benefit to all readers and potential future users to know how robust cas9 expression is in the melanocyte lineage.

      We have revised Figure 1C to show additional melanocytes and added a new quantification of Cas9 RNA expression in melanocytes (S1D). 

      The second major concern is whether this model is genuinely more valuable than MAZERATI. A more elaborate discussion would benefit potential future users to guide their decision regarding which tool best suits their experimental goals.

      As noted above, we agree with this statement. The reviewers are correct in that we did not directly compare our system to MAZERATI, and therefore cannot make any claims about efficiency in a comparative regard. Therefore, in our revised Discussion, we talk about the relative strengths and weaknesses of each approach, and emphasize that our approach mainly has the advantage of retaining endogenous regulatory elements for mitfa, but that each user should decide which is the best approach for their problem.

      We hope the comments in both the public and private reviews will help improve the manuscript.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      While that authors show the indel charts for the Crispr mutations generated in the supplement. However, I wonder if there is a way to analyze the percentage of cells that are mutated in each animal to understand the variability that can exist across animals with the method.

      We have revised Figure 1C to show additional melanocytes and added a new quantification of Cas9 RNA expression in melanocytes (S1D). 

      The analysis of the scRNA sequencing could be described more fully.

      More details have been added to the scRNA sequencing analysis including the functions that were used. 

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      This was an excellent read, and I'm very interested in seeing it in its final form. Congratulations! My larger critiques are outlined in the public reviews. A few smaller points:

      Are the mitfaCas9 fish used as homozygotes before the first cross? If so, might be nice to include their nacre-like phenotype in diagrams like Fig 2A.

      For these studies, heterozygous mitfaCas9 fish were used for all breedings and progeny were sorted for BFP+ eyes. This enabled the comparison to sibling controls without Cas9 expression. 

      BFP+ eye screening for mitfaCas9 is elegant and included nicely in the diagrams. Are germline sgRNA integrants identified in F1 with melanocyte GFP? Or present at a high enough efficiency that this is not relevant? This would be good to include in the diagrams.

      Germline sgRNA integrants are identified with melanocyte GFP in embryos. Figure 2A has been edited to show GFP expression. 

      Most cells are GFP positive in S3C (the F0 "mosaic"). It might be nice to show a single GFP stripe like in the other panels for direct comparison of edited/non-edited in the same fish.

      This figure (now S3E) has been edited to show a clear comparison between GFP+ and GFP- cells in the same fish. 

      177 - My understanding is that CRISPR-Seq is basically amplicon sequencing. This would measure efficiency but not "specificity" as described. Off-target activity would have to be measured at other loci etc. Not necessary to do in my opinion, but I don't think measured.

      In this case, “specificity” refers to cell type specificity, not genomic specificity. We are measuring cell type specificity by comparing on-target cutting in GFP+ cells (melanocytes) versus GFP- cells (non-mitfa expressing cells). We did not look at off-target activity of Cas9 in this study and have edited the text to make this clearer. 

      219 -"several gaps were visible"

      Fixed

      286 - TUBA1A should be italicized

      Fixed

      399 - I think I understand the logic of the DepMap argument, and the importance of studying tumor initiation in vivo stands for itself. But here is maybe not the best example (or might need clarification)? - SOX9's most enriched dependency in DepMap is cutaneous melanoma and its top co-essential gene is SOX10. I'm not sure the SOX9/SOX10 interaction couldn't be parsed from DepMap alone.

      This is true, and the DepMap was actually somewhat of an inspiration for our own studies. We have modified the line to acknowledge this and explain the main advantage of our system is in vivo confirmation of what the DepMap had alluded to.

      433 - "fewer animals since all F1 animals (even those for recessive alleles) are informative."

      The fact that this is approach is faster and more efficient per animal is important to highlight (and very believable), but is this technically true given not all F1 fish will have Cas9 or a germline sgRNA integration?

      In considering this statement, we agree with you and decided to remove it from the text.

    1. eLife Assessment

      This important study reveals that Excitatory Amino Acid Transporters play a role in chromatic information processing in the retina. The combination of (double) mutants, behavioral assays, immunohistochemistry, and electroretinograms provides solid evidence supporting the appropriately conservative conclusions. The work will be of interest to neurobiologists working on color vision or retinal processing.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript by Garbelli et al. investigates the roles of excitatory amino acid transporters (EAATs) in retinal bipolar cells. The group previously identified that EAAT5b and EAAT7 are expressed at the dendritic tips of bipolar cells, where they connect with photoreceptor terminals. The previous study found that the light responses of bipolar cells, measured by electroretinogram (ERG) in response to white light, were reduced in double mutants, though there was little to no reduction in light responses in single mutants of either EAAT5b or EAAT7.

      The current study further explores the roles of EAAT5b and EAAT7 in bipolar cells' chromatic responses. The authors found that bipolar cell responses to red light, but not to green or UV-blue light, were reduced in single mutants of both EAAT5b and EAAT7. In contrast, UV-blue light responses were reduced in double mutants. Additionally, the authors observed that EAAT5b, but not EAAT7, is strongly localized in the UV cone-enriched area of the eye, known as the "Strike Zone (SZ)." This led them to investigate the impact of the EAAT5b mutation on prey detection performance, which is mediated by UV cones in the SZ. Surprisingly, contrary to the predicted role of EAAT5b in prey detection, EAAT5b mutants did not show any changes in prey detection performance compared to wild-type fish. Interestingly, EAAT7 mutants exhibited enhanced prey detection performance, though the underlying mechanisms remain unclear.

      The distribution of EAAT7 protein in the outer plexiform layer across the eye correlates with the distribution of red cones. Based on this, the authors tested the behavioral performance driven by red light in EAAT5b and EAAT7 mutants. The results here were again somewhat contrary to predictions based on ERG findings and protein localization: the optomotor response was reduced in EAAT5b mutants, but not in EAAT7 mutants.

      Strengths:

      Although the paper lacks cohesive conclusions, as many results contradict initial predictions as mentioned above, the authors discuss possible mechanisms for these contradictions and suggest future avenues for study. Nevertheless, this paper demonstrates a novel mechanism underlying chromatic information processing.<br /> The manuscript is well-written, the data are well-presented, and the analysis is thorough.

      Weaknesses:

      I have only a minor comment. The authors present preliminary data on mGluR6b distribution across the eye. Since this result is based on a single fish, I recommend either adding more samples or removing this data, as it does not significantly impact the paper's main conclusions.

      Comments on revisions:

      The authors addressed all of the concerns that I had in the original manuscript.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Garbelli et. al. set out to elucidate the function of two glutamate transporters, EAAT5b and EAAT7, in the functional and behavioral responses to different wavelengths of light. The question is an interesting one because these transporters are well-positioned to affect responses to light, and their distribution in the retina suggests that they could play differential roles in visual behaviors. However, the resolution of the functional and behavioral data presented here means that the conclusions are necessarily a bit vague.

      In Figure 1, the authors show that the double KO has a decreased ERG response to UV/blue and red wavelengths. However, the individual mutations both only affect the response to red light, suggesting that they might affect behaviors such as OMR that typically rely on this part of the visual spectrum. However, there was no significant change in the response to UV/blue light of any intensity, making it unclear whether the mutations could individually play roles in detection of UV prey. Based on the later behavioral data, it seems likely that at least the EAAT7 KO should affect retinal responses to UV light, but it may be that the ERG does not have the spatial or temporal resolution to detect the difference, or that the presence of blue light overwhelmed any effect of the individual knockouts on the response to UV light.

      In Figures 5 and 6, the authors compare the two knockouts to wild-type fish in terms of their sensitivity to UV prey in a hunting assay. The EAAT5b KO showed no significant impairment in UV sensitivity, while the EAAT7 KO fish actually had an increased hunting response to UV prey. However, there is no comparison of the KO and WT responses to different UV intensities, only in bulk, so we cannot conclude that the EAAT7 KO is allowing the fish to detect weaker prey-like stimuli.

      In Figure 7, the EAAT5b KO seems to cause a decrease in OMR behavior to red grating stimuli, but only one stimulus is tested, so it is unclear whether this is due to a change in visual sensitivity or resolution.

      The conclusions made in the manuscript are appropriately conservative; the abstract states that these transporters somehow influence prey detection and motion sensing, and this is likely true.

      In terms of impact on the field, this work highlights the potential importance of these two transporters to visual processing, but further studies will be required to say how important they are and exactly what they are doing.

    4. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript by Garbelli et al. investigates the roles of excitatory amino acid transporters (EAATs) in retinal bipolar cells. The group previously identified that EAAT5b and EAAT7 are expressed at the dendritic tips of bipolar cells, where they connect with photoreceptor terminals. The previous study found that the light responses of bipolar cells, measured by electroretinogram (ERG) in response to white light, were reduced in double mutants, though there was little to no reduction in light responses in single mutants of either EAAT5b or EAAT7.

      The current study further explores the roles of EAAT5b and EAAT7 in bipolar cells' chromatic responses. The authors found that bipolar cell responses to red light, but not to green or UV-blue light, were reduced in single mutants of both EAAT5b and EAAT7. In contrast, UV-blue light responses were reduced in double mutants. Additionally, the authors observed that EAAT5b, but not EAAT7, is strongly localized in the UV cone-enriched area of the eye, known as the "Strike Zone (SZ)." This led them to investigate the impact of the EAAT5b mutation on prey detection performance, which is mediated by UV cones in the SZ. Surprisingly, contrary to the predicted role of EAAT5b in prey detection, EAAT5b mutants did not show any changes in prey detection performance compared to wild-type fish. Interestingly, EAAT7 mutants exhibited enhanced prey detection performance, though the underlying mechanisms remain unclear.

      The distribution of EAAT7 protein in the outer plexiform layer across the eye correlates with the distribution of red cones. Based on this, the authors tested the behavioral performance driven by red light in EAAT5b and EAAT7 mutants. The results here were again somewhat contrary to predictions based on ERG findings and protein localization: the optomotor response was reduced in EAAT5b mutants, but not in EAAT7 mutants.

      Strengths:

      Although the paper lacks cohesive conclusions, as many results contradict initial predictions as mentioned above, the authors discuss possible mechanisms for these contradictions and suggest future avenues for study. Nevertheless, this paper demonstrates a novel mechanism underlying chromatic information processing.

      The manuscript is well-written, the data are well-presented, and the analysis is thorough.

      We are happy about the perceived strengths of our manuscript.

      Weaknesses:

      I have only a minor comment. The authors present preliminary data on mGluR6b distribution across the eye. Since this result is based on a single fish, I recommend either adding more samples or removing this data, as it does not significantly impact the paper's main conclusions.

      We agree that the mGluR6 result is statistically underpower (we would never claim differently). The data is based on only one clutch of fish, comprising 11 eyes. Since the data is anyway in the supplement and not part of the main story, we would like to keep it to spur further investigations into anisotropic distribution of synaptic proteins.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Garbelli et. al. set out to elucidate the function of two glutamate transporters, EAAT5b and EAAT7, in the functional and behavioral responses to different wavelengths of light. The question is an interesting one, because these transporters are well positioned to affect responses to light, and their distribution in the retina suggests that they could play differential roles in visual behaviors. However, the low resolution of both the functional and behavioral data presented here means that the conclusions are necessarily a bit vague.

      In Figure 1, the authors show that the double KO has a decreased ERG response to UV/blue and red wavelengths. However, the individual mutations only affect the response to red light, suggesting that they might affect behaviors such as OMR which typically rely on this part of the visual spectrum. However, there was no significant change in the response to UV/blue light of any intensity, making it unclear whether the mutations could individually play roles in the detection of UV prey. Based on the later behavioral data, it seems likely that at least the EAAT7 KO should affect retinal responses to UV light, but it may be that the ERG does not have the spatial or temporal resolution to detect the difference, or that the presence of blue light overwhelmed any effect of the individual knockouts on the response to UV light.

      In Figures 5 and 6, the authors compare the two knockouts to wild-type fish in terms of their sensitivity to UV prey in a hunting assay. The EAAT5b KO showed no significant impairment in UV sensitivity, while the EAAT7 KO fish actually had an increased hunting response to UV prey. However, there is no comparison of the KO and WT responses to different UV intensities, only in bulk, so we cannot conclude that the EAAT7 KO is allowing the fish to detect weaker prey-like stimuli.

      We have now reported in both in the results paragraph and in the methods section that response-comparison of intensity-specific responses were non-significant in all instances of analyses (Chi-square test with p>0.05). We decided not to add the information to the figure as it does not add to the data and risks causing excessive clutter of an already complex graph.

      As reviewer #2 rightfully states, we cannot conclude that EAAT7 KO is allowing the fish to detect weaker prey-like stimuli. We only intend to suggest that a lack of EAAT7 might facilitate prey detection events as the number of hunting events in total, is increased compared to WT.

      In Figure 7, the EAAT5b KO seems to cause a decrease in OMR behavior to red grating stimuli, but only one stimulus is tested, so it is unclear whether this is due to a change in visual sensitivity or resolution.

      We fully agree that further experiments presenting different stimuli in the setup may very well reveal more details on the nature of the observed defect and thank reviewer #2 for the suggestion. We feel that identifying the reason of the defect lies outside of the scope of this paper, but should definitely be investigated in future studies.

      The conclusions made in the manuscript are appropriately conservative; the abstract states that these transporters somehow influence prey detection and motion sensing, and this is probably true. However, it is unclear to what extent and how they might be acting on these processes, so the conclusions are a bit unsatisfying.

      In terms of impact on the field, this work highlights the potential importance of these two transporters to visual processing, but further studies will be required to say how important they are and what they are doing. The methods presented here are not novel, as UV prey and red OMR stimuli and behaviors have previously been described.

      We agree that this study is not fully conclusive but a first step towards a clarification of the role of glutamate transporters in shaping visual behavior.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Suggestions for improved or additional experiments, data, or analyses:

      Figure 3:

      (a) What is the intensity of the light emitted by the UV and yellow LEDs and experienced by the larva, e.g. in nW? This is necessary in order to be able to compare and replicate the results.

      Stimuli intensities in microwatts are now included and reported in the Materials and Methods sections

      (b) In Figure 3D, are all the example eye movement events hunting initiations? Does right eye/left eye positive or negative angle change denote convergence?

      As indicated in the figure legend, hunting initiations are indicated by black dots on the graph. In Stytra’s eye tracking system, eye convergence is indicated by an increase in the left eye angle and a decrease in the right eye angle. Both these points have now been clarified in the figure legend.

      (c) Also in 3D, the tail angle plot and x-axis are too small to read.

      Figure 3D has been reformatted to be more legible.

      (d) How much eye convergence constitutes a response? In order to compare the findings to previous studies of prey capture, it would be best to use a bimodal distribution of eye angles to set a convergence threshold for each fish (e.g. Paride et. al., eLife 2019), but there should at least be a clear threshold mentioned.

      We have expanded the explanation of how the response detection paradigm was calculated. We acknowledge that this analysis has limitations in terms of comparability with previous studies, as it was developed de novo, based on the format of eye coordinate data provided by Stytra and refined through iterative comparison with experimental video recordings. Since the threshold was defined relative to the average noise level of the trace, it is difficult to specify an exact value. However, we are happy to share the Python scripts used for the analysis to facilitate further investigation.

      (e) The previous study using artificial UV prey stimuli to trigger hunting (Khan et. al., Current Biology 2023) should be acknowledged.

      This is an indeed an embarrassing omission, not excused by the first version of this section being drafted before the Khan publication. We have now cited this important study.

      Figure 5:

      Was the response at any individual intensity significantly lower in the mutant? If not, this should be clearly stated.

      Yes, and this is now clearly stated in the main text

      Figure 6:

      Again, it would be more informative to know for which intensities the KO response was significantly greater than WT.

      This is now also clearly stated in the main text

      Figure 7:

      (a) What are the intensity units?

      We now clarified in the figure that the intensity shown in the graph is digital intensity

      (b) Similar to Figures 5 and 6, it would be more informative to know at which intensities the KO response was significantly different from WT.

      We now report the measured optical powers relative to the digital intensities in the Materials and Methods sections.

      Suggestion for writing:

      The discussion was a bit discursive. A more structured discussion, sequentially explaining each of the key results, would be easier for the reader to follow. And, it would be helpful to have hypotheses for how these transporter mutants could cause each of the changes in visual behaviors that were observed.

      We agree that the discussion needed improvements. We have completely rewritten the discussion and hope that it now more concisely put our results into context.

    1. eLife Assessment

      This study proposes a useful assay to identify relative social ranks in mice incorporating the competitive drive for two basic resources - food and living space. Using this new protocol, the authors provide solid evidence of stable ranking among male and female pairs, while reporting more fluctuant hierarchies among triads of males. The evidence is, however, limited by the lack of ethologically based validation, assessment of the influence of competitor recognition, and proof of concept of application to neuroscience. This manuscript may be of interest to those interested in social behavior and related neuroscience.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary

      The authors present a new protocol to assess social dominance in pairs and triads of C57BL/6j mice, based on a competition to access a hidden food pellet. Using this new protocol, the authors have been able to identify stable ranking among male and female pairs, while reporting more fluctuant hierarchies among triads of males. Ranking readout identified with this new apparatus was compared to the outcome obtained with the same animals competing in the tube and in the warm spot tests, which have been both commonly used during the last decade to identify social ranks in rodents under laboratory conditions.

      Strengths

      FPCT allows for an easy and fast identification of a winner and loser in a context of food competition. The apparatus and the protocol are relatively easy and quick to implement in the lab and free from any complex post processing/analysis, which qualifies it for wide distribution, particularly within laboratories that do not have the resources to implement more sophisticated protocols. Hierarchical readout identified through the FPCT correlates with social ranks identified with the tube and the warm spot tests, which have been widely adopted during the last decade and allow for study comparison.

      Weaknesses

      While the FPCT is validated by the tube and the warm spot test, this paper would have gained strength by providing a more ethologically based validation. Tube and warm spot tests have been shown to provide conflicting results and might not be a sufficient measurement for social ranking (see Varholik et al, Scientific Reports, 2019; Battivelli et al, Biological Psychiatry, 2024). Instead, a general consensus pushing toward more ethological approaches for neuroscience studies is emerging.<br /> Other papers already successfully identified social ranks dyadic food competition, using relatively simple scoring protocol (see, for example, Merlot et al., 2006), within a more naturalistic set-up, allowing the 2 opponents to directly interact while competing for the food. A potential issue with the FPCT, is that the opponents being isolated from each other, the normal inhibition expected to appear in subordinates in presence of a dominant to access food, could be diminished, and usually avoiding subordinates could be more motivated to push for the access to the food pellet.

      Comments on revisions:

      We thank the authors for the significant improvement of the English in the revised version and for the replacement of some conceptual terms that now seem more relevant and appropriate. We only noticed that the term "society" remains in use, although it might not be appropriate to describe a mouse colony (see previous review).

      Conclusive remarks

      Although this protocol aims to provide a novel approach to evaluate social ranks in mice, it is not clear how it really brings a significant advance in neuroscience research. The FPCT dynamic is very similar to the one observed in the tube test, where mice compete to navigate forward in a narrow space, constraining the opponent to go backwards. The main difference between the FPCT and the tube test is the presence of food between the opponents. In the tube test, food reward was initially used to increase motivation to cross the tube and push the opponent upon the testing day. This component has been progressively abandoned, precisely because it was not necessary for the mice to compete in the tube.<br /> This paper would really bring a significant contribution to the field by providing a neuronal imaging or manipulation correlate to the behavioral outcome obtained by the application of the FPCT.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this study, the authors have devised a novel assay to measure relative social rank in mice that is aimed at incorporating multiple aspects of social competition while minimizing direct contact between animals. Forming a hierarchy often involves complex social dynamics related to competitive drives for different fundamental resources, including access to food, water, territory and sexual mates. This makes the study of social dominance and its neural underpinnings hard, warranting the development of new tools and methods that can help understand both social function as well as dysfunction.

      Strengths:

      This study showcases an assay called the Food Pellet Competition Test, where cagemate mice compete for food, without direct contact, by pushing a block in a tube from opposite directions. This task ran with stranger mice leads to more variable outcomes, suggesting co-housing helps stabilize outcomes. The authors have attempted to quantify motivation to obtain the food independent of other factors by running the assay under two conditions: one where the food is accessible and one where it isn't. This assay results in high outcome consistency across days for females and males paired housed and for male groups of three. Further, the determined social ranks correlate strongly with two common assays: the tube test and the warm spot test.

      Weaknesses:

      This new assay has limited ethological validity since mice do not compete for food without touching each other with a block in the middle. In addition, the assay may only be valid for a single trial per day, making its utility for recording neural recordings and manipulations limited to a single sample per mouse. The authors claim, as currently stated in results, for the new control experiment in 1H-J is not warranted given that 6/8 mice had majority winning or losing across all strangers.

    4. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The laboratory mouse is an ideal animal to study the neural and psychological underpinnings of social dominance behavior because of its economic cost and the animals' readiness to display dominant and subordinate behaviors in simple and testable environments. Here, a new and novel method for measuring dominance and the individual social status of mice is presented using a food competition assay. Historically, food competition assays have been avoided because they occur in an open arena or the home cage, and it can be difficult to assess who gets priority access to the resource and to avoid aggressive interactions such as bite wounding. Now, the authors have designed a narrow rectangular arena separated in half by a sliding floor-to-ceiling obstacle, where the mice placed at opposite sides of the obstacle compete by pushing the obstacle to gain priority access to a food pellet resting on the arena floor under the obstacle. One can also place the food pellet within the obstacle to restrict priority access to the food and measure the time or effort spent pushing the obstacle back and forth. As hypothesized, the outcomes in the food competition test were significantly consistent with those of the more common tube test (space competition) and warm spot competition test. This suggests that these animals have a stereotypic dominance organization that exists across multiple resource domains (i.e., food, space, and temperature). Only male and female C57 mice in same-sex pairs or triads were tested.

      Strengths:

      The design of the apparatus and the inclusion of females are significant strengths within the study.

      Weaknesses:

      There are at least two major weaknesses of the study: the test with unfamiliar non-cagemates and not providing the mice time to recognize who they are competing with.

      The authors conclude in the first section of the results that they "did not detect significant difference in winning/losing results between unfamiliar non-cagemate male mice." Given the data and analysis provided, I believe this statement is false. My understanding is that the authors would like to show that the establishment of social relationships (i.e., familiarity) is necessary for FPCT to distinguish social ranks of mice. There are many ways to test this. The simplest would be to randomly pair unfamiliar non-cagemates that are housed in isolation with one another and see if they perform at chance, individually. The more involved empirical way would be to measure the ranks of mice in a social group, then test them with unfamiliar non-cagemate mice to see if they maintain their outcomes regardless of social familiarity, or return to chance outcomes when paired with non-cagemates. Figure 1I clearly shows that they did not perform at chance. Since the outcome is win or lose, then the probability of getting all of one outcome 4 times in a row would be 1 in 16. The data shows that this occured twice, so 2 mice of 8 had the same outcome 4 times in a row (i.e., Mouse B3 and A1). So, they did not perform at chance. I am not even sure if there are enough animals here to test this question. One may need to consult a mathematician. Moreover, the original tube-test study by Lindzey et al. 1961 (https://www.nature.com/articles/191474a0) used unfamiliar non-cagemate male mice, and showed that 100% of the A/alb strain won more than half of their oppositions against C3H and DBA/8 mice. Thus, A/alb mice were more "dominant" mice relative to C3H or DBA/8. Taking into consideration the results, is mouse A1 naturally dominant? So maybe it doesn't matter what mouse you pair with it, it will always win? If this is true, is "individual identification of the partner" actually necessary to get this outcome? All they have to do is push to get the food reward, does it matter who is on the other side? If one wants to measure social dominance relationships, then it should matter who is on the other side. If one would like to measure attributes of dominant behavior (e.g., pushing), then one may do so and not insinuate a social link. Studying dominance relationships (i.e., social ranking) of animals is an extremely difficult task. We must ensure that we are not assigning something about a relationship that does not exist. Please read "Dominance: The baby and the bathwater" but Irwin Bernstein, https://annas-archive.org/scidb/10.1017/s0140525x00009614/

      Unlike the tube test and warm spot test, the food competition test presented here provides no opportunity for the animals to identify their opponent. That is, they cannot sniff their opponent's fur or anogenital region, which would allow them an opportunity to identify them individually. Thus, as the authors state, the test only measures a psychological motivation to get a food reward. Notably, the outcome in the direct and indirect testing of food competition is in agreement, leaving many to wonder whether they are measuring the social relationship or the effort an individual puts forth in attaining a food reward regardless of the social opponent. Specifically, in the direct test, an individual can retrieve the food reward by pushing the obstacle out of the way first. In the indirect test, the animals cannot retrieve the reward and can only push the obstacle back and forth, which contains the reward inside. In Figure 2F, you can see that winners spent more time pushing the block in the indirect test--albeit not significantly. Thus, whether the test measures a social relationship or just the likelihood to gain priority access to food is unclear. To rectify this issue, the authors could provide an opportunity for the animals to interact before lowering the obstacle and raising(?) a food reward. They may also create a very long one-sided apparatus to measure the amount of effort an individual mouse puts forth in the indirect test with only one individual-or any situation with just one mouse where the moving obstacle is not pushed back, and the animal can just keep pushing until they stop. This would require another experiment. It also may not tell us much more since it remains unclear whether inbred mice can individually identify one another (see https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2000.1057 for more details).

    5. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors present a new protocol to assess social dominance in pairs and triads of C57BL/6j mice, based on a competition to access a hidden food pellet. Using this new protocol, the authors have been able to identify stable ranking among male and female pairs, while reporting more fluctuant hierarchies among triads of males. Ranking readouts identified with this new apparatus were compared to the outcomes obtained with the same animals competing in the tube and in the warm spot tests, which have been both commonly used during the last decade to identify social ranks in rodents under laboratory conditions.

      Strengths:

      FPCT allows for easy and fast identification of a winner and a loser in the context of food competition. The apparatus and the protocol are relatively easy and quick to implement in the lab and free from any complex post-processing/analysis, which qualifies it for wide distribution, particularly within laboratories that do not have the resources to implement more sophisticated protocols. Hierarchical readouts identified through the FPCT correlate with social ranks identified with the tube and the warm spot tests, which have been widely adopted during the last decade and allow for study comparison.

      Weaknesses:

      While the FPCT is validated by the tube and the warm spot test, this paper would have gained strength by providing a more ethologically based validation. Tube and warm spot tests have been shown to provide conflicting results and might not been a sufficient measurement for social ranking (see Varholik et al, Scientific reports, 2019; Battivelli et al, Biological psychiatry, 2024). Instead, a general consensus pushing toward more ethological approaches for neuroscience studies is emerging.

      We appreciate all the reviewers for recognizing the strength of the FPCT setup and the data. We also appreciate the reviewers for pointing out weakness and giving us valuable suggestions that help us to improve the quality of our manuscript through revision.

      In this manuscript, we found the ranking results of the FPCT were largely consistent with the tube and the warm spot tests. Such a finding was unexpected by us as we considered that different competitive targets of different paradigms should provide the mice with distinct appeals and enable them to exert their specific advantages. However, the consistency between the FPCT and tube test was observed in the pairs of female mice, pairs of male mice and triads of male mice. The consistency between the FPCT, tube test and warm spot test was observed in pairs of male mice and triads of male mice. Thus, we concluded that there is a social rank-order stability of mice. 

      We acknowledge that it’d better if this conclusion could be validated by more ethological approaches like urine-marking analysis and water competition test. Whereas, we did not rule out inconsistency of ranking results between two or more paradigms. Actually, there were inconsistent cases in our experiments. The inconsistency of ranking results between paradigms, even between FPCT and tube test, could be amplified if the tests were operated with other details of experimental protocols and conditions. This is in that too many factors and aspects can affect the readouts, such as formation of colony, tasks, test protocols, habituation and training. Using tube test itself, both stable 1,2 and unstable 3 ranking results have been reported.

      Other papers already successfully identified social ranks dyadic food competition, using relatively simple scoring protocol (see for example Merlot et al., 2006), within a more naturalistic set-up, allowing the 2 opponents to directly interact while competing for the food. A potential issue with the FPCT, is that the opponents being isolated from each other, the normal inhibition expected to appear in subordinates in the presence of a dominant to access food, could be diminished, and usually avoiding subordinates could be more motivated to push for the access to the food pellet.

      The hierarchical structure of mice colony could be established on the basis of physical aspects—such as muscular strength, vigorousness of fighting—and psychological aspects— such as boldness, focused motivation, active self-awareness of status. In the contexts of currently available food contest paradigms where the mice compete with bodily interaction, the physical and psychological aspects are intermingled in the interpretation of the mice’s winning/losing. In the FPCT, the opponents are isolated from each other so that the importance of direct bodily interaction in a competition is minimized, facilitating the exposure of psychological factors contributing to the establishment and/or expression of social status of the mice. In this study, the overall stable ranking results across the FPCT, tube test and warm spot test indicate that the status sense of animals is part of a comprehensive identify of self-recognition of individuals in an established mice social colony.

      There are issues with use of the English language throughout the text. Some sentences are difficult to understand and should be clarified and/or synthesized.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out language issues. We have carefully corrected the grammar errors.

      Open question:

      Is food restriction mandatory? Palatable food pellet is not sufficient to trigger competition? Food restriction has numerous behavioral and physiological consequences that would be better to prevent to be able to clearly interpret behavioral outcomes in FPCT (see for example Tucci et al., 2006).

      We thank the reviewer for raising this question. In the preliminary experiments, we noticed that food restriction was mandatory and palatable food pellet was not sufficient to trigger competition. In order to limit the potential influence of food restriction on competitive behavior, the mice underwent only a 24-hour food deprivation period at the beginning of training, followed by mild restriction of food supply to meet basic energy requirement.

      Conclusive remarks:

      Although this protocol attempts to provide a novel approach to evaluate social ranks in mice, it is not clear how it really brings a significant advance in neuroscience research. The FPCT dynamic is very similar to the one observed in the tube test, where mice compete to navigate forward in a narrow space, constraining the opponent to go backward. The main difference between the FPCT and the tube test is the presence of food between the opponents. In the tube test, a food reward was initially used to increase motivation to cross the tube and push the opponent upon the testing day. This component has been progressively abandoned, precisely because it was not necessary for the mice to compete in the tube.

      This paper would really bring a significant contribution to the field by providing a neuronal imaging or manipulation correlate to the behavioral outcome obtained by the application of the FPCT.

      Thank the reviewer for this comment on the significance of the FPCT paradigm. In this manuscript, we think it is interesting to report that the ranking results were consistent across the FPCT, tube test and warm spot test. This finding indicates that the status sense of animals might be a part of a comprehensive identify of self-recognition of individuals in an established social colony. 

      Moreover, we are conducting researches on biological consequences and mechanisms of social competition. Hopefully, the results of the on-going project will be published in the near future.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this study, the authors have devised a novel assay to measure relative social rank in mice that is aimed at incorporating multiple aspects of social competition while minimizing direct contact between animals. Forming a hierarchy often involves complex social dynamics related to competitive drives for different fundamental resources including access to food, water, territory, and sexual mates. This makes the study of social dominance and its neural underpinnings hard, warranting the development of new tools and methods that can help understand both social functions as well as dysfunction.

      Strengths:

      This study showcases an assay called the Food Pellet Competition Test where cagemate mice compete for food, without direct contact, by pushing a block in a tube from opposite directions. The authors have attempted to quantify motivation to obtain the food independent of other factors such as age, weight, sex, etc. by running the assay under two conditions: one where the food is accessible and one where it isn't. This assay results in an impressive outcome consistency across days for females and males paired housed and for male groups of three. Further, the determined social ranks correlate strongly with two common assays: the tube test and the warm spot test.

      Weaknesses:

      This new assay has limited ethological validity since mice do not compete for food without touching each other with a block in the middle. In addition, the assay may only be valid for a single trial per day making its utility for recording neural recordings and manipulations limited to a single sample per mouse. Although the authors attempt to measure motivation as a factor driving who wins the social competition, the data is limited. This novel assay requires training across days with some mice reaching criteria before others. From the data reported, it is unclear what effects training can have on the outcome of social competition. Beyond the data shown, the language used throughout the manuscript and the rationale for the design of this novel assay is difficult to understand.

      We appreciate the reviewers for the valuable comments on the strength and weakness of our manuscript. 

      The design mentality of the FPCT was to (1) provide researchers with a choice of new food competition paradigm and (2) expose psychological factors influencing the establishment and/or expression social status in mice by avoiding direct physical competition between contenders (see revised Abstract and the last paragraph in the Introduction).

      As a result, the consistent ranking across the FPCT, tube test and warm spot test might indicate that the status sense of animals is part of a comprehensive identify of self-recognition of individuals in an established social colony. 

      We suggest to perform the FPCT test one trial per day per mouse as the mice might lose interest in the food pellet if it is tested frequently in a day, but it is practical to perform the FPCT assay for several days. 

      Regarding the training, we suggest 4-5 days for training as we did. In this revision, we add training data which show the progressing latency of food-getting of mice (Figure 1). At the last day of training, the mice would go directly to push the block and eat the food after they entered the arena.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out language issues. We have carefully corrected the errors.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The laboratory mouse is an ideal animal to study the neural and psychological underpinnings of social dominance behavior because of its economic cost and the animals' readiness to display dominant and subordinate behaviors in simple and testable environments. Here, a new and novel method for measuring dominance and the individual social status of mice is presented using a food competition assay. Historically, food competition assays have been avoided because they occur in an open arena or the home cage, and it can be difficult to assess who gets priority access to the resource and to avoid aggressive interactions such as bite wounding. Now, the authors have designed a narrow rectangular arena separated in half by a sliding floor-to-ceiling obstacle, where the mice placed at opposite sides of the obstacle compete by pushing the obstacle to gain priority access to a food pellet resting on the arena floor under the obstacle. One can also place the food pellet within the obstacle to restrict priority access to the food and measure the time or effort spent pushing the obstacle back and forth. As hypothesized, the outcomes in the food competition test were significantly consistent with those of the more common tube test (space competition) and warm spot competition test. This suggests that these animals have a stereotypic dominance organization that exists across multiple resource domains (i.e., food, space, and temperature). Only male and female C57 mice in same-sex pairs or triads were tested.

      Strengths:

      The design of the apparatus and the inclusion of females are significant strengths within the study.

      Weaknesses:

      There are at least two major weaknesses of the study: neglecting the value of test inconsistency and not providing the mice time to recognize who they are competing with.

      Several studies have demonstrated that although inbred mice in laboratory housing share similar genetics and environment, they can form diverse types of hierarchical organizations (e.g., loose, stable, despotic, linear, etc.) and there are multiple resource domains in the home cage that mice compete over (e.g., space, food, water, temperature, etc.). The advantage of using multiple dominance assays is to understand the nuances of hierarchical organizations better. For example, some groups may have clear dominant and subordinate individuals when competing for food, but the individuals may "change or switch" social status when competing for space. Indeed, social relationships are dynamic, not static. Here, the authors have provided another test to measure another dimension of dominance: food competition. Rather than highlight this advantage, the authors highlight that the test is in agreement with the standard tube test and warm spot test and that C57 mice have stereotypic dominance across multiple domains. While some may find this great, it will leave many to continue using the tube test only (which measures the dimension of space competition) and avoid measuring food competition. If the reader looks at Figures 6E, F, and G they will see examples of inconsistency across the food competition test, tube test, and warm spot test in triads of mice. These groups are quite interesting and demonstrate the diversity of social dynamics in groups of inbred mice in highly standardized environmental conditions. Scientists interested in dominance should study groups that are consistent and inconsistent across multiple dimensions of dominance (e.g., space, food, mates, etc.).

      Unlike the tube test and warm spot test, the food competition test presented here provides no opportunity for the animals to identify their opponent. That is, they cannot sniff their opponent's fur or anogenital region, which would allow them an opportunity to identify them individually. Thus, as the authors state, the test only measures psychological motivation to get a food reward. Notably, the outcome in the direct and indirect testing of food competition is in agreement, leaving many to wonder whether they are measuring the social relationship or the effort an individual puts forth in attaining a food reward regardless of the social opponent. Specifically, in the direct test, an individual can retrieve the food reward by pushing the obstacle out of the way first. In the indirect test, the animals cannot retrieve the reward and can only push the obstacle back and forth, which contains the reward inside. In Figure 4E, you can see that winners spent more time pushing the block in the indirect test. Thus, whether the test measures a social relationship or just the likelihood of gaining priority access to food is unclear. To rectify this issue, the authors could provide an opportunity for the animals to interact before lowering the obstacle and raising(?) a food reward. They may also create a very long one-sided apparatus to measure the amount of effort an individual mouse puts forth in the indirect test with only one individual - or any situation with just one mouse where the moving obstacle is not pushed back, and the animal can just keep pushing until they stop. This would require another experiment. It also may not tell us much more since it remains unclear whether inbred mice can individually identify one another

      (see https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2000.1057 for more details).

      A minor issue is that the write-up of the history of food competition assays and female dominance research is inaccurate. Food competition assays have a long history since at least the 1950s and many people study female dominance now.

      Food competition: https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.1950.9712776, https://psycnet.apa.org/fullte xt/1953-03267-

      001.pdf, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbi.2003.11.007, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-02204507-5

      Female dominance: history  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2023.03.020,  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0 031-9384(01)00494-2,  https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.99.4.411

      We thank the reviewers very much for so many helpful comments and suggestions.

      In this manuscript, we want to address the overall and averagely consistency of ranking results between FPCT, tube test and warm spot test) as an unexpected finding. We agree that the inconsistency of social ranking occurred between trials and between paradigms should not be ignored. In the revision, we added description and discussion of inconsistent part of the different test paradigms (paragraph 2 in the section 3 of the Result, last 2 sentences of paragraph 4 in the Discussion)

      Although the two opponents were separated each other, they were able to see and sniff each other because the block is transparency, there are holes in the lower portion of the block, and there is the gap between the block and chamber (Supplementary figures 1 and 2). In the female but not male groups, the presence of a cagemate opponent during the test 1 could significantly disturb the female mice and increase the its latency to get the food, comparing with last day of training when there was no opponent (Figure 3A). This indicates that one mouse, at least female mouse, could identify the existence of the opponent in the opposite side of the chamber. To further see whether social relation was influential to readouts of the FPCT, we performed additional experiments using two groups of non-cagemate mice to perform the competition. We did not detect obviously different ranks between the two groups (Figure 1H-1J), suggesting that establishment of social colony is necessary for FPCT to distinguish social ranks of mice.

      Thank the reviewer for reminding us to recognize the history of food competition assays. We have added the citations and discussions of related literatures, both for male (paragraph 2 in the Introduction; paragraph 3 in the Discussion) and female (paragraph 1 of section 3 in the Results; paragraph 4 in the Discussion) mice. 

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      There are issues with use of the English language throughout the text. Some sentences are difficult to understand and should be clarified and/or synthesized.

      We appreciate the reviewer for constructive comments and helpful corrections.

      “Despite that 6 in 9 groups of mice display some extent of flipped ranking (Figures 6B-6G) and only 3 in 9 groups displayed continuously unaltered ranking (Figure 6H) during a total of 9 trials consisting of 3 trials of FPCT, 3 trials of tube test and 1 trial of WST, an obvious stable linear intragroup hierarchy was observed throughout all the trials and tasks"

      The above sentence has been re-written as: The ranking result showed that 6 in 9 groups of mice displayed some extent of flipped ranking (Figures 4B-4G), and only 3 in 9 groups displayed continuously unaltered ranking (Figure 4H). Averagely, in the totally 27 trials consisting of 12 trials of FPCT, 12 trials of tube test and 3 trials of WST, an obvious stable linear intragroup hierarchy was observed across all the trials and tasks (paragraph 1 of section 4 in the Results).

      "it is hard to attribute winning a competition in a shared space to stronger motivation rather than muscular superiority".

      The above sentence has been deleted and re-written in paragraph 1 of section 4 in the Results and paragraph 3 in the Discussion.

      "Unexpectedly, in most of the trials the mice preserved the winner or loser identity acquired in FPCT into tube test and WST (Figures 5L-5O)".

      Why this is unexpected? Instead, it looks like this result is expected (tube test has been successfully applied to identify ranks in females, see Leclair et al, eLife, 2021).

      We thank the reviewer for raising this point. FPCT is different from tube test and warm spot test at least in two aspects: competition for food vs space; presence vs absence of direct bodily interaction during competition. Some mice might be active in food competition, but not in space competition, while others might be on the contrary. Some mice might be good at physical contest, while others might be good at play tricks. Therefore, these factors made us expect task-specific outcomes of ranking results.

      Vocabulary issues:

      "Stereotypic", to talk about rank stability in a different context does not look appropriate. In behavioral neuroscience, stereotypy is more excepted to intend abnormal repetitive behaviors. The stability that the authors seem to indicate with the word "stereotype" refers rather to the concept of "consistency" or "stability".

      We thank the reviewer for this detailed explanation. We have chosen to use "stability" to describe the data.

      "Society", to talk about groups or colonies of animals sounds a bit odd. Society evokes more abstract concepts more likely to fit with human organization. I suggest the use of "group" or "colony".

      "Hide" to qualify the block preventing access to the food pellet. It is said that the block is transparent. We suggest the use of "inaccessible" instead of hidden.

      We strongly encourage the authors to further edit the entire script to improve language.

      Thank the reviewer for kind correction. We have corrected the above vocabulary misuse. 

      Technical issues / typos:

      Figure 1. The picture does not seem optimal to visualize the apparatus.

      Missing unit legend in Figure 4E.

      Supplementary videos 2 and 4 are missing.

      We have added a frontal view of the apparatus in the figure (Supplementary Figure 1), added a unit to the Figure 2F (previous Figure 4E), and we will make sure to upload the missing videos.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      While the assay shows promise as a tool for studying social dominance, the study suffers from some limitations such as lack of ethological relevance. In addition, there is a lack of rationale and methodological clarity in the manuscript that can impact the ability of other scientists to be able to perform this novel assay.

      (1) Related to lack of scientific rigor:

      a. In the first paragraph of the introduction, the authors mention that "disability in social recognition and unsatisfied social status are associated with brain diseases such as autism, depression and schizophrenia". Both papers that they cited refer to mouse models, not humans (which is the species that is attributed these diagnoses clinically). In addition, neither citation discusses schizophrenia. While social dysfunctions can indeed be related to these diseases, to my knowledge this is not caused by a change in "social status" and there is no human data with patient populations and social status. Therefore, this sentence is inaccurate and there is no research that demonstrates that.

      We thank the reviewer for raising this point. To express the opinion and cite literatures more accurately, we improved the sentence in the 1st paragraph of Introduction as follows: “Impaired awareness of social competition has been documented in individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD)4,5, and reduced social interaction has been characterized in corresponding animal models6. Similarly, maladaptive responses to social status loss has been associated with patient depressive disorders7,8 and animal models of depression1,9”. The reviewer is right that no patient disease is causally related with social status, and only depression has been proposedly associated with change of social status7,8.

      b. In the second paragraph of the introduction, the authors mention a scarcity of research papers with designs for food competition-based social hierarchy assays for mice. At least two such papers have been published in the past few years (DOIs https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-

      021-04000-5 and https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-022-04507-5). The authors should acknowledge the existence of these and other assays and discuss how their work would be related. In the same paragraph, they also mention that existing assays suffer from "hierarchy instability" and "complex calculations" without showing any citations or details for these claims.

      We thank the reviewer for raising this point. We acknowledged that there are some available food competitions to measure social hierarchy for mice. But relative to space competition, food competition tests have not been used so commonly and widely. No food competition paradigm has been accepted as generally as some space competition paradigms like tube test and warm spot test. To improve the language and scientific expression, we revised the sentences as follows: “Relative to space competition, food competition tests for mice have been designated and applied less commonly in animal studies despite its long history 28-30. Several issues could be thought to be the underlying limitations for the application of food competition paradigms. First, there are methodological issues in some of these approaches, such as long video recording duration and difficulty in analyzing animal’s behaviors during competitive physical interaction in videos, hindering their application by laboratories that cannot afford sophisticated equipment and analysis”. Corresponding citations have been updated (see paragraph 3 in the Introduction).

      c. The authors say that their study is the first to demonstrate that female mice follow social ranks. This is not the first study to do so and the authors should acknowledge existing publications that have done the same (eg DOI https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.71401).

      We have followed the reviewer’s suggestion to increase citations regarding social ranking of female mice tested by competition paradigms, especially food competition paradigms (see paragraph 1 of section 3 in the Results; paragraph 4 in the Discussion).

      (2) Related to problems with interpretation of data:

      a. The authors showed the assay works for females and males in pairwise housing, but two mice don't make a hierarchy, as hierarchies require a minimum of three individuals. Therefore, whether the assay works for females caged in three is an important question that is unaddressed in this study and is a caveat. extended the competition assay to male mice that are housed in cages of three. It would be important to show whether the assay generalizes well for female mice with this three-animal housing as well as discuss the effect of using even bigger groups of mice on the results of the assay.

      We thank the reviewer for raising questions related to the interpretation of data and giving us the insightful the suggestions. We agree that it is interesting and important to probe if FPCT works for a group of three female mice. Although social rankings of pairs of male and female mice were not significantly different (new Figure 2D-2F and 3F-3H), that of triads of male and female mice could be different. We have tested trads of male mice and found that the mice displayed an overall linear hierarchical ranking. We would like to use FPCT to investigate the rankings of trads of female mice and even bigger group of mice in the future. In the present manuscript we’d like to address the feasible application of the FPCT in smaller groups. In the Discussion, we add contents commenting group size effect on social competition tests (see paragraph 4 in the Discussion).

      b. The authors claim that "test 2" of their assay helps assert the motivation of mice for social competition as in Figure 4E. This could simply be a readout of how strong the mice are (muscle mass). To claim that this is indeed related to motivation during the FPCT assay, the authors should show the correlation of this readout with the latency to push the block during the social competition task.

      We appreciate the reviewer for raising this question. The dimensions establishing the social structures include physical and psychological factors. In the FPCT paradigm, the two contenders are separated so that physical factors are minimized in this context and psychological factors should play more important role in competition in comparison with previous reported food competition paradigms. Therefore, in the revised manuscript we consider to attribute the ranking results mainly to psychological factors, rather than only motivation which is just one of the numerous psychological factors (paragraph 3 of Discussion). Moreover, in the Discussion we point out that we could not exclude physical factors still participate in the determination of competitive outcomes since some of mice pairs pushed the block simultaneously (paragraph 3 of Discussion).

      c.The authors mention that they are interested to understand which factors lead to the outcome of the competition such as age, sex, physical strength, training level, and intensity of psychological motivation. However, in all their runs of the assay, they always matched these variables between the competitors. They should clarify that they were instead controlling for these variables. Another thing to note here is that while they controlled the body mass of the animals, that isn't the same as physical strength, as a lighter mouse can have more muscle mass than a heavier mouse. They should either specify this limitation or quantify the additional metric of "muscle mass" which is a much better proxy for physical strength. Thus, the claim that the outcome of the competition is solely affected by motivation is not convincing since they didn't rule out the others such as quantifying the rate of learning during training and strength.

      We thank the reviewer for addressing this question. As our response to the question in (c), we acknowledge that it is not accurate to ascribe the outcomes of FPCT to psychological motivation. In the revised manuscript, the dimensions of contributing factors to the outcomes of FPCT have been simplified to physical and psychological factors. We consider that the psychological factor could be the main driver of mice participating in FPCT (see paragraph 3 of Discussion).

      d. In the discussion, the authors mention that their task only requires a single day of food deprivation (the day before the first trial) while other assays suffer from a continued food deprivation protocol. However, the authors also use 10g per cage as the amount of food instead of giving them ad libitum access. Limited food is a food deprivation method. Thus, this is an inaccurate claim.

      We thank the reviewer for raising this point. We have clarified the requirement of food restriction for FPCT in the revision. The mice were deprived of food for 24 hours while water consumption remained normally to enhance the appeal of the food pellet to the mice. Then, after 24 hours of food deprivation, each cage of mice was given 10 g of food every morning to meet their daily food requirements until the end of the test (see FPCT procedure section in Methods and materials).

      e.In the second section of the results, the authors run their assay with female mice that are housed in cages of two. This section suffers from the same limitations as the first and can be improved by showing the training data, correlations of competition outcome with "motivation" and ruling out the other factors that could contribute to the outcome. Further, the authors saying that their FPCT assay is enough to show that female mice follow a social hierarchy by itself is a weak claim. They should instead include their cross-validation with the others to strengthen it.

      We appreciate the reviewer for raising this question. We have taken the reviewer’s suggestion to show the training data (Figures 1E, 2A and 3A). As the factors contributing to the outcomes of FPCT are diverse, we’d like not to control and determine the exact factor in the current manuscript. We agree with the reviewer that cross-validation with different paradigms is suggested for the studies to rank social hierarchy as the ranking results could be variable with tasks, procedures and operations.

      f.  In the last paragraph of the introduction, the authors mention how their assay involves "peaceful competition" since the mice are not in direct contact and hence cannot exhibit aggression. The authors do not address the limitation that a lack of physical contact actually makes the assay less ethological. Further, since the mice are housed in groups of two and three, it is not guaranteed that the mice will not be aggressive during their time in the home cage, which could affect their behavior during the competition assay. Whether the assay causes more aggression in the cage due to the lack of physical contact during the competition is not addressed in this study.

      We thank the reviewer for raising this point. Diverse factors affect the outcomes of a food competition test, some of which belong to psychological factors and others belong to physical factors. We agree that a lack of physical contact makes the assay less naturally ethological. However, when the social statuses have been established during habituation housing a group of mice for enough time, the win/lose outcomes in the FPCT could be a readout of the expression of social statuses since the mice cannot exhibit aggression in the test. We have revised the Introduction and Discussion (paragraph 3 of Discussion). Thank you.

      (3) Related to lack of methodological rigor and rationale clarity:

      a. In the first section of the results, the authors run their assay with male mice that are housed in cages of two. While the data that they display is promising, we do not see how mice change behavior across days of training and how that relates to the outcome of the competition. It would be valuable to also show the training data for the mice, answering questions related to competency and any inter-animal variabilities prior to rank assessment. Plotting the training data across all days would be helpful for the other parts of the results as well. This is especially important because the methods mention that mice are trained until they get to the criterium, so this means that different individuals get different amounts of training.

      We appreciate the reviewer for addressing the importance of showing training data. We have taken the reviewer’s suggestion and shown the training data (Figures 1E, 2A and 3A).

      b.  It is unclear why the assay was run only once per mouse pair per day since most protocols for the tube test involve multiple repetitions each day while alternating the side from which the mice enter. The authors should address whether a single trial per day is enough to show consistent results and that it wouldn't vary with more.

      We suggest to run the FPCT once or twice per mouse per day under conditions of mild food restriction, training and test procedures in this manuscript. Frequent tests might make the mice’s interest in the food pellet gradually diminished because the food supply was not fully deprived. According to our data, the outcomes of FPCT in 4 consecutive days were overall stable.

      c.  In the results the authors say that they "raised 3 male mice" which may be incorrect because they report in the methods buying the mice buy mice and they housed all their mice for only three days before running the assay which might be too little for the hierarchy to stabilize. The authors should comment on what was the range of the cohabitation across different cages and whether it had an impact on the results.

      According to our experiments, housing the mice for 3 days is enough to establish a mice social colony with relative stable status structure. Prolonged housing may produce either similar, stabler or more dynamic social colony.

      d. There are also some formatting and/or convention issues in the results. The first figure callout in the results is for Figure 4 instead of Figure 1 (which is the standard). This is because the authors do not explain how the mice are trained for the task in the results section and show limited data about the training of the task. Not showing comprehensive training data would make replication of this study very difficult.

      We appreciate the reviewer for raising this question. We have re-arranged the figures. The new arrangement of figures started with schematic drawing of FPCT procedure and training data (Figure 1).

      e. The authors don't report the exact p-values in the figures

      We reported the difference level in the figures in the revised manuscript. Thank you.

      4. The writing of the manuscript suffers from a lack of clarity in most sections of the manuscript.

      Here are several examples that are critical:

      a. In the title and abstract, it isn't clear what the authors mean by "stereotype". It could be a behavior during the competition, or that the social ranks across assays are correlated or that the rank for the new assay is consistent across days.

      b. There are several instances where the authors anthropomorphize mice using human features such as "urbanization" and "society" which are not established factors affecting mouse hierarchy. This further extends to anthropomorphizing mice in ways that are not standard such as an animal being "timid" or "bold" which would be hard to measure in mice, if not impossible.

      c. Across the social dominance literature, relative social rank is described using more general "dominant" and "subordinate" titles instead of "superior" and "inferior" that are sometimes used in the manuscript. The authors should follow the standard language so that readers understand.

      d.  In the third paragraph of the introduction, the authors say "Thus, it is more likely expected that different paradigms to weigh the social competency and status may lead to diverse readouts, given that competitive factors are included in competition paradigms." This sentence suffers from multiple syntax errors thereby reducing clarity

      e. There are several typos in the manuscript such as using "dominate" instead of "dominant", "grades" instead of "outcomes" and "forth" instead of "fourth", to give a few examples.

      We thank the reviewer for careful reading of the manuscript and very helpful comments. We have taken the above suggestions and improved the writing of the manuscript. For examples, "stereotype" was replaced by “stability”, mice "society" was expressed by "colony", the sentence “Thus, it is more.... in competition paradigms” has been deleted.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) The justification for the design of this new test paradigm is unclear. In the abstract, you state that the field needs a reliable, valid, and easily executable test. Your test provides this, as you state, but how is it better than the tube test? Does the tube test suffer from taskspecific win-or-lose outcomes? Can you provide evidence for this? The nature methods protocol for the tube test (https://doi.org/10.1038/s41596-018-0116-4) "strongly suggest using more than two dominance measures, for example, by also carrying out the warm spot test, or territory urine marking or ultrasonic courtship vocalization assays." This would suggest that results from the tube test can be task-specific, but I am not convinced that you have demonstrated that results from your food competition test are not task-specific. Indeed, by your title, one must run multiple tests.

      This same problem is apparent in the introduction. In the second paragraph, there is a discussion of the tube test, warm spot test, and food competition tests. What is the problem with these tests?

      I believe that social dominance relationships are complex and dynamic social relationships indicating who has priority access to a resource between multiple animals that live together. In these living situations, several resources can often be capitalized competed over-for example, space, food, mates, temperature, etc. Currently, we have tests to measure space via the tube test or urine marking, mates via ultrasonic vocalization, temperature via warm spot test, and food via food competition assays. The tube test, urine marking assay, and ultrasonic vocalization test have been demonstrated to be reliable, valid, and easily executable. However, the food competition assays are often difficult to execute because it is difficult to interpret the dominant behaviors and aggressive behaviors like bite wounding can occur during the test. Here, you present a new food competition assay to address these issues and show that it can be used in conjunction with other assays to measure social dominance across multiple resources easily. In doing so, you revealed that many same-sex groups of C57 mice have a stereotypic pattern of dominance behavior when competing across multiple types of resources: space, temperature, and food.

      I ask that you please rebut if you disagree with me, and adjust your abstract, introduction, and discussion accordingly.

      We thank the reviewer for all the constructive comments. We have adjusted the Abstract, Introduction and Discussion of the manuscript.

      We recognize and appreciate the valuable tube test, warm spot test and many other competition tests, including food competitions. Tube test and warm spot test are space competition tasks. Relative to space competition, food competition tests for mice have been designated and applied less commonly in animal studies. Several issues (such as methodological issue, aggressive behaviors occurring in competition, and prolonged food deprivation) could be thought to be the underlying limitations of the application of food competition paradigms (paragraph 3 in the Introduction). Therefore, we clarify that the justification for the design of FPCT was “to have a new choice of food competition paradigm for mice, and to facilitate the exposure of psychological aspects contributing to the winning/losing outcomes in competitions” (last paragraph in the Introduction).

      FPCT is different from tube test and warm spot test at least in two ways. FPCT is food completion task where the mice need no physical contact during competition, while tube test and WST are space competition tasks where the mice need direct physical contact during competition. Therefore, we expected inconsistent evaluation results of competitiveness and rankings if we compared FPCT with typically available competition paradigms—tube test and WST (last paragraph in the Introduction).

      (2)  The design of the test needs to be described before the results. You can either move the methods section before the results or add a paragraph in the introduction to better describe the test. Here, you can also reference Figures 1 through 3 so that the figures are presented in the order of which they are mentioned in the paper. (It is very confusing that the first reference to a figure is Figure 4, when it should be Figure 1).

      We appreciate the reviewer for raising this point and giving us suggestions. We have added a new section (section 1) in the Results. In the revised manuscript, the figures in the Results start with Figure 1 which shows schematic drawing of FPCT procedure, training data and some test results (Figure 1).

      (3)  The sentence describing Figure 4H. You argue that this shows that the mice are well and equally trained. It also shows that they have the same motivation or preference for the food.

      We appreciate the reviewer for this helpful comment. Data in previous Figures 4H and 5I have been presented as new Figures 2A and 3A, respectively, of revised manuscript. These retrospect analysis of training data displayed similar training level of food-getting and craving state for food (Sections 2 and 3 in the Results).

      (4)  "Social ranking of multiple cagemate mice using FPCT, tube test and WST"

      Here, you claim that "comparison of inter-task consistency revealed that the ranks evaluated by FPCT, tube test and WST did not differ from each other...Figure 6K." Okay, however, it is important to discuss the three cases when there wasn't consistency between the tests! Figure 6E-G.

      We appreciate the reviewer for raising this point. In the revised manuscript, we add description and discussion of inconsistent part of the different test paradigms (paragraph 2 in the section 3 of the Result, last 2 sentences of paragraph 4 in the Discussion)

      (5)  Replace all instances of "gender" with "sex". Animals do not have a gender.

      (6)  Adjust the strain of the mice to C57BL/6JNifdc.

      We have replaced "gender" with "sex" and “C57BL/6J” with “C57BL/6JNifdc”. Thank you for your careful correction.

      (7)  What is the justification for running the warm spot test for one day and the other tests for four days?

      From the consecutive FPCT and tube test, we already knew that the ranking results were overall stable. This stability was still observed in the day of warm spot test. A bad point for frequent warm spot test is that mice get much stress due to exposure in ice-cold environment. Therefore, we terminated the competition test after only one trial of warm spot test.

      (8)  Grammar

      The second sentence of the abstract: ...recognized as a valuable...

      Results, sentence after "...was observed (Figure 4G)." it should be "Fourth"

      We have corrected these and other grammar errors. We appreciate the reviewers for very careful review and all helpful comments.

    1. eLife Assessment

      In this study Wang et. al. mined publicly available RNA-seq data from The Genotype-Tissue Expression (GTEx) database spanning multiple tissues to ask the question of how transcriptomes are changed with age and in both sexes. The authors provide solid evidence reporting widespread gene expression changes and alternative splicing events that vary in an age- and sex-dependent manner. An important finding is that many of these changes coincide with the time sex hormones begin to decline; additionally, the rate of aging is faster in males than in females.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, Wang et al analyze ~17,000 transcriptomes from 35 human tissues from the GTEx database and address transcriptomic variations due to age and sex. They identified both gene expression changes as well as alternative splicing events that differ among sexes. Using breakpoint analysis, the authors find sex dimorphic shifts begin with declining sex hormone levels with males being affected more than females. This is an important pan-tissue transcriptomic study exploring age and sex-dependent changes although not the first one.

      Strengths:

      (1) The authors use sophisticated modeling and statistics for differential, correlational and predictive analysis.

      (2) The authors consider important variables such as genetic background, ethnicity, sampling bias, sample sizes, detected genes etc.

      (3) This is likely the first study to evaluate alternative splicing changes with age and sex at a pan-tissue scale.

      (4) Sex dimorphism with age is an important topic and is thoroughly analyzed in this study.

    3. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this study, Wang et al utilized the available GTEx data to compile a comprehensive analysis that attempt to reveal aging-related sex-dimorphic gene expression as well as alternative splicing changes in human. The key conclusions based upon their analysis are that 1) extensive sex-dimorphisms during aging with distinct patterns of change in gene expression and alternative splicing (AS), and 2) the male-biased age-associated AS events have a stronger association with Alzheimer's disease, and 3) the females-biased events are often regulated by several sex-biased splicing factors that may be controlled by estrogen receptors. They further performed break-point analysis and reveal in males there are two main breakpoints around ages 35 and 50, while in female only one breakpoint at 45.

      Strengths:

      This study sets an ambitious goal, leveraging the extensive GTEx dataset to investigate aging-related, sex-dimorphic gene expression and alternative splicing changes in humans. The research addresses a significant question, as our understanding of sex-dimorphic gene expression in the context of human aging is still in its early stages. Advancing our knowledge of these molecular changes is vital for identifying therapeutic targets for age-related diseases and extending human healthspan. The study is highly comprehensive, and the authors are commendable for their attempted thorough analysis of both gene expression and alternative splicing-an area often overlooked in similar studies.

    4. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews: 

      Reviewer #1 (Public review): 

      Summary:  

      Wang et al. investigate sexual dimorphic changes in the transcriptome of aged humans. This study relies upon analysis of the Genotype-Tissue Expression dataset that includes 54 tissues from human donors. The authors investigate 17,000 transcriptomes from 35 tissues to investigate the effect of age and sex on transcriptomic variation, including the analysis of alternative splicing. Alternative splicing is becoming more appreciated as an influence in the aging process, but how it is affected by sexual dimorphism is still largely unclear. The authors investigated multiple tissues but ended up distilling brain tissue down to four separate regions: decision, hormone, memory, and movement. Building upon prior work, the authors used an analysis method called principal component-based signal-to-variation ratio (pcSVR) to quantify differences between sex or age by considering data dispersion. This method also considers differentially expressed genes and alternative splicing events. 

      Strengths:  

      (1) The authors investigate sexual dimorphism on gene expression and alternative splicing events with age in multiple tissues from a large publicly available data set that allows for reanalysis. 

      (2) Furthermore, the authors take into account the ethnic background of donors. Identification of agingmodulating genes could be useful for the reanalysis of prior data sets. 

      Weaknesses:  

      The models built off of the GTEx dataset should be tested in another data set (ex. Alzheimer's disease) where there are functional changes that can be correlated. Gene-length-dependent transcription decline, which occurs with age and disease, should also be investigated in this data set for potential sexual dimorphism. 

      We appreciate the reviewer’s constructive feedback and acknowledgment of the strengths of our study. The detailed results are included in the ‘Recommendations for the authors’ from the editorial office. Below we summarize our feedback that address the concerns of this reviewer:

      (1) Independent Alzheimer’s disease (AD) datasets:

      We acknowledge the importance of validating our models beyond GTEx to assess their generalizability aging to Alzheimer’s disease. While GTEx provides valuable transcriptomic data across multiple tissues, it lacks direct functional assessments linked to disease states. We have already analyzed RNA-seq data from ROSMAP and GEO in Figure 4, focusing on sex-biased gene expression and splicing changes between aging and AD.  The results showed a male-biased association with Alzheimer’s disease at AS resolution, indicating that the AS changes during aging could contribute more to AD in males than females. We added a highlight to this analysis in the manuscript (Pages 6-7).

      (2) Sexual dimorphism in Gene-Length-Dependent Transcription Decline (GLTD) 

      We appreciate the reviewer’s suggestion to explore gene-length-dependent transcription decline (GLTD), which has been implicated in both aging and disease. As the reviewer suggested, our analysis revealed that GLTD exhibits sex-biased patterns in different tissues, aligning with recent literature on sex-dimorphic transcriptional aging. Our findings also revealed that longer genes with greater transcriptional decline are enriched in AD-related pathways. We have incorporated this new analysis in the ‘Recommendations for the authors’ in Author response image 5-6 and expanded the discussion of the biological relevance. 

      Reviewer #2 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      In this manuscript, Wang et al analyze ~17,000 transcriptomes from 35 human tissues from the GTEx database and address transcriptomic variations due to age and sex. They identified both gene expression changes as well as alternative splicing events that differ among sexes. Using breakpoint analysis, the authors find sex dimorphic shifts begin with declining sex hormone levels with males being affected more than females. This is an important pan-tissue transcriptomic study exploring age and sex-dependent changes although not the first one. 

      Strengths:  

      (1) The authors use sophisticated modeling and statistics for differential, correlational, and predictive analysis. 

      (2) The authors consider important variables such as genetic background, ethnicity, sampling bias, sample sizes, detected genes, etc. 

      (3) This is likely the first study to evaluate alternative splicing changes with age and sex at a pan-tissue scale. 

      (4) Sex dimorphism with age is an important topic and is thoroughly analyzed in this study.  Weaknesses:  

      (1) The findings have not been independently validated in a separate cohort or through experiments. Only selective splicing factor regulation has been verified in other studies. 

      (2) It seems the authors have not considered PMI or manner of death as a variable in their analysis. 

      (3) The manuscript is very dense and sometimes difficult to follow due to many different types of analyses and correlations. 

      (4) Short-read data can detect and quantify alternative splicing events with only moderate confidence and therefore the generalizability of these findings remains to be experimentally validated. 

      We appreciate the thorough review and thoughtful feedback. We have addressed the reviewer’s concerns and added clarification. The detailed results are included in Recommendations for the authors. Here are the summaries.

      (1) Challenge of independent validation in separate cohorts

      • The GTEx dataset includes the most comprehensive transcriptome resource for studying population-level differences in age and sex across tissues, particularly including large-scale brain samples. This provides a unique opportunity to analyze sex-dimorphic aging and the relevance of age-associated diseases.  Several technical issues, including cell type heterogeneity, postmortem artifacts, as well as sequencing biases, lead to technical challenges in different cohorts.

      • As the reviewer mentioned, we analyzed transcriptomic data from Shen et al. (2024) and compared them with GTEx results (Author response image 2). Limited overlap in differentially expressed genes again highlighted the challenges in cross-dataset validation due to the differences in cell composition and data processing (peripheral blood mononuclear cells (PBMCs) vs whole blood). 

      • Due to the limited human brain transcriptome data covering different age and sex groups, we found mouse hippocampus datasets from Mass spectrometry (MS), including young and old, as well as female and male groups.  The results validated the expression of splicing factors in brain (Author response image 9). This cross-species consistency supports the robustness of our findings in human brain aging.

      (2) Effects of Postmortem Interval, Manner of Death, and Time of Death

      • We agree that the sample collections could introduce confounding effects. To address this, we calculated the correlations between the confounding factors with Postmortem Interval (PMI), Manner of Death (DTHMNNR), or Time of Death (DTHTIME and DTHSEASON). We observed strong correlations in some surrogate variables in most tissues, indicating that those factors could be well-regressed during our analysis (Recommendations for the authors, Figure S4 and R8). 

      • In addition, we re-evaluated our analyses while incorporating PMI as a covariate in our models. Our results align with our initial findings (Author response image 1), suggesting that age- and sex-dependent transcriptomic changes are not strongly confounded by PMI and confirming that our model has controlled PMI. These results are detailed in ‘Recommendations for the authors’ and included in Figure S4C-E with the description in text, Page 5. 

      (3) Readability of manuscript and flow of analyses

      • In summary, our study first examined global alternative splicing (AS) and gene expression (GE) across all tissues before focusing on specific regions for deeper insights. To improve clarity, we have made the following revisions:

      • Add clearer statements when transitioning between all-tissue and brain-specific analyses (Page 6-7).

      • Modify the subtitle of Results to highlight all-tissue vs. brain analyses (Page 6).

      • These refinements could enhance the manuscript’s structure, making the flow of analysis and conclusions more intuitive for readers.

      (4) Limitations of short-read RNA-seq for splicing analysis

      • Short-read RNA-seq provides only moderate confidence in detecting and quantifying full-length isoforms. However, its higher sequencing depth makes it more suitable for quantifying changes in alternative splicing (AS) events.

      • Our analysis focused on splicing event-level quantification, applying stringent filters and using our GPU-based tool, which showed strong concordance with RT-PCR and other pipelines. Therefore, we also cited and included the updated Paean manuscript that benchmarks its performance in AS analysis.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review): 

      Summary:  

      In this study, Wang et al utilized the available GTEx data to compile a comprehensive analysis that attempt to reveal aging-related sex-dimorphic gene expression as well as alternative splicing changes in humans. 

      The key conclusions based on their analysis are that. 

      (1) extensive sex-dimorphisms during aging with distinct patterns of change in gene expression and alternative splicing (AS), and 

      (2) the male-biased age-associated AS events have a stronger association with Alzheimer's disease, and  (3) the female-biased events are often regulated by several sex-biased splicing factors that may be controlled by estrogen receptors. They further performed break-point analysis and revealed that in males there are two main breakpoints around ages 35 and 50, while in females, there is only one breakpoint at 45. 

      Strengths:  

      This study sets an ambitious goal, leveraging the extensive GTEx dataset to investigate aging-related, sexdimorphic gene expression and alternative splicing changes in humans. The research addresses a significant question, as our understanding of sex-dimorphic gene expression in the context of human aging is still in its early stages. Advancing our knowledge of these molecular changes is vital for identifying therapeutic targets for age-related diseases and extending the human health span. The study is highly comprehensive, and the authors are commendable for their attempted thorough analysis of both gene expression and alternative splicing - an area often overlooked in similar studies. 

      We thank this reviewer for the insightful review and recognition of our study's significance.  We agree with the reviewer on how to examine sex-dimorphic gene expression and alternative splicing in aging by using the GTEx dataset.  This is indeed an essential aspect of developing potential therapeutic targets for agerelated diseases to promote human health span.

      Weaknesses:  

      Due to the inherent noise within the GTEx dataset - which includes numerous variables beyond aging and sex - there are significant technical concerns surrounding this study. Additionally, the lack of crossvalidation with independent, existing data raises questions about whether the observed gene expression changes genuinely reflect those associated with human aging. For instance, the break-point analysis in this study identifies two major breakpoints in males around ages 35 and 50, and one breakpoint in females at age 45; however, these findings contradict a recent multi-omics longitudinal study involving 108 participants aged 25 to 75 years, where breakpoint at 44 and 60 years was observed in both male and females (Shen et al, 2024). These issues cast doubt on the robustness of the study's conclusions. Specific concerns are outlined below: 

      References: 

      Ferreira PG, Muñoz-Aguirre M, Reverter F, Sá Godinho CP, Sousa A, Amadoz A, Sodaei R, Hidalgo MR, Pervouchine D, Carbonell-Caballero J et al (2018) The effects of death and post-mortem cold ischemia on human tissue transcriptomes. Nature Communications 9: 490. 

      Shen X, Wang C, Zhou X, Zhou W, Hornburg D, Wu S, Snyder MP (2024) Nonlinear dynamics of multiomics profiles during human aging. Nature Aging. 

      Wucher V, Sodaei R, Amador R, Irimia M, Guigó R (2023) Day-night and seasonal variation of human gene expression across tissues. PLOS Biology 21: e3001986. 

      (1) The primary method used in this study is linear regression, incorporating age, sex, and age-by-sex interactions as covariates, alongside other confounding factors (such as ethnicity) as unknown variables. However, the analysis overlooks two critical known variables in the GTEx dataset: time of death (TOD) and postmortem interval (PMI). Both TOD and PMI are recorded for each sample and account for substantial variance in gene expression profiles. A recent study by Wucher et al.(Wucher et al, 2023) demonstrated the powerful impact of TOD on gene expression by using it to reconstruct human circadian and even circannual datasets. Similarly, Ferreira et al. (Ferreira et al, 2018) highlighted PMI's influence on gene expression patterns. Without properly adjusting for these two variables, confidence in the study's conclusions remains limited at best. 

      We appreciate the reviewer for raising this important point regarding the impact of post-mortem interval (PMI) and time of death (TOD) on gene expression, including the death seasons (DTHSEASON) and daytime (DTHTIME). To address this point, we carefully evaluated whether our linear model controlled for these factors as potential confounders. 

      Our results showed that PMI and TOD significantly correlated with the estimated covariates in most tissues, suggesting that their effects could be effectively regressed out using our model (Figure S4).  As the reviewers and editors suggested, we have now included this correlation analysis in the updated Figure S4C-E and the text in the Results section, citing relevant literature [1,2] (Page 5). 

      Author response image 1.

      The results of differential gene expression analysis with vs without the inclusion of PMI correction as a known covariate. The scatter plots show the correlations of significance levels (pvalues, left panel) and effect sizes (coefficients, right panel) of sex (A) and age (B). Whole-blood tissue is used as an example.

       

      In addition, we did the differential analysis that incorporated PMI as a covariate in the regression models and re-evaluated the age- and sex-related transcriptomic changes. Using WholeBlood gene expression as an example, our revised analysis shows that the inclusion of PMI in the covariates has minimal impact on the significance levels and effects of sex and age (i.e., p-values and coefficients, respectively), indicating that our findings are robust using confounding factors (Author response image 1). 

      (2) To demonstrate that their analysis is robust and that the covariates TOD and PMI are otherwise negligible - the authors should cross-validate their findings with independent datasets to confirm that the identified gene expression changes are reproducible for some tissues. For instance, the recent study by Shen et al. (Shen et al., 2024) in Nature Aging offers an excellent dataset for cross-validation, particularly for blood samples. Comparing the GTEx-derived results with this longitudinal transcriptome dataset would enable verification of gene expression changes at both the individual gene and pathway levels. Without such validation, confidence in the study's conclusions remains limited. 

      We thank the reviewer for the insightful suggestion regarding cross-validation with independent datasets. We understand that validating findings across datasets is crucial for ensuring robustness. As the reviewers suggested, we see whether there are some shared findings in the GTEx data with the study by Shen et al. (2024) in Nature Aging. However, after performing comparisons with our GTEx results in whole blood tissue, we found that the overlaps of differentially expressed genes are limited (Fig. 3). In our results, we found a large proportion of age-associated genes in the GTEx data, whereas just 54 genes are age-associated from Shen et al.’s PBMC data. 3 in 7 genes are differentially expressed in both datasets (Fig. 3A). Additionally, we performed the functional enrichment analysis on the GTEx-specific age-associated genes.

      We observed a strong enrichment in the biological pathways related to neutrophil functions and innate immune responses, which are specific to the cell compositions in whole blood rather than PBMC (Fig. 3B).

      Author response image 2.

      The comparison between the gene expression of whole blood tissue from GTEx and PBMCs from Shen et al. (A) The bar plot shows the number of age (left panel) or sex-associated  (right panel) genes in the two datasets. The grey bars highlight the proportion of overlapped genes in both datasets. (B) The top 10 significantly enriched biological processes in the GTEx-specific age-associated genes. The color bar shows the number of age-associated genes in specific pathways.

      These discrepancies highlighted the crucial factors in cross-dataset comparison:

      • Cell compositions: GTEx used whole blood, which contains all blood components, including neutrophils and erythrocytes, whereas PBMCs contain lymphocytes and monocytes. Under the influence of granulocytes and red blood cells in whole blood, the gene expression profiles between these two datasets are different.

      • Biological functions: Whole blood includes both innate and adaptive immune components; thus, aging-related gene expression changes in whole blood may include a broader systemic response than those in PBMCs. This difference in biological context contributes to the observed variation in the differentially expressed genes, as demonstrated by our functional enrichment analysis (Fig. 3B). 

      • Sequencing biases and data processing: The two datasets were generated using different RNAseq processing pipelines, including distinct normalization, batch correction, and quantification methodologies. These technical differences may introduce systematic variations that complicate direct cross-validation.

      Due to these fundamental problems, a direct one-to-one validation between the two datasets is challenging. We understand the importance of independent dataset validation and appreciate the reviewer’s suggestion. However, future studies could be performed more precisely if comparable whole-blood-based datasets are available. In addition, GTEx data provides nearly thousands of samples in whole blood, which is a largescale, comprehensive, and clinically relevant dataset for studying aging-related changes, particularly in innate immunity and inflammation, which are not well captured in PBMCs.

      (3) As a demonstration of the lack of such validation, in the Shen et al. study (Shen et al., 2024), breakpoints at 44 and 60 years were observed in both males and females, while this study identifies two major breakpoints in males around ages 35 and 50, and one breakpoint in females at age 45. What caused this discrepancy? 

      We thank the reviewer and the editors for both coming up with the non-linear multi-omic aging patterns observed by Shen et al.  They observed two prominent crests around the ages of 45 and 60 from omics data.

      Similarly, we also identified two breakpoints in our analysis, with some differences in specific age breakpoints. These could be the result of sample preparation methods and breakpoint definition. These responses are also included in the editor’s recommendations.

      Definition of breakpoints vs crests:

      • Crests represent age-related molecular changes at each time point across the human lifespan. They indicate the number of molecules that are differentially expressed during aging (q < 0.05), without considering individual expression levels.

      • Our breakpoints, in contrast, are identified after filtering the chronological trends using the Autoregressive Integrated Moving Average (ARIMA) model. We calculated the rate of change at each age point using the smooth approach and sliding windows. Breakpoints are defined as local maxima where the distance to the nearest minimum, relative to the global maximum. We indeed found some local wide peaks around 60 in some tissues, shown in Figure S10, however, we excluded these due to our strict cutoffs to remove noise.

      Differences and similarities between sequenced tissues: 

      • Whole-blood vs PBMC: In the GTEx RNA-seq data used in our study, whole blood samples from donors were sequenced, whereas their study used PBMCs. Whole blood contains all blood components, including red blood cells, platelets, granulocytes (e.g., neutrophils), lymphocytes, and monocytes, while PBMCs represent a subset of white blood cells, primarily consisting of lymphocytes (T cells, B cells, NK cells) and monocytes, excluding granulocytes and erythrocytes. As we mentioned in the previous responses, the gene expression changes observed in whole blood capture the contributions of neutrophils and other granulocytes, which are neglected in the PBMC profile (also shown in Figure S11C). 

      • For the shared tissues in two studies – skin, we looked at the non-linear changes during aging and found the same two breakpoints: 43 and 58. 

      Novelties in our study:

      • Whole blood can serve as a readily accessible resource for testing age-related disease biomarkers without cell separation, making it more practical for clinical applications.

      • Our analysis was performed on females and males, respectively. The main object of our analysis is to compare the differences in aging rates between sexes. Our results reveal clear sex-specific differences across multiple human tissues. Therefore, the identified breakpoints may differ when sex effects are not taken into account, highlighting the specificity of our analysis. 

      • Additionally, our breakpoints are integrated across multiple tissues. Our results showed that there is a large diversity of aging patterns in different tissues.

      As the reviewers and editors suggested, we have added the following statements to clarify this distinction in the Discussion section: ‘Our analysis observed the non-linear aging patterns with two breakpoints, which is consistent with recent findings, with differences in specific age points due to sex differences as well as tissue diversities 3.’ (Page 14), and ‘These breakpoints could represent key junctures in the aging process that align with the non-linear patterns of aging and disease progression.’ (Page 15)

      (4) Although the alternative splicing analysis is intriguing, the authors did not differentiate between splicing events that alter the protein-coding sequence and those that do not. Many splicing changes occurring in the 5' UTR and 3' UTR regions do not impact protein coding, so it is essential to filter these out and focus specifically on alternative splicing events that can modify protein-coding sequences. 

      The reviewer raises an important point. In our study, we included the AS events in protein-coding genes to gain a comprehensive understanding of sex-biased age-associated splicing. As the reviewer suggested, focusing on coding-sequence-altering events is particularly relevant to protein function. To address this, we performed an additional analysis to specifically annotate sBASEs occurring within the coding sequence (defeined as CDS-altering sBASEs) and reanalyzed their functional pathways and AD-associations (Author response image 3).  

      Our analysis revealed that most of the sBASEs are relevant to protein-coding sequences (CDS) across multiple tissues (Author response image 3A).  We then confirmed our findings using CDS-altering sBASEs. We found that those sBASEs in brain regions were significantly enriched in pathways related to amyloid-beta formation and actin filament organization (Author response image 3B). Notably, male-biased sBASEs in decision-related brain regions were particularly associated with dendrite development and regulation of cell morphogenesis, highlighting the sex-specific roles of sBASEs in brain functions. Additionally, we performed a random forest classification using only CDS-altering sBASEs in AD datasets (Author response image 3C-D), again confirming the malebiased association between aging and AD.

      Overall, we found that most of the identified sBASEs could modify protein-coding sequences, and our main conclusions remain consistent even after filtering out non-coding events. 

      Nevertheless, in addition to AS events that impact protein sequences, alternative splicing in untranslated regions (UTRs) also plays a critical regulatory role. Splicing events in the 5′ UTR can influence translation efficiency by modifying upstream open reading frames (uORFs) or RNA secondary structures, while splicing in the 3′UTR can affect mRNA stability, localization, and translation by altering microRNA binding sites and RNA-binding protein interactions. Given these functional implications, we believe that UTR-targeted AS events should also be considered to supplement the understanding of post-transcriptional gene regulation in future research.

      Author response image 3.

      The distribution and functional relevance of sBASEs with coding effects. (A) The number of sBASEs and CDS-altering sBASEs across multiple tissues. The deeper bars show the number of sBASEs whose alternative splice sites are located at protein-coding regions. (B) GO biological pathways in each sex and brain region. Heatmap shows the sex-specific pathways that are significantly enriched by CDS-altering sBASEs in more than 2 brain regions and sex. (C) Correlation between ADassociated and age-associated AS changes across the CDS-altering sBASEs that alter protein-coding sequences in females and males. (D) Performances of sex-stratified models predicted by CDS-altering sBASEs in 100 iterations using the random forest approach

      (5) One of the study's main conclusions - that "male-biased age-associated AS events have a stronger association with Alzheimer's disease" - is not supported by the data presented in Figure 4A, which shows an association with "regulation of amyloid precursor formation" only in female, not male, alternative splicing genes. Additionally, the gene ontology term "Alzheimer's disease" is absent from the unbiased GO analysis in Figure S6. These discrepancies suggest that the focus on Alzheimer's disease may reflect selective data interpretation rather than results driven by an unbiased analysis. 

      We thank the reviewer for this point. In our functional analysis, we identified distinct biological processes enriched in female- and male-biased AS genes, such as the regulation of amyloid precursor formation in females and structural constituents of the cytoskeleton in males. However, Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is a complex neurodegenerative disorder with multiple pathological mechanisms beyond amyloid-beta (Aβ) formation, many of which are strongly age-related in both sexes. This complexity motivates us to explore novel relationships between splicing and AD in distinct sexes.

      Although Figure 4A shows the enrichment of “regulation of amyloid precursor formation” in female-biased AS events, this does not contradict the broader enrichment of AD-related processes in male-biased AS events. Our disease ontology analysis supports this finding, as male-biased age-associated AS events are enriched in neurodegenerative diseases, including cognitive disorders. Additionally, we considered not only individual GO terms but also the disease-associated transcriptomic signatures from AD-related datasets, which collectively indicate a stronger association in males. 

      Regarding Figure S6 mentioned by the reviewer, the GO term “Alzheimer’s disease” is not explicitly listed in the heatmap because we filtered the pathways that are consistently enriched in multiple tissues. As noted in the figure legend, we only displayed sex-specific GO terms that were significant in at least 15 tissues. Then, since the brain is highly affected by age-related processes and neurological conditions show sex differences, the sex-biased AS events could help explain differential susceptibility to age-related cognitive decline and neurodegeneration. That’s why we chose the brain data for detailed analysis.

      To improve clarity, we have revised the text to describe the purpose of our analysis in brain rather than other tissues (Page 6-7). We appreciate the reviewer’s feedback, and we will consider additional analyses to further explore the sex-biased AS as well as disease risk in other tissues.

      (6) The experimental data presented in Figures 5E - I merely demonstrate that estrogen receptor regulates the expression of two splicing factors, SRSF1 and SRSF7, in an estradiol-dependent manner. However, this finding does not support the notion that this regulation actually contributes to sex-dimorphic alternative splicing changes during human aging. Notably, the authors do not provide evidence that SRSF1 and SRSF7 expression changes actually occur in a sex-dependent manner with human aging (in a manner similar to TIA1). As such, this experimental dataset is disconnected from the main focus of the study and does not substantiate the conclusions on sex-dimorphic splicing during human aging. The authors performed RNAseq in wild-type and ER mutant cells, and they should perform a comprehensive analysis of ER-dependent alternative splicing and compare the results with the GTEx data. It should be straightforward. 

      Thanks for the reviewer’s feedback. The main purpose of the analyses in Figures 5E-I was to explore which factors affect the sex-biased expression of splicing factors during aging and substantially regulate alternative splicing (AS). To address the reviewer’s concerns, we have included additional analysis and explained the challenge of linking estrogen receptor (ER)-regulated splicing factors to sex-dimorphic AS changes during human aging in specific human cell types. 

      • As suggested by the reviewer, we first examined the expression changes of SRSF1 and SRSF7 during aging in males and females, like TIA1 in decision-related brain regions (Fig. 5I).

      • Secondly, the regulation is based on a highly complex regulatory network involving multiple splicing factors and cell heterogeneity. Due to these complexities, we did not overlap ER-dependent AS changes with sBASEs from GTEx datasets directly. As far as the reviewer is concerned, we supplemented the AS analysis in the GSE89888 dataset (Fig. 5H) and identified the estrogenregulated AS events mediated by ESR1. We found that ~6% (26/396) of female-specific ageassociated AS events were regulated by ESR1, of which 6 sBASEs can be regulated by femalebiased splicing factors. The low overlaps could be represented by the limited coverage of different RNA-seq datasets and cell types used across these analyses. Notably, the results indicated that only a fraction of AS could be directly accounted for by estrogen via ESR1, suggesting the complexity of transcriptional and splicing regulatory networks during aging. 

      • Meanwhile, we downloaded independent experimental datasets to discover the regulation by our candidate splicing factors. Due to SRSF1 is identified as a potential regulator of sex-biased splicing, we analyzed RNA-seq data with SRSF1 knock-down (KD) glioblastoma cell lines (U87MG and U251), a type of brain cancer formed from astrocytes that support nerve cells 4.  As a result, we indeed found that some sBASEs are regulated by SRSF1 during aging through this experiment using brain cell lines (Author response image 4). Together, these results suggested that some of the SF-RNA regulatory relationships can be observed in another cellular system, further supporting our findings. 

      Due to the limitations of cell-based models and the complexity in the splicing regulatory network, it is challenging to directly validate aging regulation, particularly between different sexes, based on ER treatments in vivo. However, our findings still provide valuable mechanistic insights into ER-regulated splicing factors, implying their potential role in sex-biased aging.

      Author response image 4.

      SRSF1 regulations on specific sBASEs using SRSF1 knock-down RNA-seq data in GBM cells. Three examples are shown to be regulated during aging with significant changes between SRSF1 KD vs control in U251 and U87MG cell lines. The splicing diagrams are shown below.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      The authors found that alternative splicing was affected by both sex and age across many tissues, with gene expression differences affected by both parameters only present in some tissues. This trend was consistent when the effects of sex chromosomes were subtracted from the analysis. The effect of aging on differential gene expression and alternative splicing was more prevalent in male than female samples. For analysis purposes, young subjects were deemed to be anyone under 40, and old subjects were over 60 years old. The authors then investigated if specific genes or alternative splicing events were responsible for these effects. Some candidate genes or splicing events were identified but there was little overlap between tissues, suggesting no universal gene or event as a driver of aging. Surrogate variables like the ethnic backgrounds of donors were also investigated. Ultimately the authors found that alternative splicing events showed a stronger sexual dimorphic effect with age than did differential gene expression and that at least for the brain, alternative splicing changes showed a bias for Alzheimer's disease in male samples. This was highlighted by examples of exon skipping in SCL43A2 and FAM107A in males that were associated respectively with plaques and tangles. 

      The authors go on to identify sexual dimorphic differences in splicing factors in particular brain regions during age. Finally, the authors performed analysis for aging-modulated genes, identifying nearly 1000 across the tissues, nearly 70% of which are sex-specific. Their work suggests that further analysis of these aging-modulated genes could be differentially modulating the transcriptome based on sex. The work is novel and interesting, especially investigating sexual dimorphism in alternative splicing. However, the work is still preliminary, and these assumptions need to be applied to other data sets beyond GTEx for validation as well as some other phenomena that need to be considered. I recommend major revisions to address the points below. 

      (1) At the beginning of the results section, the authors state that the brain is stratified into four functional regions. It would be useful to explicitly state those four regions in the text at that point. 

      We agree that specifying these regions early in the text will improve clarity and provide the reader with a clear understanding of the analysis. As the reviewer’s suggestion, we revised the Results section (Page 3) to explicitly state the four functional brain regions as follows: ‘Due to data sparseness, the brain tissues were recombined into four functional regions (table S1), including hormone- or emotion-related region, movement-related region, memory-related region, and decision-related region (See Methods).’. This ensures that the regions are clearly defined before the subsequent analysis is presented. 

      (2) The manuscript becomes a bit confusing when the authors shift from all the tissues as a whole specifically to the brain and then back to the larger tissue set to make assumptions. This can be a bit confusing and should be better delineated.

      We thank the reviewer and editor for the feedback regarding the transitions between the analysis of all tissues and the brain-specific analysis. In our study, we first conducted a broad analysis of alternative splicing (AS) and gene expression (GE) across all tissues. For the AS analyses, we did sBASEs analysis in all tissues and then focused on specific tissue (i.e., brain) whose splicing changes are functionally enriched with age-related diseases.  For the GE analyses, we also analyzed the aging rate across tissues and identified the tissue-specific/shared patterns. 

      We agree that the shifts of the tissues for AS and GE may cause some confusion, and have made the following revisions to delineate why we focused on different tissues for distinct analyses:

      • We have added clear statements to better delineate when we shift focus from the analysis of all tissues to the region-specific analysis and vice versa. For instance, in the Results section (Page 67), we include a transitional phrase: ‘Having established patterns across all tissues, we now turn to a more focused analysis to investigate tissue-specific alternative splicing changes.’

      • To improve the overall structure, we have reorganized the Results section, adding distinct subheadings for the analysis of all tissues and the brain (Page 6), which should make the transition between these sections smoother and more intuitive for the reader.

      We believe that these revisions will make the manuscript’s structure clearer and allow the reader to better follow the flow of the analysis and the subsequent conclusions.

      (3) Gene-length-dependent transcription decline (GLTD) is another phenomenon that occurs with aging and is known to be associated with Alzheimer's disease [PMID38519330]. The authors should make some statement if this is present in their dataset and if any sexual dimorphism in tissues is present. 

      We thank the editors and reviewers for bringing up the possible connection of gene-length-dependent transcription decline (GLTD), which was reported to be associated with both aging and Alzheimer’s disease (AD). We appreciate the reviewer’s suggestion and have addressed whether GLTD is present in our dataset and whether any sex differences are observed in this context.

      We evaluated GLTD using the correlation between gene length with age-associated changes (i.e., the coefficients of the ‘age’ term in the linear regression model) in GTEx data. We did observe strong evidence of GLTD, particularly in the brain, heart, muscle, pancreas, spleen, skin, muscle, etc (Author response image 5A). In brain, we performed the functional enrichment analysis on the genes with Foldchange > 2 and length > 10<sup>5</sup> bp (Author response image 5B). We found that these extremely long genes are significantly relevant to synapse and neuron functions. These findings align with previous studies showing that GLTD can occur with aging in the tissues that are relevant to Alzheimer’s disease, cardiovascular diseases, and common failures of metabolism (e.g., diabetes) [5,6]. Additionally, it was not a ubiquitous phenomenon across all tissues. The correlations could be positive in tissues like adipose and artery.  These findings suggested the GLTD could be varied and tissuespecific in its manifestation during aging. 

      Author response image 5.

      (A) The correlation between gene length and age-associated changes across GTEx tissues in human samples. The correlation tests are evaluated using Spearman’s approach. The color bar indicates the -log10 transformed p-values in the correlation test. (B) The results of GO enrichment analysis using the genes with Foldchange > 2 and length > 10<sup>5</sup> bp. The parent terms calculated by ‘rrvgo’ with a similarity threshold of 0.9 are shown.

      Regarding sexual dimorphism, we conducted this analysis in females and males, respectively (Author response image 6). We found GLTD exists in both females and males in most tissues, such as brain, whole blood, muscle, etc, consistent with the previous results without considering the sex groups. Interestingly, we observed sexbiased patterns in certain tissues. In particular, the left ventricle, pancreas, and hippocampus showed notable male-biased patterns in the degree of transcriptional decline with gene length, whereas skin, liver, small intestine, and esophagus showed that in females. These findings suggest that GLTD could be relevant to aging and age-related diseases; the levels of expression and sexual dimorphism may vary depending on the tissue type. We hope this clarification addresses the reviewer’s concern and provides a more comprehensive understanding of the GLTD and sex differences observed in our dataset. 

      Author response image 6.

      The correlation between gene length and age-associated changes across tissues in females and males, respectively. The correlation tests are evaluated using the Spearman’s approach. The red dots indicate the significant correlations in females, while the navy dots show those in males.

      (4) Because the majority of this work has been performed in the GTEx dataset, applying this analysis to another publicly available dataset would be useful validation. For instance, the authors have interesting findings in the brain and correlations to Alzheimer's disease. Analysis of an existing RNAseq dataset from Alzheimer's disease patients and controls (with functional outcomes) would provide more evidence beyond the preliminary findings from GTEx. 

      We appreciate the reviewer’s suggestion on the validation of our findings by applying our analysis to independent RNA-seq datasets from Alzheimer’s disease patients. 

      • We have used two Alzheimer’s disease datasets, GEO and ROSMAP, to investigate the correlation between aging and Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and included these analyses in our study (Fig. 4B-C and Figure S8C).

      • In the Results section (Page 7), we have presented the results of this validation, where we identified correlations between sex-biased aging-related splicing changes and AD-related changes. These findings support the conclusions from the GTEx dataset and further strengthen the relevance of our results to AD.

      As suggested, we have updated the manuscript to more explicitly highlight this validation in the Discussion section (Page 12), noting: ‘We further validated our findings using Alzheimer’s disease dataset, ROSMAP, where we observed consistent correlations between aging-related splicing changes and Alzheimer’s disease-related changes, providing additional evidence for the robustness of our results.’ 

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      (1) In the text (Introduction and Discussion), the authors mention analyzing 54 tissues, the abstract states 35 tissues, Table S1 lists 48, and Figure 2A-B shows 33. Could the authors please clarify exactly how many tissues they used? I am also confused by the sample numbers in Table S1. For example: for adiposesubcutaneous tissue, the total number of females is listed as 218 but the sum of young and old females is only 110. Does this mean some samples were excluded? What is the exclusion criterion? 

      We thank the reviewers and editors for pointing out the discrepancies regarding the number of tissues analyzed and the sample numbers in Table S1. We appreciate the opportunity to clarify these points:

      Number of tissues analyzed:

      • We downloaded and analyzed 17,382 samples in 54 tissues from GTEx in total (31 tissues and 13 brain regions), as mentioned in the Results, Methods, and Discussion sections. Table S1 lists 48 tissues (31 tissues, 13 brain regions, and 4 merged brain regions), which include a refined classification of the tissues we analyzed, accounting for the variations in brain region categorization in the dataset.

      • The discrepancy also arises from the different sample size cutoffs in specific analyses. For pcSVR analysis (Figure 2A-B), we did the subsampling for the permutation analysis for certain key findings, so we filtered a subset of 33 tissues (29 tissues and 4 merged brain regions), which included at least 3 samples in each age group in females or males. 

      • To resolve this, we have clarified the total number of tissues analyzed and aligned the numbers across the manuscript. In the revised manuscript, we now explicitly state in both the Abstract and Methods sections that 54 tissues were analyzed in the context of this study. We added a note in Methods to clarify that 35 tissues are 31 tissues and 4 merged brain regions (Page 16). In Figure 2A-B, we clarified that the 33 tissues are filtered due to the usage in this analysis (Page 17).

      Sample numbers in Table S1:

      • Regarding the sample sizes of age groups, the discrepancy occurred due to the classification of the age groups. We classify the samples into three: Young, Middle, and Old, as mentioned in the Results section (Page 4). 

      • Additionally, we excluded the sample sizes in 13 single brain regions. We aligned the total tissue number to 35 with our texts.

      We hope this resolves the confusion regarding the number of tissues and the sample sizes used in the analysis. These clarifications have been incorporated into the revised manuscript to ensure consistency.

      (2) Was post-mortem interval (PMI) or manner of death considered in the model? For example, traumatic death may have major consequences on gene expression. Similarly, a few tissues have low sample numbers, for example, kidney cortex and brain. The pooling of brain samples is explained and the kidney cortex is excluded, so why is it listed in Table S1? 

      Thank you for raising this important point regarding the potential impact of post-mortem interval (PMI) and manner of death (DTHMNNR) on gene expression. We carefully considered both factors as potential confounders in our analysis. 

      Specifically, to evaluate their impacts, we calculated the correlations between the coefficients of PMI or manner of death, with the confounding factors. Our results showed that PMI and DTHMNNR are significantly correlated with the covariates in most tissues, suggesting that their effects could be effectively regressed in our model (Figure S4). As we have mentioned in Figure S4 and Author response image 1, we conducted a differential analysis that incorporated PMI as a covariate in the regression models and re-evaluated the age- and sex-related transcriptomic changes to address this concern. The high correlations showed the minor effect size of PMI when including the covariates in the model. As suggested by the reviewers and editors, we have now included this correlation analysis in Figure S4C-E and updated the text in the results section (Page 5).

      Additionally, as the responses above, Table S1 provides the general sample sizes of all GTEx tissues without filtering. We have modified the table to include a total of 35 tissues, including 31 non-brain tissues and 4 brain regions.

      (3) It might be important to show a simple visual of cohort details such as age ranges, sexes, ethnicities, PMIs, etc. 

      To address this, we added summary figures to illustrate the distributions of key demographic variables, including age, sex, BMI, ethnicity, post-mortem intervals (PMIs), and manner of death (DTHMNNR) (Author response image 7 and Author response image 8). This will provide readers with a clearer overview of the dataset composition and potential covariates affecting the analysis. 

      Author response image 7.

      Age (left panel), BMI (Body Mass Index) (middle panel), and PMI (Post-Mortem Interval) (right panel) distribution in GTEx v8 cohort.

      Author response image 8.

      Sex (left panel), ethnicity (middle panel), and manner of death (DTHMNNR) (right panel) distribution in GTEx v8 cohort.

      (4) Since this study is highly correlative, it is impossible to determine if the findings hold true without an independent cohort validation or experimental validation. They used the ROSMAP cohort for AD samples, and some splicing factors regulation but the generalizability to the age and sex effects have not been independently tested.

      The reviewer raises an important point regarding the independent validation of sex- and age-associated splicing changes associated with AD. We used GTEx primarily because it includes approximately 17,000 RNA-seq samples across multiple human tissues, making it the most comprehensive public resource for studying population-level differences in age and sex. In particular, its large-scale brain samples provide a unique opportunity to analyze transcriptomic changes in sex-dimorphic aging.

      We understand the reviewer’s concern that our findings are mainly supported by correlative evidence, which could be affected by dataset-specific biases. However, there are several technical issues in crossvalidation with transcriptomes across different datasets, including limited comparability due to cell type heterogeneity, postmortem artifacts, and sequencing biases.

      Specifically, GTEx data is bulk RNA-seq that does not capture cell-type-specific transcriptomic changes. Given the cellular complexity of the brain and other tissues, observed differences in gene expression and splicing may be influenced by shifts in cellular composition rather than intrinsic transcriptional regulation. For example, we compared our results from GTEx whole blood with the analysis using an external dataset from Peripheral Blood Mononuclear Cells (PBMCs) provided by Shen et al. (2024) [3] (Author response image 2).  We observed limited overlap in differentially expressed genes between these datasets (probably because the whole blood contains diverse immune cell populations), highlighting the challenges in cross-dataset validation due to differences in tissue composition and sample processing.

      Therefore, we applied surrogate variable analysis (SVA) to minimize technical and biological confounders. This approach helped reduce biases from genetic background to hidden batch effects, including postmortem artifacts, sequencing biases (Figure S4), and other covariates. This approach could help us identify whether sex-biased splicing events are biologically meaningful rather than technical artifacts.  

      In addition, to address the reviewer’s concern on the splicing factor regulation, we managed to find a dataset in decision-related brain regions. Due to the limitation of human brain data covering different age and sex groups, we used mouse hippocampus datasets, including young and old, as well as female and male groups [7].  The analysis of protein levels from MS data identified sex-biased age-associated splicing factors, including Srsf1 and Srsf7.  We found that the changes are consistent with the findings from GTEx (Author response image 9), aligning with our sex-biased splicing factor expression during aging in the same region of the human brain. This cross-species consistency supports the robustness of our findings in human brain aging.

      Author response image 9.

      Protein levels of some male-specific splicing factors in human hippocampus quantified using MS data. The Y-axis shows the protein intensity. Different facets mean different sample batch sets. The yellow boxes indicate the protein levels in the young group, while the brown boxes indicate those in the old group.

      In summary, despite the inherent limitations of RNA-seq studies in sex- and age-related transcriptomics, we have made our best efforts to address these concerns through comparisons with external datasets, statistical corrections, and validation using proteomic data. We appreciate the reviewer’s feedback and include additional discussion on these points (Page 13). 

      (5) Are AS predictions from short-read data accurate enough to make the predictions the authors report? 

      The reviewer is correct that the short-read sequencing has inherent limitations in reconstructing full-length isoforms.  However, the higher sequencing depth for short reads makes it a better choice in quantifying the relative change of each AS event across different conditions.  As a result, short-read data are extensively used in the splicing field to quantitatively measure the AS changes.  For this reason, we focused on the levels of alternative splicing events, rather than the quantification of full-length isoforms.  We used a series of stringent filters in our analyses to increase the reliability of our results.

      Specifically, we filtered the read counts of the junction read counts (JC) of most differential AS events that were higher than 10, as mentioned in the Methods section. Also, we used our GPU-based gene expression quantification tool, Paean, which performed better in cross-validation with quantitative RT-PCR results. The results of Paean are consistent with other pipelines. We cited an updated version of Paean that included the comparison with other tools in analyzing AS for consistency.  The manuscript on the new Paean version is being reviewed in another journal, and we included the PDF of that manuscript (Fig. 3 in the Paean manuscript) in the revised documents. 

      (6) Along the same lines, the finding that male age-related AS events are linked to Alzheimer's disease somewhat contradicts epidemiological studies that show that even after adjusting for age, women still have a greater risk of developing Alzheimer's than men. The authors show a significant overlap with AD GE events in females but don't explain the discrepancy. 

      We appreciate the editor’s comment regarding these discrepancies with the epidemiological studies. Previous studies suggested that the disease manifestations of Alzheimer’s Disease (AD) showed sex differences in AD phenotypes, including cognitive decline and brain atrophy [8].  The analyses on the sex/age effect of AD are indeed pretty complex, depending on the molecular criteria (GE or AS vs epidemiological data) in distinct studies, probably due to the difficulty in capturing how environmental exposures interact with biological pathways.  We hope to bring up three related points regarding this concern, which were also discussed in the revised manuscript. 

      • As we have mentioned in the Discussion section, an early study investigated the relationship between age, sex, and cognitive function in a large cohort of 17,127 UK Biobank participants [9]. Their study highlighted more apparent age-related changes in cognitive function among men, suggesting a potential vulnerability of men to cognitive decline with age.  Their main conclusion is consistent with our findings. 

      • While men and women can both suffer from Alzheimer's disease, women are more likely to be diagnosed, possibly due to longer lifespans and potential differences in brain structure or other factors. Although women exhibit a higher overall risk of AD, they may also have distinct molecular compensatory mechanisms that influence disease progression. 

      • To avoid the age effect, in our AD datasets, including ROSMAP, we filtered the samples over 90 years old to match the number of both sexes and the age distribution between the AD and control groups. Our analysis avoided the age biases in comparing AD and control, suggesting the crucial roles of sBASEs in AD during male aging.

      Moreover, for gene expression (GE), we showed distinct patterns of AD-related genes in females with AS. These two molecular processes do not necessarily have the same functional impact. AS changes may precede or contribute to disease onset in different ways compared to GE alterations. Our study came up with the underlying mechanisms linking cognitive disorders and alternative splicing (AS) at a higher molecular resolution.   

      (7) Could the authors explain which sBASE subset they used for their random forest prediction model and what was the rationale? 

      We are sorry for missing the details in selecting sBASEs (sex-biased age-associated splicing events) for the random forest prediction model. We specifically used sBASEs that exhibited specific sex-biased changes in splicing associated with aging. This subset of sBASEs was chosen in terms of those that could also be detected in the ROSMAP AD dataset due to different sequencing depths or technical biases across datasets. These sBASEs were further input to a prediction model with the feature selection algorithm RFE, and then evaluated their contributions. In the revised manuscript, we added the details of this selection in the Methods (Page 7).

      (8) The breakpoint analysis is particularly interesting. Can this be speculated to correlate with the recent non-linear multi-omic aging patterns observed by Shen et al in Nature Aging? 

      Thank you for highlighting the interesting aspects of our breakpoint analysis and suggesting its potential correlation with the non-linear aging patterns observed by Shen et al. 

      Shen et al. observed two prominent crests around the ages of 45 and 60 using omics data. Similarly, we also identified the non-linear aging patterns with two breakpoints in our analysis. However, there are some notable differences in specific breakpoints between these two studies, resulting from the breakpoint definition, as well as the sample preparations. According to the response in Author response image 2, the differences come from the following aspects:

      The definition of breakpoints vs crests:

      • Crests represent age-related molecular changes at each time point across the human lifespan. They indicate the number of molecules that are differentially expressed during aging (q < 0.05), without considering individual expression levels.

      • Our breakpoints, in contrast, are identified after filtering the chronological trends based on the expression levels and calculating the rate of change at each age point using sliding windows. Breakpoints are defined as local maxima where the distance to the nearest minimum, relative to the global maximum, exceeds 10%. We indeed found some local wide peaks around 60 in some tissues, shown in Figure S10, however, we excluded these due to our strict cutoffs.

      The sequenced biosamples: 

      • Whole-blood vs Peripheral Blood Mononuclear Cells (PBMC): As mentioned in previous responses, in GTEx, whole blood samples from donors were sequenced, whereas their study used PBMCs. Whole blood contains all blood components, including red blood cells, platelets, granulocytes (e.g., neutrophils), lymphocytes, and monocytes, while PBMCs only represent a subset of white blood cells, primarily consisting of lymphocytes (T cells, B cells, NK cells) and monocytes, excluding granulocytes and erythrocytes. Gene expression changes observed in whole blood capture the contributions from neutrophils and other granulocytes, which are absent in PBMC analyses (as shown in Figure S11C and Author response image 2). Additionally, whole blood can serve as a readily accessible biomarker source for testing age-related diseases without the need for cell separation, making it a more practical option for clinical applications.

      • For both studies, we share a tissue, which is skin, we looked at the non-linear changes during aging and found the same two breakpoints: 43 and 58. 

      Sex-specific analysis in females and males:

      • The main object of our analysis is to compare the differences in aging rates between sexes. Notably, the identified breakpoints may differ when sex effects are not taken into account, highlighting the importance of analyzing males and females separately.

      We have added the following statements to further clarify this connection: ‘Our analysis observed the nonlinear aging patterns with two breakpoints, which is consistent with recent findings (Nature Aging, 2024), with differences in specific age points due to the sex differences as well as tissue diversities.’ (Page 14), and ‘These breakpoints could represent key junctures in the aging process that align with the non-linear patterns of aging and disease progression.’ (Page 15)

      (9) Minor - the authors should refer to figures in the Discussion. They do so in some cases but this needs to be more extensive. 

      Thank you for pointing this out. In response, we have reviewed the Discussion section and added references to relevant figures where appropriate. In the section discussing the discrepancies between the profiles of GE vs. AS, we now refer to Figure 3 to highlight the earlier onset of different transcriptomic resolutions (Page 12); When describing the sex-specific age-associated AS changes and their associations with Alzheimer’s disease, we have added references to Figure 4 (Page 12); In the discussion of estrogen-mediated regulation of splicing factors, we have referred to Figure 5A, which detail the construction of RBP-RNA regulatory network integrating muti-dimensional data obtained through several orthogonal state-of-the-art approaches (Page 14).

      Reference:

      (1) Ferreira, P.G. et al. The effects of death and post-mortem cold ischemia on human tissue transcriptomes. Nature communications 9, 490 (2018).

      (2) Wucher, V., Sodaei, R., Amador, R., Irimia, M. & Guigó, R. Day-night and seasonal variation of human gene expression across tissues. PLoS Biology 21, e3001986 (2023).

      (3) Shen, X. et al. Nonlinear dynamics of multi-omics profiles during human aging. Nature aging, 116 (2024).

      (4) Zhou, X. et al. Splicing factor SRSF1 promotes gliomagenesis via oncogenic splice-switching of MYO1B. The Journal of clinical investigation 129, 676-693 (2019).

      (5) Soheili-Nezhad, S., Ibáñez-Solé, O., Izeta, A., Hoeijmakers, J.H. & Stoeger, T. Time is ticking faster for long genes in aging. Trends in Genetics 40, 299-312 (2024).

      (6) Brouillette, M. Gene length could be a critical factor in the aging of the genome. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 121, e2416630121 (2024).

      (7) Keele, G.R. et al. Global and tissue-specific aging effects on murine proteomes. Cell reports 42(2023).

      (8) Ferretti, M.T. et al. Sex differences in Alzheimer disease—the gateway to precision medicine. Nature Reviews Neurology 14, 457-469 (2018).

      (9) Foo, H. et al. Age-and sex-related topological organization of human brain functional networks and their relationship to cognition. Frontiers in aging neuroscience 13, 758817 (2021).

    1. eLife Assessment

      Peukes et al. report compelling ultrastructures of excitatory synapses in the mouse forebrain that will serve as a reference for future work in the field. Their important findings using correlated fluorescence and cryo-electron tomography challenge the textbook view of synaptic structure that emerged from chemically fixed and metal-stained tissues. Instead of a post-synaptic density, these authors reveal the architecture of the cytoskeletal, neurotransmitter receptor clusters, and organelles in the 'synaptoplasm'.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review): <br /> The authors survey the ultrastructural organization of glutamatergic synapses by cryo-ET and image processing tools using two complementary experimental approaches. The first approach employs so-called "ultra-fresh" preparations of brain homogenates from a knock-in mouse expressing a GFP-tagged version of PSD-95, allowing Peukes and colleagues to specifically target excitatory glutamatergic synapses. In the second approach, direct in-tissue (using cortical and hippocampal regions) targeting of the glutamatergic synapses employing the same mouse model is presented. In order to ascertain whether the isolation procedure causes any significant changes in the ultrastructural organization (and possibly synaptic macromolecular organization) the authors compare their findings using both of these approaches. The quantitation of the synaptic cleft height reveals an unexpected variability, while the STA analysis of the ionotropic receptors provides insights into their distribution with respect to the synaptic cleft.

      The main novelty of this study lies in the continuous claims by the authors that the sample preservation methods developed here are superior to any others previously used. This leads them as well to systematically downplay or directly ignore a substantial body of previous cryo-ET studies of synaptic structure. Without comparisons with the cryo-ET literature, it is very hard to judge the impact of this work in the field. Furthermore, the data does not show any better preservation in the so-called "ultra-fresh" preparation than in the literature, perhaps to the contrary as synapses with strangely elongated vesicles are often seen. Such synapses have been regularly discarded for further analysis in previous synaptosome studies (e.g. Martinez-Sanchez 2021). Whilst the targeting approach using a fluorescent PSD95 marker is novel and seems sufficiently precise, the authors use a somewhat outdated approach (cryo-sectioning) to generate in-tissue tomograms of poor quality. To what extent such tomograms can be interpreted in molecular terms is highly questionable. The authors also don't discuss the physiological influence of 20% dextran used for high-pressure freezing of these "very native" specimens.

      Lastly, a large part of the paper is devoted to image analysis of the PSD which is not convincing (including a somewhat forced comparison with the fixed and heavy-metal staining room temperature approach). Despite being a technically challenging study, the results fall short of expectations.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors set out to visualize the molecular architecture of the adult forebrain glutamatergic synapses in a near-native state. To this end, they use a rapid workflow to extract and plunge-freeze mouse synapses for cryo-electron tomography. In addition, the authors use knockin mice expression PSD95-GFP in order to perform correlated light and electron microscopy to clearly identify pre- and synaptic membranes. By thorough quantification of tomograms from plunge- and high-pressure frozen samples, the authors show that the previously reported 'post-synaptic density' does not occur at high frequency and therefore not a defining feature of a glutamatergic synapse.

      Subsequently, the authors are able to reproduce the frequency of post-synaptic density when preparing conventional electron microscopy samples, thus indicating that density prevalence is an artifact of sample preparation. The authors go on to describe the arrangement of cytoskeletal components, membraneous compartments, and ionotropic receptor clusters across synapses.

      Demonstrating that the frequency of the post-synaptic density in prior work is likely an artifact and not a defining feature of glutamatergic synapses is significant. The descriptions of distributions and morphologies of proteins and membranes in this work may serve as a basis for the future of investigation for readers interested in these features.

      Strengths:

      The authors perform a rigorous quantification of the molecular density profiles across synapses to determine the frequency of the post-synaptic density. They prepare samples using two cryogenic electron microscopy sample preparation methods, as well as one set of samples using conventional electron microscopy methods. The authors can reproduce previous reports of the frequency of the post-synaptic density by conventional sample preparation, but not by either of the cryogenic methods, thus strongly supporting their claim.

    4. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors use cryo-electron tomography to thoroughly investigate the complexity of purified, excitatory synapses. They make several major interesting discoveries: polyhedral vesicles that have not been observed before in neurons; analysis of the intermembrane distance, and a link to potentiation, essentially updating distances reported from plastic-embedded specimen; and find that the postsynaptic density does not appear as a dense accumulation of proteins in all vitrified samples (less than half), a feature which served as a hallmark feature to identify excitatory plastic-embedded synapses.

      Strengths:

      (1) The presented work is thorough: the authors compare purified, endogenously labeled synapses to wild-type synapses to exclude artifacts that could arise through the homogenation step, and, in addition, analyse plastic embedded, stained synapses prepared using the same quick workflow, to ensure their findings have not been caused by way of purification of the synapses. Interestingly, the 'thick lines of PSD' are evident in most of their stained synapses.

      (2) I commend the authors on the exceptional technical achievement of preparing frozen specimens from a mouse within two minutes.

      (3) The approaches highlighted here can be used in other fields studying cell-cell junctions.

      (4) The tomograms will be deposited upon publication which will enable neurobiologists and researchers from other fields to carry on data evaluation in their field of expertise since tomography is still a specialized skill and they collected and reconstructed over 100 excellent tomograms of synapses, which generates a wealth of information to be also used in future studies.

      (5) The authors have identified ionotropic receptor positions and that they are linked to actin filaments, and appear to be associated with membrane and other cytosolic scaffolds, which is highly exciting.

      (6) The authors achieved their aims to study neuronal excitatory synapses in great detail, were thorough in their experiments, and made multiple fascinating discoveries. They challenge dogmas that have been in place for decades and highlight the benefit of implementing and developing new methods to carefully understand the underlying molecular machines of synapses.

      Impact on community:

      The findings presented by Peukes et al. pertaining to synapse biology change dogmas about the fundamental understanding of synaptic ultrastructure. The work presented by the authors, particularly the associated change of intermembrane distance with potentiation and the distinct appearance of the PSD as an irregular amorphous 'cloud' will provide food for thought and an incentive for more analysis and additional studies, as will the discovery of large membranous and cytosolic protein complexes linked to ionotropic receptors within and outside of the synaptic cleft, which are ripe for investigation. The findings and tomograms available will carry far in the synapse fields and the approach and methods will move other fields outside of neurobiology forward. The method and impactful results of preparing cryogenic, unlabeled, unstained, near-native synapses may enable the study of how synapses function at high resolution in the future.

    5. Author Response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review): 

      The authors survey the ultrastructural organization of glutamatergic synapses by cryo-ET and image processing tools using two complementary experimental approaches. The first approach employs so-called "ultra-fresh" preparations of brain homogenates from a knock-in mouse expressing a GFP-tagged version of PSD-95, allowing Peukes and colleagues to specifically target excitatory glutamatergic synapses. In the second approach, direct in-tissue (using cortical and hippocampal regions) targeting of the glutamatergic synapses employing the same mouse model is presented. In order to ascertain whether the isolation procedure causes any significant changes in the ultrastructural organization (and possibly synaptic macromolecular organization) the authors compare their findings using both of these approaches. The quantitation of the synaptic cleft height reveals an unexpected variability, while the STA analysis of the ionotropic receptors provides insights into their distribution with respect to the synaptic cleft.

      The main novelty of this study lies in the continuous claims by the authors that the sample preservation methods developed here are superior to any others previously used. This leads them as well to systematically downplay or directly ignore a substantial body of previous cryo-ET studies of synaptic structure. Without comparisons with the cryo-ET literature, it is very hard to judge the impact of this work in the field. Furthermore, the data does not show any better preservation in the so-called "ultra-fresh" preparation than in the literature, perhaps to the contrary as synapses with strangely elongated vesicles are often seen. Such synapses have been regularly discarded for further analysis in previous synaptosome studies (e.g. Martinez-Sanchez 2021). Whilst the targeting approach using a fluorescent PSD95 marker is novel and seems sufficiently precise, the authors use a somewhat outdated approach (cryo-sectioning) to generate in-tissue tomograms of poor quality. To what extent such tomograms can be interpreted in molecular terms is highly questionable. The authors also don't discuss the physiological influence of 20% dextran used for high-pressure freezing of these "very native" specimens.

      Lastly, a large part of the paper is devoted to image analysis of the PSD which is not convincing (including a somewhat forced comparison with the fixed and heavy-metal staining room temperature approach). Despite being a technically challenging study, the results fall short of expectations. 

      Our manuscript contains a discussion of both conventional EM and cryoET of synapses. We apologise if we have omitted referencing or discussing any earlier cryoET work. This was certainly not our intention, and we include a more complete discussion of published cryoET work on synapses in our revised manuscript.

      The reviewer is concerned that the synaptic vesicles in some synapse tomograms are “stretched” and that this may reflect poor preservation.  We would like to point out that such non-spherical synaptic vesicles have also been previously reported in cryoET of primary neurons grown on EM grids (Tao et al., J. Neuro, 2018). Indeed, there is no reason per se to suppose synaptic vesicles are always spherical and there are many diverse families of proteins expressed at the synapse that shape membrane curvature (BAR domain proteins, synaptotagmin, epsins, endophilins and others). We will add further discussion of this issue in the revised manuscript.

      The reviewer regards ‘cryo-sectioning’ as outdated and cryoET data from these preparations as “poor quality”. We respectfully disagree. Preparing brain tissues for cryoET is generally considered to be challenging. The first successful demonstration of preparing such samples was before the advent of the cryoEM resolution revolution (with electron counting detectors) by Zuber et al (Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.,2005) preparing cryo-sections/CEMOVIS of in vitro brain cultures. We followed this technique to prepare tissue cryo-sections for cryoET in our manuscript. Recently, cryoFIB-SEM liftout has been developed as an alternative method to prepare tissue samples for cryoET (Mahamid et al., J. Struct. Biol., 2015) and only more recently this method became available to more laboratories. Both techniques introduce damage as has been described (Han et al., J. Microsc., 2008; Lucas et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci., 2023). Importantly no like-for-like, quantitative comparison of these two methodologies has yet been performed. We have recently demonstrated that the molecular structure of amyloid fibrils within human brain is preserved down to the protein fold level in samples prepared by cryo-sectioning (Gilbert et al., Nature, 2024). We will add further detail on the process by which we excluded poor quality tomograms from our analysis, which we described in detail in our methods section.

      The reviewer asks what the physiological effect is of adding 20% w/v ~40,000 Da dextran? This is a reasonable concern since this could in principle exert osmotic pressure on the tissue sample. While we did not investigate this ourselves, earlier studies have (Zuber et al, 2005) showing cell membranes were not damaged by and did not have any detectable effect on cell structure in the presence of this concentration of dextran.

      The reviewer is not convinced by our analysis of the apparent molecular density of macromolecules in the postsynaptic compartment that in conventional EM is called the postsynaptic density. However, the reviewer provides no reasoning for this assessment nor alternative approaches that could be attempted. We would like to add that we have tested multiple different approaches to objectively measure molecular crowding in cryoET data, that give comparable results. We believe that our conclusion – that we do not observe an increased molecular density conserved at the postsynaptic membrane, and that the PSD that we and others observed by conventional EM does not correspond to a region of increased molecular density - is well supported by our data.  We and the other reviewers consider this an important and novel observation.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review)

      Summary: 

      The authors set out to visualize the molecular architecture of the adult forebrain glutamatergic synapses in a near-native state. To this end, they use a rapid workflow to extract and plunge-freeze mouse synapses for cryo-electron tomography. In addition, the authors use knockin mice expression PSD95-GFP in order to perform correlated light and electron microscopy to clearly identify pre- and synaptic membranes. By thorough quantification of tomograms from plunge- and high-pressure frozen samples, the authors show that the previously reported 'post-synaptic density' does not occur at high frequency and therefore not a defining feature of a glutamatergic synapse.

      Subsequently, the authors are able to reproduce the frequency of post-synaptic density when preparing conventional electron microscopy samples, thus indicating that density prevalence is an artifact of sample preparation. The authors go on to describe the arrangement of cytoskeletal components, membraneous compartments, and ionotropic receptor clusters across synapses.

      Demonstrating that the frequency of the post-synaptic density in prior work is likely an artifact and not a defining feature of glutamatergic synapses is significant. The descriptions of distributions and morphologies of proteins and membranes in this work may serve as a basis for the future of investigation for readers interested in these features.

      Strengths: 

      The authors perform a rigorous quantification of the molecular density profiles across synapses to determine the frequency of the post-synaptic density. They prepare samples using two cryogenic electron microscopy sample preparation methods, as well as one set of samples using conventional electron microscopy methods. The authors can reproduce previous reports of the frequency of the post-synaptic density by conventional sample preparation, but not by either of the cryogenic methods, thus strongly supporting their claim. 

      We thank the reviewer for their generous assessment of our manuscript.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      The authors use cryo-electron tomography to thoroughly investigate the complexity of purified, excitatory synapses. They make several major interesting discoveries: polyhedral vesicles that have not been observed before in neurons; analysis of the intermembrane distance, and a link to potentiation, essentially updating distances reported from plastic-embedded specimen; and find that the postsynaptic density does not appear as a dense accumulation of proteins in all vitrified samples (less than half), a feature which served as a hallmark feature to identify excitatory plastic-embedded synapses. 

      Strengths: 

      (1)The presented work is thorough: the authors compare purified, endogenously labeled synapses to wild-type synapses to exclude artifacts that could arise through the homogenation step, and, in addition, analyse plastic embedded, stained synapses prepared using the same quick workflow, to ensure their findings have not been caused by way of purification of the synapses. Interestingly, the 'thick lines of PSD' are evident in most of their stained synapses.

      (2)I commend the authors on the exceptional technical achievement of preparing frozen specimens from a mouse within two minutes.

      (3)The approaches highlighted here can be used in other fields studying cell-cell junctions.

      (4)The tomograms will be deposited upon publication which will enable neurobiologists and researchers from other fields to carry on data evaluation in their field of expertise since tomography is still a specialized skill and they collected and reconstructed over 100 excellent tomograms of synapses, which generates a wealth of information to be also used in future studies.

      (5) The authors have identified ionotropic receptor positions and that they are linked to actin filaments, and appear to be associated with membrane and other cytosolic scaffolds, which is highly exciting.

      (6) The authors achieved their aims to study neuronal excitatory synapses in great detail, were thorough in their experiments, and made multiple fascinating discoveries. They challenge dogmas that have been in place for decades and highlight the benefit of implementing and developing new methods to carefully understand the underlying molecular machines of synapses.

      Weaknesses: 

      The authors show informative segmentations in their figures but none have been overlayed with any of the tomograms in the submitted videos. It would be helpful for data evaluation to a broad audience to be able to view these together as videos to study these tomograms and extract more information. Deposition of segmentations associated with the tomgrams would be tremendously helpful to Neurobiologists, cryo-ET method developers, and others to push the boundaries.

      Impact on community: 

      The findings presented by Peukes et al. pertaining to synapse biology change dogmas about the fundamental understanding of synaptic ultrastructure. The work presented by the authors, particularly the associated change of intermembrane distance with potentiation and the distinct appearance of the PSD as an irregular amorphous 'cloud' will provide food for thought and an incentive for more analysis and additional studies, as will the discovery of large membranous and cytosolic protein complexes linked to ionotropic receptors within and outside of the synaptic cleft, which are ripe for investigation. The findings and tomograms available will carry far in the synapse fields and the approach and methods will move other fields outside of neurobiology forward. The method and impactful results of preparing cryogenic, unlabelled, unstained, near-native synapses may enable the study of how synapses function at high resolution in the future.

      We thank the reviewer for their supportive assessment of our manuscript.  We thank the reviewer for suggesting overlaying segmentations with videos of the raw tomographic volumes. We will include this in our revised manuscript.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      Major comments: 

      (1) The previous literature on synaptic cryo-ET studies is systematically ignored. The results presented here (and their novelty) must be compared directly with this body of work, rather than with classical EM.

      Our submitted manuscript included a 3-paragraph discussion of earlier synaptic cryoET studies, albeit we apologize that a seminal citation was missing, which we have corrected in our revised manuscript. We have now also included an additional brief discussion related to several more recent cryoET studies (see citations below) that were published after our pre-print was first deposited in 2021.

      (1) Held, R.G., Liang, J., and Brunger, A.T. (2024). Nanoscale architecture of synaptic vesicles and scaffolding complexes revealed by cryo-electron tomography. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 121, e2403136121. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2403136121.

      (2) Held, R.G., Liang, J., Esquivies, L., Khan, Y.A., Wang, C., Azubel, M., and Brunger, A.T. (2024). In-Situ Structure and Topography of AMPA Receptor Scaffolding Complexes Visualized by CryoET. bioRxiv, 2024.10.19.619226. https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.10.19.619226.

      (3)Matsui, A., Spangler, C., Elferich, J., Shiozaki, M., Jean, N., Zhao, X., Qin, M., Zhong, H., Yu, Z., and Gouaux, E. (2024). Cryo-electron tomographic investigation of native hippocampal glutamatergic synapses. eLife 13, RP98458. https://doi.org/10.7554/elife.98458.

      (4)Glynn, C., Smith, J.L.R., Case, M., Csöndör, R., Katsini, A., Sanita, M.E., Glen, T.S., Pennington, A., and Grange, M. (2024). Charting the molecular landscape of neuronal organisation within the hippocampus using cryo electron tomography. bioRxiv, 2024.10.14.617844. https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.10.14.617844.

      We discuss the above papers in our revised manuscript with the following:

      “Since submission of our manuscript, several reports of synapse cryoET from within cultured primary neurons (Held et al., 2024a, 2024b)  and mouse brain(Glynn et al., 2024; Matsui et al., 2024) were prepared by cryoFIB-milling. These new datasets are largely consistent with the data reported here. CryoFIB-SEM has the advantage of overcoming the local knife damage caused by cryo-sectioning but introduces amorphization across the whole sample that diminishes the information content (Al-Amoudi et al., 2005; Lovatt et al., 2022; Lucas and Grigorieff, 2023). We have recently shown cryoET data is capable of revealing subnanometer resolution in-tissue protein structure from vitreous cryo-sections (Gilbert et al., 2024) and near-atomic structures within cryo-sections has recently been demonstrated (Elferich et al., 2025).”

      Although there is variation between individual synapses, PSDs are clearly visible in several previous cryo-ET studies (even if it's not as striking as in heavy-metal stained samples). In fact, although the contrast of the images is generally poor, PSDs are also visible in several examples shown in Figure 1 - Supplement 3. Not being able to detect them seems more of a problem of the workflow used here than of missing features. The authors should also discuss why heavy-metal stains would accumulate on a non-existing structure (PSD) in conventional EM.

      We agree that apparent higher molecular density can be observed in example tomographic data of earlier cryoET studies. We also report individual examples of similar synapses in our dataset. A key strength of our approach is that we have assessed the molecular architecture of large numbers of adult brain synapses acquired by an unbiased approach (solely guided by PSD95 cryoCLEM), which indicate that a higher molecular density proximal to the postsynaptic membrane is not a conserved feature of glutamatergic synapses in the adult brain. There is no rationale for our cryoCLEM approach being a ‘problem of the workflow’.

      The reviewer misunderstands the weaknesses of conventional/room temperature EM workflows (including resin-embedding and freeze substitution). It is unavoidable that most proteins are damaged by denaturation and/or washed away by washing samples in organic solvents (methanol/acetone that directly denature most proteins) during tissue preparation for conventional EM. It is therefore conceivable that in such preparations a relative increase in contrast proximal to the postsynaptic membrane (‘PSD’) would appear if cytoplasmic proteins were washed away during these harsh organic solved washing steps, leaving only those denatured proteins that are tethered to the postsynaptic membrane. It is not that the PSD is absent in cryoEM, rather that this difference in molecular crowding is not evident when tissues are imaged directly by cryoEM and have not undergone the harsh sample preparation required for conventional/room temperature EM.

      (2) Whether the synapses examined here are in a more physiological state than those analyzed in other papers remains absolutely unclear. For example, the quality of the tomographic slice shown in Figure 1C is poor, with the majority of synaptic vesicles looking suspiciously elongated. 

      We addressed this in our public reviews.

      (3) How were actin filaments segmented and quantified (e.g. for Fig 1E)? Apart from actin, can the authors show some examples of other macromolecular complexes (e.g. ribosomes) that they are able to identify in synapses (based on the info in supplementary tables)? Also, the mapping of glutamatergic receptors is not convincing, as the molecules were picked manually. To analyze their distribution, they should be mapped as comprehensively as possible by e.g. template matching.

      Actin filaments identified by ~7 nm diameter with ~70° branch points were manually segmented in IMOD. The number of filaments was counted per postsynaptic compartment. We have amended the methods section to include this description.

      “In the PoSM, F-actin formed a network with ~70° branch points (Figure 1–figure supplement 1C) likely formed by Arp2/3, as expected(Pizarro-Cerdá 2017,Fäßler 2020) . Putative filament copy number in the PoSM was estimated by manual segmentation in IMOD.” Manual picking was validated by the quality of the subtomogram average, which although only reached modest resolution (25 Å) is consistent with the identification of ionotropic glutamate receptors.

      (4) In the section "Synaptic organelles" the authors should provide some general information on the average number and size of synaptic vesicles (for the in-tissue tomograms).

      We have provided this information in the methods section:

      “The average diameter of synaptic vesicles was 40.2 nm and the minimum and maximum dimensions ranged from 20 to 57.8 nm, measured from the outside of the vesicle that included ellipsoidal synaptic vesicles similar to those previously reported (Tao et al., 2018).” A detailed survey of the presynaptic compartment, including the number of presynaptic vesicles was not the focus of our manuscript. We have deposited all tomograms from our dataset for any further data mining.

      Can the "flat tubular membranes compartments" be attributed to ER? The angular vesicles certainly have a typical ER appearance, as such morphology has been seen in several cryo-ET studies of neuronal and non-neuronal cells.

      In neuronal cells we regard it as unsafe to describe an intracellular organelle as being endoplasmic reticulum on the basis of morphology alone (eg. Smooth ER described widely in conventional EM) because of the apparent diversity of distinct organelles. As described in our methods section, we could have confidence that a membrane compartment is ER when we observe ribosomes tethered to the membrane. In instances where flat/tubular membranes did not have associated ribosomes, we take the cautious view that there is not sufficient evidence to define these as ER.

      Importantly, polyhedral vesicles were distinct from the flat/tubular membranes that resembled ER and are at present organelles of unknown identity. It will be important in future experiments to determine what are the protein constituents of these distinct organelle types to understand both their functions and how these distinct membrane architectures are assembled.

      Therefore, the sentences in lines 198-199 are simply wrong. Additionally, features of even higher membrane curvature are common in the ER (e.g. Collado et al., Dev Cell 2019). 

      We thank the reviewer for bringing our attention to this excellent paper (Collado et al.). We agree that the sentence describing the curvature being higher than all other membranes except mitochondrial cristae is wrong. We have removed this sentence in the revised manuscript.

      (5)The quality of the tomographic data for the in-tissue sample is low, likely due to cryo-sectioning-induced artifacts, as extensively documented in the literature. Additionally, the authors used 20% dextran as cryo-protectant for high-pressure freezing, which contrasts with statements like those in lines 342-344. Given that several publications describing the in-tissue targeting of synapses (e.g. from Eric Gouaux's lab) are available, the quality of the tomographic data presented in this work is underwhelming and limits the conclusions that can be drawn, not providing a solid basis for future studies of in-tissue synapse targeting. However, the complete workflow (excluding the sectioning part) can be adapted for a cryo-FIB approach. The authors should discuss the limitations of their approach. 

      Our manuscript preprint was deposited in the Biorxiv several years before Matsui/Gouaux’s recent ELife paper that reported a novel work-flow for in-tissue cryoET. It is difficult to directly compare data from our and Matsui/Gouaux’s approach because the latter reported a dataset of only 3 tomograms. Note also that Matsui/Gouaux followed our approach of using 20% dextran 40,000 as a cryo-preservative. The use of 20% dextran 40,000 as a cryo-protectant was first established by Zuber et al., 2005 (PMID: 16354833) and shown avoid hyper-osmotic pressure and cell membrane rupture. However, Matsui/Gouaux additionally included 5% sucrose in their cryoprotectant. We did not include sucrose as cryo-preservative because this exerts osmotic pressure and was not necessary to achieve vitreous tissues in our workflow.

      Before high-pressure freezing, Matsui/Gouaux also incubated tissue slices in a HEPES-buffered artificial cerebrospinal fluid (that included 2 mM CaCl2 but did not include glucose as an energy source) for 1 h at room temperature to label AMPA receptors with Fab fragment-Au conjugates. Under these conditions, neurons can elicit both physiological and excitotoxic action potentials (even though AMPARs were themselves antagonised with ZK-200775). The absence of glucose is a concern, and it is unclear to what extent tissue viability is affected by this incubation step. In contrast, we chose to use an NMDG-based artificial cerebrospinal fluid for slice preparation and high-pressure freezing that is a well-established method for preserving neuronal viability (Ting et al., 2018).

      We addressed the supposed limitations of cryo-sectioning versus cryoFIB-SEM in our public response. In particular, we have recently shown that cryo-sectioning produced a  subnanometer resolution in-tissue structure of a protein, that has so far only been achieved for ribosome within cryoFIB-SEM sample preparations. A discussion of cryo-sectioning versus cryoFIB-SEM must be informed by new data that directly compares these methods, which is not the subject of our eLife paper. We also cite a recent preprint directly comparing cryoFIB-milled lamellae with cryo-sections and showing that near atomic resolution structures can also be obtained from the latter sample preparations (Elferich et al., 2025).

      (6) The authors show (in Supplementary) putative tethers connecting SV and the plasma membrane. Is it possible to improve the image quality (e.g. some sort of filtering or denoising) so that the tethers appear more obvious? Can the authors observe connectors linking synaptic vesicles? 

      We have tested multiple iterative reconstruction and denoising approaches, including SIRT and noise2noise filtering in Isonet. We observed instances of macromolecular complexes linking one synaptic vesicle with another. However, there was no question we sought to answer by performing a quantitative analysis of these linkers.

      (7) Figure 4F is missing. 

      Thank you for spotting this omission. We have corrected this in the revised manuscript.

      (8) Most quantifications lack statistical analyses. These need to be included, and only statistically significant findings should be discussed. Terms like "significantly" (e.g. Line 144) should only be used in these cases.

      We used the term ‘significantly’ in the results section (line 143 and line 166 in revised text, we cite figure 1H and 2F showing analyses in which we have in fact performed statistical tests (t-tests with Bonferroni correction) comparing the voxel intensities in regions of the cytoplasm that are proximal versus distal to the postsynaptic membrane. We have amended the main text to include the details of the statistical test that we performed. Also, we neglected to include a description of the statistical test in line 241, which cites Figure 3G. We have corrected this in the revised text.

      Minor comments: 

      (1) Can the authors comment on why only 1-2 grids are prepared per mouse brain (in M&M -section)?

      We prepared only two grids in order to have prepared samples within 2 minutes, to limit deterioration of the sample.

      (2) Figure 1 Supplement 2 and its legend are confusing (averaging of non-aligned versus aligned post-synaptic membrane). Can the authors describe more clearly their molecular density profile analysis?

      We apologise that this figure legend was insufficient. We have included a detailed description of our molecular density profile analysis in the methods section entitled ‘Molecular density profile analysis’. In the revised manuscript we have now also included a citation to this methods section in Figure – figure 1 supplement 2 legend.

      (3) Please clarify with higher precision the areas were recorded in relation to the fluorescent spots (e.g. Figures 3A-C).

      We have included a white rectangular annotation in the cryoCLEM inset panels of Figures 3A-C to indicate the field of view of each corresponding tomographic slice. This shows that PSD95-GFP puncta localise to the postsynaptic compartments in each tomogram.

      (4) Figure 4 Supplement 2D is not clear: the connection between receptors and actin should be shown in a segmentation.

      We agree with the reviewer. A ‘connection’ is not clear, which is expected because the cytoplasmic domain of ionotropic glutamate receptor subunits is composed of a non-globular/intrinsically disordered sequence. We have amended our description of the proximity of actin cytoskeleton to ionotropic glutamate receptor clusters in the main text replacing “associated with” to “adjacent to”.

      (5) Line 341: the reference is referred to by a number (56) at the end of the sentence, rather than by name.

      Good spot. We have corrected this in the revised manuscript.

      (6) Line 968: tomograms is misspelled. 

      Good spot. We have corrected this error (line 1018 in our revised manuscript).

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      (1) On page 11: "The position of (i)onotropic receptor...". 

      Good spot. We have corrected this.

      (2) On page 13: "Slightly higher relative molecular density..." this line ends with a citation to reference '56', but the works cited are not numbered.

      Good spot. We have corrected this in the revised manuscript.

      (3) On page 46: "as described in (69)..." the works cited are not numbered. 

      Good spot. We have corrected this in the revised manuscript.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors): <br /> (1) The title does not do the work justice. The authors make many exciting discoveries, e.g. PSD appearance, new polyhedral vesicles, ionotropic receptor positions, and intermembrane distance changes even within the synaptic cleft, but title their manuscript "The molecular infrastructure of glutamatergic synapses in the mammalian forebrain". It is also a bit misleading, since one would have expected more molecular detail and molecular maps as part of the work, so the authors may think about updating the title to reflect their exciting work. 

      We thank the reviewer for recognising the exciting discoveries in our manuscript. Summarising all these in a title is challenging. We intend ‘molecular infrastructure’ to mean a structure composed of many molecules including proteins (by analogy ‘transport infrastructure’ is composed of many roads, ports and train lines).

      (2) It would be in the spirit of eLife and open science if the authors could submit their segmentations alongside the tomographic data to either EMPIAR or pdb-dev (if they accept it) or the new CZII cryoET data portal for neurobiologists, method developers, and others to use. 

      We agree with the reviewer. We have deposited in subtomogram averaged map of AMPA receptor in EMDB, and all tilt series and 4x binned tomographic reconstructions described in our manuscript (figure 1- table1 and figure 2 -table 2), together with segmentations in EMPIAR.  

      (3) Methods: the authors establish an exciting new workflow to get from living mice to frozen specimens within 2 minutes and perform many unique analyses that would be useful to different fields. Their methods section overall is well described and contains criteria and details that should allow others to apply experiments to their scientific problems. However, it would be very helpful to expand on the methods in the 'annotation and analysis [...]' and "Subtomogram averaging" sections, to at least in short describe the steps without having to embark on a reference journey for each method and generally provide more detail. For the annotation section, the software used for annotation is not listed. Table 1 only contains the list of the counts of organelles etc. identified in each tomogram, no processing details. 

      We have revised the methods section ‘annotation and analysis’ including software used (IMOD). We have also included a slightly more detailed description of subtomogram averaging. We did not include ‘processing details’ because there are none - identification of constituents in each tomogram was carried out manually, as described in the methods section.

      (4) Some of the tomograms submitted as videos may have slipped through as an early version since they appear to be originating from not perfectly aligned tiltseries; vesicles and membranes can be observed 'rubberbanding'. The authors should go through and check their videos. 

      We thank the referee for suggesting we double check our tomogram videos. All movies are representative tomographic reconstructions from ultra-fresh synapse preparations (Figure 1 – videos 1-7) and synapses in tissue cryo-sections (Figure 2 – videos 1-2). We have double checked that the videos correspond to tomograms that were aligned as good as possible. In general, tissue cryo-section tomograms reconstructed less well than ultra-fresh synapse tomograms, which limits the information content of these data, as expected. Consequently, the reconstructions shown in these videos were all reconstructed as best we could (testing multiple approaches in IMOD, and more recent software packages, eg. AreTomo). While we think it is important to share all tomograms, regardless of quality, we were careful to exclude tomograms for analysis that did not contain sufficient information for analysis (as described in the methods section).

      Minor suggestions: 

      (1) Page 13, line 341, reference 56, but references are not numbered. Please update.

      Good spot. We have corrected this in the revised manuscript.

      (2) Page 33, line 746, the figure legend is not referencing the correct figure panels G-K should be I-K;

      We have amended the Figure 3 legend to “(G-K) Snapshots and quantification of membrane remodeling within glutamatergic synapses”.

      (3) Page 33, line 750; reads 'same as E', but should be 'same as G'. 

      Good spot. We have corrected this in the revised manuscript.

      (4) Page 35, Figure 4: Please use more labels: Figure 4B: it would be helpful to use different colors for each view and match to the tomogram - then non-experts could easily relate the projections and real data; Figure 4C: please label domains; Figure 4F: the figure panel got lost. 

      This is an interesting idea. While our subtomgram average of 2522 subvolumes provided decent evidence that these are ionotropic receptors, we are reluctant to label specific putative domains of individual subvolumes in the raw tomographic slice because the resolution of the raw tomogram (particularly in the Z-direction) is worse and may not be sufficient to resolve definitely each domain layer. We hope the reviewer appreciates our cautious approach.

      (5) Page 42, line 933: incomplete sentence. 

      Good spot. We have corrected this in the revised manuscript.

      (6) Page 46, line 1038; Reference 69 is in brackets, but references are not numbered. Please update.

      Good spot. We have corrected this in the revised manuscript.

    1. eLife Assessment

      This paper is an important overview of the currently published literature on low-intensity focussed ultrasound stimulation (TUS) in humans, providing a meta-analysis of this literature that explores which stimulation parameters might predict the directionality of the physiological stimulation effects. The overall synthesis, except for the section on TPS and AD, is convincing though could be streamlined at places. The database proposed by the paper has the potential to become a key community resource if carefully curated and developed.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This paper is a relevant overview of the currently published literature on low-intensity focused ultrasound stimulation (TUS) in humans, with a meta-analysis of this literature that explores which stimulation parameters might predict the directionality of the physiological stimulation effects.

      The pool of papers to draw from is small, which is not surprising given the nascent technology. It seems nevertheless relevant to summarize the current field in the way done here, not least to mitigate and prevent some of the mistakes that other non-invasive brain stimulation techniques have suffered from, most notably the theory- and data-free permutation of the parameter space.

      The meta-analysis concludes that there are, at best, weak trends toward specific parameters predicting the direction of the stimulation effects. The data have been incorporated into an open database that will ideally continue to be populated by the community and thereby become a helpful resource as the field moves forward.

      Strengths:

      The current state of human TUS is concisely and well summarized. The methods of the meta-analysis are appropriate. The database is a valuable resource.

      Suggestions:<br /> - The paper remains lengthy and somewhat unfocused, to the detriment of readability. One can understand that the authors wish to include as much information as possible, but this reviewer is sceptical that this will aid the use of the databank, or help broaden the readership. For one, there is a good chunk of repetition throughout. The intro is also somewhat oscillating between TMS, tDCS and TUS. While the former two help contextualizing the issue, it doesn't seem necessary. In the section on clinical applications of TUs and possible outcomes of TUS, there's an imbalance of the content across examples. That's in part because of the difference in knowledge base but some sections could probably be shortened, eg stroke. In any case, the authors may want to consider whether it is worth making some additional effort in pruning the paper

      - The terms or concept of enhancement and suppression warrant a clearer definition and usage. In most cases, the authors refer to E/S of neural activity. Perhaps using terms such as "neural enhancement" etc helps distinguish these from eg behavioural or clinical effects. Crucially, how one maps onto the other is not clear. But in any case, a clear statement that the changes outlined on lines 277ff do not

      - Re tb-TUS (lines 382ff), it is worth acknowledging here that independent replication is very limited (eg Bao et al 2024; Fong et al bioRxiv 2024) and seems to indicate rather different effects

      - The comparison with TPS is troublesome. For one, that original study was incredibly poorly controlled and designed. Cherry-picking individual (badly conducted) proof-of-principle studies doesn't seem a great way to go about as one can find a match for any desired use or outcome.

      Moreover, other than the concept of "pulsed" stimulation, it is not clear why that original study would motivate the use of TUS in the way the authors propose; both types of stimulation act in very different ways (if TPS "acts" at all). But surely the cited TPS study does not "demonstrate the capability for TUS for pre-operative cognitive mapping". As an aside, why the authors feel the need to state the "potential for TPS... to enhance cognitive function" is unclear, but it is certainly a non-sequitur. This review feels quite strongly that simplistic analogies such as the one here are unnecessary and misleading, and don't reflect the thoughtful discussion of the rest of the paper. In the other clinical examples, the authors build their suggestions on other TUS studies, which seems more sensible.

    3. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewing Editor Comments:

      Focus and Scope:

      The paper attempts to address too many topics simultaneously, resulting in a lack of focus and insufficient depth in the treatment of individual components.

      We have moved this selective clinical review section that was previously Part I in the paper now to Part II, given the importance of leading off with the meta-analysis and resource before doing a selective review, which are now Part I. In the lead in to Part II, we now indicate that the review is not intended to be comprehensive, because there are other recent comprehensive reviews, which we cite. This part of the paper merely aims to generate hypotheses on the directionality of effects ripe for testing on how TUS could be used to excite or suppress function, illustrated with specific clinical examples. The importance of this section, even though not comprehensive, is that it should provide the reader with examples on how the directionality of TUS could be used specifically in a range of clinical applications. The reader will find that the same hypotheses do not apply to different clinical disorder. Therefore, patient specific hypotheses need to be motivated and then subsequently tested with empirical application of TUS, which Part II provides.

      Part II. Selective TUS clinical applications review and TUS directionality hypotheses starts at line 458. Part I, the meta-analysis and resource section starts at line 199, after the Introduction on TUS and the importance on understanding how the directionality of TUS effects could be better understood.

      Strengthening the Meta-Analysis:

      The meta-analysis is the strongest aspect of the paper and should be expanded to include the relevant statistics. However, it currently omits several key concepts, studies, and discussion points, particularly related to replication and the dominance of results from specific groups. These omissions should be addressed even with a focus on meta-analysis.

      We thank the reviewer for their enthusiasm about the meta-analysis, which we have now promoted to Part I in the revised paper. We have substantially updated the latest database (inTUS_DATABASE_1-2025.csv) and ensured that the R markdown script can re-generate all of the results and statistical values. We have inserted additional statistical values in the main manuscript, as requested. The inTUS Resource is located here (https://osf.io/arqp8/ under Cafferatti_et_al_inTUS_Resource), and we have aimed to make it as user friendly to use and contribute to as possible. For instance, the reader can find them all in the HTML link summarizing the R markdown output with all statistical values here: https://rpubs.com/BenSlaterNeuro/1268823, a part of the inTUS resource.

      Since the last submission, there has been a tremendous increase in the number of TUS studies in healthy participants. We have curated and included all of the relevant studies we could find in the 1-2025 database, as the next large expansion of the database (now including 52 experiments in healthy participants). We then reran and report the results of the statistical tests via the R markdown script (starting at line 336). Finally, the online database (inTUS_DATABASE_1-2025.csv) has additional columns, suggested by the reviewers, including one to identify the same groups that conducted the TUS study, based on a social network analysis. The manuscript figures (Table 1 and Table 2) did not have the space to expand the data tables, but these additional columns are available in the database online. Finally, we have ensured that the resource is as easy to use as possible (line 862 has the Introduction to the inTUS Resource – which is also the online READ ME file), and we have been in contact with the iTRUSST consortium leads who are interested in discussing hosting the resource and helping it to become self-sustaining.

      Conceptual Development:

      The more conceptual part of the paper is underdeveloped. It lacks sufficient supporting data, a well-articulated argument, and a clear derivation or development of a concrete model.

      To ensure that the conceptual sections are well developed, we have revised the introduction, including the background on TUS and bases for the interest in the directionality of effects. We have also revised the TUS mechanisms background as suggested by the reviewers. For Part I, the meta-analysis basis and hypotheses we have ensured the rationale is clearer. The hypotheses are based on several lines of research in the animal model and human literature as cited (starting with line 211). For Part II, the selective clinical review, we have revised this section as well to have each section on lowintensity TUS and end in a hypothesis on the directionality of TUS effects. Starting at line 199 we have clarified the scope of the review and ensured that all the relevant experiments in healthy participants (n = 52 experiments) have now been included in the next key update of the resource and meta-analysis in this key paper update.

      Database Curation:

      The authors should provide more detailed information about how the database will be curated and made accessible. They may consider collaborating with ITRUSST.

      We have expanded the information on the Resource documents (starting at line 862) to make the resource as user friendly as possible. At the beginning of the resource development stage we had contacted but not heard from the ITRUSST consortium. Encouraged by this comment we again reached out and are now in contact with the ITRUSST consortium leads who are interested in discussing sustaining the resource. It would be wonderful to have the resource linked to other ITTRUST tools, since it was inspired by the organization. Practically what this means is that the resource rather than being hosted on Open Science Framework, would potentially be hosted on the ITRUSST web site (https://itrusst.com/). These discussions are in progress, but the next key update to the database (1-2025) is already available and reported in this key update to our original paper.

      Reviewer #1: (Public Review)

      Summary:

      This paper is a relevant overview of the currently published literature on lowintensity focussed ultrasound stimulation (TUS) in humans, with a meta-analysis of this literature that explores which stimulation parameters might predict the directionality of the physiological stimulation effects.

      The pool of papers to draw from is small, which is not surprising given the nascent technology. It seems nevertheless relevant to summarize the current field in the way done here, not least to mitigate and prevent some of the mistakes that other non-invasive brain stimulation techniques have suffered from, most notably the theory- and data-free permutation of the parameter space.

      The meta-analysis concludes that there are, at best, weak trends toward specific parameters predicting the direction of the stimulation effects. The data have been incorporated into an open database, that will ideally continue to be populated by the community and thereby become a helpful resource as the field moves forward.

      Strengths:

      The current state of human TUS is concisely and well summarized. The methods of the meta-analysis are appropriate. The database is a valuable resource.

      Weaknesses:

      These are not so much weaknesses but rather comments and suggestions that the authors may want to consider.

      We thank the reviewer for their support of the resource and meta-analysis. We have implemented the suggestions next as follows.

      I may have missed this, but how will the database be curated going forward? The resource will only be as useful as the quality of data entry, which, given the complexity of TUS can easily be done incorrectly.

      We have added a paragraph on how authors could use the Qualtrics form to submit their data and the curation process involved (from line 891). Currently, this process cannot be automated because we continue to find that reported papers do not report the TUS parameters that ITRUSST has encouraged the community to report (Martin et al., 2024). We can dedicate for a TUS expert to ensure that every 6 or 12 months the data base is curated and expanded. The current version is the latest 1-2025 update to the data base. Longer term we are in discussion with ITRUSST on whether the resource could become self sustaining when TUS papers regularly reporting all the relevant parameters such that the database expansion becomes trivial, and then the Resource R markdown script and other tools can be used to re-evaluate the statistical tests and the user can conduct secondary hypothesis testing on the data.

      It would be helpful to report the full statistics and effect sizes for all analyses. At times, only p-values are given. The meta-analysis only provides weak evidence (judged by the p-values) for two parameters having a predictive effect on the direction of neuromodulation. This reviewer thinks a stronger statement is warranted that there is currently no good evidence for duty cycle or sonication direction predicting outcome (though I caveat this given the full stats aren't reported). The concern here is that some readers may gallop away with the impression that the evidence is compelling because the p-value is on the correct side of 0.05.

      We have ensured that the R script can generate the full statistics from the tests and the effect sizes for all the analyses, and now also report more of the key statistical values in the revised paper (starting at line 336). As suggested, we have also ensured that the interpretation is sufficiently nuanced given the small sample sizes and the p-values below 0.1 but above 0.05 are interpreted as a statistical trend.

      This reviewer thinks the issue of (independent) replication should be more forcefully discussed and highlighted. The overall motivation for the present paper is clearly and thoughtfully articulated, but perhaps the authors agree that the role that replication has to play in a nascent field such as TUS is worth considering.

      We completely agree and have added additional columns to the online database to identify unique groups, using a social network analysis, and independent replications. These expanded tables did not fit in the manuscript versions of Tables 1 and 2 but are fully available in the Resource data tables ready for further analysis by interested resource users.

      A related point is that many of the results come from the same groups (the so-called theta-TUS protocol being a clear example). The analysis could factor this in, but it may be helpful to either signpost independent replications, which studies come from the same groups, or both.

      In the expanded database tables (inTUS_DATABASE_1-2025.csv: https://osf.io/arqp8/ under Cafferatti_et_al_inTUS_Resource) we have added a column to identify independent replication.

      The recent study by Bao et al 2024 J Phys might be worth including, not least because it fails to replicate the results on theta TUS that had been limited to the same group so far (by reporting, in essence, the opposite result).

      Thank you. We have added this study and over a dozen recent TUS studies in healthy participants to the database and redone the analyses.

      The summary of TUS effects is useful and concise. Two aspects may warrant highlighting, if anything to safeguard against overly simplistic heuristics for the application of TUS from less experienced users. First, could the effects of sonication (enhancing vs suppressing) depend on the targeted structure? Across the cortex, this may be similar, but for subcortical structures such as the basal ganglia, thalamus, etc, the idiosyncratic anatomy, connectivity, and composition of neurons may well lead to different net outcomes. Do the models mentioned in this paper account for that or allow for exploring this? And is it worth highlighting that simple heuristics that assume the effects of a given TUS protocol are uniform across the entire brain risk oversimplification or could be plain wrong? Second, and related, there seems to be the implicit assumption (not necessarily made by the authors) that the effects of a given protocol in a healthy population transfer like for like to a patient population (if TUS protocol X is enhancing in healthy subjects, I can use it for enhancement in patient group Y). This reviewer does not know to which degree this is valid or not, but it seems simplistic or risky. Many neurological and psychiatric disorders alter neurotransmission, and/or lead to morphological and structural changes that would seem capable of influencing the impact of TUS. If the authors agree, this issue might be worth highlighting.

      We agree that given the divergence in circuits and cellular constituents between cortical and subcortical areas, it is important to distinguish studies that have focused on cortical or subcortical brain areas. The online data tables identify the target region. The analyses can be used to focus on the cortical or subcortical sites for analysis, although for the current version of the database there are too few subcortical sites with which to conduct analyses on subcortical sites. On the second point, that pathology may have affected the results, we completely agree and have clarified that the current database only includes healthy participant experiments for this reason. We are considering future updates to the resource may include clinical patient results (Line 247).

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Minor edits (I wouldn't call them "corrections").

      We sincerely appreciate the constructive comments and have aimed to address them all as suggested.

      Perhaps the most relevant edit pertains to the statistics.

      We now report the more complete statistical results (line 336) and the R markdown script can re-generate all the statistical values for the tests.

      The issue of replication also seems relevant and ought to be raised. This reviewer does not want to prescribe what to do or impose the view the authors ought to adopt.

      In the online version of the data tables for the latest dataset, we have added a column in the data table as suggested that identifies independent groups and replications.

      The other points are left to the authors' discretion.

      We have aimed to address all of the reviewer’s points. Thank you for the constructive input which has helped to improve the expanded database and resource.

      Reviewer #2: (Public Review)

      Summary:

      This paper describes a number of aspects of transcranial ultrasound stimulation (TUS) including a generic review of what TUS might be used for; a meta-analysis of human studies to identify ultrasound parameters that affect directionality; a comparison between one postulated mechanistic model and results in humans; and a description of a database for collecting information on studies.

      Strengths:

      The main strength was a meta-analysis of human studies to identify which ultrasonic parameters might result in enhancement or suppression of modulation effects. The meta-analysis suggests that none of the US parameters correlate significantly with effects. This is a useful result for researchers in the field in trying to determine how the parameter space should be further investigated to identify whether it is possible to indeed enhance or suppress brain activity with ultrasound.

      The database is a good idea in principle but would be best done in collaboration with ITRUSST, an international consortium, and perhaps should be its own paper.

      Weaknesses:

      The paper tries to cover too many topics and some of the technical descriptions are a bit loose. The review section does not add to the current literature. The comparison with a mechanistic model is limited to comparing data with a single model at a time when there is no general agreement in the field as to how ultrasound might produce a neuromodulation effect. The comparison is therefore of limited value.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s assessment and interest in the meta-analysis and database to guide the development of TUS for more systematic control of the directionality of neuromodulation. With this next key expansion of the database (inTUS_DATABASE_1-2025.csv) we have added over a dozen new studies that have been published since our original submission (n = 52 experiments). We have also moved the ‘review’ part of the paper below the meta-analysis and resource description. We have clarified that the clinical review section (now Part II in the revised manuscript) is not intended as a comprehensive review but as a selective review showing how hypotheses on the directionality of TUS effects need to be carefully developed for specific patient groups that require different effects to be induced at specific brain areas. Finally, we have gotten in contact with the ITRUSST consortium leads, as suggested, and are in discussion on whether the inTUS resource could be hosted by ITRUSST. Since these discussions are ongoing practically what this might mean is moving the resource from the Open Science Framework to ITRUSST webpages, which would be a trivial update of the link to the resource in OSF.

      We also sincerely appreciate the time and care the reviewer has given to provide us with the below guidance, all of which we have aimed to take on board in the revised paper.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Line 24/25 - I suggest avoiding using the term "deep brain stimulation" in reference to TUS as the term is normally used to describe electrically implanted electrodes.

      We have removed the term “deep” brain stimulation in reference to TUS to avoid confusion with electrical DBS for patient treatment [Line 24].

      Line 25 - I don't think "computational modelling" has changed how TUS can be done. There is still much to be understood about mechanisms. I think the modelling aspects of the paper should be toned down. Indeed the NICE data that is presented later appears to have a weak, if any, correlation to the outcomes.

      We have revised the manuscript text throughout to ensure that the computational modeling contributions are not overstated, as noted, given the lack of strong correlation to the NICE model outcomes by the meta-analysis including in the latest results with the more extensive database (n = 52).

      Line 32 - "exponentially increasing" is a well-defined technical term and the increase in studies should be quantified to ensure it is indeed exponential. I agree that TUS studies in humans are increasing but a quick tally of the data by year in the meta-analysis reported here doesn't suggest that it follows an "exponential" growth.

      We have changed “exponential” to “to increase”. [Line 32]

      Line 50 - I would suggest using the term sub-MHz rather than 100-1,000 kHz as it is challenging to deliver ultrasound at 1 MHz through the skull. The highest frequency in the meta-analysis is 850 kHz; but the majority are in the 200-500 kHz range.

      We have made this correction to sub-MHz. [Line 54]

      Line 58/59 - Is the FDA publication on diagnostic imaging relevant for saying that 50 W/cm2 is a lowintensity TUS? I think it's perhaps reasonable to say that intensities below diagnostic thresholds are "low intensities" but that is not clear in the text. I would refer to ITRUSST on what is appropriate for defining what is low, medium, or high.

      We have cut the reference to the FDA here since it is, as noted, not as relevant as pointing to the ITRUSST definition.

      Line 65/66 - I agree that ultrasound for neuromodulation is gaining traction and there is an increase in activity, but it also has a long history with the work of the Fry brothers published in the 1950s; and extensive work of Gavrilov in humans starting in the 1970s.

      We have added citations to the Fry brothers and Gavrilov to the text in this section. [Line 69/70]

      Line 75 - I think the intermembrane cavitation mechanism is unlikely to be due to "microbubbles" in a lipid membrane. The predicted displacements are on the order of nanometres, so they are unlikely to generate microbubbles. The work on comparing with NICE is limited. Note there are a number of experimental papers that have reported an absence of intra-membrane cavitation, including the Yoo et al 2022 which is referenced later in the paragraph. Also, there are other models, such as Liao et al 2021 (https://www.nature.com/articles/s41598020-78553-2).

      As suggested, we have removed this phrase on microbubble formation as a likely mechanism. We have also added the Liao paper to this paragraph as it is relevant.

      Line 83 - "At the lower intensities..." it is not clear whether this means all TUS intensities or the lower end of intensities used in TUS.

      We now use the following wording here: “low intensities”. [Line 86]  

      Line 85/86 - "more continuous stimulation" the modulation paradigms haven't been described yet and so pulse vs continuous hasn't been made clear to the reader. Also "more continuous" is very loose terminology. Something is either continuous or it isn't.

      We agree and have removed “more” to be clear that the stimulation is continuous. [Line 88]

      Line 87/88 - "TUS does not .. cavitation ..when ..ISPTA...<14 W/cm2". You can't use ISPTA to determine cavitation. It is the peak negative pressure which is the key driver for cavitation and the MI which is the generally accepted (although grudgingly by some) metric for assessing cavitation risk. You can link the negative pressure to ISPPA but not really to ISPTA. In histotripsy for example the ISPTA is low due to the low duty cycles to avoid heating but the cavitation is a huge effect. Technical terminology is loose.

      We have corrected this to “TUS does not appear to cause significant heating or cavitation of brain tissue when the intensity remains low, based on Mechanical and Thermal Index values and recommendations of use”. [Line 90/91]

      Line 89 - What is meant by "low intensity TUS"? I think all TUS used in the literature counts as low intensity - in that it is below the level allowed for diagnostic imaging.

      We have ensured that the text is focused on TUS being low-intensity and only in the introduction do we distinguish low intensity TUS from moderate and high intensity TUS, such as used for thermal ablation [Lines 62-66].

      Line 88/89 - Most temperature rises in brain tissue in TUS are well below 1 C - will this really change membrane capacitance significantly? If so it would have been good to consider a model for it.

      We have revised this statement as “thermal effects could at least minimally alter cell membrane capacitance…”. [Line 93]

      Line 111 - The text refers to "recent studies" but then the next two references are from 1990 and 2005 which I would argue don't count as "recent".

      We have corrected this wording to “previous studies”. [Line 114]

      Lines 122/129 - This paragraph on TMS pulsing should be linked to the TUS paragraph on pulsing (lines 109/116). The intervening paragraph on anaesthesia is relevant but breaks the flow.

      We have merged the paragraph on anesthesia to the prior one on TUS so that the TMS paragraph is linked more closely to it [starting on line 112].

      Line 130/131 - It is not clear to me that current studies are being guided by computational models. I think there is still no generally accepted theory for mechanisms. If the authors want to do a mechanisms paper then they should compare a few.

      We have revised this as suggested to not overstate the contribution of the limited computational modeling studies throughout the manuscript.

      Line 132 on - There are a number of studies that suggest that NICE is likely not the mechanism by which TUS produces neuromodulation.

      We have revised this sentence as follows: “Although it remains questionable whether intramembrane cavitation is a key mechanism for TUS, the NICE model simulations explored a broad set of TUS parameters, including TUS intensity and the continuity of stimulation (duty cycle) on modelled neuronal responses.” [Lines 139/142]

      Lines 137-140 - Terms are defined after their use. Things like ISPPTA, PRF, TI, and MI have been discussed already and so the terms should have been defined earlier. The authors should think carefully about how the material is presented to make it more logical for the reader.

      We have ensured that the definitions precede the use of abbreviations and have added abbreviations to the tables.

      Part I Line 180-437 - The review of potential applications for TUS reads like an introductory chapter of a thesis. It is entirely proper for a thesis to have a chapter like this, but it is not really relevant for a peer-reviewed research article. There are also numerous applications, e.g. mapping areas associated with decisions, or treating patients with addiction, which are not included, so it is not exhaustive. I would suggest this part be removed.

      We have moved the ‘review’ part of the paper to Part II, given the metaanalysis and resource should be more prominent as Part I. In the review now Part II of the paper we also now make it clear that there are recent comprehensive reviews of the clinical literature ( line 465/467). Namely, the purpose of our selective review is to demonstrate how directionality of TUS effects need to be specific for the clinical application intended, given the great variability in clinical effects that might be desired, brain areas targeted and pathology being treated. We have also aimed to ensure that each section summary is scholarly and academically written to a high level. All the co-authors contributed to these sections so we have also edited to have some consistency across sections, with sections ending with directionality of TUS hypotheses that could be developed for empirical testing.

      Line 453 - It is stated that "ISPTA, which mathematically integrates ISSPA by the sonication DC" It sounds rather grand to mathematically integrate but you can't integrate with respect to DC, you can integrate with respect to time. If you integrate intensity with respect to time over pulse and over the sonication time then one finds that ISPTA = DC x ISPPA, multiplication is also an important mathematical function and should be given its due. Lastly, I think there is a typo and ISSPA should read ISPPA

      We have corrected the typo and the statement to “mathematically multiplies ISPPA by the continuity of sonication”. [Line 221/222]

      Line 454 - I don't think ISPTA is a good measure of "dose." In radiation physics dose is well defined in terms of absorbed energy. The equivalent has yet to be defined for TUS so I would avoid using dose. The ISPTA does relate to TI - although it depends not just on the spatial peak but also on the spatial distribution and the frequency-dependent absorption coefficient of the tissue. I would just avoid the use of "dose" until the field has a better idea of what is going on.

      We have cut this phrase on dose as suggested.

      Page 16 Box 1 - TI is defined as diagnostic ultrasound imaging it is based on. Also, I think TI is dimensionless; it is referenced to a 1-degree temperature rise and so it can be interpreted in terms of celsius or kelvin; but to be technically accurate it is dimensionless.

      We have made TI dimensionless in Box 1

      Page 17 Box 2 - Here you have no units for TI - which is correct but inconsistent with Box 1. But the legend suggests a 2 K temperature rise where as your Box allows for 6 K. The value of 6 is consistent with FDA but my understanding of the BMUS guidelines is the TI must be less than or equal to 0.7 for unlimited time or less than 3 if the duration is less than 1 minute. I accept that the table is labelled FDA limits, but the bold table caption is "Recommendations for TUS parameters" I think you should give the ITRUSST values rather than FDA.

      We have revised this Box legend to better distinguish the FDA and ITRUSST recommendation where they differ (e.g., the importance of ISPTA and the TI values). See revised legend for Box 2.

      Page 18 Box 3 - Not sure what this is trying to show? Also, what is "higher intensity" and "lower intensity"?

      Why not just give a range of values in each box?

      We agree that the higher and lower intensities likely to lead to enhancement or suppression are poorly defined and have noted this in the legend: “Note that the threshold for ISPPA qualifying as ‘higher’ or ‘lower’ intensity is currently poorly understood, or may non-linearly interact with other factors” [Line 751/754, Box 3].

      Line 444 - The hypotheses should be stated more clearly. Maybe I am just dense, but it is not obvious to me from box 3.

      We provide the basis for the hypotheses in the manuscript text on the paragraph [Lines 106-179].

      Line 481/482 - The intensity of a diagnostic ultrasound system is very well characterised. It just might be that the authors didn't report it. It is not clear what is meant by the "continuity." I guess it's to do with pulsing - which is also well defined but perhaps also not reported.

      We agree and have revised this as follows “For the meta-analysis, we only included studies that either reported a basic set of TUS stimulation parameters or those sufficient for estimating the required parameters or those sufficient for estimating the required parameters necessary for the meta-analysis” [Lines 256/258]

      Figure 2 - What is the purpose of this figure? Did you carry out simulations for all the studies? It doesn't seem to be relevant to the data here.

      This figure illustrates the TUS targeting approach and simulations, in this case conducted in k-plan. These were conducted to evaluate approximations to ISPPA in brain values from the studies that did not report these values [Lines 264/268]).  

      Figure 4 - The data in these figures is nice (and therefore doesn't need to have a NICE curve) To me it clearly shows that the data in the literature does not obviously segment into enhancement vs suppression with DC. I suspect it is the same with PRF. I think it would have been better if C and D had PRF on the horizontal axis for on-line and off-line so that effect could be seen more clearly.

      We have kept the NICE curve only for a reference that some readers familiar with the NICE model might want to see overlaid in the figure, but have ensured that the text throughout makes clear that the NICE model predictions are not as statistically robust as initially anecdotally thought. PRF results are not significant but we do show a panel with the PRF measures on one axis (Fig. 4D). Figure 5 also shows box plot results with PRF as well as the other key TUS parameters. Moreover, in the inTUS resource we have provided an app for users to explore the data (https://benslaterneuro.shinyapps.io/Caffaratti_inTUS_Resource/).

      Figure 5 - The text on the axes is too small to read. Was the DC significant for both on-line and offline? What about ISPPA for off-line. At least by eye, it looks as different as DC. Figure 5C doesn't add anything.

      We have boosted the font for Figure 5 and have cut panel 5C since it was not adding much. We have also checked whether DC parameter was significant separately for on-line and off-line effects, but the sample sizes were too small for significance, and the statistical test was not significantly different for Online and Offline effects even in the 12025 database. Therefore they might look stronger for Offline effects in some of the plots in Figure 5, but are currently statistically indistinguishable [Lines 347/348].

      Table 1 - There is a typo in the 3rd column. FF should have units of kHz, not KHz. In addition, SD should have units of s as that is the SI symbol for seconds. I would swap columns 9 and 10 so that ISPPA in water and ISPPA in the brain are next to each other.

      We have corrected the typo in the 3rd column and ensured that units are kHz. SD in the tables has units of ‘s’ for seconds and have put ISPPA in water and in brain next to each other in the data tables.

      Line 767 - "M.K. was supported..." There are TWO MKs in the author list.

      We have changed this to M.Ka. for Marcus Kaiser.

    1. eLife Assessment

      This important study presents a new method for longitudinally tracking cells in two-photon imaging data that addresses the specific challenges of imaging neurons in the developing cortex. It provides compelling evidence demonstrating reliable longitudinal identification of neurons across the second postnatal week in mice. The study should be of interest to development neuroscientists engaged in population-level recordings using two-photon imaging.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript presents a compelling and innovative approach that combines Track2p neuronal tracking with advanced analytical methods to investigate early postnatal brain development. The work provides a powerful framework for exploring complex developmental processes such as the emergence of sensory representations, cognitive functions, and activity-dependent circuit formation. By enabling the tracking of the same neurons over extended developmental periods, this methodology sets the stage for mechanistic insights that were previously inaccessible.

      Strengths:

      (1) Innovative Methodology:<br /> The integration of Track2p with longitudinal calcium imaging offers a unique capability to follow individual neurons across critical developmental windows.

      (2) High Conceptual Impact:<br /> The manuscript outlines a clear path for using this approach to study foundational developmental questions, such as how early neuronal activity shapes later functional properties and network assembly.

      (3) Future Experimental Potential:<br /> The authors convincingly argue for the feasibility of extending this tracking into adulthood and combining it with targeted manipulations, which could significantly advance our understanding of causality in developmental processes.

      (4) Broad Applicability:<br /> The proposed framework can be adapted to a wide range of experimental designs and questions, making it a valuable resource for the field.

      Weaknesses:

      No major weaknesses were identified by this reviewer. The manuscript is conceptually strong and methodologically sound. Future studies will need to address potential technical limitations of long-term tracking, but this does not detract from the current work's significance and clarity of vision.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript by Majnik and colleagues introduces "Track2p", a new tool designed to track neurons across imaging sessions of two-photon calcium imaging in developing mice. The method addresses the challenge of tracking cells in the growing brain of developing mice. The authors showed that "Track2p" successfully tracks hundreds of neurons in the barrel cortex across multiple days during the second postnatal week. This enabled the identification of the emergence of behavioral state modulation and desynchronization of spontaneous network activity around postnatal day 11.

      Strengths:

      The manuscript is well written, and the analysis pipeline is clearly described. Moreover, the dataset used for validation is of high quality, considering the technical challenges associated with longitudinal two-photon recordings in mouse pups. The authors provide a convincing comparison of both manual annotation and "CellReg" to demonstrate the tracking performance of "Track2p". Applying this tracking algorithm, Majnik and colleagues characterized hallmark developmental changes in spontaneous network activity, highlighting the impact of longitudinal imaging approaches in developmental neuroscience. Additionally, the code is available on GitHub, along with helpful documentation, which will facilitate accessibility and usability by other researchers.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The main critique of the "Track2p" package is that, in its current implementation, it is dependent on the outputs of "Suite2p". This limits adoption by researchers who use alternative pipelines or custom code. One potential solution would be to generalize the accepted inputs beyond the fixed format of "Suite2p", for instance, by accepting NumPy arrays (e.g., ROIs, deltaF/F traces, images, etc.) from files generated by other software. Otherwise, the tool may remain more of a useful add-on to "Suite2p" (see https://github.com/MouseLand/suite2p/issues/933) rather than a fully standalone tool.

      (2) Further benchmarking would strengthen the validation of "Track2p", particularly against "CaIMaN" (Giovannucci et al., eLife, 2019), which is widely used in the field and implements a distinct registration approach.

      (3) The authors might also consider evaluating performance using non-consecutive recordings (e.g., alternate days or only three time points across the week) to demonstrate utility in other experimental designs.

    4. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, Majnik et al. developed a computational algorithm to track individual developing interneurons in the rodent cortex at postnatal stages. Considerable development in cortical networks takes place during the first postnatal weeks; however, tools to study them longitudinally at a single-cell level are scarce. This paper provides a valuable approach to study both single-cell dynamics across days and state-driven network changes. The authors used Gad67Cre mice together with virally introduced TdTom to track interneurons based on their anatomical location in the FOV and AAVSynGCaMP8m to follow their activity across the second postnatal week, a period during which the cortex is known to undergo marked decorrelation in spontaneous activity. Using Track2P, the authors show the feasibility of tracking populations of neurons in the same mice, capturing with their analysis previously described developmental decorrelation and uncovering stable representations of neuronal activity, coincident with the onset of spontaneous active movement. The quality of the imaging data is compelling, and the computational analysis is thorough, providing a widely applicable tool for the analysis of emerging neuronal activity in the cortex. Below are some points for the authors to consider.

      Major points:

      (1) The authors used 20 neurons to generate a ground truth dataset. The rationale for this sample size is unclear. Figure 1 indicates the capability to track ~728 neurons. A larger ground truth data set will increase the robustness of the conclusions.

      (2) It is unclear how movement was scored in the analysis shown in Figure 5A. Was the time that the mouse spent moving scored after visual inspection of the videos? Were whisker and muscle twitches scored as movement, or was movement quantified as the amount of time during which the treadmill was displaced?

      (3) The rationale for binning the data analysis in early P11 is unclear. As the authors acknowledged, it is likely that the decoder captured active states from P11 onwards. Because active whisking begins around P14, it is unlikely to drive this change in network dynamics at P11. Does pupil dilation in the pups change during locomotor and resting states? Does the arousal state of the pups abruptly change at P11?

    1. eLife Assessment

      This is an important study providing molecular insight into how cross-talk between histone modifications regulates the histone H3K36 methyltransferase SETD2. The manuscript contains excellent quality data, and the conclusions are convincing and justified. This work will be of interest to many biochemists working in the field of chromatin biology and epigenetics.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, Mack and colleagues investigate the role of posttranslational modifications, including lysine acetylation and ubiquitination, in methyltransferase activity of SETD2 and show that this enzyme functions as a tumor suppressor in a KRASG12C-driven lung adenocarcinoma. In contrast to H3K36me2-specific oncogenic methyltransferases, the deletion of SETD2, which is capable of H3K36 trimethylation, increases lethality in a KRASG12C-driven lung adenocarcinoma mouse tumor model. In vitro, the authors demonstrate that polyacetylation of histone H3, particularly of H3K27, H3K14, and H3K23, promotes the catalytic activity of SETD2, whereas ubiquitination of H2A and H2B has no effect.

      Strengths:

      Overall, this is a well-designed study that addresses an important biological question regarding the functioning of the essential chromatin component. The manuscript contains excellent quality data, and the conclusions are convincing and justified. This work will be of interest to many biochemists working in the field of chromatin biology and epigenetics.

      Weaknesses:

      A minor comment: labels should be added in the Figures and should be uniform across all Figures (some are distorted).

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Human histone H3K36 methyltransferase Setd2 has been previously shown to be a tumor suppressor in lung and pancreatic cancer. In this manuscript by Mack et al., the authors first use a mouse KRASG12D-driven lung cancer model to confirm in vivo that Setd2 depletion exacerbates tumorigenesis. They then investigate the enzymatic regulation of the Setd2 SET domain in vitro, demonstrating that H2A, H3, or H4 acetylation stimulates Setd2-SET activity, with specific enhancement by mono-acetylation at H3K14ac or H3K27ac. In contrast, histone ubiquitination has no effect. The authors propose that H3K27ac may regulate Setd2-SET activity by facilitating its binding to nucleosomes. This work provides insight into how cross-talk between histone modifications regulates Setd2 function. However, the manuscript lacks a clear discussion on how Setd2's in vivo tumor suppressor role and the in vitro mechanistic regulation findings are connected. Additionally, some experiments require more controls and better data quality for proper interpretation.

      Specific comments:

      (1) As for Figure 2F, Setd2-SET activity on WT rNuc (H3) appears to be significantly lower compared to what is extensively reported in the literature. This is particularly puzzling given that Figure 2B suggests that using 3H-SAM, H3-nuc are much better substrates than K36me1, whereas in Figure 3F, rH3 is weaker than K36me1. It is recommended for the authors to perform additional experimental repeats and include a quantitative analysis to ensure the consistency and reliability of these findings.

      (2) The additional bands observed in Figure 4B, which appear to be H4, should be accompanied by quantification of the intensity of the H3 bands to better assess K36me3 activity. Additionally, the quantification presented in Figure 4C for SAH does not seem accurate as it potentially includes non-specific methylation activity, likely from H4. This needs to be addressed for clarity and accuracy.

      (3) In Figure 4E, the differences between bound and unbound substrates are not sufficiently pronounced. Given the modest differences observed, authors might want to consider repeating the assay with sufficient replicates to ensure the results are statistically robust.

      (4) Regarding labeling, there are multiple issues that need correction: In the depiction of Epicypher's dNuc, it is crucial to clearly mark H2B as the upper band, rather than ambiguously labeling H2A/H2B together when two distinct bands are evident. In Figure 3B and D, the histones appear to be mislabeled, and the band corresponding to H4 has been cut off. It would be beneficial to refer to Figure 3E for correct labeling to maintain consistency and accuracy across figures.

      (5) There are issues with the image quality in some blots; for instance, Figure 2EF and Figure 2D exhibit excessive contrast and pixelation, respectively. These issues could potentially obscure or misrepresent the data, and thus, adjustments in image processing are recommended to provide clearer, more accurate representations.

      (6) The authors are recommended to provide detailed descriptions of the materials used, including catalog numbers and specific products, to allow for reproducibility and verification of experimental conditions.

      (7) The identification of Setd2 as a tumor suppressor in KrasG12C-driven LUAD is a significant finding. However, the discussion on how this discovery could inspire future therapeutic approaches needs to be more balanced. The current discussion (Page 10) around the potential use of inhibitors is somewhat confusing and could benefit from a clearer explanation of how Setd2's role could be targeted therapeutically. It would be beneficial for the authors to explore both current and potential future strategies in a more structured manner, perhaps by delineating between direct inhibitors, pathway modulators, and other therapeutic modalities.

    4. Author response:

      We thank the Reviewers for their thoughtful and helpful critiques. Below we provide a point-bypoint response to the comment raised.

      Reviewer #1:

      (1) Labels should be added in the Figures and should be uniform across all Figures (some are distorted).

      We thank the Reviewer for pointing out this issue. As requested, labels have been edited to ensure they are legible and are consistent in font, size, and style.  

      Reviewer #2:

      (1) As for Figure 2F, Setd2-SET activity on WT rNuc (H3) appears to be significantly lower compared to what is extensively reported in the literature. This is particularly puzzling given that Figure 2B suggests that using 3H-SAM, H3-nuc are much better substrates than K36me1, whereas in Figure 3F, rH3 is weaker than K36me1. It is recommended for the authors to perform additional experimental repeats and include a quantitative analysis to ensure the consistency and reliability of these findings.  

      We appreciate the Reviewer’s points. We respectfully suggest that these comments may reflect potential confusion around interpreting how different assays detect in vitro methylation, what data can and cannot be compared, and the nature of the different substrates used. 

      With respect to point 1 (Western signal significantly lower compared to extensive literature): To the best of our knowledge, it would be extremely challenging to make a quantitative argument comparing the strength of the Western signal in Figure 2F with results reported in the literature. Specifically, comparing our results with previous studies would require (1) all the studies to have used the exact same antibodies as antibody signal intensities vary depending on the specific activity and selectively of a particular antibody and even its lot number, (2) similar in vitro methylation reaction condition, (3) the same type of recombinant nucleosomes used, and so on. Further, given that these are Western blots, we do not understand how one could interpret an absolute activity level. In the figure, all we can conclude is that in in vitro methylation reactions, our recombinant SETD2 protein methylates rNucs to generate mono-, di-, and tri-methylation at K36 (using vetted antibodies (see Fig. 2e)). If there is a specific paper within the extensive literature that the Reviewer highlights, we could look more into the details of why the signals are different (our guess is that any difference would largely be due to the use of different antibodies). We add that it might be challenging to find a similar experiment performed in the literature; we are not aware of a similar experiment. 

      With respect to comparing Figure 2B and 2F: We do not understand how one can meaningfully compare incorporation of radiolabeled SAM to antibody-based detection on film using an antibody against specific methyl states. In particular, regarding the question regarding comparing rH3 vs H3K36me1 nucleosomes, we point out that in using recombinant nucleosomes installed with native modifications (e.g. H3K36me1), in which the entire population of the starting material is mono-methylated, then naturally the Western signal with an anti-H3K36me1 antibody will be strong. In Fig. 2b, the assay is incorporation of radiolabeled methyl, which is added to the preexiting mono-methylated substrate. In other words, the results are entirely consistent if one understands how the methylation reactions were performed, how methylation was detected, and the nature of the reagents.

      (2) The additional bands observed in Figure 4B, which appear to be H4, should be accompanied by quantification of the intensity of the H3 bands to better assess K36me3 activity. Additionally, the quantification presented in Figure 4C for SAH does not seem accurate as it potentially includes non-specific methylation activity, likely from H4. This needs to be addressed for clarity and accuracy. 

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. The additional bands observed in Figure 4B represent degradation products of histone H3, not H4 methylation. This is commonly seen in in vitro reactions using recombinant nucleosomes, where partial proteolysis of H3 can occur under the assay conditions.  

      (3) In Figure 4E, the differences between bound and unbound substrates are not sufficiently pronounced. Given the modest differences observed, authors might want to consider repeating the assay with sufficient replicates to ensure the results are statistically robust.

      In Figure 4E, we observe a clear difference between the bound and unbound substrate. To aid interpretation, we have clarified in the figure where the bound complex migrates on the gel, while the unbound nucleosomes migrate at the bottom of the gel. The differences are indeed subtle, which we highlight in the text.  

      (4) Regarding labeling, there are multiple issues that need correction: In the depiction of Epicypher's dNuc, it is crucial to clearly mark H2B as the upper band, rather than ambiguously labeling H2A/H2B together when two distinct bands are evident. In Figure 3B and D, the histones appear to be mislabeled, and the band corresponding to H4 has been cut off. It would be beneficial to refer to Figure 3E for correct labeling to maintain consistency and accuracy across figures. 

      Thank you for pointing this out. To avoid any confusion, we have delineated the H2B and H2A markers and indicate the band corresponding to H4.

      (5) There are issues with the image quality in some blots; for instance, Figure 2EF and Figure 2D exhibit excessive contrast and pixelation, respectively. These issues could potentially obscure or misrepresent the data, and thus, adjustments in image processing are recommended to provide clearer, more accurate representations. 

      Contrast adjustments were applied uniformly across each entire image and were not used to modify any specific region of the blot. We have corrected the issue of increased pixelation in Figure 2D. 

      (6) The authors are recommended to provide detailed descriptions of the materials used, including catalog numbers and specific products, to allow for reproducibility and verification of experimental conditions. 

      We have added the missing product specifications and catalog numbers to ensure clarity and reproducibility of the experiments.

      (7) The identification of Setd2 as a tumor suppressor in KrasG12C-driven LUAD is a significant finding. However, the discussion on how this discovery could inspire future therapeutic approaches needs to be more balanced. The current discussion (Page 10) around the potential use of inhibitors is somewhat confusing and could benefit from a clearer explanation of how Setd2's role could be targeted therapeutically. It would be beneficial for the authors to explore both current and potential future strategies in a more structured manner, perhaps by delineating between direct inhibitors, pathway modulators, and other therapeutic modalities. 

      SETD2 is a tumor suppressor in lung cancer (as we show here and many others have clearly established in the literature) and thus we would recommend avoiding a SETD2 inhibitor to treat solid tumors, as it could have a very much unwanted affect.  Our discussion addresses a different point regarding the relative importance of the enzymatic activity versus other, nonenzymatic functions of SETD2. We believe that a detailed exploration of the therapeutic potential of inhibiting SETD2 would be better suited in a review or a more therapy-focused manuscript.

    1. eLife Assessment

      In this important manuscript, Cassell and colleagues set out on a mechanistic and pharmacological exploration of an engineered chimeric small conductance calcium-activated potassium channel 2 (SK2). They show convincing evidence that the SK2 channel possesses a unique extracellular structure that modulates the conductivity of the selectivity filter, and that this structure is the target for the SK2 inhibitor apamin. While the interpretations are sound and the writing is clear, the manuscript would be strengthened by providing more detailed information for the electrophysiological experiments and the structural analyses attempted, in addition to relating dilation of the filter to mechanisms of inactivation in other potassium channels. This high-quality study will be of interest to membrane protein structural biologists, ion channel biophysicists, and chemical biologists, and will inform future drug development targeting SK channels.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      The small conductance calcium-activated potassium channel 2 (SK2) is an important drug target for treating neurological and cardiovascular diseases. However, structural information on this subtype of SK channels has been lacking, and it has been difficult to draw conclusions about activator and inhibitor binding and action in the absence of structural information.

      Here the authors set out to (1) determine the structure of the transmembrane regions of a mammalian SK2 channel, (2) determine the binding site of apamin, a historically important SK2 inhibitor whose mode of action is unclear, and (3) use the structural information to generate a novel set of activators/inhibitors that selectively target SK2.

      The authors largely achieved all the proposed goals, and they present their data clearly.

      Unable to solve the structure of the human SK2 due to excessive heterogeneity in its cytoplasmic regions, the authors create a chimeric construct using SK4, whose structure was previously solved, and use it for structural studies. The data reveal a unique extracellular structure formed by the S2-S3 loop, which appears to directly interact with the selectivity filter and modulate its conductivity. Structures of SK2 in the absence and presence of the activating Ca2+ ions both possess non-K+-selective/conductive selectivity filters, where only sites 3 and 4 are preserved. The S6 gates are captured in closed and open states, respectively. Apamine binds to the S2-S3 loop, and unexpectedly, induces a K+ selective/conductive conformation of the selectivity filter while closing the S6 gate.

      Through high-throughput screening of small compound libraries and compound optimization, the group identified a reasonably selective inhibitor and a related compound that acts as an activator. The characterization shows that these compounds bind in a novel binding site. Interestingly, the inhibitor, despite binding in a site different from that of apamine, also induces a K+ selective/conductive conformation of the selectivity filter while the activator induces a non-K+ selective/conductive conformation and an open S6 gate.

      The data suggest that the selectivity filter and the S6 gate are rarely open at the same time, and the authors hypothesize that this might be the underlying reason for the small conductance of SK2. The data will be valuable for understanding the mechanism of SK2 channel (and other SK subtypes).

      Overall, the data is of good quality and supports the claims made by the authors. However, a deeper analysis of the cryo-EM data sets might yield some important insights, i.e., about the relationship between the conformation of the selectivity filter and the opening of the S6 gate.

      Some insight and discussion about the allosteric networks between the SF and the S6 gate would also be a valuable addition.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors have used single-particle cryoEM imaging to determine how small-molecule regulators of the SK channel interact with it and modulate their function.

      Strengths:

      The reconstructions are of high quality, and the structural details are well described.

      Weaknesses:

      The electrophysiological data are poorly described. Several details of the structural observations require a mechanistic context, perhaps better relating them to what is known about SK channels or other K channel gating dynamics.

      The most pressing point I have to make, which could help improve the manuscript, relates to the selectivity filter (SF) conformation. Whether the two ion-bound state of SK2-4 (Figure 4A) represents a non-selective, conductive SF occluded by F243 or represents a C-type inactivated SF, further occluded by F243, is unclear. It would be important to discuss this. Reconstructions of Kv1.3 channels also feature a similar configuration, which has been correlated to its accelerated C-type inactivation.

      Furthermore, binding of a toxin derivative to Kv1.3 restores the SF into a conductive form, though occluded by the toxin. It appears that apamin binding to SK2-4 might be doing something similar. Although I am not sure whether SK channels undergo C-type inactivation like gating, classical MTS accessibility studies have suggested that dynamics of the SF might play a role in the gating of SK channels. It would be really useful (if not essential) to discuss the SF dynamics observed in the study and relate them better to aspects of gating reported in the literature.

      The SF of K channels, in conductive states, are usually stabilized by an H-bond network involving water molecules bridged to residues behind the SF (D363 in the down-flipped conformation and Y361). Considering the high quality of the reconstructions, I would suspect that the authors might observe speckles of density (possibly in their sharpened map) at these sites, which overlap with water molecules identified in high-resolution X-ray structures of KcsA, MthK, NaK, NaK2K, etc. It could be useful to inspect this region of the density map.

    4. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      This is a fundamentally important study presenting cryo-EM structures of a human small conductance calcium-activated potassium (SK2) channel in the absence and presence of calcium, or with interesting pharmacological probes bound, including the bee toxin apamin, a small molecule inhibitor, and a small molecule activator. As efforts to solve structures of the wild-type hSK2 channel were unsuccessful, the authors engineered a chimera containing the intracellular domain of the SK4 channel, the subtype of SK channel that was successfully solved in a previous study (reference 13). The authors present many new and exciting findings, including opening of an internal gate (similar to SK4), for the first time resolving the S3-S4 linker sitting atop the outer vestibule of the pore and unanticipated plasticity of the ion selectivity filter, and the binding sites for apamin, one new small molecule inhibitor and another small molecule activator. Appropriate functional data are provided to frame interpretations arising from the structures of the chimeric protein; the data are compelling, the interpretations are sound, and the writing is clear. This high-quality study will be of interest to membrane protein structural biologists, ion channel biophysicists, and chemical biologists, and will be valuable for future drug development targeting SK channels.

      The following are suggestions for strengthening an already very strong and solid manuscript:

      (1) It would be good to include some information in the text of the results section about the method and configuration used to obtain electrophysiological data and the limitations. It is not until later in the text that the Qube instrument is mentioned in the results section, and it is not until the methods section that the reader learns it was used to obtain all the electrophysiological data. Even there, it is not explicitly mentioned that a series of different internal solutions were used in each cell where the free calcium concentration was varied to obtain the data in Figure1C. Also, please state the concentration of free calcium for the data in Figure 1B.

      (2) The authors do a nice job of discussing the conformations of the selectivity filter they observed here in SK as they relate to previous work on NaK and HCN, but from my perspective the authors are missing an opportunity to point out even more striking relationships with slow C-type inactivation of the selectivity filter in Shaker and Kv1 channels. C-type inactivation of the filter in Shaker was seen in 150 mM K using the W434F mutant (PMC8932672) or in 4 mM K for the WT channel (PMC8932672), and similar results have been reported for Kv1.2 (PMC9032944; PMC11825129) and for Kv1.3 (PMC9253088; PMC8812516) channels. For Kv1.3, C-type inactivation occurs even in 150 mM K (PMC9253088; PMC8812516). Not unlike what is seen here with apamin, binding of the sea anemone toxin (ShK) with a Fab attached (or the related dalazatide) inserts a Lys into the selectivity filter and stabilizes the conducting conformation of Kv1.3 even though the Lys depletes occupancy of S1 by potassium (PMC9253088; PMC8812516). What is known about how the functional properties of SK2 channels (where the filter changes conformation) differ from SK4, where the filter remains conducting (reference 13)? Is there any evidence that SK2 channels inactivate? Or might the conformation of the filter be controlled by regulatory processes in SK2 channels? I think connecting the dots here would enhance the impact of this study, even if it remains relatively speculative.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      We thank the reviewers and editors for their careful consideration of our work and pointing out areas where the current version lacked clarity or necessary experiments. Based on the reviews we have made the following significant changes to the revised version:

      (1) Revised the text to focus on the distinct pathogen responses to indole in isolation versus fecal material.

      We believe the key takeaway from this work is that the native context of a given effector, in this case indole, can elicit markedly different bacterial responses compared to the pure compound in isolation. This is because natural environments contain multiple, often conflicting, stimuli that complicate predictions of overall chemotactic behavior. For example, while indole has been proposed to mediate chemorepulsion and contribute to colonization resistance against enteric pathogens, our findings challenge this model. We provide evidence that feces, the intestinal source of indole, actually induces attraction, and that indole taxis may in fact benefit the pathogen through prioritizing niches with low microbial competition. Put another way, the biological reservoir of indole, fecal material, generates an attraction response but indole regulated the degree of attraction.

      Most current understanding of chemotaxis is based on responses to individual, purified effectors. Our study highlights the need to investigate chemotactic responses in the presence of native mixtures, which better reflect the complexity of natural environments and may reveal new functional insights relevant for disease.

      Reviewer comments indicated that these core points above were not clearly conveyed in the previous version, and that the manuscript's logical flow needed improvement. In this revised version, we have substantially rewritten the text and removed extraneous content to sharpen the focus on these central findings. We have also aligned our discussion more closely with the experimental data. While we appreciated the reviewers’ thoughtful suggestions, we chose not to expand on topics that fall outside the scope of our current experiments.

      (2) Provide new chemotaxis data with mixtures of fecal effectors (Fig. 5).

      Related to the above, the reviewers and editors brought up concerns that our discovery of pathogen fecal attraction was underexplored. Although we showed Tsr to be important for mediating fecal attraction, even the tsr mutant showed attraction to a lesser degree, and the reviewers noted that we did not identify what other fecal attractants could be involved.

      Fecal material is a complex biological material (as noted by Reviewer 3) and contains effectors already characterized as chemoattractants and chemorepellents. It would be ideal to be able to perform some experiment where individual effectors are removed from fecal material and then quantify chemotaxis. We considered methods to do this but ultimately found this approach unfeasible. Instead, we employed a reductionist approach and developed a synthetic approximate of fecal material containing a mixture of known chemoeffectors at fecal-relevant concentrations (Fig. 5). We used this defined system as a way to test the specific roles of the Tsr effectors L-Ser (attractant) and indole (repellent) in relation to glucose, galactose, and ribose (sensed through the chemoreceptor Trg), and L-Asp (sensed through the chemoreceptor Tar). We chose these effectors as they have reasonable structure-function relationships established in prior work, and had information available about their concentrations in fecal material. We present these data as a new Figure 5, and also provide videos clearly showing the responses to each treatment (Movies 7-10).

      This defined system provided several new insights that help understand and model indole taxis amidst other fecal effectors. First, the complete effector mixture, like fecal treatment, elicits attraction. Second, L-Ser is able to negate indole chemorepulsion in cotreatments of the two effectors, and also other chemoattractants in the absence of L-Ser also negate this repulsion, albeit to a lesser degree, helping to explain why the tsr mutant still shows attraction to fecal material. Lastly, we also show that the degree of attraction in this system is controlled by indole, with mixtures containing greater indole showing less attraction. We feel this is an important addition to the study because it provides a new view on how indole-taxis functions in pathogen colonization; rather than causing the pathogen to swim away (like pure indole does) indole helps the pathogen rank and prioritize its attraction to fecal effector mixtures, biasing navigation toward lower indolecontaining niches.

      We also acknowledge that this defined system does not capture all possible interactions. Indeed, there are even a few chemoreceptors in Salmonella for which the sensing functions remain poorly understood. Nonetheless, we believe the data offer mechanistic context for understanding fecal attraction and suggest that factors beyond Tsr, L-Ser, and indole also contribute to the observed behaviors, aligning with other data we present.

      (3) Provide new data that show that E. coli MG1655, and disease-causing clinical isolate strains of the Enterobacteriaceae Tsr-possessing species E. coli, Citrobacter koseri, and Enterobacter cloacae exhibit fecal attraction (Fig. 4).

      An important new finding from this study is our direct test of whether indole-rich fecal material elicits repulsion. Contrary to expectations, given that for E. coli indole is a wellcharacterized strong chemorepellent, we show that fecal material instead elicits attraction in non-typhoidal Salmonella.

      Reviewers raised the question of whether our observations regarding indole taxis and attraction to indole-rich feces in Salmonella are similar or relevant to E. coli. While a full dissection of indole taxis in E. coli is beyond the scope of this study and has been the focus of extensive prior research, we sought to address this point by examining whether other enteric pathogens respond similarly to the native indole reservoir, fecal material. To this end, we present new data demonstrating that, like S. Typhimurium, E. coli and other representative enteric pathogens and pathobionts possessing Tsr are also attracted to indole-rich feces (Fig. 4, Movies 4–6, Fig. S4).

      Notably, these new results represent some of the first characterizations of chemotactic behavior in the clinical isolates we examined, including E. coli NTC 9001 (a urinary tract infection isolate), Citrobacter koseri, and Enterobacter cloacae, adding another element of novelty to this work.

      (4) Repeated all of the explant Salmonella Typhimurium infection studies and added a new experimental control competition between WT and an invasion-deficient mutant (invA).

      Although our new colonic explant system was noted as a novelty and strength of this work, it was also seen as a weakness in that some of the results were surprising and difficult to link to chemotactic behavior. Reviewer 3 also brought up the need to be clear about our usage of the term ‘invasion’ in reference to S. Typhimurium entering nonphagocytic host cells, and requested we test an invasion-inhibited mutant (which we do in new experiments, now Fig. S1). We also note that some of the interpretations of these data were made challenging by result variability.

      To help address these issues we performed additional replicates for all of our explant experiments (contained within Figure 1, Fig. S1-S2, and Data S1), to provide greater power for our analyses. These new data provide a clearer view of this system that revise our interpretations from the prior version of this study. While treatment with indole alone does suppress the WT advantage over chemotactic mutants for both total colonization and cellular invasion, essentially all other treatments have a similar result with a timedependent increase in both colonization and invasion, dependent on chemotaxis and Tsr. A remaining unique feature of fecal treatment is an increase in the cellular invaded population of the cells at 3 h post-infection. As requested by Reviewer 3, we provide new experimental data showing that in competitions between WT and an invasion-deficient mutant (invA), with fecal material pretreatment, we see the WT has an advantage only for the gentamicin-treated qualifications, providing some support that our model selects for the invaded sub-population. Although we note that the invA still can invade through alternative mechanisms (as discussed in earlier work such as here: https://doi.org/10.1111/1574-6968.12614), so the relative amount of presumed cellular invasion is less than WT, and not zero, in our experiments (Fig. S1).

      One point of confusion in the previous version of the text was the assay design for the explant experiments, which is important to understand in order to interpret the results. During the explant infection bacteria are not immersed in the effector treatment solution, rather the tissue is soaked in the effector solution beforehand and then exposed to a 300 µl buffer solution containing the bacteria. This means that the bacteria experience only the residue of that treatment at concentrations far lower. We have added clarity about this through revising Fig. 1 to include a conceptual diagram of the assay (Fig. 1C), and added a new supplementary Fig. S5 that summarizes the explant data in this same conceptual model. We provide detail on the method in the text in lines 115-137. In describing the results, and synthesizing them in the discussion, we now state:

      Line 112: “This establishes a chemical gradient which we can use to quantify the degree to which different effector treatments are permissive of pathogen association with, and cellular invasion of, the intestinal mucosa (Fig. 1C).”

      And, a new section in the discussion devoted to describing the explant infections:

      Line: 366: “Our explant experiments can be thought of as testing whether a layer of effector solution is permissive to pathogen entry to the intestinal mucosa, and whether chemotaxis provides an advantage in transiting this chemical gradient to associate with, and invade, the tissue (Fig. 1C, Fig. S5).”

      As mentioned above, we have honed the text to focus on the disparity between the effects of indole alone versus treatments with indole-rich feces to help clarify how these data advance our understanding of the indole taxis in directing pathogenesis. While our explant studies still confirm the role of factors other than L-Ser, indole, and Tsr in directing Salmonella infection and cellular invasion, we now include further analyses of other fecal effectors (described above) that provide some insights into how fecal effectors have some redundancy in their impact.

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The study shows, perhaps surprisingly, that human fecal homogenates enhance the invasiveness of Salmonella typhimurium into cells of a swine colonic explant. This effect is only seen with chemotactic cells that express the chemoreceptor Tsr. However, two molecules sensed by Tsr that are present at significant concentrations in the fecal homogenates, the repellent indole and the attractant serine, do not, either by themselves or together at the concentrations in which they are present in the fecal homogenates, show this same effect. The authors then go on to study the conflicting repellent response to indole and attractant response to serine in a number of different in vitro assays.

      Strengths:

      The demonstration that homogenates of human feces enhance the invasiveness of chemotactic Salmonella Typhimurium in a colonic explant is unexpected and interesting. The authors then go on to document the conflicting responses to the repellent indole and the attractant serine, both sensed by the Tsr chemoreceptor, as a function of their relative concentration and the spatial distribution of gradients.

      Thank you for your summary and acknowledgement of the strengths of this work. We hope the revised text and additional data we provide further improve your view of the study.

      Weaknesses:

      The authors do not identify what is the critical compound or combination of compounds in the fecal homogenate that gives the reported response of increased invasiveness. They show it is not indole alone, serine alone, or both in combination that have this effect, although both are sensed by Tsr and both are present in the fecal homogenates. Some of the responses to conflicting stimuli by indole and serine in the in vitro experiments yield interesting results, but they do little to explain the initial interesting observation that fecal homogenates enhance invasiveness.

      Thank you for noting these weaknesses. We have provided new data using a defined mixture of fecal effectors to further investigate the roles of L-Ser, indole, and other effectors present in feces that we did not initially study. We have refined our discussion of these results to hopefully improve the clarity of our conclusions. We show now both in explant studies (Fig. 1I) and chemotaxis responses to a defined fecal effector system (Fig. 5) that L-Ser is able to abolish both the suppression of indole-mediated WT advantage and also indole chemorepulsion, respectively. We also show the latter can be accomplished by other fecal chemoattractants (Fig. 5). This is in line with our earlier finding that Tsr, the sensor of indole and L-Ser, is an important mediator of fecal attraction but not the sole mediator.

      As this reviewer points out, there are indeed other factors mediating invasion that we do not elucidate here, but we do note these possibilities in the text (lines: 125-127):

      “This benefit may arise from a combination of factors, including sensing of host-emitted effectors, redox or energy taxis, and/or swimming behaviors that enhance infection [5,30,31,35].”

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript presents experiments using an ex vivo colonic tissue assay, clearly showing that fecal material promotes Salmonella cell invasion into the tissue. It also shows that serine and indole can modulate the invasion, although their effects are much smaller. In addition, the authors characterized the direct chemotactic responses of these cells to serine and indole using a capillary assay, demonstrating repellent and attractant responses elicited by indole and serine, respectively, and that serine can dominate when both are present. These behaviors are generally consistent with those observed in E. coli, as well as with the observed effects on cell invasion.

      Strengths:

      The most compelling finding reported here is the strong influence of fecal material on cell invasion. Also, the local and time-resolved capillary assay provides a new perspective on the cell's responses.

      Thank you for acknowledging these aspects of the study.

      Weaknesses:

      The weakness is that indole and serine chemotaxis does not seem to control the fecal-mediated cell invasion and thus the underlying cause of this effect remains unclear.

      In addition, the fact that serine alone, which clearly acts as a strong attractant, did not affect cell invasion (compared to buffer) is somewhat puzzling. Additionally, wild-type cells showed nearly a tenfold advantage even without any ligand (in buffer), suggesting that factors other than chemotaxis might control cell invasion in this assay, particularly in the serine and indole conditions. These observations should probably be discussed.

      Addressed above.

      Final comment. As shown in reference 12, Tar mediates attractant responses to indole, which appear to be absent here (Figure 3J). Is it clear why? Could it be related to receptor expression?

      Thank you for noting this. We now mention this in the discussion. In the course of this work, we encountered a number of apparent inconsistencies, or differences, between what we were observing with S. Typhimurium and what had been reported previously in studies of Tsr function in E. coli. We indeed noted that some studies had investigated a role of Tar for indole taxis (in E. coli), hence why we determined whether, and confirmed, that Tsr is required for indole taxis for S. Typhimurium (Fig. 6).

      We do not know the reason for this apparent difference between the two bacteria, but we have previously shown with our same strain of S. Typhimurium IR715, under the same growth assay, and preparation protocol, that L-Asp is a strong chemoattractant for both WT and the tsr mutant (see Glenn et al. 2024, eLife, Fig. 5G: https://iiif.elifesciences.org/lax:93178%2Felife-93178-fig5-v1.tif/full/1500,/0/default.jpg).

      This supports that this strain of Salmonella indeed has a functional Tar present and is expressed at a level sufficient for sensing L-Asp. So, if Tar generally mediates indole sensing we do not know why we would not see that in Salmonella. Hence, we do not see any role for Tar in indole chemorepulsion in our strain of study, which is different than reported for E. coli, but we cannot confirm the reason.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, Franco and colleagues describe careful analyses of Salmonella chemotactic behavior in the presence of conflicting environmental stimuli. By doing so, the authors describe that this human pathogen integrates signals from a chemoattractant and a chemorepellent into an intermediate "chemohalation" phenotype.

      Strengths:

      The study was clearly well-designed and well-executed. The methods used are appropriate and powerful. The manuscript is very well written and the analyses are sound. This is an interesting area of research and this work is a positive contribution to the field.

      Thank you for your comments.

      Weaknesses:

      Although the authors do a great job in discussing their data and the observed bacterial behavior through the lens of chemoattraction and chemorepulsion to serine and indole specifically, the manuscript lacks, to some extent, a deeper discussion on how other effectors may play a role in this phenomenon. Specifically, many other compounds in the mammalian gut are known to exhibit bioactivity against Salmonella. This includes compounds with antibacterial activity, chemoattractants, chemorepellers, and chemical cues that control the expression of invasion genes. Therefore, authors should be careful when making conclusions regarding the effect of these 2 compounds on invasive behavior.

      Thank you for this comment, and we agree with your point. We hope we have revised the text and provided new data to address your concern. We have also chosen for clarity to keep our text close to our experimental data and so have refrained from speculating about some topics, even though you are absolutely correct about the immense complexity of these systems.

      It is important that the word invasion is used in the manuscript only in its strictest sense, the ability displayed by Salmonella to enter non-phagocytic host cells. With that in mind, authors should discuss how other signals that feed into the control of Salmonella invasion can be at play here.

      Thank you for your recommendation. We have revised the text to hopefully be clearer on our meaning of invasion in regard to Salmonella entering non-phagocytic host cells, essentially changing our usage to ‘cellular invasion’ throughout.

      It is also a commonly-used phrase in reference to enteric infections and the colonization resistance conferred by the microbiome to refer to ‘invading pathogens’ (i.e. invasion in the sense of a new microbe colonizing the intestines), For instance, this recent review on Salmonella makes use of the term invading pathogen (https://www.nature.com/articles/s41579-021-00561-4). We acknowledge the confusion by this dual use of the term. We have mostly removed our statements using invasion in this context. We hope our language is clearer in this revised version.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      It was difficult to understand the true intent or importance of the study described in this manuscript. The first figure in the paper showed that a Salmonella Typhimurium strain lacking either CheY, and thus incapable of any chemotaxis, or the Tsr chemoreceptor, and thus incapable of sensing serine or indole, was modestly inferior to the wild-type version of that strain in invading the cells of a swine colonic explant. It then showed that, in the presence of a human fecal homogenate, the wild-type strain had a much greater advantage in invading the colonic cells. Thus, the presence of the fecal homogenate significantly increased invasiveness in a way that depends on chemotaxis and the Tsr chemoreceptor.

      As human feces were determined to contain 882 micromolar indole and 338 micromolar serine, the effects of those concentrations of either indole or serine alone or in combination were tested. The somewhat surprising finding was that neither indole nor serine alone nor in combination changed the result from the experiment done with just buffer in the colonic explant.

      The clear conclusion of this initial study is that both chemotaxis in general and chemotaxis mediated by Tsr improve the invasiveness of S. Typhimurium. They provide a much bigger advantage in the presence of human feces. However, two molecules present in the feces that are sensed by Tsr, serine, and indole, seem to have no effect on invasiveness either alone or in combination.

      At this point, the parsimonious interpretation is that there is something else in human feces that is responsible for the increased invasiveness, and the authors acknowledge this possibility. However, they do not take what appears to be the obvious approach: to look for additional factors in human feces that might be responsible, either by themselves or in combination with indole and/or serine, for the increased invasiveness. Instead, they carry out a detailed examination of the counteracting effects of indole as a repellent and of serine as an attractant as a function of their relative concentrations and their spatial distributions.

      Thank you for your comments. In our revised version, we have undertaken some additional studies of other fecal effectors that help better understand the relationship between L-Ser and indole, but also the roles of other chemoattractants (glucose, galactose, ribose, L-Asp) in mediating fecal attraction (Fig. 5). We agree with the reviewer and conclude that fecal attraction and the cell invasion phenotype mediated by fecal treatment are influenced by factors other than only Tsr, indole, and L-Ser. Our new data do show that L-Ser is sufficient to block both the invasion suppression effects of indole (negating the WT advantage) and also indole chemorepulsion, therefore making our detailed examination of the counteracting effects more relevant for understanding this system.

      What they find is what other studies have shown, primarily with S. Typhimurium's relative, the gamma-proteobacterium Escherichia coli.

      At high indole and low serine concentrations, the repulsion by indole wins out. At low indole and high serine concentrations, attraction by serine wins out. What is perhaps novel is what happens at an intermediate ratio of concentrations. Repulsion by indole dominates at short distances from the source, so there is a zone of clearing. At longer distances, attraction by serine dominates, so there is an accumulation of cells in a "halo" around the zone of clearing. Thus, assuming that serine and indole diffuse equally, the repulsive effect of indole dominates until its concentration falls below some critical level at which the concentration of serine is still high enough to exert an attractive effect.

      They go on to show, using ITC, that serine binds to the periplasmic ligand-binding domain (LBD) of Tsr, something that has been studied extensively with very similar E. coli Tsr.

      They also show that indole does not bind to the Tsr LBD, which also is known for E. coli Tsr.

      This would be newsworthy only if the results were different for S. Typhimurium than for E. coli. As it is, it is merely confirmatory of something that was already known about Tsr of enteric bacteria.

      An idea that the authors introduce, if I understand it correctly, is that a repellent response to something in feces, perhaps indole, drives S. Typhimurium chemotactically competent cells out of the colonic lumen and promotes invasion of the bacteria into the cells of the colonic lining. If the feces contain both an attractant and a repellent, bacteria might be attracted by the feces to the lining of the intestine and then enter the colonic cells to escape a repellent, perhaps indole. That is an interesting proposition.

      In summary, I think that the initial experimental approach is fine. I do not understand the failure to follow up on the effect of the fecal homogenates in promoting invasion by chemotactic bacteria possessing Tsr. It seems there must be something else in the homogenates that is sensed by Tsr. Other amino acids and related compounds are also sensed by Tsr. Perhaps it is energy or oxygen taxis, which is partially mediated by Tsr, as the authors acknowledge.

      Much of the work reported here is quasi-repetitive with work done with E. coli Tsr. Minimally, previous work on E. coli Tsr should be explained more thoroughly rather than dealt with only as a citation.

      Thank you for your comments.

      We would like to confirm our agreement that E. coli and S. enterica indeed possess similarities. They are Gammaproteobacteria and inhabit/infect the gut. But also we note they diverged evolutionarily during the Jurassic period (ca. 140 million years ago, see: PMC94677). In the context of colonizing humans, the former is a pathobiont, indoleproducer, and a native member of the microbiome, whereas the latter is a frank pathogen and does not produce indole. Hence, there are many reasons to believe one is not an approximate of the other, especially when it comes to causing disease.

      We agree that much of what is known about indole taxis has come from excellent studies in well-behaved laboratory strains of E. coli, a powerful model. We believe that expanding this work to include clinically relevant pathogens is important for understanding its role in human disease. In this study, we contribute to that broader understanding by providing new mechanistic insights into Tsr-mediated indole taxis in S. Typhimurium, along with data demonstrating fecal attraction in other enteric pathogens and pathobionts. These findings help define a more general role for Tsr in enteric colonization and disease. While some of our results indeed confirm and extend prior findings, we respectfully believe that such confirmation in relevant pathogenic strains adds value to the field.

      Regarding our ITC studies, to our knowledge no other study has investigated, using ITC whether indole does or does not bind the LBD (which we show it does not), nor investigated whether it interferes with L-Ser sensing (which we show it does not). Hence, these are not duplicate findings, although we do acknowledge this leaves the mechanism of indolesensing undiscovered. If we are incorrect in this regard, please provide us a citation and we will be happy to include it and revise our comments.

      We now clarify in the text on lines 378-381: “While these leave the molecular mechanism of indole-sensing unresolved, it does eliminate two possibilities that have not, to our knowledge, been tested previously. Overall, our data add support to the hypothesis that a non-canonical sensing mechanism is employed by Tsr to respond to indole [8,18,69].”

      Lastly, as noted by the reviewer, and which we mention in the text, essentially all prior studies on indole taxis were conducted in E. coli, and this is not what is new and novel about the work we present, which is focused on S. Typhimurium and testing the prediction that fecal indole protects against pathogen invasion. We have added in a few additional points of comparisons between our results and prior studies. While we appreciate that much understanding has come from E. coli as a model for indole taxis, we feel discussing prior work in extensive detail would be more suitable for a review and would occlude our new findings about Salmonella, and other enterics.

      In an earlier version of the manuscript, we included more background on E. coli indole taxis. However, we found that the historical literature in this area was somewhat inconsistent, with different assays using varying time points and indole concentrations, often leading to results that were difficult to reconcile. Providing sufficient context to explain these discrepancies required considerable space and, ultimately, detracted from the focus of our current study. Hence, we have only brought in comparisons with E. coli where most relevant to the present work. Also, we provide new data that E. coli also exhibits fecal attraction, and so there is reason to believe the mechanisms we study here are also relevant to that system.

      Some minor points

      (1) Hyphens are not needed with constructs like "naturally occurring" or "commonly used".

      Thank you. Revisions made throughout.

      (2) The word "frank" as in "frank pathogen" seems odd. It seems "potent" would be better.

      Thank you for this comment. Per your recommendation, we have removed this term.

      The term ‘frank pathogen’ is standard usage in the field of bacterial pathogenesis in reference to a microbe that always causes disease in its host (in this case humans) and causes disease in otherwise healthy hosts (example: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1369527420300345). We actually used this specific term to distinguish an aspect of novelty of our study because E. coli can, sometimes, be a pathogen (i.e. a pathobiont) and of course E. coli indole taxis has been previously studied. Ours is the first study of indole taxis in a frank pathogen.

      (3) It is unnecessary to coin a new word, chemohalation, to describe a phenomenon that is a simple consequence of repulsion by higher concentrations of a repellent and attraction by lower concentrations of attractant to generate a halo pattern of cell distribution.

      Thank you for your opinion on this. We have softened our statements on this point, and in the newly revised version of the text less space is devoted to this idea. We now state in line 304-307:

      “There exists no consensus descriptor for taxis of this nature, and so we suggest expanding the lexicon with the term “chemohalation,” in reference to the halo formed by the cell population, and which is congruent with the commonly-used terms chemoattraction and chemorepulsion.”

      We appreciate the reviewer’s perspective and agree that the behavior we describe can be viewed as the result of competing attractant and repellent cues. However, we find that the traditional framework of “chemoattraction” and “chemorepulsion” is often insufficient to describe the spatial positioning behaviors we observe in our system. In our experience presenting and discussing this work, especially with audiences outside the chemotaxis field, it has been challenging to convey these dynamics clearly using only those two terms.

      For this reason, we introduced the term chemohalation to describe this more nuanced behavior, which appears to reflect a balance of signals rather than a simple unidirectional response. More bacteria enter the field of view, but they are clearly positioned differently than regular ‘chemoattraction.’ We also note that Reviewers 2 and 3 did not raise concerns about the term, and after careful consideration, we have opted to retain it in the revised manuscript.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Lines 143-156 seem somewhat overcomplicated and may be confusing. For example: in line 143: "However, when colonic tissue was treated with purified indole at the same concentration, the competitive advantage of WT over the chemotactic mutants was abolished compared to fecaltreated tissue...". But indole was tested alone, so it did not abolish the response; rather the absence of fecal material did.

      We appreciate your point. We have made revisions throughout to help improve the clarity of how we discuss the explant infection data and provide new visuals to help explain the experiment and data (Fig. 1C, Fig. S5).

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Line 46 - Are references 9-11 really about topography?

      Thank you. You are correct. Revised and eliminated this statement.

      (2) Lines 87-89 - It seems to me that a bit more information on this would be helpful to the reader.

      In our revision of the text, to make it more centered on our primary findings of the differences between indole taxis when indole is the sole effector versus amidst other effectors, we have removed this section.

      (3) Line 112 - When mentioning the infection of the cecum and colon, authors should specify that this is in mice.

      Thank you for this comment. In our revised version we provide references both for animal model infections and work in human patients (ex: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/abs/pii/S0140673676921000)

      We have revised our statement to be (Line 99-100: “Salmonella Typhimurium preferentially invades tissue of the distal ileum but also infects the cecum and colon in humans and animal models [42–46].”

      (4) Lines 122-123 - Authors state that "This experimental setup simulates a biological gradient in which the effector concentration is initially highest near the tissue and diffuses outward into the buffer solution.". Was this experimentally demonstrated? If not, authors should tone this down.

      We have removed this comment and instead present a conceptual diagram illustrating this idea (Fig. 1C). Also, addressed by above.

      (5) When looking at the results in Figure 1, I wonder what the results of this experiment would be if the authors tested an invasion mutant of Salmonella. In a strain that is able to perform chemotaxis (attraction and repulsion) but unable to actively invade, would there be a phenotype here? Is it possible that the fecal material affects cellular uptake of Salmonella, independently of active invasion? I don't think the authors necessarily need to perform this experiment, but I think it could be informative and this possibility should at least be discussed.

      Thank you for your comments and suggestions. We have included new data of an explant co-infection experiment with WT and an invasion-deficient mutant invA (Fig. S1). Under these conditions, WT exhibits an advantage in the gentamicin-treated homogenate, but not the untreated homogenate, suggestive of an advantage in cellular invasion.

      However, we did not repeat all experiments with this genetic background. We felt that would be outside the scope of this work, and would probably require dual chemotaxis/invA deletions to assess the impact of each, which also could be difficult to interpret. The hypothesis mentioned by the Reviewer is possible, but we were not able to devise a way to test this idea, as it seems we would need to deactivate all other mechanisms of Salmonella invasion.

      (6) Lines 137-140 - Because this is a competition experiment and results are plotted as CI, the reader can't readily assess the impact of human feces on invasion by WT Salmonella.

      Thank you for pointing this out. We want to mention that the data are plotted as CI in the main text, but the supplemental contains the disaggregated CFU data (Fig. S1-2) and the numerical values (Data S1).

      Please include the magnitude of induction in this sentence, compared to the buffer control.

      The text of this section has been changed to account for new data.

      Additionally, although unlikely, the presence of the chemotaxis mutants in the same infection may be a confounding factor. In order to irrefutably ascertain that feces induces invasion, I suggest authors perform this experiment with the wildtype strain (and mutant) alone in different conditions.

      Thank you for this suggestion, although after careful consideration we have decided not to repeat these explant studies with monoinfections. Coinfections are a common tool in Salmonella pathogenesis studies, including prior chemotaxis studies which our work builds upon (ex: https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC3630101/). The explant experiments, even controlling as many aspects as we did, still show lots of variability and one way to mitigate this is through competition experiments so that each strain experiences the same environment.

      We agree that a cost of this approach is that one strain may affect the other, or may alter the environment in a way that impacts the other. Thus, the resulting data must also be understood through this lens. We have revised the text to stay closer to the competitive advantage phenotype.

      (7) Line 150 - Authors state that bacterial loads are similar. However, authors should perform and report statistical analyses of these comparisons, at least in the supplementary data.

      We have removed this statement as requested. We do note, however, that the mean CFU values across treatments at identical time points appear qualitatively similar, which is an observation that does not require statistical testing.

      (8) Lines 154-154 - This seems incorrect, as the effect observed with the mixture of indole and serine is very similar to the addition of serine alone. Therefore, there was no "neutralization" of their individual effects.

      We have revised this statement.

      (9) Line 159-161 - I strongly suggest authors reword this sentence. I don't think this is the best way to describe these results. The stronger phenotype observed was with the fecal material. Therefore, it is the indole (alone) condition that does not "elicit a response". Focusing on indole too much here ignores everything else that is present in feces and also the fact that there was a drastic phenotype when feces were used.

      Thank you for your opinion on this. We believe this is one of the ways in which our earlier draft was unclear. It was actually a primary motivation of this work to test whether there were differences in pathogen infection, mediated by chemotaxis, in the presence of indole as a singular effector or in its near-native context in fecal material, and our revised text centers our study around this question. We believe this distinction is important for the reasons mentioned earlier.

      Relative to buffer treatment, indole changes the behavior of the system, eliminating the WT advantage, and this is the effect we refer to. We have made many revisions to the text of these sections and hope it better conveys this idea. We expect we may still have differences regarding the interpretation of these results, but regardless, thank you for your suggestions and we have tried to implement them to improve the clarity of the text.

      (10) Line 162 - Again, I disagree with this. Indole does not have an effect to be cancelled out by serine.

      Addressed above, and this text has been changed. Also, we provide new chemotaxis data that at fecal-relevant concentrations of indole and L-Ser, indole chemorepulsion is overridden (Fig. 5).

      (11) Lines 166-168 - Again, this is a skewed analysis. Indole and serine could not possibly provide an "additive effect" since they do not provide an effect alone. There is nothing to be added.

      This text has been deleted.

      (12) Lines 168-170 - Most of the citations provided to this sentence are inadequate. Our group has previously shown that the mammalian gut harbors thousands of small molecules (Antunes LC et al. Antimicrob Agents Chemother 2011). You obviously do not have to cite our work, but there is significant literature out there about the complexity of the gut metabolome.

      Thank you for this comment. We have revised this particular text, but do make mention of potential other effectors driving these effects, which was also requested by the other reviewers.

      Your work and others indeed support there being thousands of molecules in the gut, but our work centers on chemotaxis, and bacteria have a small number of chemoreceptors and only sense a very tiny fraction of these molecules as effectors. Since the impacts of infection of the explants depends on chemotaxis, we keep our comments restricted to those, but agree that there are likely many interactions involved, such as those impacting gene expression.

      Please note our more detailed description of the explant infection assay (and shown in Fig. 1C) that may change your view on the significance of non-chemotaxis effects. The bacteria only experience the effectors at low concentration, not the high concentration that is used to soak and prepare the tissue prior to infection.

      (13) Figure 2 - The letter 'B' from panel B is missing.

      Thank you very much for bringing this oversite to our attention. We have fixed this.

      (14) Legend of Figure 3 - Panel J is missing a proper description. Figure legends need improvement in general, to increase clarity.

      Thank you for noting this. This is now Fig. 6E. We have provided an additional description of what this panel shows. We have edited the legend text to read: “E. Shows a quantification of the relative number of cells in the field of view over time following treatment with 5 mM indole for a competition experiment with WT and tsr (representative image shown in F).”

      We also have made other edits to figure legends to improve their clarity and add additional experimental details and context. By breaking up larger figures into smaller figures, we also hope to have improved the clarity of our data presentation.

      (15) Lines 264-265 - Maybe I am missing something, but I do not see the ITC data for serine alone.

      We have clarified in the text that this was measured in our previous study https://elifesciences.org/articles/93178). The present study is a ‘Research Advance’ article format, and so builds on our prior observation.

      We have revised the text to read: “To address these possibilities, we performed ITC of 50 μM Tsr LBD with L-Ser in the presence of 500 μM indole and observed a robust exothermic binding curve and KD of 5 µM, identical to the binding of L-Ser alone, which we reported previously (Fig. 6H) [36].”

      (16) Lines 296-297 - What is the effect of these combinations of treatments on bacterial cells? I commend the authors for performing the careful growth assays, but I wonder if bacterial lysis could be a factor here. I am not doubting the effect of chemotaxis, but I am wondering if toxic effects could be a confounding factor. For instance, could it be that the "avoidance" close to the compound source and subsequent formation of a halo suggest bacterial death and lysis? I suggest the authors perform a very simple experiment, where bacteria are exposed to the compounds at various concentrations and combinations, and cells are observed over time to ensure that no bacterial lysis occurs.

      Thank you for mentioning this possibility. If we understand correctly, the Reviewer is asking if the chemohalation effect we report could be from the bacteria lysing near the source. Our data actually argue against this possibility through a few lines of evidence.

      First, if this were the case in experiments with the cheY mutant, we would also see an effect near the source. But actually, in experiments with either the cheY mutant or the tsr mutant, neither of which can sense indole, the bacteria just ignore the stimulus and show an even distribution (see current Fig. 6F).

      Second, our calculations suggest that in the chemotaxis assay (CIRA), the bacteria only experience rather low local concentration of indole, mostly I the nM concentration range, because as soon as the effector treatment is injected into the greater volume, it is immediately diluted. This means the local concentration is far below what we see inhibits growth of the cells in the long run and may not be toxic (Fig. 7, Fig. S3).

      Lastly, in the representative video presented we can observe individual cells approach and exit the treatment (Movie 11). Due to the above we have not performed additional experiments to test for lysis.

      (17) Lines 310-311 - Isn't this the opposite of the model you propose in Figure 5? The higher the concentration of indole in the lumen the more likely Salmonella is to swim away from it and towards the epithelium, favoring invasion, no?

      We appreciate the opportunity to clarify this point and apologize for any confusion caused. In response, we have revised the text to place less emphasis on chemohalation, and the specific statement and model in question have now been removed. Instead, we provide a summary of our explant data in light of the other analyses in the study (Fig. S5).

      What we meant here was in relation to the microscopic level, not whether or not a host/intestine is colonized. To put it another way, we think our data supports that the pathogen colonizes and infects the host regardless of indole presence, but it uses indole as a means to prioritize which tissues are optimal for colonization at the microscopic level. The prediction made by others was that bacteria swim away from indole source and therefor this could prevent or inhibit pathogen colonization of the intestines, which our data does not support.

      (18) Lines 325-326 - Maybe, but feces also contain several compounds with antibacterial activity, as well as other compounds that could elicit chemorepulsion. This should be stated and discussed.

      We have removed this statement since we did not explicitly test the growth of the bacteria with fecal treatments. We have refrained from speculating further in the text since we do not have direct knowledge of how that relationship with differing effectors could play out.

      We agree with the reviewer that the growth assays are reductionist and give insight only into the two effectors studied. We provide evidence from several different types of enterics that they all exhibit fecal attraction, and it seems unlikely the bacteria would be attracted to something deleterious, but we have not confirmed.

      (19) Lines 371-374 - How preserved (or not) is the mucus layer in this model? The presence of an inhibitory molecule in the lumen does not necessarily mean that it will protect against invasion. It is possible that by sensing indole in the lumen Salmonella preferentially swims towards the epithelium, thus resulting in enhanced evasion.

      The text in question has been removed. However, we acknowledge the reviewer’s point, and that these explant tissues do not fully model an in vivo intestinal environment. Other than a gentle washing with PBS to remove debris prior to the experiment the tissue is not otherwise manipulated, and feasibly the mucus layer is similar to its in vivo state.

      In mentioning this hypothesis about indole, which our data do not support, we were echoing a prediction from the field, proposed in the studies we cite. We agree with the reviewer that there were other potential outcomes of indole impacting chemotaxis and invasion, and indeed our data supports that.

      (20) Lines 394-395 - The authors need to remember that the ability to invade the intestinal epithelium is not only a product of chemoattraction and repulsion forces. Several compounds in the gut are used by Salmonella as cues to alter invasion gene expression. See PMID: 25073640, 28754707, 31847278, and many others.

      Thank for you for this point, and we now include these citations. We have revised the text in question, stating:

      “In addition to the factors we have investigated, it is already well-established in the literature that the vast metabolome in the gut contains a complex repertoire of chemicals that modulate Salmonella cellular invasion, virulence, growth, and pathogenicity [79–81].”

      Our intent is not to diminish the role of other intestinal chemicals but rather to put our new findings into the context of bacterial pathogenesis. We do provide evidence that specific chemoeffectors present in fecal material alter where bacteria localize through chemotaxis, which is one method of control over colonization.

      (21) Line 408 - I think it could be hard to observe this using your experimental approach.

      Because you need to observe individual cells, the number of cells you observe is relatively small. If, in a bet-hedging strategy, the proportion of cells that were chemoattracted to indole was relatively low you likely would not be able to distinguish it from an occasional distribution close to the repellent source. You may or may not want to discuss this.

      Thank you for this observation. It is indeed challenging to both observe large scale population behaviors and also the behaviors of individual cells in the same experiment. Our ability to make this distinction is similar to the approach used in the study we cite, so that is our comparison.

      But, if there was a subpopulation that was attracted we would predict a ‘bull’s-eye’ population structure, with some cells attracted and other avoiding the source, which we do not see - we see the halo. So, we find no evidence of the bet-hedging response seen in a different study using E. coli and using different time scales than we have.

      (22) Lines 410-411 - What could the other attractants be? Would it be possible/desirable to speculate on this?

      We have changed the text here, but we present new data that examines some of these other attractants (Fig. 5).

      (23) Line 431 - What exactly do you mean by "running phenotype"? Please, provide a brief explanation.

      We have removed this text, but a running phenotype means the swimming bacteria rarely make direction changes (i.e. tumbles), which has been associated with promoting contact with the epithelium, described in the references we cite. Hence, this type of swimming behavior could contribute to the effects we observe in the explant studies, potentially explaining some of the Tsr-mediated advantage that was not dependent on L-Ser/indole.

      (24) Line 441 - Other work has shown that feces contain inhibitors of invasion gene expression. The authors should integrate this knowledge into their model. In fact, indole has been shown to repress host cell invasion by Salmonella, so it is important that authors understand and discuss the fact that the impact of indole is multifaceted and not only a reflection of its action as a chemorepellent. PMID: 29342189, 22632036.

      We agree with the reviewer about this point, and mention this in the text (lines 55-57): “Indole is amphipathic and can transit bacterial membranes to regulate biofilm formation and motility, suppress virulence programs, and exert bacteriostatic and bactericidal effects at high concentrations [16–18,20–22].”

      We have added in the references suggested.

      What we test here is the specific hypothesis made by others in the field about indole chemorepulsion serving to dissuade pathogens from colonizing.

      For instance, the statement from: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0190613

      “Since indole is also a chemorepellent for EHEC [23], it is intriguing to speculate that in addition to attenuating Salmonella virulence, indole also attenuates the recruitment and directed migration of Salmonella to its infection niche in the GI tract.”

      And from: https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1916974117

      “We propose that indole spatially segregates cells based on their state of adaptation to repel invaders while recruiting beneficial resident bacteria to growing microbial communities within the GI tract.”

      And

      “Thus, foreign ingested bacteria, including invading pathogens such as E. coli O157:H7 and S. enterica, are likely to be prevented by indole from gaining a foothold in the mucosa.”

      As shown by others, indole certainly does have many roles in controlling pathogenesis, and there are other chemicals we do not investigate that control invasion and bacterial growth, but we keep our statements here restricted to chemotaxis since that is what are experiments and data show.

      (25) Line 472 - "until fully motile". How long did this take, how variable was it, and how was it determined?

      Thank you for asking for this clarification. We have added that the time was between 1-2 h, and confirmed visually. Our methods are similar to those described in earlier chemotaxis studies (ex: 10.1128/jb.182.15.4337-4342.2000).

      (26) Line 487 - I worry that the fact fecal samples were obtained commercially means that compound stability/degradation may be a factor to consider here. How long had the sample been in storage? Is this information available?

      Thank you for this question. We agree that the fecal sample we used serves as a model system and we cannot rule out that handling by the supplier could potentially alter its contents in some way that would impact bacterial chemosensing. However, we note that the measurements of L-Ser and indole we obtained are in the appropriate range for what other studies have shown.

      The fecal sample used for all work in the study were from a single healthy human donor, obtained from Lee Biosolutions (https://www.leebio.com/product/395/fecal-stool-samplehuman-donor-991-18). The supplier did not state the explicit date of collection, nor indicated any specific handline or storage methods that would obviously degrade its native metabolites, but we cannot rule that out. In our hands, the fecal sample was collected and kept frozen at -20 C. For research purposes, portions were extracted and thawed as needed, maintaining the frozen state of the original sample to limit degradation from freeze-thaws.

    2. eLife Assessment

      In this manuscript, Franco and colleagues present compelling evidence that fecal extracts containing high concentrations of indole, a known repellent, enhance rather than protect against invasion of colonic tissue by Salmonella. The authors describe important findings that lead to the conclusion that the competing effects of attractants present in fecal matter, including L-serine, also sensed by the Tsr chemoreceptor that senses indole, override the repulsive effect of indole.

    3. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The study shows, perhaps surprisingly, that human fecal homogenates enhance the invasiveness of Salmonella typhimurium into cells of a swine colonic explant. This effect is only seen with chemotactic cells that express the chemoreceptor Tsr. However, two molecules sensed by Tsr that are present at significant concentrations in the fecal homogenates, the repellent indole and the attractant serine, do not, either by themselves or together at the concentrations in which they are present in the fecal homogenates, show this same effect. The authors then go on to study the conflicting repellent response to indole and attractant response to serine in a number of different in vitro assays.

      Strengths:

      The demonstration that homogenates of human feces enhance the invasiveness of chemotactic Salmonella Typhimurium in a colonic explant is unexpected and interesting. The authors then go on to document the conflicting responses to the repellent indole and the attractant serine, both sensed by the Tsr chemoreceptor, as a function of their relative concentration and the spatial distribution of gradients.

      Weaknesses:

      The authors do not identify what is the critical compound or combination of compounds in the fecal homogenate that gives the reported response of increased invasiveness. They show it is not indole alone, serine alone, or both in combination that have this effect, although both are sensed by Tsr and both are present in the fecal homogenates. Some of the responses to conflicting stimuli by indole and serine in the in vitro experiments yield interesting results, but they do little to explain the initial interesting observation that fecal homogenates enhance invasiveness.

    4. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, Franco and colleagues describe careful analyses of Salmonella chemotactic behavior in the presence of conflicting environmental stimuli. By doing so, the authors describe that this human pathogen integrates signals from a chemoattractant and a chemorepellent into an intermediate "chemohalation" phenotype.

      Strengths:

      The study was clearly well-designed and well-executed. The methods used are appropriate and powerful. The manuscript is very well written, and the analyses are sound. This is an interesting area of research, and this work is a positive contribution to the field.

      Weaknesses:

      No significant weaknesses noted.

    1. eLife Assessment

      This is a potentially useful study that provides solid, yet confirmatory structural findings about the complex (FtsEX) that controls peptidoglycan remodeling during bacterial cell division. The authors capitalize on the fact that ATP binding stabilizes the FtsEX complex allowing structural characterization for this system. A model is then developed to explain ATP regulation but there is a gap between the model presented here and in vivo data reported previously.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      In this paper, Li and colleagues overcome solubility problems to determine the structure of FtsEX bound to EnvC from E. coli.

      Strengths:

      The structural work is well done, and the work is consistent with previous work on the structure of this complex from P. aerugionsa.

      Weaknesses:

      The model does not take into account all the information that the authors obtained, as well as known in vivo data.

      The work lacks a clear comparison to the Pseudomonas structure highlighting new information that was obtained so that it is readily available to the reader.

      The authors set out to obtain the structure of FtsEX-EnvC complex from E. coli. Previously, they were unable to do so but were able to determine the structure of the complex from P. aeruginosa. Here they persisted in attacking the E. coli complex since more is known about its involvement in cell division and there is a wealth of mutants in E. coli. The structural work is well done and recapitulates the results this lab obtained with this complex from P. aeruginosa. It would be helpful to compare more directly the results obtained here with the E. coli complex with the previously reported P. aeruginosa complex - are they largely the same or has some insight been obtained from the work that was not present in the previous complex from P. aeruginosa. This is particularly the case in discussing the symmetrical FtsX dimer binding to the asymmetrical EnvC, since this is emphasized in the paper. However, Figures 3C & D of this paper appear similar to Figures 2D & E of the P. aeruginosa structure. Presumably, the additional information obtained and presented in Figure 4 is due to the higher resolution, but this needs to be highlighted and discussed to make it clear to a general audience.

      The main issue is the model (Figure 6). In the model ATP is shown to bind to FtsEX before EnvC, however, in Figure 1c, it is shown that ADP is sufficient to promote binding of FtsEX to EnvC.

      The work here is all done in vitro, however, information from in vivo needs to be considered. In vivo results reveal that the ATP-binding mutant FtsE(D162N)X promotes the recruitment of EnvC (Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2011 108:E1052-60). Thus, even FtsEX in vivo can bind EnvC without ATP (not sure if this mutant can bind ADP).

      Perhaps the FtsE protein from E. coli has to have bound nucleotides to maintain its 3D structure.

      Comments after revision:

      The most interesting aspect of this complex is that it has yet to be determined the order of events in the ATPase cycle as the authors acknowledge. Although the authors have responded quite well to the comments, I am still worried about the significance of the in vitro results compared to the in vivo results reported by others. In vivo ATP binding does not appear required for complex formation (of course it is possible that ADP is responsible in vivo). Have the authors tried to solve the complex with ADP since they suggested that it is sufficient to hold the complex together). If possible, it would confirm the role of ATP binding by comparing the structures. Also, it is not clear if ADP binds to any of the mutants made by the Bernhardt lab (D162N, K41M). If they do not bind ADP then FtsEX without nucleotide is able to bind EnvC as the authors indicate is the case in Pseudomonas. It is also unclear the significance of the ATPase activity of FtsEX in vitro with or without EnvC. Could the activity be some basal activity that is not relevant to the in vivo situation. If EnvC caused FtsEX to hydrolyze ATP it would be a futile cycle as FtsEX and EnvC are localized to the septum long before they are involved septal hydrolysis.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      Peptidoglycan remodeling, particularly that carried out by enzymes known as amidases, is essential for the later stages of cell division including cell separation. In E. coli, amidases are generally activated by the periplasmic proteins EnvC (AmiA and AmiB) and NlpD (AmiC). The ABC family member, FtsEX, in turn, has been implicated as a modulator of amidase activity through interactions with EnvC. Specifically, how FtsEX regulates EnvC activity in the context of cell division remains unclear.

      Strengths:

      Li et al. make two primary contributions to the study of FtsEX. The first, the finding that ATP binding stabilizes FtsEX in vitro, enables the second, structural resolution of full-length FtsEX both alone (Figure 2) and in combination with EnvC (Figure 3). Leveraging these findings, the authors demonstrate that EnvC binding stimulates FtsEX-mediated ATP hydrolysis approximately two-fold. The authors present structural data suggesting EnvC binding leads to a conformational change in the complex. Biochemical reconstitution experiments (Figure 5) provide compelling support for this idea.

      Weaknesses:

      The potential impact of the study is curtailed by the lack of experiments testing the biochemical or physiological relevance of the model which is derived almost entirely from structural data.

      Altogether the data support a model in which interaction with EnvC, results in a conformational change stimulating ATP hydrolysis by FtsEX and EnvC-mediated activation of the amidases, AmiA and AmiB. However, the study is limited in both approach and scope. The importance of interactions revealed in the structures to the function of FtsEX and its role in EnvC activation are not tested. Adding biochemical and/or in vivo experiments to fill in this gap would allow the authors to test the veracity of the model and increase the appeal of the study beyond the small number of researchers specifically interested in FtsEX.

      Comments after revision:

      Although I appreciate the authors' desire to save future biochemical experiments for a separate publication, the lack of in vitro data verifying their model makes it challenging to reconcile with published studies from other groups. The other reviewer's point about EnvC activating FtsEX ATPase activity resulting in a futile cycle since both are recruited to the septum well before constriction, is a good example of the disconnect between the model presented here and in vivo data.

    4. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      In this paper, Li and colleagues overcome solubility problems to determine the structure of FtsEX bound to EnvC from E. coli.

      Strengths:

      The structural work is well done and the work is consistent with previous work on the structure of this complex from P. aerugionsa.

      Weaknesses:

      The model does not take into account all information that the authors obtained as well as known in vivo data.

      The work lacks a clear comparison to the Pseudomonas structure highlighting new information that was obtained so that it is readily available to the reader.

      The authors set out to obtain the structure of FtsEX-EnvC complex from E. coli. Previously, they were unable to do so but were able to determine the structure of the complex from P. aeruginosa. Here they persisted in attacking the E. coli complex since more is known about its involvement in cell division and there is a wealth of mutants in E. coli. The structural work is well done and recapitulates the results this lab obtained with this complex from P. aeruginosa. It would be helpful to compare more directly the results obtained here with the E. coli complex with the previously reported P. aeruginosa complex - are they largely the same or has some insight been obtained from the work that was not present in the previous complex from P. aeruginosa. This is particularly the case in discussing the symmetrical FtsX dimer binding to the asymmetrical EnvC, since this is emphasized in the paper. However, Figures 3C & D of this paper appear similar to Figures 2D & E of the P. aeruginosa structure. Presumably, the additional information obtained and presented in

      Figure 4 is due to the higher resolution, but this needs to be highlighted and discussed to make it clear to a general audience.

      The main issue is the model (Figure 6). In the model ATP is shown to bind to FtsEX before EnvC, however, in Figure 1c it is shown that ADP is sufficient to promote binding of FtsEX to EnvC.

      The work here is all done in vitro, however, information from in vivo needs to be considered. In vivo results reveal that the ATP-binding mutant FtsE(D162N)X promotes the recruitment of EnvC (Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2011 108:E1052-60). Thus, even FtsEX in vivo can bind EnvC without ATP (not sure if this mutant can bind ADP).

      Perhaps the FtsE protein from E. coli has to have bound nucleotides to maintain its 3D structure.

      Thank you for your thoughtful feedback and valuable suggestions. We have carefully revised the manuscript to address these concerns, incorporating additional analysis and discussion to enhance clarity and improve the accuracy of our interpretation.

      Regarding the relationship between EnvC binding and nucleotide binding to FtsEX, our previous study on P. aeruginosa FtsEX demonstrated that FtsEX can bind EnvC even in the absence of nucleotide (PMID: 37186861, Fig. 3C). However, for E. coli FtsEX (Fig. S1 in this study), ATP is required to stabilize the complex in vitro, preventing us from directly testing whether EnvC binding is ATP-dependent. The reviewer raised an important point about the FtsED162N mutant study, from which previous studies suggests that this mutant may still retain ATP binding, as observed in its homolog MacB (PMID: 29109272, PMID: 32636250). Additionally, previous work (PMID: 22006325) has shown that the PLD domain of FtsX can bind EnvC directly, even in the absence of the NBD domain, a finding further supported by Crow’s lab (PMID: 33097670). Taken together, these studies indicate that EnvC binding to FtsEX is likely nucleotideindependent, while ATP binding primarily stabilizes FtsE dimerization, reinforcing FtsEX complex formation.

      In line with these findings, our results suggest a stabilizing role of ATP in FtsEX assembly, whereas EnvC binding does not appear to be nucleotide-dependent. However, we acknowledge that the precise sequence of ATP binding and EnvC recruitment within the cell remains unresolved. To reflect this, we have revised the manuscript to incorporate these insights (L190-201, L445-451), clearly stated the limitations (L450-451, L887-890), and updated our model (Fig. 6) to avoid assigning a definitive sequence to EnvC and ATP binding.

      Additionally, we have strengthened the structural comparison between E. coli and P. aeruginosa FtsEX, as the reviewer suggested. We have now included a detailed comparative analysis (L282-306, Fig. S9), which reveals that the transmembrane and nucleotide-binding domains are highly superimposable. The primary structural distinction lies in a slight tilting difference in the bound EnvC, which appears to stem from the conformation of the X-lobes within the PLD domains. Highlighting these differences helps clarify how our new structural data provide additional insights beyond what was previously observed in P. aeruginosa.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      Peptidoglycan remodeling, particularly that carried out by enzymes known as amidases, is essential for the later stages of cell division including cell separation. In E. coli, amidases are generally activated by the periplasmic proteins EnvC (AmiA and AmiB) and NlpD (AmiC). The ABC family member, FtsEX, in turn, has been implicated as a modulator of amidase activity through interactions with EnvC. Specifically how FtsEX regulates EnvC activity in the context of cell division remains unclear.

      Strengths:

      Li et al. make two primary contributions to the study of FtsEX. The first, the finding that ATP binding stabilizes FtsEX in vitro, enables the second, structural resolution of fulllength FtsEX both alone (Figure 2) and in combination with EnvC (Figure 3). Leveraging these findings, the authors demonstrate that EnvC binding stimulates FtsEX-mediated ATP hydrolysis approximately two-fold. The authors present structural data suggesting EnvC binding leads to a conformational change in the complex. Biochemical reconstitution experiments (Figure 5) provide compelling support for this idea.

      Weaknesses:

      The potential impact of the study is curtailed by the lack of experiments testing the biochemical or physiological relevance of the model which is derived almost entirely from structural data.

      Altogether the data support a model in which interaction with EnvC, results in a conformational change stimulating ATP hydrolysis by FtsEX and EnvC-mediated activation of the amidases, AmiA and AmiB. However, the study is limited in both approach and scope. The importance of interactions revealed in the structures to the function of FtsEX and its role in EnvC activation are not tested. Adding biochemical and/or in vivo experiments to fill in this gap would allow the authors to test the veracity of the model and increase the appeal of the study beyond the small number of researchers specifically interested in FtsEX.

      Thank you for your thoughtful review and constructive feedback. We appreciate your recognition of our study’s contributions, particularly the structural resolution of fulllength E coli FtsEX, its interaction with EnvC, and our biochemical characterization of EnvC-stimulated ATP hydrolysis.

      We understand the importance of further biochemical and in vivo validation to support our model. While our study primarily provides a structural framework for understanding FtsEX function, many key residues identified in our E. coli structures have already been tested in prior cell physiological studies. For example, residues critical for the FtsEXEnvC interaction were examined in our collaborator David Roper’s lab in collaboration with Crow’s lab (PMID: 33097670, L319-321).

      With the structural blueprint provided by our full-length E. coli FtsEX-EnvC complex, we now have a foundation to explore several key functional aspects of this system. Future mutagenesis studies will help dissect the roles of specific residues in ATP binding/hydrolysis, coupling between the TMD and NBD domains, interactions between the PLD and TMD domains of FtsX, and signal transduction from the NBD, through the TMD and PLD to EnvC. Additionally, we aim to investigate how the symmetrical PLD domain recruits asymmetrical EnvC and how the dynamics of PLD of FtsX and CCD domains of EnvC contribute to the complex’s function.

      As these experiments require specialized expertise in cell physiology and PG degradation assays, we are actively collaborating with experts in these areas to pursue them. We are committed to furthering this work and providing deeper biochemical and in vivo insights into the function of the FtsEX complex in cell division.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      (1) As mentioned, two things could strengthen the paper. One is to take into account that ADP or possibly nucleotide-free FtsEX can bind EnvC. The second is to highlight any differences between the structures from E. coli and P. aeruginosa.

      Thank you for these insightful suggestions. In our revision, we have (1) carefully considered the possibility of EnvC binding independently of nucleotide and (2) have incorporated a detailed comparison between the newly obtained E. coli FtsEX/EnvC structure and that of P. aeruginosa.

      Regarding the relationship between EnvC binding and ATP binding to FtsEX, our previous study on P. aeruginosa FtsEX demonstrated that FtsEX can bind EnvC in the absence of nucleotide (PMID: 37186861, Fig 3C). However, for E. coli FtsEX systems (Fig S1 in this study), ATP is necessary for FtsEX stabilization in vitro, which limited us from further directly testing whether EnvC binding is ATP-dependent or not.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s reference to the FtsE(D162N) mutant study. Previous studies suggest that D162N mutant may still retain ATP binding, similar to its homolog MacB (PMID: 29109272; PMID: 32636250). Additionally, findings from Winkler’s lab (PMID: 22006325) indicate that the PLD domain of FtsX can bind EnvC directly, even in the absence of the NBD domain, a result further supported by study from Crow’s lab (PMID: 33097670). Collectively, these studies suggest that EnvC binding to FtsEX is nucleotide-independent, while ATP binding likely stabilizes FtsE dimerization, thereby reinforcing FtsEX complex formation, as the reviewer suggested.

      Thus, consistent with previous studies, our results so far support a stabilizing role of ATP in FtsEX assembly, while EnvC binding itself does not appear to be nucleotidedependent. However, the available evidence remains inconclusive, and the precise sequence of ATP binding and EnvC recruitment within the cell is still unclear. In our revision, we have now incorporated these analyses in L190-201 and L445-451, stated the limitations (L450-451 and L887-890) and updated our model (Fig. 6) to avoid assigning a definitive sequence to EnvC and ATP binding.

      For the structural comparison between E. coli and P. aeruginosa FtsEX, we have added a detailed analysis in L282-306 and Supplementary figure 9. In summary, we found that the transmembrane domain and nucleotide-binding domain are highly superimposable, with only minor differences observed. The primary distinction lies in a slight tilting difference in the bound EnvC, which appears to come from the conformation of the X-lobes within the PLD domains.

      (2) Line 129. Concerning the role of ATP in stabilizing the complex. It is clear that ADP can do it as well (Figure 1c). This is mentioned in line 131 but not considered in the model.

      Thank you for pointing this out. We have now revised the relevant sections in the manuscript (L190-201 and L445-451) and updated the model (Fig 6) accordingly. In the revised manuscript, we acknowledge the reviewer’s point that ATP may primarily serve to stabilize the FtsEX complex. Additionally, we have explicitly clarified that EnvC binding appears to be nucleotide-independent. Regarding the model, we state that the current study does not provide sufficient evidence to determine the precise sequence of EnvC and ATP binding to FtsEX in the cell. We believe these revisions, incorporating the reviewer’s suggestions, improve the accuracy of our interpretation.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      (1) The introduction is written for an audience with significant expertise in bacterial PG synthesis and is thus difficult for those outside the field to follow.

      Thank you for your feedback. We have revised the introduction, particularly the first passage (L51–63), to improve readability and make it more accessible to a broader audience.

      (1) Figure 1: Please express ATP hydrolysis data in ATP/FtsEX/minute. (It is currently nmol/mg/min).

      Changed accordingly, thank you!

      (2) Figure 4: Please clarify in the legend and in the figure itself which structures correspond to full-length data from cryoEM data or truncated (FtsEX-PLD domain) protein data from previous crystallographic studies.

      Both the FtsEX and FtsEX/EnvC complex structures shown in Figure 4 were obtained from our cryo-EM data using full-length proteins. To avoid any confusion, we have now further clarified this in the figure legend (L857).

    1. eLife Assessment

      This valuable work used molecular biology, cell biology, and genetic approaches to unravel individual genes and potential pathways that contribute to paternal mitochondrial inheritance using C. elegans as the model organism. Their microscopy method is cutting edge, with sufficient biological replicates, proper control, and appropriate statistics. These findings are convincing and are of general interest for understanding mitochondrial inheritance in C. elegans, which could have implications for understanding similar biological processes in other organisms.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Melin et al. developed a quantitative assay to measure the fate of paternal mitochondria after fertilization. They combine this assay with C. elegans genetics to show that multiple genes contribute to paternal mitochondrial elimination. However, despite their claims, they unconvincingly place these genes into distinct pathways and fail to determine whether additional unknown genes are involved in the process.

      Strengths:

      Melin et al. develop a new assay to quantify the fate of paternal mitochondria during embryonic development in C. elegans. They use complex C. elegans genetics to disrupt 5 different genes and nicely measure their contributions to paternal mitochondrial elimination. In an attempt to place these genes into pathways, the authors interrupt genes in various combinations and measure paternal mitochondrial persistence. The authors discovered that disrupting 4 of the genes known to contribute to paternal mitochondrial elimination still resulted in paternal mitochondrial elimination, suggesting that more genes also contribute to this process. Finally, the authors discovered that pink-1, which had previously been discounted, indeed contributes to paternal mitochondrial elimination when the major pathway involving allo-1 is also disrupted.

      Weaknesses:

      In the introduction, the authors describe the importance of studying the maternal inheritance of mitochondrial DNA. However, the authors mostly study the inheritance of paternally-derived mitochondrial proteins (HSP6::GFP). While the authors do use a PCR approach to measure paternal mitochondrial DNA, their results are not as quantitative and thorough (applied to multiple mutant combinations) as their microscopy assay. Using their microscopy assay, the authors did not combine mutants for all 5 genes. Therefore, they cannot support or discount the possibility that undiscovered paternal mitochondrial elimination mechanisms exist. The author's genetic epistasis experiments are incomplete and occasionally improperly interpreted (as described below). Finally, the authors were unable to achieve paternal mitochondrial transmission to the F2 generation (which admittedly has not been achieved in any experimental system).

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Mitochondrial DNA (mtDNA) is exclusively maternally transmitted in almost all species. Paternal mitochondria, with their mtDNA, must be rapidly degraded after fertilisation to prevent their transmission to progeny, which could lead to subsequent detrimental mito-nuclear incompatibilities. Multiple layers of mechanisms contribute to blocking the transmission of paternal mitochondria and their mtDNA to progeny. Endonuclease activity and mitophagy form a part of these strategies. However, other key regulatory mechanisms remain to be elucidated, as inactivating endonuclease and mitophagy activity only delays the clearance of paternal mitochondria. In this study, the authors mainly focused on genes involved in endonuclease function (csp-6) and autophagy (allo-1) in C. elegans, demonstrating a synergic genetic interaction that potentialize their activity. They also revealed a contribution by pink-1/pink1, in the absence of allo-1.

      Strengths:

      The majority of data relies on confocal microscopy images and corresponding image analysis and quantification. Images are clear, and quantifications are supported by several biological replicates of >10 n and standard statistical tests. Mutants used were obtained from the Caenorhabditis Genetics Center (CGC) and were previously validated and confirmed by the C. elegans community. The scientific approach is solid and rigorous and in line with state-of-the-art C. elegans methods. Proper controls have been performed to rule out the effect of animal viability on observed results or to confirm the staining validity of TUBES on subcellular structures surrounding paternal mitochondria. Controls validating uaDf5 PCR specificity were conducted.

      Weaknesses:

      However, the embryonic expression of paternally contributing genes in feminised animals cannot be completely ruled out, as RNAi was used to alter gene expression levels. An issue inherent to RNAi approaches. Also, the impact of pink-1/pink1 is significant, but there is a lack of evidence demonstrating its mitophagic function.

      Goal achievements and data supportive of conclusions:

      In the first part of the study, the authors strongly and clearly demonstrate the synergistic interaction between the csp-6 and allo-1 in delaying paternal mitochondria degradation and associated mtDNA in the fertilised egg. In wild-type animals, paternal mitochondria are visible (using a mitochondrial HSP-::GFP marker) until the 4-cell stage embryo. In the csp-6; allo-1 double mutant genetic background, paternal mitochondria very significantly perdures until the 2-fold embryonic stage. The uaDf5 mitochondrial deletion, detectable by PCR, that was introduced by crossing with a male, followed the same trend. In addition, loss of fncd-1/fndc1 and phb-2 did not extend the perdurance of paternal mitochondria. In the second part of the study, the authors demonstrate a contribution of the loss of pink-1/pink1, in the absence of allo-1, in delaying paternal mitochondria degradation until the 100-cell stage. Overall, the conclusions are in accordance with the data shown.

      Impact on the field:

      Endonuclease activity and mitophagy aren't sufficient to prevent the transmission of paternal mitochondria and associated mtDNA to progeny, but they still contribute significantly to regulating the perdurance of paternal mitochondria in early embryos. Understanding how these two functions work in concert to potentialize their activity is important, as they could potentially be manipulated/enhanced to improve paternal mitochondrial degradation in the future. Here, the authors demonstrate a detailed synergistic genetic interaction between these functions. Also, they pointed out a new potential contribution of pink-1/pink1, which may underlie a potentially more complex mitophagic protective function.

    4. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The present study examines the cooperation among four allophagy/mitophagy factors, ALLO-1, CPS-6, FNDC-1, and PHB-2, implicated in the elimination of the sperm-derived mitochondria in C. elegans embryos. The key finding of the cumulative effect of ALLO-1 and CPS-6 inactivation causing delayed sperm mitophagy is significant for the understanding of mitochondrial inheritance in the nematode model and in general. Below are some specific suggestions on how the impact of the article could be elevated:

      Abstract:

      The authors should shorten the description of previously identified mitophagy factors and provide more detail on the present study results. An impact statement should be added at the end, with significance for understanding mitochondrial inheritance across taxa, all the way to mammals/humans.

      Introduction:

      The authors should provide more details on ALLO-1 and its interaction with LC-3. Also, it should be specified which of those previously identified allophagy factors are unique to worms and which ones are conserved. See also my comment below about including a diagram and a table of pathways and determinants involved in allophagy/paternal mitophagy.

      Results:

      If I understand the mtDNA data correctly, paternal mtDNA is maintained throughout the lifespan of the F1 generation but absent from the F2 generation. This is reminiscent of past studies of interspecific Mus musculus/Mus spretus mouse crosses by Kaneda/Shitara in which the paternal mtDNA was maintained F1 generation, resulting in heteroplasmy, but was lost from the F2 generation after back-crossing. Are CPS-6 and ALLO-1 effectors, but not determinants of maternal mtDNA inheritance in the nematode?

      The finding that PINK-1 inactivation stabilizes sperm-derived mitochondria in the embryos is interesting. Are the substrates of PINK1 known in C. elegans? This could provide a clue concerning the aforementioned mitophagy determinants acting independently of ALLO-1.

      Discussion:

      A summary-diagram compiling the intersecting allophagy pathways would be helpful to accompany discussion, in addition to or expanding on the simple diagram presented as Figure 5; also, a table listing all the factors implicated in nematode allophagy next to those implicated in human/mammalian sperm mitophagy, which would highlight the divergences and overlaps between vertebrates and invertebrates.

      Is it known how CPC-6 enters/gets imported into the sperm mitochondria inside the embryo? This pathway could potentially be targeted to decipher the allophagy mechanism.

      PINK/PARKIN/PACRG and FUNDC1/2 pathways have been implicated in mammalian neurodegeneration as well as in mitophagy, including but not limited to sperm mitophagy after fertilization. These pathways in mammals should be briefly reviewed as they may provide further clues to how the allophagy pathways intersect in C. elegans.

    1. eLife Assessment

      This valuable study addresses the structural basis of voltage-activation of BK channels using atomistic simulations of several microseconds, to assess conformational changes that underlie both voltage-sensing and gating of the pore. The findings, including movement of specific charged residues, combined with the degree to which these movements are coupled to pore movements, provide a solid basis for understanding voltage-gating mechanisms in this class of channels. This paper will likely be of interest to ion channel biologists and biophysicists focused on voltage-dependent channel gating mechanisms.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study provides new insight into the non-canonicial voltage-gating mechanism of BK channels through prolonged (10 us) MD simulations of the Slo1 transmembrane domain conformation and K+ conduction in response to high imposed voltages (300, 750 mV). The results support previous conclusions based on functional and structural data and MD simulations that the voltage-sensor domain (VSD) of Slo1 undergoes limited conformational changes compared to Kv channels, and predicts gating charge movement comparable in magnitude to experimental results. The gating charge calculations further indicate that R213 and R210 in S4 are the main contributors owing to their large side chain movements and the presence of a locally focused electric field, consistent with recent experimental and MD simulation results by Carrasquel-Ursulaez et al.,2022. Most interestingly, changes in pore conformation and K+ conduction driven by VSD activation are resolved, providing information regarding changes in VSD/pore interaction through S4/S5/S6 segments proposed to underly electromechanical coupling.

      Strengths:

      Include that the prolonged timescale and high voltage of the simulation allow apparent equilibration in the voltage-sensor domain (VSD) conformational changes and at least partial opening of the pore. The study extends the results of previous MD simulations of VSD activation by providing quantitative estimates of gating charge movement, showing how the electric field distribution across the VSD is altered in resting and activated states, and testing the hypothesis that R213 and R210 are the primary gating charges by steered MD simulations. The ability to estimate gating charge contributions of individual residues in the WT channel is useful as a comparison to experimental studies based on mutagenesis which have yielded conflicting results that could reflect perturbations in structure. Use of dynamic community analysis to identify coupling pathways and information flow for VSD-pore (electromechanical) coupling as well as analysis of state-dependent S4/S5/S6 interactions that could mediate coupling provide useful predictions extending beyond what has been experimentally tested.

      Weaknesses:

      Weaknesses include that a truncated channel (lacking the C-terminal gating ring) was used for simulations, which is known to have reduced single channel conductance and electromechanical coupling compared to the full-length channel. In addition, as VSD activation in BK channels is much faster than opening, the timescale of simulations was likely insufficient to achieve a fully open state as supported by differences in the degree of pore expansion in replicate simulations, which are also smaller than observed in Ca-bound open structures of the full-length channel. Taken together, these limitations suggest that inferences regarding coupling pathways and interactions in the fully open voltage-activated channel may be only partially supported and therefore incomplete. That said, adequate discussion regarding these limitations are provided together with dynamic community analysis based on the Ca-bound open structure. The latter supports the main conclusions based on simulations, while providing an indication of potential interaction differences between simulated and fully open conformations. Another limitation is that while the simulations convincingly demonstrate voltage-dependent channel opening as evidenced by pore expansion and conduction of K+ and water through the pore, single channel conductance is underestimated by at least an order of magnitude, as in previous studies of other K+ channels. These quantitative discrepancies suggest that MD simulations may not yet be sufficiently advanced to provide insight into mechanisms underlying the extraordinarily large conductance of BK channels.

      Comments on revisions:

      My previous questions and concerns have been adequately addressed.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript by Jia and Chen addresses the structural basis of voltage-activation of BK channels using computational approaches. Although a number of experimental studies using gating current and patch-clamp recording have analyzed voltage-activation in terms of observed charge movements and the apparent energetic coupling between voltage-sensor movement and channel opening, the structural changes that underlie this phenomenon have been unclear. The present studies use a reduced molecular system comprising the transmembrane portion of the BK channel (i.e. the cytosolic domain was deleted), embedded in a POPC membrane, with either 0 or 750 mV applied across the membrane. This system enabled acquisition of long simulations of 10 microseconds, to permit tracking of conformational changes of the channel. The authors principal findings were that the side chains of R210 and R213 rapidly moved toward the extracellular side of the membrane (by 8 - 10 Å), with greater displacements than any of the other charged transmembrane residues. These movements appeared tightly coupled to movement of the pore-lining helix, pore hydration, and ion permeation. The authors estimate that R210 and R213 contribute 0.25 and 0.19 elementary charges per residue to the gating current, which is roughly consistent with estimates based on electrophysiological measurements that used the full-length channel.

      Strengths:

      The methodologies used in this work are sound, and these studies certainly contribute to our understanding of voltage-gating of BK channels. An intriguing observation is the strongly coupled movement of the S4, S5, and S6 helices that appear to underlie voltage-dependent opening. Based on Fig 2a-d, the substantial movements of the R210 and R213 side chains occur nearly simultaneously to the S6 movement (between 4 - 5 usec of simulation time). This seems to provide support for a "helix-packing" mechanism of voltage gating in the so-called "non-domain-swapped" voltage-gated K channels.

      Weaknesses:

      The main limitation is that these studies used a truncated version of the BK channel, and there are likely to be differences in VSD-pore coupling in the context of the full-length channels that will not be resolved in the present work. Nonetheless, the authors provide a strong rationale for their use of the truncated channel, and the results presented will provide a good starting point for future computational studies of this channel.

    4. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewing Editor Comments:

      The resubmitted version of the manuscript adequately addressed several initial comments made by reviewing editors, including a more detailed analysis of the results (such as those of bilayer thickness). This version was seen by 2 reviewers. Both reviewers recognize this work as being an important contribution to the field of BK and voltage-dependent ion channels in general. The long trajectories and the rigorous/novel analyses have revealed important insights into the mechanisms of voltage-sensing and electromechanical coupling in the context of a truncated variant of the BK channel. Many of these observations are consistent with structural and functional measurements of the channel, available thus far. The authors also identify a novel partially expanded state of the channel pore that is accessed after gating-charge displacement, which informs the sequence of structural events accompanying voltage-dependent opening of BK.

      However, there are key concerns regarding the use of the truncated channel in the simulations. While many gating features of BK are preserved in the truncated variant, studies have suggested that opening of the channel pore to voltage-sensing domain rearrangement is impaired upon gating-ring deletion. So the inferences made here might only represent a partial view of the mechanism of electromechanical coupling.

      It is also not entirely clear whether the partially expanded pore represents a functionally open, sub-conductance, or another closed state. Although the authors provide evidence that the inner pore is hydrated in this partially open state, in the absence of additional structural/functional restraints, a confident assignment of a functional state to this structure state is difficult. Functional measurements of the truncated channel seem to suggest that not only is their single channel conductance lower than full-length channels, but they also appear to have a voltage-independent step that causes the gates to open. It is unclear whether it is this voltage-independent step that remains to be captured in these MD trajectories. A clean cut resolution of this conundrum might not be feasible at this time, but it could help present the various possibilities to the readers.

      We appreciate the positive comments and agree that there will likely be important differences between the mechanistic details of voltage activation between the Core-MT and full-length constructs of BK channels. We also agree that the dilated pore observed in the simulation may not be the fully open state of Core-MT.

      Nonetheless, the notion that the simulation may not have captured the full pore opening transition or the contribution of the CTD should not render the current work “incomplete”, because a complete understanding of BK activation would be an unrealistic goal beyond the scope of this work. We respectfully emphasize that the main insights of the current simulations are the mechanisms of voltage sensing (e.g., the nature of VSD movements, contributions of various charged residues, how small charge movements allow voltage sensing, etc.) as well as the role of the S4-S5-S6 interface in VSD-pore coupling. As noted by the Editor and reviewers, these insights represent important steps towards establishing a more complete understanding of BK activation.

      Below are the specific comments of the two experts who have assessed the work and made specific suggestions to improve the manuscript.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Although the successful simulation of V-dependent K+ conduction through the BK channel pore and analysis of associated state dependent VSD/pore interactions and coupling analysis is significant, there are two related questions that are relevant to the conclusions and of interest to the BK channel community which I think should be addressed or discussed.

      One key feature of BK channels is their extraordinarily large conductance compared to other K+ selective channels. Do the simulations of K+ conductance provide any insight into this difference? Is the predicted conductance of BK larger than that of other K+ channels studied by similar methods? Is there any difference in the conductance mechanism (e.g., the hard and soft knock-on effects mentioned for BK)?

      The molecular basis of the large conductance of BK channels is indeed an interesting and fundamental question. Unfortunately, this is beyond the scope of this work and the current simulation does not appear to provide any insight into the basis of large conductance. It is interesting to note, though, the conductance is apparently related to the level of pore dilation and the pore hydration level, as increasing hydration level from ~30 to ~40 waters in the pore increases the simulated conductance from ~1.5 to 6 pS (page 8). This is consistent with previous atomistic simulations (Gu and de Groot, Nature Communications 2023; ref. 33) showing that the pore hydration level is strongly correlated with observed conductance. As noted in the manuscript, the conductance mechanism through the filter appears highly similar to previous simulations of other K+ channels (Page 8). Given the limit conductance events observed in the current simulations, we will refrain from discussing possible basis of the large conductance in BK channels except commenting on the role of pore hydration (page 8; also see below in response to #5).

      The pore in the MD simulations does not open as wide as the Ca-bound open structure, which (as the authors note) may mean that full opening requires longer than 10 us. I think that is highly likely given that the two 750 mV simulations yielded different degrees of opening and that in BK channels opening is generally much slower than charge movement. Therefore, a question is - do any of the conclusions illustrated in Figures 6, S5, S6 differ if the Ca-bound structure is used as the open state? For example, I expect the interactions between S5 and S6 might at least change to some extent as S6 moves to its final position. In this case, would conclusions about which residues interact, and get stronger or weaker, be the same as in Figures S6 b,c? Providing a comparison may help indicate to what extent the conclusions are dependent on achieving a fully open conformation.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s suggestion and have further analyzed the information flow and coupling pathways using the simulation trajectory initiated from the Ca<sup>2+</sup>-bound cryo-EM structure (sim 7, Table S1). The new results are shown in two new SI Figures S7 and S8, and new discussion has been added to pages 14-15. Comparing Figures 5 and S7, we find that dynamic community, coupling pathways, and information flow are highly similar between simulation of the open and closed states, even though there are significant differences in S5 contacts in the simulated open state vs Ca<sup>2+</sup>-bound open state (Figure S8). Interestingly, there are significant differences in S4-S5 packing in the simulated and Ca<sup>2+</sup>-bound open states (Figure S8 top panel), which likely reflect important difference in VSD/pore interactions during voltage vs Ca<sup>2+</sup> activation.

      (2) P4 Significance -"first, successful direct simulation of voltage-activation"

      This statement may need rewording. As noted above Carrasquel-Ursulaez et al.,2022 (reference 39) simulated voltage sensor activation under comparable conditions to the current manuscript (3.9 us simulation at +400 mV), and made some similar conclusions regarding R210, R213 movement, and electric field focusing within the VSD. However, they did not report what happens to the pore or simulate K+ movement. So do the authors here mean something like "first, successful direct simulation of voltage-dependent channel opening"?

      We agree with the reviewer and have revised the statement to “ … the first successful direct simulation of voltage-dependent activation of the big potassium (BK) channel, ..”

      (3) P5 "We compare the membrane thickness at 300 and 750 mV and the results reveal no significant difference in the membrane thickness (Figure S2)"

      The figure also shows membrane thickness at 0 mV and indicates it is 1.4 Angstroms less than that at 300 or 750 mV. Whether or not this difference is significant should be stated, as the question being addressed is whether the structure is perturbed owing to the use of non-physiological voltages (which would include both 300 and 750 mV).

      We have revised the Figure S2 caption to clarify that one-way ANOVA suggest the difference is not significant.

      (4) P7 "It should be noted that the full-length BK channel in the Ca2+ bound state has an even larger intracellular opening (Figure 2f, green trace), suggesting that additional dilation of the pore may

      occur at longer timescales."

      As noted above, I agree it is likely that additional pore dilation may occur at longer timescales. However, for completeness, I suppose an alternative hypothesis should be noted, e.g. "...suggesting that additional dilation of the pore may occur at longer timescales, or in response to Ca-binding to the full length channel."

      This is a great suggestion. Revised as suggested.

      (5) Since the authors raise the possibility that they are simulating a subconductance state, some more discussion on this point would be helpful, especially in relation to the hydrophobic gate concept. Although the Magleby group concluded that the cytoplasmic mouth of the (fully open) pore has little impact on single channel conductance, that doesn't rule out that it becomes limiting in a partially open conformation. The simulation in Figure 3A shows an initial hydration of the pore with ~15 waters with little conductance events, suggesting that hydration per se may not suffice to define a fully open state. Indeed, the authors indicate that the simulated open state (w/ ~30-40 waters) has 1/4th the simulated conductance of the open structure (w/ ~60 waters). So is it the degree of hydration that limits conductance? Or is there a threshold of hydration that permits conductance and then other factors that limit conductance until the pore widens further? Addressing these issues might also be relevant to understanding the extraordinarily large conductance of fully open BK compared to other K channels.

      We agree with the reviewer’s proposal that pore hydration seems to be a major factor that can affect conductance. This is also well in-line with the previous computational study by Gu and de Groot (2023). We have now added a brief discussion on page 8, stating “Besides the limitation of the current fixed charge force fields in quantitively predicting channel conductance, we note that the molecular basis for the large conductance of BK channels is actually poorly understood (78). It is noteworthy that the pore hydration level appears to be an important factor in determining the apparent conductance in the simulation, which has also been proposed in a previous atomistic simulation study of the Aplysia BK channel (33).”

      Minor points

      (1) P5 "the fully relaxed pore profile (red trace in Figure S1d, top row) shows substantial differences compared to that of the Ca2+-free Cryo-EM structure of the full-length channel."

      For clarity, I suggest indicating which is the Ca-free profile - "... Ca2+-free Cryo-EM structure of the full-length channel (black trace)."

      We greatly appreciate the thoughtful suggestion. Revised as suggested.

      (2) P8 "Consistent with previous simulations (78-80), the conductance follows a multi-ion mechanism, where there are at least two K+ ions inside the filter"

      For clarity, I suggest indicating these are not previous simulations of BK channels (e.g., "previous simulations of other K+ channels ...").

      Author response: Revised as suggested. Thank you.

      (3) Figure 2, S1 - grey traces representing individual subunits are very difficult to see (especially if printed). I wonder if they should be made slightly darker. Similar traces in Figure 3 are easier to see.

      The traces in Figure S1 are actually the same thickness in Figure 3 and they appear lighter due to the size of the figure. Figure 2 panels a-c have been updated to improve the resolution.

      (4) Figure 2 - suggest labeling S6 as "S6 313-324" (similar to S4 notation) to indicate it is not the entire segment.

      Figure 2 panel d) has been updated as suggested.

      (5) Figure 2 legend - "Voltage activation of Core-MT BK channels. a-d)..."

      It would be easier to find details corresponding to individual panels if they were referenced individually. For example:

      "a-d) results from a 10-μs simulation under 750 mV (sim2b in Table S1). Each data point represents the average of four subunits for a given snapshot (thin grey lines), and the colored thick lines plot the running average. a) z-displacement of key side chain charged groups from initial positions. The locations of charged groups were taken as those of guanidinium CZ atoms (for Arg) and sidechain carboxyl carbons (for Asp/Glu) b) z-displacement of centers-of-mass of VSD helices from initial positions, c) backbone RMSD of the pore-lining S6 (F307-L325) to the open state, and d) tilt angles of all TM helices. Only residues 313-324 of S6 were included inthe tilt angle calculation, and the values in the open and closed Cryo-EM structures are marked using purple dashed lines. "

      We appreciate the thoughtful suggestion and have revised the caption as suggested.

      (6) Figure S1 - column labels a,b,c, and d should be referenced in the legend.

      The references to column labels have been added to Figure S1 caption.

      (7) References need to be double-checked for duplicates and formatting.

      a) I noticed several duplicate references, but did not do a complete search: Budelli et al 2013 (#68, 100), Horrigan Aldrich 2002 (#22,97), Sun Horrigan 2022 (#40, 86), Jensen et al 2012 (#56,81).

      b) Reference #38 is incorrectly cited with the first name spelled out and the last name abbreviated.

      We appreciate the careful proofreading of the reviewer. The duplicated references were introduced by mistake due to the use of multiple reference libraries. We have gone through the manuscript and removed a total of 5 duplicated references.

      Response to additional reviewer comments

      My only new comment is that the numbering of residues in Fig. S8 does not match the standard convention for hSlo and needs to be doublechecked. For the residues I checked, the numbers appear to be shifted 3 compared hSlo (e.g. Y315, P317, E318, G324 should be Y318, P320, E321, G327).

      We greatly appreciate the reviewer for catching the errors in residue labels. Figure S8 has now been updated to include correct residue labels. Thanks!

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      This manuscript has been through a previous level of review. The authors have provided their responses to the previous reviewers, which appear to be satisfactory, and I have no additional comments, beyond the caveats concerning interpretations based on the truncated channel, which are noted above.

      We greatly appreciate the constructive comments and insightful advice. Please see above response to the Reviewing Editor’s comments for response and changes regarding the caveats concerning interpretations of the current simulations.

    1. eLife Assessment

      The open-source software Chromas tracks and analyses cephalopod chromatophore dynamics. The software features a user-friendly interface alongside detailed instructions for its application, with compelling exemplary applications to two widely divergent cephalopod species, a squid and a cuttlefish, over time periods large enough to encompass new chromatophore development among existing ones. It demonstrates accurate tracking of the position and identity of each chromatophore. The software and methods outlined therein will become an important tool for scientists tracking dynamic signaling in animals.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study provides comprehensive instructions for using the chromatophore tracking software, Chromas, to track and analyse the dynamics of large numbers of cephalopod chromatophores across various spatiotemporal scales. This software addresses a long-standing challenge faced by many researchers who study these soft-bodied creatures, known for their remarkable ability to change colour rapidly. The updated software features a user-friendly interface that can be applied to a wide range of applications, making it an essential tool for biologists focused on animal dynamic signalling. It will also be of interest to professionals in the fields of computer vision and image analysis.

      Strengths:

      This work provides detailed instructions for this tool kit along with examples for potential users to try. The Gitlab inventory hosts the software package, installation documentation, and tutorials, further helping potential users with a less steep learning curve.

      Weaknesses:

      The evidence supporting the authors' claims is solid, particularly demonstrated through the use of cuttlefish and squid. However, it may not be applicable to all coleoid cephalopods yet, such as octopuses, which have an incredibly versatile ability to change their body forms.

      Comments on revisions:

      I am pleased to see the more detailed version of this useful tool along with tutorials designed for diverse users who are interested in animal dynamic colouration. This study provides detailed instructions for using the chromatophore tracking software Chromas to track and analyse the dynamics of large numbers of cephalopod chromatophores across various spatiotemporal scales. The software features a user-friendly interface that is highly compelling and can be applied to a wide range of applications.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors developed a computational pipeline named CHROMAS to track and analyze chromatophore dynamics, which provides a wide range of biological analysis tools without requiring the user to write code.

      Strengths:

      (1) CHROMAS is an integrated toolbox that provides tools for different biological tasks such as: segment, classify, track and measure individual chromatophores, cluster small groups of chromatophores, analyze full-body patterns, etc.

      (2) It could be used to investigate different species. The authors have already applied it to analyze the skin of the bobtail squid Euprymna berryi and the European cuttlefish Sepia officinalis.

      (3) The tool is open-source and easy to install. The paper describes in detail the experiment requirements, command to complete each task and provides relevant sample figures, which are easy to follow.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) There are some known limitations for the current version. The users should read the "Discussion" chapter carefully before preparing their experiments and using CHROMAS.

    4. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study provides comprehensive instructions for using the chromatophore tracking software, Chromas, to track and analyse the dynamics of large numbers of cephalopod chromatophores across various spatiotemporal scales. This software addresses a long-standing challenge faced by many researchers who study these soft-bodied creatures, known for their remarkable ability to change colour rapidly. The updated software features a user-friendly interface that can be applied to a wide range of applications, making it an essential tool for biologists focused on animal dynamic signalling. It will also be of interest to professionals in the fields of computer vision and image analysis.

      Strengths:

      This work provides detailed instructions for this toolkit along with examples for potential users to try. The Gitlab inventory hosts the software package, installation documentation, and tutorials, further helping potential users with a less steep learning curve.

      Weaknesses:

      The evidence supporting the authors' claims is solid, particularly demonstrated through the use of cuttlefish and squid. However, it may not be applicable to all coleoid cephalopods yet, such as octopuses, which have an incredibly versatile ability to change their body forms.

      The reviewer is right to highlight this limitation. We clarified, in the revised manuscript, that CHROMAS relies on the assumption that chromatophore activity occurs primarily in a plane — a condition that is valid most of the time in squid and cuttlefish, where the majority of skin deformations are in-plane (with small occasional papillae). In cephalopods such as octopuses, however, in which the skin may undergo large 3-dimensional deformations through the action of papillary musculature, this assumption may not always hold. Although octopods’ bodies are more spherical (less flat) than those of squid and cuttlefish, CHROMAS should still be usable and useful if applied to smaller skin areas, especially because chromatophore density is often even higher in octopoda than in sepiidae.

      We added the following paragraph in the discussion:

      Another known limitation concerns the biological assumptions underlying the current version of CHROMAS. The pipeline is designed for surfaces that remain reasonably planar and undergo deformations primarily in two dimensions. In cephalopods such as octopuses, in which the skin can undergo substantial three-dimensional morphological changes, analysing chromatophore dynamics may require complementary three-dimensional tracking of the skin surface to correct for out-of-plane deformations and maintain accurate measurement of chromatophore activity.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors developed a computational pipeline named CHROMAS to track and analyse chromatophore dynamics, which provides a wide range of biological analysis tools without requiring the user to write code.

      Strengths:

      (1) CHROMAS is an integrated toolbox that provides tools for different biological tasks such as: segment, classify, track and measure individual chromatophores, cluster small groups of chromatophores, analyse full-body patterns, etc.

      (2) It could be used to investigate different species. The authors have already applied it to analyse the skin of the bobtail squid Euprymna berryi and the European cuttlefish Sepia officinalis.

      (3) The tool is open-source and easy to install. The paper describes in detail the command format to complete each task and provides relevant sample figures.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The generality and robustness of the proposed pipeline need to be verified through more experimental evaluations. For example, the implementation algorithm depends on relatively specific or obvious image features, clean backgrounds, and objects that do not move too fast.

      (2) The pipeline lacks some kind of self-correction mechanism. If at one moment there is a conflicting match with the previous frames, how does the system automatically handle it to ensure that the tracking results are accurate over a long period of time?

      We thank the reviewer for raising this important point. CHROMAS does rely on relatively clean imaging conditions for optimal performance. However, the computational features of the pipeline — segmentation, tracking, and downstream analysis — have been designed to perform reliably as long as the segmentation models are trained on frames that reflect the diversity of the dataset (e.g., variations in lighting or minor background noise). It is correct, however, that acquiring the necessary quality of input data is both important and non-trivial. The pipeline is designed to work best with high-resolution footage of chromatophores under clear imaging conditions — specifically, with minimal water surface distortion, minimal particulate matter in the water column, and stable focus.

      To mitigate issues arising from motion blur or focus loss, CHROMAS includes an automatic frame quality control step that detects and discards frames that are out of focus, including those where the animal moves too fast for reliable tracking.

      To assist future users, we have now added a section under Discussion detailing the recommended recording conditions and video characteristics for effective analysis with CHROMAS. It reads:

      Recommended Video Parameters for Optimal Use of CHROMAS

      The performance of CHROMAS depends on the quality of the input videos. Although the pipeline analyses each frame independently and has no frame rate requirement, we recommend recording at 20 frames per second at least, to capture chromatophore dynamics accurately. Sharp, in-focus frames are critical, particularly for moving subjects, where higher shutter speeds help minimize motion blur. For reliable segmentation, each chromatophore should cover at least 10 pixels across its fully expanded diameter. Higher spatial resolution, with chromatophores covering around 50 pixels in diameter, are recommended if sub-chromatophore dynamics are of interest. Recording conditions should minimize background noise, and the water column should be as clear as possible, free of particles or debris. The water surface should be kept as calm and planar as possible to avoid optical artifacts. If wide-angle lenses or other optics that may introduce distortion are used, lens correction algorithms should be applied during preprocessing to compensate for the optical distortions. For long-term tracking applications (e.g., developmental studies), frequent imaging sessions are recommended. Newly differentiated chromatophores are initially light colored (e.g., yellow) and thus visually distinct from mature chromatophores (which are dark); over days to weeks, however, the light chromatophores darken and become increasingly difficult to differentiate from older ones. Recording at appropriate and regular intervals thus helps track individual chromatophores across developmental stages and improves the reliability of long-term analyses. Following these recommendations will help segmentation, tracking, and analysis with CHROMAS.

      CHROMAS does not implement an active self-correction mechanism in the sense of real-time error recovery. Yet, several steps are in place to ensure the reliability of registration and tracking over time. During registration, a set of points is tracked across frames using optical flow. If the displacement of a point between two frames exceeds a biologically plausible threshold, that point is automatically discarded from the registration calculation to prevent error propagation. If too many points are discarded, the registration step fails, preventing the acceptance of a poor alignment.

      In addition, masterframes (the averages of all aligned frames in a chunk) are generated at the end of the registration process to enable the visual verification of the quality of the mapping.

      During stitching, CHROMAS calculates reprojection errors between chunks, providing a quantitative measure of stitching validity and allowing users to detect and correct potential mismatches.

      We have revised the Results section to explicitly highlight the error-checking mechanisms implemented during registration and stitching to maintain tracking accuracy over time.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Figures 2, 3, 5, 6, 8 showed the bobtail squid, however, all command lines for these figures were referred to "sepia_example.dataset".

      We thank the reviewer for noticing this inconsistency. We have corrected the labeling of the dataset name in the command line examples from "sepia_example.dataset" to the neutral term "example.dataset" to avoid any confusion regarding the species used in the figures.

      (2) It's excellent that Chromas includes a manual pre-alignment function. However, it's unclear how the authors determined the registration of selected chromatophores across different ages in the long-term tracking session. Given the rapid growth of cephalopods and presumably skin expansion with increased chromatophores, it would be helpful to provide more details or examples on this process.

      The manual pre-alignment function provides an interactive interface allowing the user to select a set of matching chromatophores across frames from different developmental stages. The accuracy of this process depends on the user's ability to recognize individual chromatophores reliably over time. Critically, it is not necessary to identify all those chromatophores; a representative subset is sufficient to interpolate the spatial mapping and align the surrounding chromatophores.

      To limit the potential challenges associated with chromatophore development, frequent imaging sessions (every few days) are recommended initially. Excessive intervals between recordings can result in relative displacements among existing chromatophores and the sudden appearance of newly matured chromatophores, both of which complicate manual matching.

      It should be noted that these challenges are not limitations of the CHROMAS pipeline itself, but rather relate to experimental design choices that affect the quality and traceability of the dataset. The exact parameters (e.g., size/duration of the datasets, spatial resolution, frame rate and intervals between recording sessions) to be used must be adapted to each experimental animal, each age, and ultimately, each question.

      Recommended video acquisition parameters, including guidance on recording frequency for long-term chromatophore tracking, have been added to the Discussion section.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) More detailed information should be given, such as operating system requirements, camera frame rate requirements, target size and speed limitations, when chunking videos into usable segments, the minimum length of each segment, etc.

      CHROMAS is platform-independent and requires only a functioning Python 3.9+ environment, regardless of the operating system or OS version, as described in “Methods – Implementation details”.

      Although CHROMAS does not require specific frame rates and because it analyses each frame independently, the quality of each image—and thus of imaging parameters—is critical to enable reliable chromatophore segmentation. If an animal remains relatively calm during recording, low shutter speeds will be adequate for image sharpness. Conversely, if the animal moves frequently or rapidly, it will be preferable to use a higher frame rate and a higher shutter speed to minimize motion blur. Recording parameters should therefore be adjusted accordingly, primarily to optimize image clarity and maintain frames in sharp focus.

      The frame rate should be sufficiently high also to capture the fast dynamics of chromatophore expansions and contractions. Although the pipeline has no specific frame rate requirement, we recommend image rates of at least 20 frames per second to sample the temporal patterns of chromatophore activity adequately, based on biological considerations.

      Each chromatophore should be represented by a sufficiently large number of pixels in each recorded image to enable the reliable estimation of its size, shape, and dynamics. If the spatial resolution is too low, individual chromatophores may appear as small pixel clusters, reducing the accuracy of area and shape measurements and introducing quantization artifacts. Based on our experience, we recommend recording conditions that result in each chromatophore covering at least 10 pixels across its diameter when fully expanded to ensure accurate segmentation and quantitative whole-chromatophore analysis. For sub-chromatophore motion analysis, we recommend a minimum of 50 pixels across the fully expanded diameter.

      These considerations relate to optimizing biological sampling and image quality for analysis, and are not technical requirements imposed by CHROMAS itself.

      We added a Discussion section outlining the recommended recording conditions and video parameters to facilitate effective use of CHROMAS.

      (2) This pipeline does not include functionality to correct for lens distortion, which may affect the results when accurate measurement of single chromatophore morphology is required.

      We thank the reviewer for this observation. We agree that lens distortion can affect the accurate measurement of chromatophore morphology if present. However, the current datasets analysed with CHROMAS were recorded using a long macro lens with minimal distortion, and visual inspections as well as quantitative assessments of chromatophore geometry did not indicate measurable optical deformation. We acknowledge that for other imaging setups —particularly those relying on the use of wide-angle lenses— lens distortion could introduce artifacts. In such cases, we recommend applying standard lens distortion correction during preprocessing, prior to analysis with CHROMAS.

      We have also addressed this point in the newly added section under the Discussion.

      (3) How to perform expansion for single chromatophores shown in Figure 6, and how to keep the expansion area consistent?

      The graph in Figure 6 illustrates the expansion of a single chromatophore over time and was generated entirely using the "areas" command and visualization tools available within CHROMAS.

      Spatial consistency is maintained because CHROMAS, through its registration and area extraction steps, tracks the identity of each chromatophore across the video, allowing the same individual to be followed reliably over time.

      (4) Tables 1 and 2: it's better to add the units of the values in each column.<br />

      We thank the reviewer for the suggestion. We have added the appropriate units to each column in Tables 1 and 2 to improve clarity.

    1. eLife Assessment

      This study presents a rather valuable finding that a combination of arginine methyltransferase inhibitors synergize with PARP inhibitors to kill ovarian and triple negative cancer cells. The evidence supporting the claims of the authors is solid, although some comments and elaborations in the main text would have enhanced the comprehension and clarity of the data. The work will be of interest to scientists working in the field of breast cancer.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors show that a combination of arginine methyltransferase inhibitors synergize with PARP inhibitors to kill ovarian and triple negative cancer cell lines in vitro and in vivo using preclinical mouse models.

      Strengths and weaknesses

      The experiments are well-performed, convincing and have the appropriate controls (using inhibitors and genetic deletions) and use statistics.

      They identify the DNA damage protein ERCC1 to be reduced in expression with PRMT inhibitors. As ERCC1 is known to be synthetic lethal with PARPi, this provides a mechanism for the synergy. They use cell lines only for their study in 2D as well as xenograph models.

    3. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The authors aimed to enhance the effectiveness of PARP inhibitors (PARPi) in treating high-grade serous ovarian cancer (HGSOC) and triple-negative breast cancer (TNBC) by inhibiting PRMT1/5 enzymes. They conducted a drug screen combining PARPi with 74 epigenetic modulators to identify promising combinations.

      Zhang et al. reported that protein arginine methyltransferase (PRMT) 1/5 inhibition acts synergistically to enhance the sensitivity of Poly (ADP-ribose) polymerase inhibitors (PARPi) in high-grade serous ovarian cancer (HGSOC) and triple-negative breast cancer (TNBC) cells. The authors are the first to perform a drug screen by combining PARPi with 74 well-characterized epigenetic modulators that target five major classes of epigenetic enzymes. Their drug screen identified both PRMT1/5 inhibitors with high combination and clinical priority scores in PARPi treatment. Notably, PRMT1/5 inhibitors significantly enhance PARPi treatment-induced DNA damage in HR-proficient HGSOC and TNBC cells through enhanced maintenance of gene expression associated with DNA damage repair, BRCAness, and intrinsic innate immune pathways in cancer cells. Additionally, bioinformatic analysis of large-scale genomic and functional profiles from TCGA and DepMap further supports that PRMT1/5 are potential therapeutic targets in oncology, including HGSOC and TNBC. These results provide a strong rationale for the clinical application of a combination of PRMT and PARP inhibitors in patients with HR-proficient ovarian and breast cancer. Thus, this discovery has a high impact on developing novel therapeutic approaches to overcome resistance to PARPi in clinical cancer therapy. The data and presentation in this manuscript are straightforward and reliable.

      Strengths:

      (1) Innovative Approach: First to screen PARPi with a large panel of epigenetic modulators.

      (2) Significant Results: Found that PRMT1/5 inhibitors significantly boost PARPi effectiveness in HR-proficient HGSOC and TNBC cells.

      (3) Mechanistic Insights: Showed how PRMT1/5 inhibitors enhance DNA damage repair and immune pathways.

      (4) Robust Data: Supported by extensive bioinformatic analysis from large genomic databases.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) Novelty Clarification: Needs clearer comparison to existing studies showing similar effects.

      (2) Unclear Mechanisms: More investigation is needed on how MYC targets correlate with PRMT1/5.

      (3) Inconsistent Data: ERCC1 expression results varied across cell lines.

      (4) Limited Immune Study: Using immunodeficient mice does not fully explore immune responses.

      (5) Statistical Methods: Should use one-way ANOVA instead of a two-tailed Student's t-test for multiple comparisons.

      We sincerely thank Reviewer #1 for the insightful and constructive feedback, as well as for the kind acknowledgment of the significance of our work: “These results provide a strong rationale for the clinical application of a combination of PRMT and PARP inhibitors in patients with HR-proficient ovarian and breast cancer. Thus, this discovery has a high impact on developing novel therapeutic approaches to overcome resistance to PARPi in clinical cancer therapy. The data and presentation in this manuscript are straightforward and reliable.” We greatly appreciate the reviewer #1’s thoughtful comments, which have significantly improved the quality of our manuscript. In response, we conducted additional experiments and analyses, and made comprehensive revisions to the text, figures, and supplementary materials. In the “Recommendations for the authors” sections, we have provided point-by-point responses to each of the reviewer’s comments, which were immensely helpful in guiding our revisions. We believe these updates have substantially strengthened the manuscript and have fully addressed all reviewer concerns.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The authors show that a combination of arginine methyltransferase inhibitors synergize with PARP inhibitors to kill ovarian and triple-negative cancer cell lines in vitro and in vivo using preclinical mouse models.

      PARP inhibitors have been the common targeted-therapy options to treat high-grade serous ovarian cancer (HGSOC) and triple-negative breast cancer (TNBC). PRMTs are oncological therapeutic targets and specific inhibitors have been developed. However, due to the insufficiency of PRMTi or PARPi single treatment for HGSOC and TNBC, designing novel combinations of existing inhibitors is necessary. In previous studies, the authors and others developed an "induced PARPi sensitivity by epigenetic modulation" strategy to target resistant tumors. In this study, the authors presented a triple combination of PRMT1i, PRMT5i and PARPi that synergistically kills TNBC cells. A drug screen and RNA-seq analysis were performed to indicate cancer cell growth dependency of PRMT1 and PRMT5, and their CRISPR/Cas9 knockout sensitizes cancer cells to PARPi treatment. It was shown that the cells accumulate DNA damage and have increased caspase 3/7 activity. RNA-seq analysis identified BRCAness genes, and the authors closely studied a top hit ERCC1 as a downregulated DNA damage protein in PRMT inhibitor treatments. ERCC1 is known to be synthetic lethal with PARP inhibitors. Thus, the authors add back ERCC1 and reduce the effects of PRMT inhibitors suggesting PRMT inhibitors mediate, in part, their effect via ERCC1 downregulation. The combination therapy (PRMT/PARP) is validated in 2D cultures of cell lines (OVCAR3, 8 and MDA-MB-231) and has shown to be effective in nude mice with MDA-MB-231 xenograph models.

      Strengths and weaknesses:

      Overall, the data is well-presented. The experiments are well-performed, convincing, and have the appropriate controls (using inhibitors and genetic deletions) and statistics.

      They identify the DNA damage protein ERCC1 to be reduced in expression with PRMT inhibitors. As ERCC1 is known to be synthetic lethal with PARPi, this provides a mechanism for the synergy. They use cell lines only for their study in 2D as well as xenograph models.

      We sincerely thank Reviewer #2 for the insightful and constructive feedback, as well as for the kind acknowledgment of the significance of our work: “Overall, the data are well-presented. The experiments are well-performed, convincing, and supported by appropriate controls (using inhibitors and genetic deletions) and statistics.” We greatly appreciate the reviewer #2’s thoughtful comments, which have significantly improved the quality of our manuscript. In response, we conducted additional experiments and analyses, and made comprehensive revisions to the text, figures, and supplementary materials. In the “Recommendations for the authors” sections, we have provided point-by-point responses to each of the reviewer’s comments, which were immensely helpful in guiding our revisions. We believe these updates have substantially strengthened the manuscript and have fully addressed all reviewer concerns.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Recent studies have revealed promising synergistic effects between PRMT inhibitors and chemotherapy, as well as DDR-targeting drugs (ref. 89-92). In the discussion, the authors should highlight what is novel in this study compared to the reported studies.

      We thank the reviewer for this important comment and fully agree that prior studies have demonstrated the potential of PRMT inhibitors to enhance the efficacy of DNA damage-targeting agents and certain chemotherapies[1-4]. In response to the reviewer’s constructive suggestion, we have now revised the discussion to highlight the novel aspects of our study compared to previously reported findings. Specifically, our work presents several key advances that go beyond prior studies. Below, we would like to emphasize the novelty of our current study as follows:

      In the clinic, a strategy termed “induced PARP inhibitor (PARPi) sensitivity by epigenetic modulation” is being evaluated to sensitize homologous recombination (HR)-proficient tumors to PARPi treatments. Together with other groups, we reported that repression of BET activity significantly reduces the expression levels of essential HR genes by inhibiting their super-enhancers[5]. This preclinical discovery is now being assessed in a Phase 1b/2 clinical trial combining the BET inhibitor ZEN-3694 with the PARPi talazoparib for the treatment of patients with metastatic triple-negative breast cancer (TNBC) who do not carry germline BRCA1/2 mutations. Promising anti-tumor activity has been observed in this ongoing trial[6]. Importantly, gene expression profiles from paired tumor biopsies demonstrated robust target engagement, evidenced by repression of BRCA1 and RAD51 mRNA expression, consistent with our preclinical findings in xenograft models. Based on these encouraging results, the trial is being expanded to a Phase 2b stage to enroll additional TNBC patients. Moreover, other combination strategies[7-13] based on this “induced PARPi sensitivity by epigenetic modulation” approach have also shown promising clinical responses in both intrinsic and acquired HR-proficient settings. Notably, these clinical studies indicate that the strategy is well-tolerated, likely due to cancer cells being particularly sensitive to epigenetic repression of DNA damage response (DDR) genes, compared with normal cells.

      However, two key clinical challenges remain for broader application of this strategy in oncology: 1) which clinically actionable epigenetic drugs can produce the strongest synergistic effects with PARPi? and 2) can a BRCA-independent approach be developed? To address these questions, we performed a drug screen combining the FDA-approved PARPi olaparib with a panel of clinically relevant epigenetic drugs. This panel includes 74 well-characterized epigenetic modulators targeting five major classes of epigenetic enzymes, comprising 7 FDA-approved drugs, 14 agents in clinical trials, and 54 in preclinical development. Notably, both type I PRMT inhibitors (PRMTi) and PRMT5 inhibitors (PRMT5i) achieved high combination and clinical prioritization scores in the screen. Functional assays demonstrated that PRMT inhibition markedly enhances PARPi-induced DNA damage in HR-proficient cancer cell lines. In line with a strong positive correlation between PRMT and DDR gene expression across primary tumors, we observed that PRMT activity supports the transcription of DDR genes and maintains a BRCAness-like phenotype in cancer cells. These findings provide strong rationale for clinical development of PRMT/PARPi combinations in patients with HR-proficient ovarian or breast cancers. Mechanistic characterization from our study further supports PRMTi clinical development by elucidating mechanisms of action, identifying rational combinations, defining predictive biomarkers, and guiding dosing strategies.

      We believe our studies will be of significant interest to the cancer research community for several reasons. First, they address major clinical challenges in women’s cancers, specifically, high-grade serous ovarian cancer (HGSOC) and TNBC, both of which are aggressive malignancies with limited therapeutic options. Second, they offer a novel solution to overcome PARPi resistance. Our earlier discovery of “induced PARPi sensitivity by epigenetic modulation” has already shown promising clinical results and represents a new path to overcome both primary and acquired resistance to PARPi and platinum therapies. Third, they focus on a clinically translatable drug class. Selective and potent PRMT inhibitors have been developed by leading pharmaceutical companies, with more than ten currently in advanced clinical trials. Fourth, they support mechanism-driven combination strategies. Preclinical evaluation of PRMTi-based combinations with other therapeutic agents is urgently needed for future clinical success. Finally, our work highlights understudied but therapeutically relevant mechanisms in cancer biology. In-depth mechanistic analysis of the PRMT regulome is essential, and our studies provide important new insights into how PRMTs regulate transcription, RNA splicing, DNA damage repair, and anti-tumor immune responses in the context of HGSOC and TNBC.

      In summary, our study identifies PRMT1 and PRMT5 as key epigenetic regulators of DNA damage repair and shows that their inhibition sensitizes HR-proficient tumors to PARP inhibitors by repressing transcription and altering splicing of BRCAness genes. Distinct from prior strategies, dual inhibition of type I PRMT and PRMT5 exhibits strong synergy, allowing for lower-dose combination treatments that may reduce toxicity. Our findings also nominate ERCC1 as a potential predictive biomarker and suggest that MYC-driven tumors may be particularly responsive to this approach. Collectively, these results offer a mechanistic rationale and translational framework to broaden the clinical application of PARP inhibitors.

      (2) In Figures 3H-J, MYC targets were likely to correlate with the expression levels of PRMT1/PRMT5 in various public datasets, supporting previous reports that the Myc-PRMT loop plays critical roles during tumorigenesis (ref. 45). "Myc-targets" signatures were also the most significant signatures correlated with the expression of PRMT1 and PRMT5. The authors suggest that under MYC-hyperactivated conditions, tumors may be extremely sensitive to PRMT inhibitors or PRMTi/PARPi combination. However, the underlying mechanism remains unclear.

      We sincerely thank the reviewer for the critical and insightful comments. We fully agree that more direct evidence is needed to establish the regulatory relationship between MYC and PRMT1/5. To investigate the effect of c-Myc on PRMT1 and PRMT5 expression, we analyzed RNA-seq data from P493-6 Burkitt lymphoma cells, which harbor a tetracycline (Tet)-repressible MYC transgene. In this system, MYC expression can be suppressed to very low levels and then reactivated, enabling a gradual increase in c-Myc protein levels[14]. Upon Tet removal to induce MYC expression, we observed a robust upregulation of both PRMT1 (4.3-fold) and PRMT5 (3.6-fold) RNA levels within 24 hours, as measured by RNA-seq. These findings indicate that MYC activation can transcriptionally upregulate PRMT1 and PRMT5. To determine whether this regulation is directly driven by MYC, we further analyzed MYC ChIP-seq profiles from the same cell line following 24 hours of MYC induction. Consistently, we observed remarkably increased MYC binding at the promoter regions of both PRMT1 and PRMT5 genes. Interestingly, MYC’s regulatory influence was not limited to PRMT1 and PRMT5, we also observed transcriptional upregulation of other PRMT family members, including PRMT3, PRMT4, and PRMT6, in response to MYC activation. Together with the data presented in Figure 3H, these new results strongly suggest that MYC directly upregulates the expression of PRMT family genes by binding to their promoter regions. Consequently, increased PRMT expression may facilitate MYC’s regulation of target gene expression and splicing in cancer cells. In cancers with MYC hyperactivation, this feed-forward loop may be amplified, creating a potential therapeutic vulnerability. In response to the reviewer’s insightful suggestion, we have further explored how MYC regulates PRMT1/5 and whether this regulation modulates the efficacy of PRMT inhibitors in oncology. These unpublished observations are currently being prepared for a separate manuscript, and we have now incorporated a discussion of these unpublished findings into the revised version of this manuscript. We thank the reviewer again for the thoughtful and constructive comments regarding the MYC–PRMT regulatory axis.

      (3) In Figure 5F, ERCC1 expression was unlikely to be reduced in cells treated with GSK025, especially in OVCAR8 cells, although other cells, including TNBC cells, are dramatically changed after treatment.

      We sincerely thank the reviewer for the critical and insightful comments. We agree with the reviewer that in Figure 5F, although GSK025 treatment reduced ERCC1 expression, the loading control Tubulin also showed a notable decrease in the OVCAR8 cell line. This may be because Tubulin expression is not specifically affected by the chemical inhibitor GSK025 in this particular cell line, or it may be secondarily reduced as a consequence of PRMT inhibitor-induced cell death. As the reviewer pointed out, this phenomenon was not observed in other cell lines, suggesting that the effect on Tubulin is not specific to PRMT inhibition. To further investigate, we employed CRISPR/Cas9-mediated knockout of PRMT1 or PRMT5 in OVCAR8 cells, a more specific genetic approach to inhibit PRMT activity. In both cases, ERCC1 expression was significantly reduced, whereas Tubulin levels remained stable (Figure 5G). These results support the conclusion that PRMT1 and PRMT5 specifically regulate ERCC1 expression in OVCAR8 cells. The inconsistent effect on Tubulin is likely due to nonspecific cellular responses to chemical inhibition, which are generally more variable and less precise than those induced by genetic perturbation.

      (4) In Figure 7H-L, MDA-MB-231 cells were implanted subcutaneously in nude immunodeficient mice to confirm the synergistic therapeutic action of the PRMTi/PARPi combination in vivo. Although PRMT inhibition activates intrinsic innate immune pathways in cancer cells, suggesting that PRMTi treatments may enhance intrinsic immune reactions in tumor cells, the use of nude immune deficient mice means that changes in the tumor immune microenvironment remain unknown.

      We sincerely thank the reviewer for the critical and insightful comments. We fully agree with the reviewer that our in vivo experiments using the human cancer cell line MDA-MB-231 in immunodeficient nude mice limit our ability to assess changes in the tumor immune microenvironment. We thank the reviewer for highlighting this important limitation. While the primary goal of the current study was to investigate the therapeutic synergy between PRMT inhibition and PARP inhibition in cancer cells, we would like to take this opportunity to share additional unpublished data that further support and extend the reviewer’s point regarding the immunomodulatory effects of PRMT inhibitors. In syngeneic mouse tumor models, we have observed that the combination of PRMT inhibition and PARP inhibition leads to a more robust anti-tumor immune response compared to either treatment alone. Specifically, we found increased infiltration of CD8⁺ cytotoxic T cells within the tumor microenvironment, suggesting enhanced immune activation and tumor immunogenicity. Furthermore, we have also obtained preliminary evidence that PRMT inhibition can potentiate immune checkpoint blockade therapy. Mechanistically, this may be mediated through the activation of the STING1 pathway and the upregulation of splicing-derived neoantigens, both of which have been implicated in promoting tumor immune visibility. These findings indicate that beyond enhancing DNA damage response, PRMT inhibition may have a broader impact on tumor-immune interactions and could serve as a promising strategy to sensitize tumors to immunotherapy. A separate manuscript detailing these results is currently in preparation and will be submitted for publication as an independent research article. In light of the reviewer’s thoughtful suggestions and in consideration of feedback from Reviewer #2, who recommended removing Figure 6 from the manuscript, we have carefully reevaluated the overall organization of the manuscript. Given the scope and focus of the current work, as well as the desire to maintain a concise and coherent narrative, we decided to move the content originally presented in Figure 6 to the supplementary materials. This figure is now included as Supplementary Figure S5 in the revised version of the manuscript. We believe this change helps streamline the main text while still making the additional data available for interested readers.

      (5) In Figures 6-7, a two-tailed Student's t-test was used to determine the statistical differences among multiple comparisons, which should be performed by one-way ANOVA followed by a post hoc test.

      We thank the reviewer for this thoughtful and important comment regarding the choice of statistical method. We fully agree with the reviewer that one-way ANOVA followed by a post hoc test is one of the standard approaches for multiple group comparisons. In response to the suggestion, we have performed one-way ANOVA on our data and found that the statistical conclusions are consistent with those obtained from the two-tailed Student’s t-tests. For example, in the first panel of Figure 6A (OVCAR8 treated with GSK715), one-way ANOVA (p = 1.1 × 10<sup>-6</sup>), followed by Tukey’s HSD test, confirmed significant differences between control and Olaparib (p = 0.000165), control and GSK715 (p = 0.000145), control and combination (p = 6.067 × 10<sup>-7</sup>), Olaparib and combination (p = 0.0003523), and GSK715 and combination (p = 0.0004015), consistent with the conclusions from the two-tailed t-test shown in Figure 6H. Additionally, we would like to explain why two-tailed Student’s t-tests were used in our current study. When comparisons are predefined and conducted pairwise (i.e., two groups at a time), a two-tailed Student’s t-test is statistically equivalent to one-way ANOVA for those comparisons. In our study, each comparison involved only two groups, and we therefore chose t-tests for hypothesis-driven, specific comparisons rather than exploratory multiple testing. This approach aligns with valid statistical principles. All statistical analyses presented in Figures 6-7 were designed to evaluate specific, biologically meaningful comparisons (e.g., treatment vs. control or treatment A vs treatment B). The study was hypothesis-driven, not exploratory, and did not involve simultaneous comparisons across multiple groups. In such cases, the t-test provides a more direct and interpretable result for targeted comparisons. The use of Student’s t-tests reflects the focused nature of the analysis, where each test directly addresses a specific biological question rather than a global group comparison. We sincerely appreciate the reviewer’s thoughtful comments on the statistical methods.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) If the authors kept the tumors of various sizes in Figure 7I, it would be important to assess the protein and/or mRNA level of ERCC1 to further support their mechanism.

      We sincerely thank the reviewer for the insightful comments. We fully agree that evaluating ERCC1 expression in drug-treated tumor samples is critical to support the proposed mechanism. Due to the limited volume of tumor specimens and extensive necrosis observed after three weeks of treatment in the condition used for Figure 7I, we were unable to obtain sufficient material for expression analysis in the original cohort. To address this, we conducted an additional experiment using xenograft-bearing mice (MDA-MB-231 model), initiating treatment when tumors reached approximately 200 mm³ to ensure adequate tissue collection. We also shortened the treatment duration to 7 days to assess early molecular responses to therapy, rather than downstream effects. Consistent with our in vitro results, both GSK715 and GSK025 significantly reduced ERCC1 RNA expression (0.79 ± 0.17, p = 0.03; 0.82 ± 0.11, p = 0.02, respectively), and the combination treatment further decreased ERCC1 expression (0.49 ± 0.20, p = 0.0003), as determined by qRT-PCR. A two-tailed Student’s t-test was used for statistical analysis. In this experiment, we used the same dosing regimen as in the three-week treatment shown in Figure 7I. Importantly, the shorter treatment period and moderate tumor size at treatment initiation minimized necrosis and did not significantly affect tumor growth, allowing for reliable molecular evaluation. We sincerely thank the reviewer for highlighting this important point.

      (2) Figure 2G: please explain why two bands remain for sgPRMT1.

      We greatly appreciate the reviewer for raising this insightful and important question. As the reviewer pointed out, an additional band appeared after PRMT1 knockdown in OVCAR8 cells using two sequence-independent gRNAs. Notably, this band was not observed in MDA-MB-231 cells. The antibody used to detect PRMT1 (clone A33, #2449, Cell Signaling Technology) is widely adopted in PRMT1 research, with over 65 citations supporting its specificity. Interestingly, previous studies[15] have identified seven PRMT1 isoforms (v1–v7), generated through alternative splicing and exhibiting tissue-specific expression patterns. Of these, three isoforms are detectable using the A33 antibody. We believe the additional band observed upon sgRNA treatment likely represents a PRMT1 isoform that is normally expressed at low levels in OVCAR8 cells. Upon knockdown of the major isoforms by CRISPR/Cas9, expression of this minor isoform may have increased as part of a compensatory feedback mechanism, rendering it detectable by immunoblotting. Because PRMT1 isoform expression is largely tissue-type specific, it is not surprising that the same band was absent in MDA-MB-231 cells, which are derived from a different lineage than OVCAR8 cells. The reviewer raised an important question regarding the role of PRMT1 isoforms in regulating DNA damage response in cancer. We agree this is an intriguing direction and will investigate it further in future studies.

      (3) Figure 4D: Please correct the figure legend so the description matches the color in the figure. Red and blue are absent.

      We sincerely thank the reviewer for the critical and insightful comments. The figure legend for Figure 4D has been corrected in the revised version of the manuscript to accurately match the colors shown in the figure. We thank the reviewer for pointing out this issue.

      (4) Figure 7A and B: please indicate the cell lines used.

      We sincerely thank the reviewer for the critical and insightful comments. In Figure 7A and 7B, human embryonic kidney 293T (HEK293T) cells were used due to their high transfection efficiency and widespread application in reporter assays. This information has been incorporated into the figure legend for Figures 7A and 7B.

      (5) What is the link with ERCC1 splicing because reduced overall ERCC1 expression is clear?

      We sincerely thank the reviewer for the critical and insightful comments. As the reviewer pointed out, although the direct impact of ERCC1 alternative splicing on its protein expression remains to be fully elucidated, it is likely that PRMT inhibition induces aberrant splicing events that result in the production of alternative ERCC1 isoforms with impaired or altered function. These splicing changes may compromise ERCC1’s role in DNA repair pathways. Furthermore, as shown in Figure 4G, we observed a reduction in the total ERCC1 mRNA reads following PRMTi treatment. This decrease may be attributed, at least in part, to the instability of the alternatively spliced ERCC1 transcripts, which could be more prone to degradation. In combination with the transcriptional downregulation of ERCC1 induced by PRMT inhibition, these alternative splicing events may lead to a further reduction in functional ERCC1 protein levels. This dual impact on ERCC1 expression, through both decreased transcription and the generation of unstable or non-functional isoforms, likely contributes to the enhanced cellular sensitivity to PARP inhibitors observed in our study. We believe this represents an important mechanistic insight into how PRMT inhibition modulates the DNA damage response in cancer cells, and further studies are warranted to investigate the precise role of ERCC1 splicing regulation in this context. We thank the reviewer for pointing out this interesting future research direction.

      (6) Figure 7J: From the graph, it seems like Olaparib+G715 and G715+G025 have a similar effect on tumor volume (two curves overlap). Please discuss.

      We sincerely thank the reviewer for the critical and insightful comments. In the current study, the doses used for single-agent treatments were selected based on prior publications. For example, the dose of GSK715 was guided by a recent study from the GSK group[16]. Our in vitro and in vivo findings, together with previously published data, consistently demonstrate that GSK715 is more potent than both GSK025 and Olaparib. Notably, treatment with GSK715 alone led to significantly greater inhibition of tumor growth compared to either GSK025 or Olaparib administered individually. This higher potency of GSK715 also explains the comparable levels of tumor suppression observed in the combination groups, including GSK715 plus Olaparib and GSK715 plus GSK025. These results suggest that GSK715 is likely the primary driver of efficacy in the two drug combination settings. Importantly, this observation provides a valuable opportunity to further refine and optimize the dosing strategy for GSK715. Specifically, because GSK715 is highly potent, its dose may be reduced when used in combination regimens without compromising therapeutic efficacy. This approach could significantly improve the safety profile of GSK715 by minimizing potential dose-related toxicities, thereby enhancing its suitability for future clinical development in combination therapy contexts.

      (7) Discussion: "PRMT5i increased global sDMA levels"-> "... aDMA levels.".

      We sincerely thank the reviewer for the critical and insightful comments. In response, we have corrected the sentence in the discussion from “PRMT5i increased global sDMA levels, which suggested that type I PRMT and PRMT5 share a substrate (i.e., MMA) and/or their functions are compensatory” to “PRMT1i increased global sDMA levels, which suggested that type I PRMT and PRMT5 share a substrate (i.e., MMA) and/or their functions are compensatory.” We apologize for the misstatement and have corrected this error in the revised version of the manuscript.

      (8) In addition to the methods, add that nude mice were used in the body of the results and the figure legend for Figure 7J.

      We sincerely thank the reviewer for the critical and insightful comments. In the revised version of the manuscript, we have added that immunodeficient nude mice were used in both the body of the Results section and the figure legend for Figure 7J, in addition to the Methods section. We thank the reviewer for this helpful suggestion.

      (9) Figure 6 can be deleted to focus the manuscript. It does not add to the PARP inhibition story, but only suggests a link to immunotherapy where this has been reported previously PMID: 35578032 and 32641491.

      We sincerely thank the reviewer for the critical and insightful comments. Reviewer #1 also raised a related concern regarding the relevance of this section to the main focus of the manuscript. In consideration of both reviewers’ comments, we have decided to move the data previously shown in Figure 6 to the supplementary section as Supplementary Figure S5. This revision allows us to streamline the main text and maintain a clear focus on the core findings related to PARP inhibition. At the same time, we believe the immunotherapy-related observation may still be of interest to some readers. By presenting these results in the supplementary materials, we ensure that this potentially relevant link remains accessible without distracting from the primary narrative of the manuscript. We greatly appreciate the reviewers’ guidance in helping us improve the clarity and focus of our work. We thank the reviewer for the thoughtful suggestion.

      References

      (1) Dominici, C., et al. Synergistic effects of type I PRMT and PARP inhibitors against non-small cell lung cancer cells. Clin Epigenetics 13, 54 (2021).

      (2) O'Brien, S., et al. Inhibiting PRMT5 induces DNA damage and increases anti-proliferative activity of Niraparib, a PARP inhibitor, in models of breast and ovarian cancer. BMC Cancer 23, 775 (2023).

      (3) Carter, J., et al. PRMT5 Inhibitors Regulate DNA Damage Repair Pathways in Cancer Cells and Improve Response to PARP Inhibition and Chemotherapies. Cancer Res Commun 3, 2233-2243 (2023).

      (4) Li, Y., et al. PRMT blockade induces defective DNA replication stress response and synergizes with PARP inhibition. Cell Rep Med 4, 101326 (2023).

      (5) Yang, L., et al. Repression of BET activity sensitizes homologous recombination-proficient cancers to PARP inhibition. Sci Transl Med 9(2017).

      (6) Aftimos, P.G., et al. A phase 1b/2 study of the BET inhibitor ZEN-3694 in combination with talazoparib for treatment of patients with TNBC without gBRCA1/2 mutations. Journal of Clinical Oncology 40, 1023-1023 (2022).

      (7) Karakashev, S., et al. BET Bromodomain Inhibition Synergizes with PARP Inhibitor in Epithelial Ovarian Cancer. Cell Rep 21, 3398-3405 (2017).

      (8) Sun, C., et al. BRD4 Inhibition Is Synthetic Lethal with PARP Inhibitors through the Induction of Homologous Recombination Deficiency. Cancer Cell 33, 401-416 e408 (2018).

      (9) Johnson, S.F., et al. CDK12 Inhibition Reverses De Novo and Acquired PARP Inhibitor Resistance in BRCA Wild-Type and Mutated Models of Triple-Negative Breast Cancer. Cell Rep 17, 2367-2381 (2016).

      (10) Iniguez, A.B., et al. EWS/FLI Confers Tumor Cell Synthetic Lethality to CDK12 Inhibition in Ewing Sarcoma. Cancer Cell 33, 202-216 e206 (2018).

      (11) Shan, W., et al. Systematic Characterization of Recurrent Genomic Alterations in Cyclin-Dependent Kinases Reveals Potential Therapeutic Strategies for Cancer Treatment. Cell Rep 32, 107884 (2020).

      (12) Muvarak, N.E., et al. Enhancing the Cytotoxic Effects of PARP Inhibitors with DNA Demethylating Agents - A Potential Therapy for Cancer. Cancer Cell 30, 637-650 (2016).

      (13) Abbotts, R., et al. DNA methyltransferase inhibitors induce a BRCAness phenotype that sensitizes NSCLC to PARP inhibitor and ionizing radiation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 116, 22609-22618 (2019).

      (14) Lin, C.Y., et al. Transcriptional amplification in tumor cells with elevated c-Myc. Cell 151, 56-67 (2012).

      (15) Goulet, I., Gauvin, G., Boisvenue, S. & Cote, J. Alternative splicing yields protein arginine methyltransferase 1 isoforms with distinct activity, substrate specificity, and subcellular localization. J Biol Chem 282, 33009-33021 (2007).

      (16) Fedoriw, A., et al. Anti-tumor Activity of the Type I PRMT Inhibitor, GSK3368715, Synergizes with PRMT5 Inhibition through MTAP Loss. Cancer Cell 36, 100-114 e125 (2019).

    1. eLife Assessment

      This study presents a valuable finding that the biomechanical force of heart contractility is required for robust endocardial id2b expression, which in return promotes valve development and myocardial function through upregulation of Neuregulin 1. The data were collected and analyzed using solid methodology and can be used as a starting point for deeper mechanistic insights into the genetic programs regulating endocardial-myocardial crosstalk during heart development.

    2. Joint Public Review:

      Summary:

      How mechanical forces transmitted by blood flow contribute to cardiac development remains incompletely understood. Using the unique advantages of the zebrafish model, Chen et al make the fundamental discovery that endocardial expression of the transcriptional repressor, Id2b, is maintained in endocardial cells by blood flow. Id1b zebrafish mutants fail to form the valve in the atrioventricular canal (AVC) and show reduced myocardial contractility that they suggest is due to impaired calcium transients. Id2b mutants are largely viable during the first 6 months of life until ~20% display cardiomyopathy characterized by visible edema, structural abnormalities, retrograde blood flow, and reduced systolic function and calcium handling. Mechanistically, the authors suggest that flow-mediated expression of Id2b leads to neuregulin 1 (nrg1) upregulation by physically interacting with and sequestering the Tcf3b transcriptional repressor from conserved tcf3b binding sites upstream of nrg1. Overall, this study advances our understanding of flow-mediated endocardial-myocardial crosstalk during heart development.

      Strengths:

      The strengths of the study are the significance of the biological question being addressed, use of the zebrafish model, data quality, and use of genetic tools. The text is generally well-written and easy to understand.

      Weaknesses:

      The main weakness that remains is the lack of rigor surrounding the molecular mechanism where the authors suggest that blood flow induces endocardial expression of Id2b, which binds to Tcf3b and sequesters it from binding the Nrg1 promoter to repress transcription. Although good faith efforts were made to bolster their model, the physical interaction between Id2b and Tcf3b is limited to overexpression of tagged proteins in HEK293 cells. Moreover, no mutagenesis was performed on the tcf3b binding sites identified in the nrg1 promoter to learn their importance in vivo.

    3. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Chen et al. identified the role of endocardial id2b expression in cardiac contraction and valve formation through pharmaceutical, genetic, electrophysiology, calcium imaging, and echocardiography analyses. CRISPR/Cas9 generated id2b mutants demonstrated defective AV valve formation, excitation-contraction coupling, reduced endocardial cell proliferation in AV valve, retrograde blood flow, and lethal effects.

      Strengths:

      Their methods, data and analyses broadly support their claims.

      Weaknesses:

      The molecular mechanism is somewhat preliminary.

      We thank the reviewer for the positive assessment of our work. A detailed point-by-point response has been incorporated in the response to “Recommendations for the authors” section.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Biomechanical forces, such as blood flow, are crucial for organ formation, including heart development. This study by Shuo Chen et al. aims to understand how cardiac cells respond to these forces. They used zebrafish as a model organism due to its unique strengths, such as the ability to survive without heartbeats, and conducted transcriptomic analysis on hearts with impaired contractility. They thereby identified id2b as a gene regulated by blood flow and is crucial for proper heart development, in particular, for the regulation of myocardial contractility and valve formation. Using both in situ hybridization and transgenic fish they showed that id2b is specifically expressed in the endocardium, and its expression is affected by both pharmacological and genetic perturbations of contraction. They further generated a null mutant of id2b to show that loss of id2b results in heart malformation and early lethality in zebrafish. Atrioventricular (AV) and excitation-contraction coupling were also impaired in id2b mutants. Mechanistically, they demonstrate that Id2b interacts with the transcription factor Tcf3b to restrict its activity. When id2b is deleted, the repressor activity of Tcf3b is enhanced, leading to suppression of the expression of nrg1 (neuregulin 1), a key factor for heart development. Importantly, injecting tcf3b morpholino into id2b-/- embryos partially restores the reduced heart rate. Moreover, treatment of zebrafish embryos with the Erbb2 inhibitor AG1478 results in decreased heart rate, in line with a model in which Id2b modulates heart development via the Nrg1/Erbb2 axis. The research identifies id2b as a biomechanical signaling-sensitive gene in endocardial cells that mediates communication between the endocardium and myocardium, which is essential for heart morphogenesis and function.

      Strengths:

      The study provides novel insights into the molecular mechanisms by which biomechanical forces influence heart development and highlights the importance of id2b in this process.

      Weaknesses:

      The claims are in general well supported by experimental evidence, but the following aspects may benefit from further investigation:

      (1) In Figure 1C, the heatmap demonstrates the up-regulated and down-regulated genes upon tricane-induced cardiac arrest. Aside from the down-regulation of id2b expression, it was also evident that id2a expression was up-regulated. As a predicted paralog of id2b, it would be interesting to see whether the up-regulation of id2a in response to tricane treatment was a compensatory response to the down-regulation of id2b expression.

      We thank the reviewer for the comment. As suggested, we performed qRT-PCR analysis to assess id2a expression in tricaine-treated heart. Our results demonstrate a significant upregulation of id2a following the inhibition of cardiac contraction, suggesting a potential compensatory response to the decreased id2b. These new results have been incorporated into the revised manuscript (Figure 1D).

      (2) The study mentioned that id2b is tightly regulated by the flow-sensitive primary cilia-klf2 signaling axis; however aside from showing the reduced expression of id2b in klf2a and klf2b mutants, there was no further evidence to solidify the functional link between id2b and klf2. It would therefore be ideal, in the present study, to demonstrate how Klf2, which is a transcriptional regulator, transduces biomechanical stimuli to Id2b.

      We have examined the expression levels of id2b in both klf2a and klf2b mutants. The whole mount in situ results clearly demonstrate a decrease in id2b signal in both mutants (Figure 3E). As noted by the reviewer, klf2 is a transcriptional regulator, suggesting that the regulation of id2b may occur at the transcriptional level. However, dissecting the molecular mechanisms underlying the crosstalk between klf2 and id2b is beyond the scope of the present study.

      (3) The authors showed the physical interaction between ectopically expressed FLAG-Id2b and HA-Tcf3b in HEK293T cells. Although the constructs being expressed are of zebrafish origin, it would be nice to show in vivo that the two proteins interact.

      We thank the reviewer for this insightful comment. As suggested, we synthesized Flag-id2b and HA-tcf3b mRNA and co-injected them into 1-cell stage zebrafish embryos. We collected 100-300 embryos at 12, 24, and 48 hpf and performed western blot analysis using the same anti-HA and anti-Flag antibodies validated in HEK293 cell experiments. Despite multiple independent attempts, we were unable to detect clear bands of the tagged proteins in zebrafish embryos. We speculate that this could be due to mRNA instability, translational efficiency, or the low abundance of Id2b and Tcf3b proteins. We have acknowledged these technical limitations in the revised manuscript and clarified that the HEK293 cell data support a potential interaction between Id2b and Tcf3b, while confirming their endogenous interaction will require further investigations (Lines 295-296).

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      How mechanical forces transmitted by blood flow contribute to normal cardiac development remains incompletely understood. Using the unique advantages of the zebrafish model system, Chen et al make the fundamental discovery that endocardial expression of id2b is induced by blood flow and required for normal atrioventricular canal (AVC) valve development and myocardial contractility by regulating calcium dynamics. Mechanistically, the authors suggest that Id2b binds to Tcf3b in endocardial cells, which relieves Tcf3b-mediated transcriptional repression of Neuregulin 1 (NRG1). Nrg1 then induces expression of the L-type calcium channel component LRRC1. This study significantly advances our understanding of flow-mediated valve formation and myocardial function.

      Strengths:

      Strengths of the study are the significance of the question being addressed, use of the zebrafish model, and data quality (mostly very nice imaging). The text is also well-written and easy to understand.

      Weaknesses:

      Weaknesses include a lack of rigor for key experimental approaches, which led to skepticism surrounding the main findings. Specific issues were the use of morpholinos instead of genetic mutants for the bmp ligands, cilia gene ift88, and tcf3b, lack of an explicit model surrounding BMP versus blood flow induced endocardial id2b expression, use of bar graphs without dots, the artificial nature of assessing the physical interaction of Tcf3b and Id2b in transfected HEK293 cells, and artificial nature of examining the function of the tcf3b binding sites upstream of nrg1.

      We thank the reviewer for the positive assessment and the constructive suggestions. We have performed additional experiments and data analysis to address these issues. A detailed point-by-point response has been incorporated in the response to “Recommendations for the authors” section.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Questions/Concerns:

      (1) In the introduction, it would be beneficial to include background information on the id2b gene, what is currently known about its function in heart development/regeneration and in other animal models than just the zebrafish.

      We thank the reviewer for the constructive suggestion. In the revised manuscript, we have added a paragraph in the Introduction to provide background on id2b and its role in heart development. Specifically, we discuss its function as a member of the ID (inhibitor of DNA binding) family of helix-loop-helix (HLH) transcriptional regulators and highlight its involvement in cardiogenesis in both zebrafish and mouse models. These additions help place our findings in a broader developmental and evolutionary context (Lines 91-100).

      (2) Of the 6 differentially expressed genes identified in Figure 1C, why did the authors choose to focus on id2b and not the other 5 downregulated genes?

      We thank the reviewer for the comments. As suggested, we have added a sentence in the revised manuscript to clarify the rationale for selecting id2b as the focus of the present study (Lines 117-121).

      (3) As the authors showed representative in situ images for id2b expression with blebbistatin treatment in Figure 1E, and tnn2a MO in Figure 1F, it would also be beneficial to show relative mRNA expression levels for id2b in conditions of blebbistatin treatment and tnn2a MO knockdown. In Fig. 1C: id2b is downregulated with tricaine, but id2a is upregulated with tricaine. Do these genes perform similar or different functions, results of gene duplication events?

      We thank the reviewer for the thoughtful suggestion. Our in situ hybridization results demonstrate reduced id2b expression following tricaine, blebbistatin, and tnn2 morpholino treatment. To further validate these observations and enhance cellular resolution, we generated an id2b:eGFP knockin line. Analysis of this reporter line confirmed a significant reduction in id2b expression in the endocardium upon inhibition of cardiac contraction and blood flow (Figure 3A-D), supporting our in situ results. The divergent expression patterns of id2a and id2b in response to tricaine treatment likely reflect functional specification following gene duplication in zebrafish. While our current study focuses on characterizing the role of id2b in zebrafish heart development, the specific function of id2a remains to be determined. 

      (4) In Fig. 2b, could the authors compare the id2b fluorescence with RNAscope ISH at 24, 48, and 72 hpf? RNAscope ISH allows for the visualization of single RNA molecules in individual cells. The authors should at least compare these in the heart to demonstrate that id2b accurately reflects the endogenous id2b expression. In Fig. 2E: Suggest showing the individual fluorescent images for id2b:eGFP and kdrl:mCherry in the same colors as top panel images instead of in black and white. In Fig. 2F: The GFP fluorescence from id2b:eGFP signals looks overexposed.

      We thank the reviewer for the valuable comment. In response, we attempted RNAscope in situ hybridization on embryos carrying the id2b:eGFP reporter to directly compare fluorescent reporter expression with endogenous id2b transcripts. However, we encountered a significant reduction in id2b:eGFP fluorescence following the RNAscope procedure, and even subsequent immunostaining with anti-GFP antibodies yielded only weak signals. Despite this technical limitation, the RNAscope results independently confirmed id2b expression in endocardial cells (Figure 2E), supporting the specificity and cell-type localization observed with the reporter line. As suggested by the reviewer, we have updated Figure 2G to display id2b:eGFP and kdrl:mCherry images in the same color scheme as the top panel to improve consistency and clarity. Additionally, we have replaced the images in Figure 2F to avoid overexposure and better represent the spatial distribution of id2b:eGFP in adult heart.

      (5) In Fig. 3A: are all the images in panel A taken with the same magnification? In Fig. 3e, could the authors show the localization of klf2 and id2b and confirm their expression in the same endocardial cells? In Fig. 3, the authors conclude that klf2-mediated biomechanical signaling is essential for activating id2b expression. This statement is somewhat overstated because they only demonstrated that knockout of klf2 reduced id2b expression.

      We thank the reviewer for these constructive comments. All images presented in Figure 3A were captured using the same magnification, as now clarified in the revised figure legend. We appreciate the reviewer’s question regarding the localization of klf2 and id2b. While we were unable to directly visualize both markers in the same embryos due to the current unavailability of klf2 reporter lines, prior studies using klf2a:H2B-eGFP transgenic zebrafish have demonstrated that klf2a is broadly expressed in endocardial cells, with enhanced expression in the atrioventricular canal region (Heckel et al., Curr Bio 2015, PMID: 25959969; Gálvez-Santisteban et al., Elife 2019, PMID: 31237233). Our id2b:eGFP reporter analysis revealed a similarly broad endocardial expression pattern. These independent observations support the likelihood that klf2a and id2b are co-expressed in the same endocardial cell population.   

      We also appreciate the reviewer’s comments regarding the connection between biomechanical signaling and id2b expression. Previous studies have already established that biomechanical cues directly regulate klf2 expression in zebrafish endocardial cells (Vermot et al., Plos Biol 2009, PMID: 19924233; Heckel et al., Curr Bio 2015, PMID: 25959969). In the present study, we observed a significant reduction in id2b expression in both klf2a and klf2b mutants, suggesting that id2b acts downstream of klf2. These observations together establish the role of biomechanical cues-klf2-id2b signaling axis in endocardial cells. Nevertheless, we agree with the reviewer that further investigation is required to elucidate the precise mechanism by which klf2 regulates id2b expression.

      (6) In Fig. 4: What's the mRNA expression for id2b in WT and id2b mutant fish hearts?

      We performed qRT-PCR analysis on purified zebrafish hearts and observed a significant reduction in id2b mRNA levels in id2b mutants compared to wild-type controls. These new results have been incorporated into the revised manuscript (Figure 4A).

      (7) In Fig. 5E, the heart rate shows no difference between id2b+/+ and id2b-/- fish according to echocardiography analysis. However, Fig. 5B indicates a difference in heart rate. Could the authors explain this discrepancy?

      We thank the reviewer for this insightful observation. In our study, we observed a reduction in heart rate in id2b mutants during embryonic stages (120 hpf), as shown in Figure 5B. However, this difference was not evident in adult fish based on echocardiography analysis (Figure 5E). While the exact reason for these changes during development remains unclear, it is possible that the reduction in cardiac output observed in id2b mutants during early development triggers compensatory mechanisms over time, ultimately restoring heart rate in adulthood. Given that heart rate is primarily regulated by pacemaker activity, further investigation will be required to determine whether such compensatory adaptations occur and to elucidate the underlying mechanisms.

      (8) In Fig. 6A: it's a little hard to read the gene names in the left most image in the panel. In Fig. 6B, the authors conducted qRT-PCR analysis of 72 hpf embryonic hearts and validated decreased nrg1 levels in id2b-/- compared to control. Since nrg1 is not specifically expressed in endocardial cells in the developing heart, the authors should isolate endocardial cells and compare nrg1 expression in id2b-/- to control. This would ensure that the loss of id2b affects nrg1 expression derived from endocardial cells rather than other cell types. In Supp Figure S6: Suggest adding an image of the UMAP projection to show tcf3b expression in endocardial cells from sequencing analysis.

      We thank the reviewer for these helpful suggestions. In response, we have increased the font size of gene names in the leftmost panel of Figure 6A to improve readability. Regarding nrg1 expression, we acknowledge the importance of assessing its cell-type specificity. Unfortunately, due to the lack of reliable transgenic or knock-in tools for nrg1, its precise expression pattern in embryonic hearts remains unclear. We attempted to isolate endocardial cells from embryonic hearts using FACS, but the limited number of cells obtained at this stage precluded reliable qRT-PCR analysis. Nonetheless, our data show that id2b is specifically expressed in endocardial cells, and publicly available single-cell RNA-seq datasets also support that nrg1 is predominantly expressed in endocardial, but not myocardial or epicardial cells during embryonic heart development (Figure 6-figure supplement 1). These findings suggest that id2b may regulate nrg1 expression in a cell-autonomous manner within the endocardium. As suggested, we have also added a UMAP image to Figure 7-figure supplement 1 to show tcf3b expression in endocardial cells, further supporting the cell identity in single-cell dataset.

      (9) In Fig. 6, Nrg1 knockout shows no gross morphological defects and normal trabeculation in larvae. Could the authors explain why they propose that endocardial id2b promotes nrg1 synthesis, thereby enhancing cardiomyocyte contractile function? Did Nrg1 knockdown with Mo lead to compromised calcium signaling and cardiac contractile function? Nrg2a has been reported to be expressed in endocardial cells in larvae, and its loss leads to heart function defects. Perhaps Nrg2a plays a more important role than Nrg1.

      We thank the reviewer for raising this important point. Although we did not directly test nrg1 knockout in our study, previous reports have shown that genetic deletion of nrg1 in zebrafish does not impair cardiac trabeculation during embryonic stages (Rasouli et al., Nat Commun 2017, PMID: 28485381; Brown et al., J Cell Mol Med 2018, PMID: 29265764). However, reduced trabecular area and signs of arrhythmia were observed in juvenile and adult fish (Brown et al., J Cell Mol Med 2018, PMID: 29265764), suggesting a potential role for nrg1 in maintaining cardiac structure and function later in development. Whether calcium signaling and cardiac contractility are affected at these stages remains to be determined. Given that morpholino-induced knockdown is limited to early embryonic stages, it is not suitable for assessing nrg1 function in juvenile or adult hearts.

      As noted by the reviewer, nrg2a is expressed in endocardial cells, and its deletion has been associated with cardiac defects (Rasouli et al., Nat Commun 2017, PMID: 28485381). To assess its potential involvement in our model, we performed qRT-PCR analysis and observed increased nrg2a expression in id2b mutant hearts (Author response image 1). This upregulation may reflect a compensatory response to the loss of id2b. Therefore, nrg2a is unlikely to play an essential role in mediating the depressed cardiac function in this context.

      Author response image 1.

      Expression levels of nrg2a. qRT-PCR analysis of nrg2a mRNA in id2b<sup>+/+</sup> and id2b<sup>-/-</sup> adult hearts. Data were normalized to the expression of actb1. N=5 biological replicates, with each sample containing two adult hearts.

      (10) In Fig. 7A of the IP experiment, it is recommended that the authors establish a negative control using control IgG corresponding to the primary antibody source. This control helps to differentiate non-specific background signal from specific antibody signal.

      As suggested, we have included an IgG control corresponding to the primary antibody species in the immunoprecipitation (IP) experiment to distinguish specific from non-specific binding. The updated data are presented in Figure 7A of the revised manuscript.

      (11) In Pg. 5, line 115: there is no reference included for previous literature on blebbistatin.

      We have added the corresponding reference (Line 126, Reference #5).

      In Pg. 5, lines 118-119; pg. 6 line 144: It would be beneficial to include a short sentence describing why choosing a tnnt2a morpholino knockdown to help provide mechanistic context to readers.

      We thank the reviewer for the constructive suggestion. In cardiomyocytes, tnnt2a encodes a sarcomeric protein essential for cardiac contraction, and its knockdown is a well-established method for abolishing heartbeat and blood flow in zebrafish embryos, thereby allowing investigation of flow-dependent gene regulation. In the revised manuscript, we have added a sentence and corresponding reference to clarify the rationale for using tnnt2a morpholino in our study (Lines 128-129, Reference #35).

      In Pg. 6, line 140: Results title of "Cardiac contraction promotes endocardial id2b expression through primary cilia but not BMP" is misleading and contradicts the results presented in this section and corresponding figure. For example, the bmp Mo knockdown experiments led to decreased id2b fluorescence and the last statement of this results section contradicts the title that BMP does not promote endocardial id2b in lines 179-180: "Collectively, these results suggest that BMP signaling and blood flow modulate id2b expression in a developmental-stage-dependent manner." It would be helpful to clarify whether BMP signaling is involved in id2b expression or not.

      We apologize for any confusion caused by the section title. Our results demonstrate that id2b expression is regulated by both BMP signaling and biomechanical forces in a developmental-stage-specific manner. Specifically, morpholino-mediated knockdown of bmp2b, bmp4, and bmp7a at the 1-cell stage significantly reduced id2b:eGFP fluorescence at 24 hpf (Figure 3-figure supplement 1A, B), suggesting that id2b is responsive to BMP signaling during early embryonic development. However, treatment with the BMP inhibitor Dorsomorphin during later stages (24-48 or 36-60 hpf) did not significantly alter id2b:eGFP fluorescence intensity in individual endocardial cells, although a modest reduction in total endocardial cell number was noted (Figure 3-figure supplement 1C, D). These results suggest that BMP signaling is required for id2b expression during early development but becomes dispensable at later stages, when biomechanical cues may play a more prominent role. To address this concern and better reflect the data, we have revised the Results section title to: "BMP signaling and cardiac contraction regulate id2b expression". This revised title more accurately reflects the dual regulation of id2b expression (Line 153).

      In line 205: Any speculation on why the hemodynamics was preserved between id2b mutant and WT siblings at 96 hpf?

      As suggested, we have included a sentence to address this observation. “Surprisingly, the pattern of hemodynamics was largely preserved in id2b<sup>-/-</sup> embryos compared to id2b<sup>+/+</sup> siblings at 96 hpf (Figure 4-figure supplement 1E, Video 1, 2), suggesting that the reduced number of endocardial cells in the AVC region was not sufficient to induce functional defects.” (Lines 223-225)

      In line 246: Fig. 6k and 6j are referenced, but should be figure 5k and 5j.

      We have corrected this in the revised manuscript.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      he manuscript was overall well explained, aside from a few minor points that would help facilitate reader comprehension:

      (1) The last paragraph of the introduction could be a brief summary of the study.

      We thank the reviewer for this constructive suggestion. As recommended, we have included a paragraph in the Introduction section summarizing our key findings to provide clearer context for the study (Lines 96-100).

      (2) Lines 127-128: 'revealed a substantial recapitulation of the... of endogenous id2b expression' may need to be rephrased.

      We thank the reviewer for the valuable suggestion. In the revised manuscript, we have changed the sentence to: “Comparison of id2b:eGFP fluorescence with in situ hybridization at 24, 48, and 72 hpf revealed that the reporter signal closely recapitulates the endogenous id2b expression pattern.” (Lines 137-139)

      (3) Line 182: '... in a developmental-stage-dependent manner' sounds a bit ambiguous, may need to slightly elaborate/ clarify what this means.

      We thank the reviewer for the helpful comment. To improve clarity, we have revised the statement to: “Collectively, these results suggest that id2b expression is regulated by both BMP and biomechanical signaling, with the relative contribution of each pathway varying across developmental stages.” (Lines 195-197)

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) The conclusion that BMP signaling prior to 24 hpf is necessary for id2b expression is not fully supported by the data. How do the authors envision pre-linear heart tube BMP signaling impacting endocardial id2b expression during later chamber stages? Id2b reporter fluorescence can be clearly visualized in the linear heart tube in panel B from Figure 1. Does id2b expression initiate prior to contraction? Can the model be refined by showing when id2b endocardial reporter fluorescence is first observed, and whether this early/pre-contractile expression is dependent on BMP signaling?

      We thank the reviewer for the important comment. As suggested, we performed morpholino-mediated knockdown of bmp2b, bmp4, and bmp7a at the 1-cell stage. Live imaging at 24 hpf showed significantly reduced id2b:eGFP fluorescence compared to controls (Figure 3-figure supplement 1A, B), suggesting that id2b is responsive to BMP signaling during early embryonic development. However, treatment with the BMP inhibitor Dorsomorphin during 24-48 or 36-60 hpf did not significantly impact id2b:eGFP fluorescence intensity in individual endocardial cells, although a reduction in endocardial cell number was observed (Figure 3-figure supplement 1C, D). These results suggest that BMP signaling is essential for id2b expression during early embryonic development, while it becomes dispensable at later stages, when biomechanical cues exert a more significant role.

      (2) Overexpressing tagged versions of TCF3b and Id2b in HEK293 cells is a very artificial way to make the major claim that these two proteins interact in endogenous endocardial cells. Can this be done in zebrafish embryonic or adult hearts?

      We thank the reviewer for this insightful comment. As suggested, we synthesized Flag-id2b and HA-tcf3b mRNA and co-injected them into 1-cell stage zebrafish embryos. We collected 100-300 embryos at 12, 24, and 48 hpf and performed western blot analysis using the same anti-HA and anti-Flag antibodies validated in HEK293 cell experiments. Despite multiple independent attempts, we were unable to detect clear bands of the tagged proteins in zebrafish embryos. We speculate that this could be due to mRNA instability, translational efficiency, or the low abundance of Id2b and Tcf3b proteins. We have acknowledged these technical limitations in the revised manuscript and clarified that the HEK293 cell data support a potential interaction between Id2b and Tcf3b, while confirming their endogenous interaction will require further investigations (Lines 295-296).

      (3) The data presented are consistent with the claim that the tcf3b binding sites are functional upstream of nrg1 to repress its transcription. To fully support this idea, those two sites should be disrupted with gRNAs if possible.

      We thank the reviewer for the valuable suggestion. In response, we attempted to disrupt the tcf3b binding sites using sgRNAs. However, we encountered technical difficulties in identifying sgRNAs that specifically and efficiently target these binding sites without affecting adjacent regions. Despite these challenges, our luciferase reporter assay, using tcf3b mRNA overexpression and morpholino knockdown, clearly demonstrated that tcf3b binds to and regulates nrg1 promoter region. Nevertheless, we acknowledge that future study using genome editing will be necessary to validate the direct binding of tcf3b to nrg1 promoter.

      Minor Points:

      (1) Must remove all of the "data not shown" statements and add the primary data to the Supplemental Figures.

      As suggested, we have removed all of the “data not shown” statements and added the original data to the revised manuscript (Figure 4E, middle panels, and Figure 4-figure supplement 1F)

      (2) Must present the order of the panels in the figure as they are presented in the text. One example is Figure 6 where 6E is discussed in the text before 6C and 6D.

      We thank the reviewer for bring up this important point. In the revised manuscript, we have carefully revised the manuscript to ensure that the order of figure panels matches the sequence in which they are discussed in the text. Specifically, we have reorganized the presentation of Figure 6 panels to align with the text flow, discussing panels 6C and 6D before panel 6E. The updated figure and corresponding text have been corrected accordingly in the revised manuscript.

      (3) Change the italicized gene names (e.g. tcf3b) to non-italicized names with the first letter capitalized (e.g. Tcf3b) when referencing the protein.

      As suggested, we have revised the manuscript to use non-italicized names with the first letter capitalized when referring to proteins.

      (4) All bar graphs should be replaced with dot bar graphs.

      We have replaced all bar graphs with dot bar graphs throughout the manuscript.

      (5) The new id2b mutant allele should be validated as a true null using quantitative RT-PCR to show that the message becomes destabilized through non-sense mediated decay or by immunostaining/western blot analysis if there is a zebrafish Id2b-specific antibody available.

      We thank the reviewer for this important suggestion. We have performed qRT-PCR analysis and detected a significant reduction in id2b mRNA levels in id2b<sup>-/-</sup> compared to id2b<sup>+/+</sup> controls. These new results are presented in Figure 4A of the revised manuscript.

      (6) Was tricaine used to anesthetize embryos for capturing heart rate and percent fractional area change? This analysis should be performed with no or very limited tricaine as it affects heart rate and systolic function. These parameters were captured at 120 hpf, but the authors should also look earlier at 72 hpf at a time when valves are not present by calcium transients are necessary to support heart function.

      We thank the reviewer for this important comment. When performing live imaging to assess cardiac contractile function, we used low-dose tricaine (0.16 mg/mL) to anesthetize the zebrafish embryos. We have included this important information in the Methods section (Line 503). As suggested, we have also included the heart function results at 72 hpf, which are now presented in Figure 5-figure supplement 2A-C of the revised manuscript.

      (7) The alpha-actinin staining in Figure 5-supplement 2D is very pixelated and unconvincing. This should be repeated and imaged at a higher resolution.

      As suggested, we have re-performed the α-actinin staining and acquired higher-resolution images. The updated results are now presented in Figure 5-figure supplement 2G of the revised manuscript.

      (8) The authors claim that reductions in id2b mutant heart contractility are due to perturbed calcium transients instead of sarcomere integrity. Why do the authors think that regulation of calcium dynamics was not observed in the DEG enriched GO-terms? Was significant downregulation of cacna1 identified in the bulk RNAseq?

      We thank the reviewer for raising this important point. In our bulk RNAseq dataset comparing id2b mutant and control hearts, GO term enrichment was primarily associated with pathways related to cardiac muscle contraction and heart contraction (Figure 5-figure supplement 1B). We speculate that the transcriptional changes related to calcium dynamics may be relatively subtle and thus were not captured as significantly enriched GO terms. In addition, our qRT-PCR analysis revealed a significant reduction in cacna1c expression in id2b mutant hearts compared to controls, suggesting that id2b deletion impairs calcium channel expression. However, this change was not detected by RNA-seq, likely due to limitations in sensitivity.

      (9) In line 277, the authors say, "To determine whether this interaction occurs in zebrafish, Flag-id2b and HA-tcf3b were co-expressed in HEK293 cells...". This should be re-phrased to, "To determine if zebrafish Id2b and Tcf3b interact in vitro, Flag-id2b and HA-tcf3b were co-expressed in HEK293 cells for co-immunoprecipitation analysis." The sentence in line 275 should be changed to, "....heterodimer with Tcf3b to limit its function as a potent transcriptional repressor."

      We thank the reviewer for these constructive comments and have revised the text accordingly (Lines 291-294).

      (10) Small text corrections or ideas:

      Line 63: emphasized

      We have corrected this in the revised manuscript.

      Line 71: studied signaling pathways

      We have corrected this in the revised manuscript.

      Line 106: the top 6 DEGS (I think that the authors mean top 6 GO-terms) and is Id2b in one of the enriched GO categories?

      id2b is one of the top DEGs. This point has been clarified in the revised manuscript (Lines 116-117).

      Line 125: a knockin id2b:eGFP reporter line

      We have corrected this in the revised manuscript (Line 136).

      Line 138: This paragraph could use a conclusion sentence.

      We have added a conclusion sentence in the revised manuscript (Lines 150-151).

      Line 190: id2b-/- zebrafish experienced early lethality

      We have revised the statement as suggested (Line 206).

      Line 193: The prominent enlargement of the atrium with a smaller ventricle has characterized as cardiomyopathy in zebrafish (Weeks et al. Cardiovasc Res, 2024, PMID: 38900908), which has also been associated with disruptions in calcium transients (Kamel et al J Cardiovasc Dev Dis, 2021, PMID: 33924051 and Kamel et al, Nat Commun 2021, PMID: 34887420). This information should be included in the text along with these references.

      We thank the reviewer for this helpful suggestion. We have incorporated these important references into the revised manuscript and included the relevant information to acknowledge the established link between atrial enlargement, cardiomyopathy, and disrupted calcium transients in zebrafish models (Reference #41, 42, and 45; Lines 210 and 260).

    1. eLife Assessment

      This important study combines agent-based modelling and in vivo experiments in medaka embryos to provide new insights into the role of the thymic niche in T cell development. The modelling yields some interesting and solid findings regarding the importance of thymic epithelial cells. This study would be of interest to oncologists, immunologists, and mathematical modelers.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study uses a cell-based computational model to simulate and study T cell development in the thymus. They initially applied this model to assess the effect of the thymic epithelial cells (TECs) network on thymocyte proliferation and demonstrated that increasing TEC size, density, or protrusions increased the number of thymocytes. They postulated and confirmed that this was due to changes in IL7 signalling and then expanded this work to encompass various environmental and cell-based parameters, including Notch signalling, cell cycle duration, and cell motility. Critical outcomes from the computational model were tested in vivo using medaka fish, such as the role of IL-7 signalling and minimal effect of Notch signalling.

      Strengths:

      The strength of the paper is the use of computational modelling to obtain unique insights into the niche parameters that control T cell development, such as the role of TEC architecture, while anchoring those findings with in vivo experiments. I can't comment on the model itself, as I am not an expert in modelling, however, the conclusions of the paper seem to be well-supported by the model.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors have worked up a ``virtual thymus' using EPISIM, which has already been published. Attractive features of the computational model are stochasticity, cell-to-cell variability, and spatial heterogeneiety. They seek to explore the role of TECs, that release IL-7 which is important in the process of thymocyte division.

      In the model, ordinary clones have IL7R levels chosen from a distribution, while `lesioned' clones have an IL7R value set to the maximum. The observation is that the lesioned clones are larger families, but the difference is not dramatic. This might be called a cell-intrinsic mechanism. One promising cell-extrinsic mechanism is mentioned: if a lesioned clone happens to be near a source of IL-7 and begins to proliferate, the progeny can crowd out cells of other clones and monopolise the IL-7 source. The effect will be more noticeable if sources are rare, so is seen when the TEC network is sparse.

      Strengths:

      Thymic disfunctions are of interest, not least because of T-ALL. New cells are added, one at a time, to simulate the conveyor belt of thymocytes on a background of stationary cells. They are thus able to follow cell lineages, which is interesting because one progenitor can give rise to many progeny.

      There are some experimental results in Figures 4,5 and 6. For example, il7 crispant embryos have fewer thymocytes and smaller thymii; but increasing IL-7 availability produces large thymii.

    4. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Tsingos et al. seek to advance beyond the current paradigm that proliferation of malignant cells in T-cell acute lymphoblastic leukemia occurs in a cell-autonomous fashion. Using a computational agent-based model and experimental validation, they show instead that cell proliferation also depends on interaction with thymic epithelial cells (TEC) in the thymic niche. One key finding is that a dense TEC network inhibits the proliferation of malignant cells and favors the proliferation of normal cells, whereas a sparse TEC network leads to rapid expansion of malignant thymocytes.

      Strengths:

      A key strength of this study is that it combines computational modeling using an agent-based model with experimental work. The original modeling and novel experimental work strengthen each other well. In the agent-based model, the authors also tested the effects of varying a few key parameters of cell proliferation.

    5. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study uses a cell-based computational model to simulate and study T cell development in the thymus. They initially applied this model to assess the effect of the thymic epithelial cells (TECs) network on thymocyte proliferation and demonstrated that increasing TEC size, density, or protrusions increased the number of thymocytes. They postulated and confirmed that this was due to changes in IL7 signalling and then expanded this work to encompass various environmental and cell-based parameters, including Notch signalling, cell cycle duration, and cell motility. Critical outcomes from the computational model were tested in vivo using medaka fish, such as the role of IL-7 signalling and minimal effect of Notch signalling.

      Strengths:

      The strength of the paper is the use of computational modelling to obtain unique insights into the niche parameters that control T cell development, such as the role of TEC architecture, while anchoring those findings with in vivo experiments. I can't comment on the model itself, as I am not an expert in modelling, however, the conclusions of the paper seem to be wellsupported by the model.

      Weaknesses:

      One potential issue is that many of the conclusions are drawn from the number of thymocytes, or related parameters such as the thymic size or proliferation of the thymocytes. The study only touches briefly on the influence of the thymic niche on other aspects of thymocyte behaviour, such as their differentiation and death.

      We thank the reviewer for this constructive feedback. Indeed, the strength of our approach lies in the close cooperation between modellers and experimentalists. One advantage of the model is its ability to manipulate challenging or even impossible variables, such as TEC dimensions, which cannot be varied experimentally with current tools. 

      The reviewer rightly pointed out that our validation focuses on comparing cell numbers or organ size as a proxy for cell numbers.

      In our previous study (Aghaallaei et al., Science Advances, 2021), we focused more on differentiation and used the computational model to predict how proportions of T-cell sublineages would vary according to different parameter values, including the IL-7 availability. One of the initial inspirations for the focus on proliferation in this manuscript was the observation in this previous work that overexpression of IL-7 in the niche resulted in overproliferation. We also focused on proliferation and organ size because these are more easily measured in experimental conditions with the tools that we have available in medaka, allowing better comparisons to the computational results.

      Regarding cell death, our experimental observations do not suggest that it plays a role before the final stages of T cell maturation. Hence, the model also does not include apoptosis before this stage either. 

      However, we do agree that taking a closer look at the regulation of differentiation and cell death would be an exciting avenue for future study!

      Please see our response to author recommendations below for more information on these points. Moreover, to make the model more accessible to non-experts, we have created new schematic figures, which we can be found in the Appendix of the revised manuscript.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors have worked up a ``virtual thymus' using EPISIM, which has already been published. Attractive features of the computational model are stochasticity, cell-to-cell variability, and spatial heterogeneity. They seek to explore the role of TECs, that release IL-7 which is important in the process of thymocyte division.

      In the model, ordinary clones have IL7R levels chosen from a distribution, while `lesioned' clones have an IL7R value set to the maximum. The observation is that the lesioned clones are larger families, but the difference is not dramatic. This might be called a cell-intrinsic mechanism. One promising cell-extrinsic mechanism is mentioned: if a lesioned clone happens to be near a source of IL-7 and begins to proliferate, the progeny can crowd out cells of other clones and monopolise the IL-7 source. The effect will be more noticeable if sources are rare, so is seen when the TEC network is sparse.

      Strengths:

      Thymic disfunctions are of interest, not least because of T-ALL. New cells are added, one at a time, to simulate the conveyor belt of thymocytes on a background of stationary cells. They are thus able to follow cell lineages, which is interesting because one progenitor can give rise to many progeny.

      There are some experimental results in Figures 4,5 and 6. For example, il7 crispant embryos have fewer thymocytes and smaller thymii; but increasing IL-7 availability produces large thymii.

      Weaknesses:

      On the negative side, like most agent-based models, there are dozens of parameters and assumptions whose values and validity are hard to ascertain.

      The stated aim is to mimic a 2.5-to-11 day-old medaka thymus, but the constructed model is a geometrical subset that holds about 100 cells at a time in a steady state. The manuscript contains very many figures and lengthy descriptions of simulations run with different parameters values and assumptions. The abstract and conclusion did not help me understand what exactly has been done and learned. No attempt to synthesise observations in any mathematical formula is made.

      The reviewer raises several important points to consider when working with mathematical or computational models.

      As in many other agent-based models, we agree that our model makes use of many parameters. Many of these parameters summarize multiple steps and are treated as phenomenological, i.e. they do not represent a microscopic event such as the rate of an individual chemical reaction, but more high-level processes such as "rate of differentiation". Realistically, this process should consist of cascades of pathway components that regulate transcription factors.

      In the supplementary material of our previous work (Aghaallaei et al., Science Advances, 2021) we provided an in-depth explanation of the mathematical formulation and rationale behind our choices in relation to the available biological data to select assumptions and restrict parameter value ranges. Four parameters that could not be characterized with pre-existing data, but which were crucial to the model's predictions, were studied in detail in that publication. Hence, the submitted manuscript starts with a well-calibrated model that has been tailored for the medaka thymus. The submitted manuscript explores the robustness of the system to lesions,  which we conceptualize as alterations in parameter values. We were surprised by how well the model recapitulated the time scales of overproliferation in the thymus of medaka embryos, which further supports the notion that our previous model calibration was successful.

      Another important point raised by the reviewer is that the "validity [of parameters and assumptions is] hard to ascertain". We agree, which is precisely the reason why we aim to test the model's predictions through experimentation. Importantly, a model does not need to be perfect to be useful. For example, in the submitted manuscript we observed a discrepancy between model predictions and experimental results that led us to hypothesize negative feedback regulation from the proliferative state to differentiation. 

      Thus, a major strength of modelling approaches is that they allow to identify erroneous or missing assumptions about the structure of the regulatory interaction network and its parametrization which can advance our scientific understanding of the underlying biology. Using models as an investigative tool is fundamental to the philosophy of systems biology (Kitano, Science, 2002), and is what we strive for.

      The reviewer rightfully points out that we only represent a geometric subset of the organ. In our preliminary work, we considered representing the full three-dimensional thymus; however, we later simplified our approach, as the organ is a symmetric ellipsoid at this developmental stage. This decision vastly reduced our computational costs, enabling us to explore parameter space more effectively.

      Nevertheless, we apologize if the submitted manuscript did not sufficiently emphasize the main insights of the paper, model limitations, and model construction. In the revised manuscript, we have improved the abstract and discussion sections to explicitly highlight the main results and limitations. We have also provided further details of the model's structure and underlying logic in the appendix.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Tsingos et al. seek to advance beyond the current paradigm that proliferation of malignant cells in T-cell acute lymphoblastic leukemia occurs in a cell-autonomous fashion. Using a computational agent-based model and experimental validation, they show instead that cell proliferation also depends on interaction with thymic epithelial cells (TEC) in the thymic niche. One key finding is that a dense TEC network inhibits the proliferation of malignant cells and favors the proliferation of normal cells, whereas a sparse TEC network leads to rapid expansion of malignant thymocytes.

      Strengths:

      A key strength of this study is that it combines computational modeling using an agent-based model with experimental work. The original modeling and novel experimental work strengthen each other well. In the agent-based model, the authors also tested the effects of varying a few key parameters of cell proliferation.

      Weaknesses:

      A minor weakness is that the authors did not conduct a global sensitivity analysis of all parameters in their agent-based model to show that the model is robust to variation, which would demonstrate that their results would still hold under a reasonable level of variation in the model and model parameters. This is a minor point, and such a supporting study would end in an appendix or supplement.

      The reviewer highlights the lack of a global sensitivity analysis as a minor weakness. 

      In our previous work (Aghaallaei et al., Science Advances, 2021), we studied parameters sensitivity for some parameters, while in the submitted manuscript, we extended this exploration to parameters that we expected to be the most meaningful for cell proliferation.

      In the revised version of the manuscript, we have included an additional supplementary figure alongside Figure 4 to show the effect of changing parameters in "control" simulations lacking a lesioned clone. These data are also provided in the source data to Figure 4. While this does not constitute an exhaustive exploration of all parameter space, it provides a useful overview of the effect of the studied parameters on thymocyte population size in the absence of lesioned clones.

      Response to reviewer recommendations

      In the revision, we have improved the manuscript to address the reviewers’ points. The following is an overview of the changes to the manuscript:

      • We wrote an extensive Appendix to better explain the model implementation.

      • The Abstract was rewritten to improve clarity on what was done and to highlight the main findings.

      • Subheadings to paragraphs were rewritten to better emphasize the main findings.

      • Font sizes in Figure 2J and Figure 4E were increased to improve readability.

      • The spacing of graphical elements in the legend of Figure 4E was improved.

      • An error in Figure 5B was corrected (the legend labels had been accidentally swapped).

      • A new supplementary figure to Figure 4 shows the sensitivity of clone size in control simulations for a subset of the tested parameter combinations.

      • The Conclusion section was rewritten to better highlight limitations of the study and Improve the summary of the main findings. 

      • Minor wording improvements were done throughout the text to improve readability.

      In the following we respond to the reviewers’ individual recommendations.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      I am not an expert in modelling, so I apologise if I missed these points in the manuscript. I am slightly confused about how differentiation and death are included in the model. At the beginning of the results you mention that you model a 5 um slice, is it known which stages of development occur in that section of the thymus? 

      We thank the reviewer for this question and appreciate the opportunity to clarify. Our virtual thymus is based on the medaka embryonic thymus, which we have extensively characterized using functional analyses and noninvasive in toto imaging (Bajoghli et al., Cell, 2009; Bajoghli et al., J Immunology, 2015; Aghaallaei et al., Science Advances, 2021; Aghaallaei, Eur J Immunology, 2022). These studies allowed us to map thymocyte developmental stages and migratory trajectories within the spatial context of a fully functional medaka thymus (see Figure 7 in Bajoghli et al., J Immunology, 2015).

      To simplify the biological system without compromising model fidelity, we chose to simulate a representative 5 µm slice from the ventral half of the thymus. Importantly, the medaka thymus is a symmetric organ (Bajoghli et al., J Immunology 2015), hence this slice captures all key events of T-cell development, including thymus homing, differentiation, proliferation, selection, and egress akin to our in vivo observations (see Figure 7 in Bajoghli et al., 2015 and Figure 7a in Aghaallaei et al., Science Advances, 2021).

      Furthermore, our model incorporates the spatial organization of the thymic cortex and medulla by including two types of thymic epithelial cells (TECs): cortical TECs positioned on the outer side, and medullary TECs on the inner side (see Figure Supplement 7 in Aghaallaei et al., Science Advances, 2021). Differentiation and cell death are modeled as discrete steps along the developmental trajectory, informed by our in vivo observations.

      We apologize to the reviewer if the workings of the model were not sufficiently clear in the original manuscript. To address this, and as also requested by reviewer 2, we provided an extensive Appendix in the revised version of the manuscript that also includes visual summaries of the model logic in the form of intuitive flowcharts.

      And is it known, or do you factor in, whether there are changes in the responsiveness of the thymocytes to signals, such as notch and IL7, depending on their state of differentiation?

      We have previously examined the roles of IL-7 (Aghaallaei et al., Science Advances, 2021) and Notch1 (Aghaallaei et al., Europ J Immunology, 2022) signaling in the medaka thymus. These studies demonstrated that T cell progenitors are responsive to both IL7 and Notch signaling, whereas more differentiated, non-proliferative thymocytes are unresponsive to IL-7. Our in vivo observations further suggest that mature thymocytes require Notch signaling during the thymic selection process. This appears to be a species-specific phenomenon (Aghaallaei et al., Europ J Immunology, 2022). 

      In the computational model, we include this state-specific responsiveness by incorporating a dependence on IL-7 and Notch signaling in the cellular decision to commit to the cell cycle (see Appendix Figure 6, and Appendix section X.) and in the decision of differentiating into αβ<sup>+</sup> or γδ<sup>+</sup> T cell subtypes (see Appendix Figure 5, and Appendix section IX.). Although the model still calculates pathway signaling activity for thymocytes in the differentiated stage belonging to the αβ<sup>+</sup> or γδ<sup>+</sup> subtype, this signaling activity has no downstream consequences for the cells’ behavior in the model.

      Note that in the computational model we do not incorporate feedback loops that regulate pathway activity (for example, it could be that thymocytes upregulate the IL7R receptor at some point in their differentiation trajectory – in the absence of speciesspecific knowledge of such regulatory feedbacks, we have chosen not to include any in our model).

      And you mention the stages of development are incorporated into the model but the main output that you discuss is thymocyte number or proliferation. It would be interesting to use the model to explore how parameters related to differentiation are changed by, for example, the level of IL7 signalling.

      We agree that examining how factors like IL-7 signaling influence thymocyte differentiation is a promising direction for future work. Based on our previous modelling work (Aghaallaei et al., Science Advances, 2021), we expect that increased IL7 availability or sensitivity should result in an increase of cells differentiating into the γδ<sup>+</sup> T cell subtype. As molecular tools for medaka continue to advance, we anticipate being able to refine and expand the model accordingly.

      Moreover, we see strong potential for adapting the current computational framework to model thymopoiesis in other species, such as mouse or human, where stage-specific markers are well characterized. We have now explicitly mentioned this opportunity for future development in the conclusion section of the revised manuscript (see page #26).

      It is also mentioned in the description of the model that the cells can die at the end of the development process. However, is death incorporated into the earlier stages of development? For instance, it is possible that when signals, such as a notch, are at low levels the thymocytes at certain stages of development will die.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. In a previous study, we mapped the spatial distribution of apoptotic cells within the medaka thymus and did not observe cell death in the region where ETPs enter the cortical thymus (Bajoghli et al., J Immunology, 2015) and where Notch1 signaling becomes activated (Aghaallaei et al., Europ J Immunology, 2021). Notch mutants exhibit a markedly reduced number of thymocytes, this reduction could be attributed either to impaired thymus homing or increased cell death within the thymus. However, our unpublished data shows that the total number of apoptotic cells in Notch1b-deficient thymus is comparable to their wild-type siblings. In fact, our in vivo observations revealed that the frequency of thymus colonization by progenitors is significantly reduced in the notch1b mutant (Aghaallaei et al., J E Immunol., 2021). Based on these in vivo observations, our computational model incorporates cell death only at the end of the thymocyte developmental trajectory. The current model does not consider cell death at earlier stages. 

      Overall, the manuscript was well-written and the figures were clear and well-presented. A minor point would be that the writing in some of the figures was too small and difficult to read, such as in Figure 4. I also sometimes struggled to find the definition of the acronyms in the figures, for example in Figure 3 it would be helpful if the definitions for D, SD, and SA were given in the figure legend as well as in the figure itself.

      We thank the reviewer for the kind words. We have reworked the figures to have larger more readable font sizes and improved figure legends as suggested.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Suppose the computational results did throw up an important new phenomenon. How might researchers seek to replicate it? If no mathematical relations can be given, can at least the code be made publicly available?

      We apologize to the reviewer if the workings of the model were not sufficiently clear in the submitted manuscript. However, we believe there may have been a misunderstanding, and we would like to clarify that both the mathematical formulations and the code used in this study were publicly available in the scientific record at the time of submission.

      Specifically, the full source code for the virtual thymus model is hosted in a permanent Zenodo repository (accessible here: https://zenodo.org/records/11656320), which includes:

      - Model files and links to source codes for the simulation environment;

      - Pre-compiled binary versions of the simulation environment (EPISIM) for both Windows and Linux platforms;

      - Detailed documentation, including step-by-step instructions on how to install and use the provided files.

      The repository link is cited in the manuscript (see page 38) and in the section “Data and materials availability”.  

      In addition, the mathematical framework that underpins the computational model has already been published and described in detail in our previous work (Aghaallaei, et al. Science Advances, 2021). In the supplementary material of this publication, we provide extensive documentation of the model, including:

      - A 13-page textual explanation of the design rationale;

      - 44 equations describing model implementation;

      - Parameter choices, partial sensitivity analysis, additional simulations, and supporting data presented in two figures and four tables.

      Nonetheless, to improve transparency, we have added an extensive Appendix in the revised version of the manuscript that also includes visual summaries of the model logic in the form of intuitive flowcharts. We hope this clarification and the new provided appendix assures the reviewer that both reproducibility and transparency have been central to our approach. 

      What about the growth of the animal and its thymus over weeks 2-11?

      We thank the reviewer for this insightful question. Indeed, our current computational model does not incorporate thymus growth over time. We decided not to model the dynamic increase in TEC numbers or organ size over time because we wanted to maintain simplicity and computational tractability. Therefore, we assumed a steadystate thymic environment. The model is therefore limited to representing thymopoiesis under homeostatic conditions, as it appears to stabilize by day 11. This is a recognized limitation of the current model. Looking ahead, we plan to develop a more advanced computational framework that incorporates thymic growth and dynamic changes in cellular composition over time. We have now included a brief note on this limitation in the conclusion of the revised manuscript (see page #26).

    1. eLife Assessment

      The authors investigate arrestin2-mediated CCR5 endocytosis in the context of clathrin and AP2 contributions. Using an extensive set of NMR experiments, and supported by microscopy and other biophysical assays, the authors provide convincing data on the roles of AP2 and clathrin in CCR5 endocytosis. This important work will appeal to an audience beyond those studying chemokine receptors, including those studying GPCR regulation and trafficking. The distinct role of AP2 and not clathrin will be of particular interest to those studying GPCR internalization mechanisms.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Petrovic et al. investigate CCR5 endocytosis via arrestin2, with a particular focus on clathrin and AP2 contributions. The study is thorough and methodologically diverse. The NMR titration data are particularly compelling, clearly demonstrating chemical shift changes at the canonical clathrin-binding site (LIELD), present in both the 2S and 2L arrestin splice variants.

      To assess the effect of arrestin activation on clathrin binding, the authors compare: truncated arrestin (1-393), full-length arrestin, and 1-393 incubated with CCR5 phosphopeptides. All three bind clathrin comparably, whereas controls show no binding. These findings are consistent with prior crystal structures showing peptide-like binding of the LIELD motif, with disordered flanking regions. The manuscript also evaluates a non-canonical clathrin binding site specific to the 2L splice variant. Though this region has been shown to enhance beta2-adrenergic receptor binding, it appears not to affect CCR5 internalization.

      Similar analyses applied to AP2 show a different result. AP2 binding is activation-dependent and influenced by the presence and level of phosphorylation of CCR5-derived phosphopeptides. These findings are reinforced by cellular internalization assays.

      In sum, the results highlight splice-variant-dependent effects and phosphorylation-sensitive arrestin-partner interactions. The data argue against a (rapidly disappearing) one-size-fits-all model for GPCR-arrestin signaling and instead support a nuanced, receptor-specific view, with one example summarized effectively in the mechanistic figure.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Based on extensive live cell assays, SEC, and NMR studies of reconstituted complexes, these authors explore the roles of clathrin and the AP2 protein in facilitating clathrin-mediated endocytosis via activated arrestin-2. NMR, SEC, proteolysis, and live cell tracking confirm a strong interaction between AP2 and activated arrestin using a phosphorylated C-terminus of CCR5. At the same time, a weak interaction between clathrin and arrestin-2 is observed, irrespective of activation.

      These results contrast with previous observations of class A GPCRs and the more direct participation by clathrin. The results are discussed in terms of the importance of short and long phosphorylated bar codes in class A and class B endocytosis.

      Strengths:

      The 15N,1H, and 13C, methyl TROSY NMR and assignments represent a monumental amount of work on arrestin-2, clathrin, and AP2. Weak NMR interactions between arrestin-2 and clathrin are observed irrespective of the activation of arrestin. A second interface, proposed by crystallography, was suggested to be a possible crystal artifact. NMR establishes realistic information on the clathrin and AP2 affinities to activated arrestin, with both kD and description of the interfaces.

      Weaknesses:

      This reviewer has identified only minor weaknesses with the study.

      (1) Arrestin-2 1-418 resonances all but disappear with CCR5pp6 addition. Are they recovered with Ap2Beta2 addition, and is this what is shown in Supplementary Figure 2D?

      (2) I don't understand how methyl TROSY spectra of arrestin2 with phosphopeptide could look so broadened unless there are sample stability problems.

      (3) At one point, the authors added an excess fully phosphorylated CCR5 phosphopeptide (CCR5pp6). Does the phosphopeptide rescue resolution of arrestin2 (NH or methyl) to the point where interaction dynamics with clathrin (CLTC NTD) are now more evident on the arrestin2 surface?

      (4) Once phosphopeptide activates arrestin-2 and AP2 binds, can phosphopeptide be exchanged off? In this case, would it be possible for the activated arrestin-2 AP2 complex to re-engage a new (phosphorylated) receptor?

      (5) Did the authors ever try SEC measurements of arrestin-2 + AP2beta2+CCR5pp6 with and without PIP2, and with and without clathrin (CLTC NTD? The question becomes what the active complex is and how PIP2 modulates this cascade of complexation events in class B receptors.

    4. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Overall, this is a well-done study, and the conclusions are largely supported by the data, which will be of interest to the field.

      Strengths:

      (1) The strengths of this study include experiments with solution NMR that can resolve high-resolution interactions of the highly flexible C-terminal tail of arr2 with clathrin and AP2. Although mainly confirmatory in defining the arr2 CBL 376LIELD380 as the clathrin binding site, the use of the NMR is of high interest (Figure 1). The 15N-labeled CLTC-NTD experiment with arr2 titrations reveals a span from 39-108 that mediates an arr2 interaction, which corroborates previous crystal data, but does not reveal a second area in CLTC-NTD that in previous crystal structures was observed to interact with arr2.

      (2) SEC and NMR data suggest that full-length arr2 (1-418) binding with the 2-adaptin subunit of AP2 is enhanced in the presence of CCR5 phospho-peptides (Figure 3). The pp6 peptide shows the highest degree of arr2 activation and 2-adaptin binding, compared to less phosphorylated peptides or not phosphorylated at all. It is interesting that the arr2 interaction with CLTC NTD and pp6 cannot be detected using the SEC approach, further suggesting that clathrin binding is not dependent on arrestin activation. Overall, the data suggest that receptor activation promotes arrestin binding to AP2, not clathrin, suggesting the AP2 interaction is necessary for CCR5 endocytosis.

      (3) To validate the solid biophysical data, the authors pursue validation experiments in a HeLa cell model by confocal microscopy. This requires transient transfection of tagged receptor (CCR5-Flag) and arr2 (arr2-YFP). CCR5 displays a "class B"-like behavior in that arr2 is rapidly recruited to the receptor at the plasma membrane upon agonist activation, which forms a stable complex that internalizes into endosomes (Figure 4). The data suggest that complex internalization is dependent on AP2 binding, not clathrin (Figure 5).

      Weaknesses:

      The interaction of truncated arr2 (1-393) was not impacted by CCR5 phospho-peptide pp6, suggesting the interaction with clathrin is not dependent on arrestin activation (Figure 2). This raises some questions.

      Overall, the data are solid, but for added rigor, can these experiments be repeated without tagged receptor and/or arr2? My concern stems from the fact that the stability of the interaction between arr2 and receptor may be related to the position of the tags.

    1. eLife Assessment

      This study introduces ambisim, a rigorously validated and well-documented simulation framework that enables the generation of synthetic, genotype-aware single-cell RNA and ATAC sequencing datasets under realistic conditions. The authors provide solid evidence of its utility by benchmarking multiple demultiplexing methods and proposing a new variant consistency metric. While the tool is valuable for guiding method selection, the interpretation of the new metric requires further clarification.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors developed a tool for simulating multiplexed single-cell RNA-seq and ATAC-seq data with various adjustable settings like ambient RNA/DNA rate and sequencing depth. They used the simulated data with different settings to evaluate the performance of many demultiplexing methods. They also proposed a new metric at single-cell level that correlates with the RNA/DNA contamination level.

      Strengths:

      The simulation tool has a straightforward design and provides adjustability in multiple parameters that have practical relevance, such as sequencing depth and ambient contamination rate. With the growing use of multiplexing in single-cell RNAseq and ATACseq experiments, the tools and results in this paper can guide the experimental design and tool selection for many researchers. The simulation tool also provides a platform for benchmarking newly developed demultiplexing tools.

      Weaknesses:

      The usefulness of the proposed new metric of "variant consistency" and how it can guide users in selecting demultiplexing methods seems a little unclear. It correlates with the level of ambient RNA/DNA contamination, which makes it look like a metric on data quality. However, it does depend on the exact demultiplexing method, yet it's not clear how it directly connects to the "accuracy" of each demultiplexing method, which is the most important property that users of these methods care about. Since the simulated data has ground truth of donor identities available, I would suggest using the simulated data to show whether "variant consistency" directly indicates the accuracy of each method, especially the accuracy within those "C2" reads.

      I also think the tool and analyses presented in this paper need some further clarification and documentation on the details, such as how the cell-type gene and peak probabilities are determined in the simulation, and how doublets from different cell types are handled in the simulation and analysis. A few analyses and figures also need a more detailed description of the exact methods used.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Li et al. describe ambisim, a tool with the goal of creating realistic synthetic single-nucleus RNA/ATAC sequencing datasets. It has become standard to pool multiple genetically distinct donors when using single-cell sequencing followed by genotype-based demultiplexing (i.e., using donor single-nucleotide variants to identify specific donor origin). A plethora of tools exist to accomplish this demultiplexing, but advanced tools to create synthetic datasets, and therefore provide definitive benchmarking, are lacking. Ambisim is a well-thought-out simulator that improves upon previous tools available by allowing for modeling of variable ambient contamination proportions and doing so in a genotype-aware fashion. This provides more realistic synthetic datasets that provide challenging scenarios for future demultiplexing tools. The authors use ambisim to benchmark a large number of available and commonly used genotype-free and -dependent demultiplexing tools. They identify the strengths and weaknesses of these tools. They also go on to define a new metric, variant consistency, to further assess demultiplexing performance across tools. Overall, this manuscript provides a useful framework to more thoroughly evaluate future demultiplexing tools, as well as provides rationale for tool selection depending on a user's experimental conditions.

      The authors provide measured conclusions that are supported by their findings. There are some aspects that are unclear.

      (1) Throughout the manuscript, the figure legends are difficult to understand, and this makes it difficult to interpret the graphs.

      (2) Since this is both a new tool and a benchmark, it would be worthwhile in the Discussion to comment on which demultiplexing tools one may want to choose for their dataset, especially given the warning against ensemble methods. From this extensive benchmarking, one may want to choose a tool based on the number of donors one has pooled, the modalities present, and perhaps even the ambient RNA (if it has been estimated previously).

      (3) What are the minimal computational requirements for running ambisim? What is the time cost?

    4. Author response:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      The usefulness of the proposed new metric of "variant consistency" and how it can guide users in selecting demultiplexing methods seems a little unclear. It correlates with the level of ambient RNA/DNA contamination, which makes it look like a metric on data quality. However, it does depend on the exact demultiplexing method, yet it's not clear how it directly connects to the "accuracy" of each demultiplexing method, which is the most important property that users of these methods care about. Since the simulated data has ground truth of donor identities available, I would suggest using the simulated data to show whether "variant consistency" directly indicates the accuracy of each method, especially the accuracy within those "C2" reads.

      I also think the tool and analyses presented in this paper need some further clarification and documentation on the details, such as how the cell-type gene and peak probabilities are determined in the simulation, and how doublets from different cell types are handled in the simulation and analysis. A few analyses and figures also need a more detailed description of the exact methods used. 

      We thank the reviewer for their suggestions. We plan on revising the manuscript to reflect their suggestions, which will include clarification of the variant consistency metric and its relationship with demultiplexing accuracy based on the simulations and additional detail regarding ambisim’s generation of multiplexed snRNA/snATAC.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      (1) Throughout the manuscript, the figure legends are difficult to understand, and this makes it difficult to interpret the graphs.

      (2) Since this is both a new tool and a benchmark, it would be worthwhile in the Discussion to comment on which demultiplexing tools one may want to choose for their dataset, especially given the warning against ensemble methods. From this extensive benchmarking, one may want to choose a tool based on the number of donors one has pooled, the modalities present, and perhaps even the ambient RNA (if it has been estimated previously).

      (3) What are the minimal computational requirements for running ambisim? What is the time cost? 

      We thank the reviewer for their suggestions. We plan on updating the manuscript to better clarify figure legends. We will also outline a set of concrete recommendations in our discussion section based on different multiplexed experimental designs. Finally, we will also include extra computational benchmarks for ambisim.

    1. eLife Assessment

      This study dissects the function of 3 outputs of a specific population of modulatory neurons, dorsal raphe dopamine neurons, in social and affective behavior. It provides valuable information that both confirms prior results and provides new insights. The strength of the evidence is convincing, based on cutting-edge approaches and analysis. This study will be of interest to behavioral and systems neuroscientists, especially those interested in social and emotional behavior.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors had previously found that a brief social isolation could increase the activity of these neurons, and that manipulation of these neurons could alter social behavior in a social rank dependent fashion. This manuscript explored which of the outputs were responsible for this, identifying the central nucleus of the amygdala as the key output region. The authors identified some discrete behavior changes associated with these outputs, and found that during photostimulation of these outputs, neuronal activity appeared altered in 'social response' neurons. In the revised manuscript, the authors address the comments in a rigorous fashion.

      Strengths:

      Rigorous analysis of the anatomy. Careful examination of the hetergenous effects on cell activity due to stimulation, linking the physiology with the behavior via photostimulation during recording in vivo.

      Weaknesses:

      The authors have responded to all of my comments.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors perform a series of studies to follow up on their previous work, which established a role for dorsal raphe dopamine neurons (DRN) in the regulation of social-isolation-induced rebound in mice. In the present study, Lee et. al, use a combination of modern circuit tools to investigate putatively distinct roles of DRN dopamine transporting containing (DAT) projections to the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST), central amygdala (CeA), and posterior basolateral amygdala (BLP). Notably, they reveal that optogenetic stimulation of distinct pathways confers specific behavioral states, with DRNDAT-BLP driving aversion, DRNDAT-BNST regulating non-social exploratory behavior, and DRNDAT-CeA promoting socialability. A combination of electrophysiological studies and in situ hybridization studies reveal heterogenous dopamine and neuropeptide expression and different firing properties, providing further evidence of pathway-specific neural properties. Lastly, the authors combine optogenetics and calcium imaging to resolve social encoding properties in the DRNDAT-CeA pathway, which correlates observed social behavior to socially engaged neural ensembles.

      Collectively, these studies provide an interesting way of dissecting out separable features of a complex multifaceted social-emotional state that accompanies social isolation and the perception of 'loneliness.' The main conclusions of the paper provide an important and interesting set of findings that increase our understanding of these distinct DRN projections and their role in a range of social (e.g., prosocial, dominance), non-social, and emotional behaviors. However, as noted below, the examination of these circuits within a homeostatic framework is limited given that a number of the datasets did not include an isolated condition. The DRNDAT-CeA pathway was investigated with respect to social homeostatic states in the present study for some of the datasets.

      Strengths:

      (1) The authors perform a comprehensive and elegant dissection of the anatomical, behavioral, molecular, and physiological properties of distinct DRN projections relevant to social, non-social, and emotional behavior, to address multifaceted and complex features of social state.

      (2) This work builds on prior findings of isolation-induced changes in DRN neurons and provides a working framework for broader circuit elements that can be addressed across social homeostatic state.

      (3) This work characterizes a broader circuit implicated in social isolation and provides a number of downstream targets to explore, setting a nice foundation for future investigation.

      (4) The studies account for social rank and anxiety-like behavior in several of the datasets, which are important consideration to the interpretation of social motivation states, especially in male mice with respect to dominance behavior.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The conceptual framework of the study is based on the premise of social isolation and perceived 'loneliness' under the framework of social homeostasis, analogous to hunger. In this framework, social isolation should provoke an aversive state and compensatory social contact behavior. In the authors' prior work, they demonstrate synaptic changes in DRN neurons and social rebound following acute social isolation. Thus, the prediction would be that downstream projections also would show state dependent changes as a function of social isolation state (e.g., grouped/socially engaged vs. isolated). In the current paper, a social isolation condition was included for some but not all experiments, which should be considered in the interpretation of the data, specifically within the context of dynamic isolation states.

      (2) Figure 1 confirms co-laterals in the BNST and CeA via anatomical tracing studies. The goal of the optogenetic studies is to dissociate functional/behavioral roles of distinct projections. One limitation of optogenetic projection targeting is the possibility of back-propagating action potentials (stimulation of terminals in one region may back-propagate to activate cell bodies, and then afferent projections to other regions), and/or stimulation of fibers of passage. However, this is addressed in the discussion and the present data are convincing, which minimizes the concern.

      (3) Sex as a biological variable should be considered in the present data, as included in the discussion.

    4. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors investigated the role of dopaminergic neurons (dopamine transporter expressing, DAT) in the dorsal raphe nucleus (DRN) in regulating social and affective behavior through projections to the central nucleus of the amygdala (CeA), bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST), and the posterior subdivision of the basolateral amygdala. The largest effect observed was in the DRN-DAT projections to the CeA. Augmenting previously published results from this group (Matthews et al., 2016), the comprehensive behavioral analysis relative to social dominance, gene expression analysis, electrophysiological profiling, and in vivo imaging provides novel insights into how DRN-DAT projections to the CeA influence the engagement of social behavior in the contexts of group housed and socially isolated mice.

      Strengths:

      Correlational analysis with social dominance is a nice addition to the study. The overall computational analyses performed are well-designed and rigorous.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) Analysis of dopamine receptor expression did not include Drd3, Drd4, or Drd5 which may provide more insights into how dopamine modulates downstream targets. This is particularly relevant to the BNST projection in which the densest innervation did not robustly co-localize with the expression of either Drd1 or Drd2. It is also possible that dopamine release from DRN-DAT neurons in any or all of these structures in modulating neurotransmitter release from inputs to these regions that contain D2 receptors on their terminals.

      (2) Although not the focus of this study, without pharmacological blockade of dopamine receptors, it is not possible to assess what the contribution of dopamine is to the behavioral outcomes. Given the co-release of glutamate and GABA from these neurons it is possible that dopamine plays only a marginal role in the functional connectivity of DRN-DAT neurons.

      (3) Photostimulation parameters used during the behavioral studies (8 pulses of light delivered at 30 Hz for several minutes) could lead to confounding results limiting data interpretation. As shown in Figure 6J, 8 pulses of light delivered at 30 Hz results in a significant attenuation of the EPSC amplitude in the BLP and CeA projection. Thus, prolonged stimulation could lead to significant synaptic rundown resulting in an overall suppression of connectivity in the later stages of the behavioral analyses.

      Comments on revisions:

      No further issues have been identified.

    5. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors had previously found that brief social isolation could increase the activity of these neurons, and that manipulation of these neurons could alter social behavior in a social rank-dependent fashion. This manuscript explored which of the outputs were responsible for this, identifying the central nucleus of the amygdala as the key output region. The authors identified some discrete behavior changes associated with these outputs, and found that during photostimulation of these outputs, neuronal activity appeared altered in 'social response' neurons.

      Strengths:

      Rigorous analysis of the anatomy. Careful examination of the heterogenous effects on cell activity due to stimulation, linking the physiology with the behavior via photostimulation during recording in vivo.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) There are some clear imbalances in the sample size across the different regions parsed. The CeA has a larger sample size, likely in part to the previous work suggesting differential effects depending on social rank/dominance. Given the potential variance, it may be hard to draw conclusions about the impact of stimulation across different social ranks for other groups.

      While it may be difficult to draw conclusions about the impact of stimulation across different social ranks, we believe that the dominance-induced variance in our dataset reveals key insights into how social history may affect the function of these circuits. However, we do recognize that there are imbalances in sample size across the different circuits that we probed. To test whether we could detect a significant effect in our DRN<sup>DAT</sup>-CeA:ChR2 group with a sample size matched to the DRN<sup>DAT</sup>-BLP:ChR2 group (the lowest sample size of the three circuits probed), we subsampled and ran tests for statistical significance using the following MATLAB code:

      Author response image 1.

      We found that out of 1000 subsamples, we detected a statistically significant effect 40.5% of the time (Author response image 2A). This suggests that the optogenetic effect exists, though it is moderate and is variable across mice (as explained by the significant correlation between social rank and optogenetic effect).

      To test whether these inconsistent effects may be an effect of variance induced by social rank, we wrote the following MATLAB code to maintain the distribution of social rank in our subsamples:

      Author response image 2.

      P-values from subsampling analysis show a moderately reproducible social preference effect in DRN<sup>DAT</sup>-CeA:ChR2 mice, but not in DRN<sup>DAT</sup>-BNST:ChR2 mice. (A-D) Histograms showing distribution of paired t-test p-values comparing OFF and ON social preference scores (as shown in Figure 4A-I) in subsampled groups (to match the sample size of the DRN<sup>DAT</sup>-BLP:ChR2 group). (A) 14 DRN<sup>DAT</sup>-CeA:ChR2 mice were randomly subsampled, a paired t-test was performed, and the resulting p-values were binned and plotted. (B) Same as (A), but ensuring that the proportion of subordinate, intermediate, and dominant mice in the subsampled groups were the same as the original distribution. (C) Same as (A), but with DRN<sup>DAT</sup>-BNST:ChR2 mice. (D) Same as (B), but with DRN<sup>DAT</sup>-BNST:ChR2 mice.

      Author response image 3.

      We found that out of 1000 subsamples, we detected a statistically significant effect 45.5% of the time when we maintained the original distribution of social rank in DRN<sup>DAT</sup>-CeA:ChR2 mice (Author response image 2B). This suggests that reducing the sample size to N=14 reduces the statistical power and indeed can make an effect harder to reliably detect. The reviewer is correct in saying that sample imbalance may skew conclusions. However, given the rank-dependent optogenetic effect on social preference seen in DRN<sup>DAT</sup>-CeA:ChR2 mice (N=29 mice, p=0.002, Figure 4H) that is notably absent in DRN<sup>DAT</sup>-BLP:ChR2 mice (N=14 mice, p=0.806, Figure 4I), we hypothesize that we would not see a significant effect of photoactivating the DRN<sup>DAT</sup>-BLP circuit on social preference, even with a larger sample size. While we acknowledge there may be evidence that there could be an effect in the DRN<sup>DAT</sup>-BLP projection, this analysis reveals that this effect is not as robust as the effect we see in the DRN<sup>DAT</sup>-CeA projection, which is the focus of this study. An in-depth exploration of the DRN<sup>DAT</sup> projection to the BLP is certainly warranted in future studies.

      Interestingly, the same analysis approach applied to DRN<sup>DAT</sup>-BNST:ChR2 mice suggest a reliably negative result, with subsampling only resulting in a significant result 1.1% of the time (Author response image 2C) and 1.7% of the time if maintaining the original rank distribution (Author response image 2D).

      (2) It is somewhat unclear why only the 'social object ratio' was used to assess the effects versus more direct measurements of social behavior.

      We decided to use ‘social:object ratio’ as we felt that measurement more directly supported our claim of increased social preference through optogenetic manipulation; however, in our updated manuscript, we included direct measurements of social behavior in the revised manuscript (Figure 4—figure supplement 1) and have updated the legend to reflect this addition (lines 1679-1684; 1698-1708).

      (3) Somewhat related, while it is statistically significant, it is unclear if the change seen in face investigation of biologically significant, on average, it looks like a few-seconds difference and that was not modulated by social rank.

      While the effect size is relatively small (4.19 seconds, 2.32% of the session), we believe we should report any statistically significant findings we discover. However, due to the small effect size, we have de-emphasized our claims regarding this finding in the text (line 172).

      (4) There are several papers studying these neurons that have explored behaviors examined here, as well as the physiological connectivity that are not cited that would provide important context for this work. In particular, multiple groups have found a dopamine-mediated IPSP in the BNST, in contrast to this work. There are technical differences that may drive these differences, but not addressing them is a major weakness.

      In the revised text, we have cited the groups who have found different effects of dopamine-mediated effects in the ovBNST (specifically from Krawczyk et al., 2011, Maracle et al., 2018, and Yu et al., 2021) and reconciled these results with those from our study (lines 422-432).

      (5) The inclusion of some markers for receptors for some of these outputs is interesting, and the authors suggest that this may be important, but this is somewhat disconnected from the rest of the work performed.

      We agree that we cannot make any causal signaling mechanism claims with the current downstream receptor RNA expression data (and we are careful in avoiding making those claims in the text), but we include these data to offer a potential mechanism and hope that these descriptive data will be useful to the field for follow up studies.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):<br /> Summary:

      The authors perform a series of studies to follow up on their previous work, which established a role for dorsal raphe dopamine neurons (DRN) in the regulation of social-isolation-induced rebound in mice. In the present study, Lee et. al, use a combination of modern circuit tools to investigate putatively distinct roles of DRN dopamine transporting containing (DAT) projections to the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST), central amygdala (CeA), and posterior basolateral amygdala (BLP). Notably, they reveal that optogenetic stimulation of distinct pathways confers specific behavioral states, with DRNDAT-BLP driving aversion, DRNDAT-BNST regulating non-social exploratory behavior, and DRNDAT-CeA promoting socialability. A combination of electrophysiological studies and in situ hybridization studies reveal heterogenous dopamine and neuropeptide expression and different firing properties, providing further evidence of pathway-specific neural properties. Lastly, the authors combine optogenetics and calcium imaging to resolve social encoding properties in the DRNDAT-CeA pathway, which correlates observed social behavior to socially engaged neural ensembles.

      Collectively, these studies provide an interesting way of dissecting out separable features of a complex multifaceted social-emotional state that accompanies social isolation and the perception of 'loneliness.' The main conclusions of the paper provide an important and interesting set of findings that increase our understanding of these distinct DRN projections and their role in a range of social (e.g., prosocial, dominance), non-social, and emotional behaviors. However, as noted below, the examination of these circuits within a homeostatic framework is limited given that a number of the datasets did not include an isolated condition. The DRNDAT-CeA pathway was investigated with respect to social homeostatic states in the present study for some of the datasets.

      Strengths: 

      (1) The authors perform a comprehensive and elegant dissection of the anatomical, behavioral, molecular, and physiological properties of distinct DRN projections relevant to social, non-social, and emotional behavior, to address multifaceted and complex features of social state.<br /> (2) This work builds on prior findings of isolation-induced changes in DRN neurons and provides a working framework for broader circuit elements that can be addressed across the social homeostatic state.<br /> (3) This work characterizes a broader circuit implicated in social isolation and provides a number of downstream targets to explore, setting a nice foundation for future investigation.<br /> (4) The studies account for social rank and anxiety-like behavior in several of the datasets, which are an important consideration to the interpretation of social motivation states, especially in male mice with respect to dominance behavior.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The conceptual framework of the study is based on the premise of social isolation and perceived 'loneliness' under the framework of social homeostasis, analogous to hunger. In this framework, social isolation should provoke an aversive state and compensatory social contact behavior. In the authors' prior work, they demonstrate synaptic changes in DRN neurons and social rebound following acute social isolation. Thus, the prediction would be that downstream projections also would show state-dependent changes as a function of social housing conditions (e.g., grouped vs. isolated). In the current paper, a social isolation condition was not included for the majority of the studies conducted (e.g., Figures 1-6 do not include an isolated condition, Figures 7-8 do include an isolated condition). Thus, while Figure 1-6 adds a very interesting and compelling set of data that is of high value to the social behavior field with respect to social and emotional processing and general circuit characterization, these studies do not directly investigate the impacts of dynamic social homeostatic state. The main claim of the paper, including the title (e.g., separable DRN projections mediate facets of loneliness-like state), abstract, intro, and discussion presents the claim of this work under the framework of dynamic social homeostatic states, which should be interpreted with caution, as the majority of the work in the paper did not include a social isolation comparison.

      In previous studies, loneliness-like phenotypes have been characterized across species as having the key dimensions of an aversive state that increases prosociality[1–5].  These two features are amplified by photostimulation of DRN DA neurons, and as we show in this manuscript, are separable across different projections to each target, and our ability to distinctly mimic different aspects of the constellation of features we characterize as “loneliness.”

      However we agree with the reviewer that we do not intend to imply that the mouse currently feels lonely.  Indeed, isolating the animals would occlude our ability to see photostimulation-induced mimicry of specific features of the loneliness-like phenotype, and this is precisely why we did not isolate animals for our ChR2 gain-of-function experiments.  To address the reviewers’ concern, we will change the title of our manuscript from making a claim of “mediating” (which we agree would rely more heavily on mediating actual (ethologically-induced) loneliness rather than “mimicry” (photostimulation-induced) behaviors associated with a loneliness-like phenotype. We have changed language regarding this claim throughout our manuscript (Lines 1, 83, 285, 369).

      For the ChR2 experiments in particular, we intended the optogenetic manipulation to be a gain-of-function one to test the hypothesis that activation of these circuits is sufficient to recapitulate different facets of a loneliness-like state (i.e. prosociality, aversion, and increased exploratory behavior). As such, that is why we only included group-housed conditions for these experiments—to mimic the phenotype of social isolation without social isolation. To test the necessity of these circuits in mediating different facets of a loneliness-like state, we agree that silencing the studied projections in an isolated state is critical, which is what we show in Figure 8. We agree that the addition of an isolated condition to understand the circuit-specific impact of dynamic social homeostatic state is important (particularly through in vivo recordings of these specific circuits during relevant behaviors), and would be a great follow-up to this study.

      (2) In Figure 1, the authors confirm co-laterals in the BNST and CeA via anatomical tracing studies. The goal of the optogenetic studies is to dissociate the functional/behavioral roles of distinct projections. However, one limitation of optogenetic projection targeting is the possibility of back-propagating action potentials (stimulation of terminals in one region may back-propagate to activate cell bodies, and then afferent projections to other regions), and/or stimulation of fibers of passage. Therefore, one limitation in the dataset for the optogenetic stimulation studies is the possibility of non-specific unintended activation of projections other than those intended (e.g., DRNDAT-CeA). This can be dealt with by administering lidocaine to prevent back-propagating action potentials.

      While back-propagating action potentials are potentially confounding for the manipulation techniques presented in this paper, we do show circuit-specific optogenetic behavioral effects despite significant collateralization (specifically between DRN<sup>DAT</sup> neurons projecting to the CeA and BNST; Figure 1H), suggesting circuit-specificity. Namely, we see that stimulation of DRN<sup>DAT</sup> terminals in CeA promotes social preference (Figure 4E,K) whereas stimulation of DRN<sup>DAT</sup> terminals in BNST promotes rearing (exploratory) behavior (Figure 3G). There is a non-negligible chance that we are stimulating DRN<sup>DAT</sup> fibers of passage, which we have addressed in a caveat disclaimer included in the revised discussion (lines 345-347).

      (3) It is unclear from the test, but in the subjects' section of the methods, it appears that only male animals were included in the study, with no mention of female subjects. It should be clear to the reader that this was conducted in males only if that is the case, with consideration or discussion, about female subjects and sex as a biological variable.

      In the revised manuscript, we have included discussion about sex as a biological variable (lines 342-345).

      (4) Averaged data are generally reported throughout the study in the form of bar graphs, across most figures. Individual data points would increase the transparency of the data.

      In an effort to increase the transparency of the data, we have prepared source data for each data panel in the final version of the manuscript and will upload it to eLife.  

      REFERENCES

      (1) Cacioppo, J.T., Hughes, M.E., Waite, L.J., Hawkley, L.C., and Thisted, R.A. (2006). Loneliness as a specific risk factor for depressive symptoms: cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses. Psychol Aging 21, 140–151. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.140.

      (2) Cacioppo, S., Capitanio, J.P., and Cacioppo, J.T. (2014). Toward a Neurology of Loneliness. Psychol Bull 140, 1464–1504. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037618.

      (3) Baumeister, R.F., and Leary, M.R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin 117, 497–529. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497.

      (4) Niesink, R.J., and Van Ree, J.M. (1982). Short-term isolation increases social interactions of male rats: A parametric analysis. Physiology & Behavior 29, 819–825. https://doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(82)90331-6.

      (5) Panksepp, J., and Beatty, W.W. (1980). Social deprivation and play in rats. Behavioral & Neural Biology 30, 197–206. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-1047(80)91077-8.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors investigated the role of dopaminergic neurons (dopamine transporter expressing, DAT) in the dorsal raphe nucleus (DRN) in regulating social and affective behavior through projections to the central nucleus of the amygdala (CeA), bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST), and the posterior subdivision of the basolateral amygdala. The largest effect observed was in the DRN-DAT projections to the CeA. Augmenting previously published results from this group (Matthews et al., 2016), the comprehensive behavioral analysis relative to social dominance, gene expression analysis, electrophysiological profiling, and in vivo imaging provides novel insights into how DRN-DAT projections to the CeA influence the engagement of social behavior in the contexts of group-housed and socially isolated mice.

      Strengths:

      Correlational analysis with social dominance is a nice addition to the study. The overall computational analyses performed are well-designed and rigorous.

      Weaknesses: 

      (1) Analysis of dopamine receptor expression did not include Drd3, Drd4, or Drd5 which may provide more insights into how dopamine modulates downstream targets. This is particularly relevant to the BNST projection in which the densest innervation did not robustly co-localize with the expression of either Drd1 or Drd2. It is also possible that dopamine release from DRN-DAT neurons in any or all of these structures modulates neurotransmitter release from inputs to these regions that contain D2 receptors on their terminals.

      Although we find that there is more Vipr2 and Npbwr1 expression compared to Drd1 and Drd2 expression in ovBNST, we still do find that a substantial proportion of cells in ovBNST express dopamine receptors (particularly D2 dopamine receptors, as shown in Figure 5C). In our revised manuscript, we have discussed potential functional mechanism through D3, D4, and D5 dopamine receptors, as well as pre-synaptic dopamine receptor expression (lines 459-461).

      (2) Although not the focus of this study, without pharmacological blockade of dopamine receptors, it is not possible to assess what the contribution of dopamine is to the behavioral outcomes. Given the co-release of glutamate and GABA from these neurons, it is possible that dopamine plays only a marginal role in the functional connectivity of DRN-DAT neurons.

      While we agree with the reviewer’s comments, we are careful to avoid making claims about dopamine-mediated physiological and behavioral effects of DRN<sup>DAT</sup> neurons (despite that these neurons are genetically identified through the expression of dopamine transporter [DAT]), mentioned in lines 222-228 in the text.

      (3) Photostimulation parameters used during the behavioral studies (8 pulses of light delivered at 30 Hz for several minutes) could lead to confounding results limiting data interpretation. As shown in Figure 6J, 8 pulses of light delivered at 30 Hz result in a significant attenuation of the EPSC amplitude in the BLP and CeA projection. Thus, prolonged stimulation could lead to significant synaptic rundown resulting in an overall suppression of connectivity in the later stages of the behavioral analyses.

      Despite attenuation of EPSC amplitude in BLP and CeA projections and potential synaptic rundown, we still observe significant behavioral effects through optogenetic manipulation of these circuits (increasing the likelihood of capturing a ‘true positive’ rather than a ‘false negative’ effect). In general, we attempt to reduce the duty cycle by sparingly delivering trains of optogenetic stimulation (eight 5-ms pulses every 5 seconds). Additionally, in the real time place preference task where stimulation of the DRN<sup>DAT</sup>-BLP projection significantly reduces the time spent in the “ON” chamber, stimulation is only delivered when the mouse is in the “ON” compartment of the apparatus. However, we do feel that the reviewer’s concern that EPSC attenuation and potential synaptic rundown may potentially explain the robust place avoidance effects in DRN<sup>DAT</sup>-BLP:ChR2 mice in the first half of the session (Figure 2G). Importantly, we show in our previous published work (Matthews et al., 2016, Cell; Figure 3) through fast-scan cyclic voltammetry (FSCV) that dopamine transients were consistently recorded in response to eight pulses of 30 Hz DRN<sup>TH</sup> stimulation delivered every 5 seconds in the BNST, though less consistently in the CeA.

    1. eLife Assessment

      This paper reports on an important study that aims to move beyond current experimental approaches in speech production by (1) investigating speech in the context of a fully interactive task and (2) employing advanced methodology to record intracranial brain activity. Together these allow for examination of the unfolding temporal dynamics of brain-behaviour relationships during interactive speech. This approach and the analyses presented in support of the authors' claims pose convincing evidence.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This paper reports an intracranial SEEG study of speech coordination, where participants synchronize their speech output with a virtual partner that is designed to vary its synchronization behavior. This allows the authors to identify electrodes throughout the left hemisphere of the brain that have activity (both power and phase) that correlates with the degree of synchronization behavior. They find that high-frequency activity in secondary auditory cortex (superior temporal gyrus) is correlated to synchronization, in contrast to primary auditory regions. Furthermore, activity in inferior frontal gyrus shows a significant phase-amplitude coupling relationship that is interpreted as compensation for deviation from synchronized behavior with the virtual partner.

      Strengths:

      (1) The development of a virtual partner model trained for each individual participant, which can dynamically vary its synchronization to the participant's behavior in real time, is novel and exciting.

      (2) Understanding real-time temporal coordination for behaviors like speech is a critical and understudied area.

      (3) The use of SEEG provides the spatial and temporal resolution necessary to address the complex dynamics associated with the behavior.

      (4) The paper provides some results that suggest a role for regions like IFG and STG in the dynamic temporal coordination of behavior both within an individual speaker and across speakers performing a coordination task.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This paper investigates the neural underpinnings of an interactive speech task requiring verbal coordination with another speaker. To achieve this, the authors recorded intracranial brain activity from the left (and to a lesser extent, the right) hemisphere in a group of drug-resistant epilepsy patients while they synchronised their speech with a 'virtual partner'. Crucially, the authors were able to manipulate the degree of success of this synchronisation by programming the virtual partner to either actively synchronise or desynchronise their speech with the participant, or else to not vary its speech in response to the participant (making the synchronisation task purely one-way). Using such a paradigm, the authors identified different brain regions that were either more sensitive to the speech of the virtual partner (primary auditory cortex), or more sensitive to the degree of verbal coordination (i.e. synchronisation success) with the virtual partner (left secondary auditory cortex and bilateral IFG). Such sensitivity was measured by (1) calculating the correlation between the index of verbal coordination and mean power within a range of frequency bands across trials, and (2) calculating the phase-amplitude coupling between the behavioural and brain signals within single trials (using the power of high-frequency neural activity only). Overall, the findings help to elucidate some of the brain areas involved in interactive speaking behaviours, particularly highlighting high-frequency activity of the bilateral IFG as a potential candidate supporting verbal coordination.

      Strengths:

      This study provides the field with a convincing demonstration of how to investigate speaking behaviours in more complex situations that share many features with real-world speaking contexts e.g. simultaneous engagement of speech perception and production processes, the presence of an interlocutor and the need for inter-speaker coordination. The findings thus go beyond previous work that has typically studied solo speech production in isolation, and represent a significant advance in our understanding of speech as a social and communicative behaviour. It is further an impressive feat to develop a paradigm in which the degree of cooperativity of the synchronisation partner can be so tightly controlled; in this way, this study combines the benefits of using pre-recorded stimuli (namely, the high degree of experimental control) with the benefits of using a live synchronisation partner (allowing the task to be truly two-way interactive, an important criticism of other work using pre-recorded stimuli). A further key strength of the study lies in its employment of stereotactic EEG to measure brain responses with both high temporal and spatial resolution, an ideal method for studying the unfolding relationship between neural processing and this dynamic coordination behaviour.

      Weaknesses:

      One limitation of the current study is the relatively sparse coverage of the right hemisphere by the implanted electrodes (91 electrodes in the right compared to 145 in the left). Of course, electrode location is solely clinically motivated, and so the authors did not have control over this. In a previous version of this article, the authors therefore chose not to include data from the right hemisphere in reported analyses. However, after highlighting previous literature suggesting that the right hemisphere likely has high relevance to verbal coordination behaviours such as those under investigation here, the authors have now added analyses of the right hemisphere data to the results. These confirm an involvement of the right hemisphere in this task, largely replicating left hemisphere results. Some hemispheric differences were found in responses within the STG; however, interpretation should be tempered by an awareness of the relatively sparse coverage of the right hemisphere meaning that some regions have very few electrodes, resulting in reduced statistical power.

    4. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Recommendations for the authors: 

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      (1) The use of the term "language network" throughout is unclear. Does this refer to work by Ev Fedorenko (i.e., does it distinguish language from other cognitive and sensorimotor domains)? There does not seem to be much in the behavior presented here that aligns with an interpretation about language per se. 

      We understand the reviewer’s point according to the work by Evelina Fedorenko considering this distinction. It is important to precise that in our present study we did not refer to her work when using the term “language network”.

      (2) Fig 4A: the "B" is missing on the figure panel to denote which Broadmann areas are shown. 

      We updated the figure panel by adding the “B” for more clarity.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      I think it would be worth mentioning the relatively sparse coverage of the right hemisphere in your abstract. 

      We agree with this suggestion, we updated the abstract as follows :  

      “Our use of language, which is profoundly social in nature, essentially takes place in interactive contexts and is shaped by precise coordination dynamics that interlocutors must observe. Thus, language interaction is highly demanding on fast adjustment of speech production. Here, we developed a real-time coupled-oscillators virtual partner that allows - by changing the coupling strength parameters - to modulate the ability to synchronise speech with a virtual speaker. Then, we recorded the intracranial brain activity of 16 patients with drug-resistant epilepsy while they performed a verbal coordination task with the virtual partner (VP). More precisely, patients had to repeat short sentences synchronously with the VP. This synchronous speech task is efficient to highlight both the dorsal and ventral language pathways. Importantly, combining time-resolved verbal coordination and neural activity shows more spatially differentiated patterns and different types of neural sensitivity along the dorsal pathway. More precisely, high-frequency activity in left secondary auditory regions is highly sensitive to verbal coordinative dynamics, while primary regions are not. Finally, while bilateral engagement was observed in the high-frequency activity of the IFG BA44— which seems to index online coordinative adjustments that are continuously required to compensate deviation from synchronisation—interpretation of right hemisphere involvement should be approached cautiously due to relatively sparse electrode coverage. These findings illustrate the possibility and value of using a fully dynamic, adaptive and interactive language task to gather deeper understanding of the subtending neural dynamics involved in speech perception, production as well as their interaction.”

      There are a few places in your results section which haven't been updated to reflect the fact that some sections refer only to the left hemisphere e.g. 

      Page 11 line 347: "Overall, neural responses are present in all six canonical frequency bands" I think this should be "In the left hemisphere, neural responses are present...". 

      Page 12 line 355: "As expected, the whole language network is strongly involved..." I think this should be "As expected, the whole left hemisphere language network is strongly involved".  Page 17 (third paragraph of the discussion): "The observed negative correlation between verbal coordination and high-frequency activity (HFa) in STG BA22" I think this should be "in left STG BA22". 

      We thank the reviewer for highlighting these important points. The updated lines are as follows:

      Page 11 line 348: ”In the left hemisphere, neural responses are present in all six canonical frequency bands…”  

      Page 12 line 356: ”As expected, the whole left hemisphere language network is strongly involved..." Page 17 lines 502-503 : “The observed negative correlation between verbal coordination and highfrequency activity (HFa) in left STG BA22 suggests a suppression of neural responses as the degree of behavioural synchrony increases.”

    1. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In microbiology, accurately characterizing microbial populations and communities is essential. One widely used approach is to measure the absolute or relative abundance of microbial species. Recent research in microbial ecology, for instance, has shown that even genetically identical hosts exposed to the same microbial pool can develop very different gut microbiota, largely due to random colonization events. This study builds on that idea but adds a valuable layer: it suggests that some of the observed variability might actually result from experimental noise, specifically the randomness introduced by dilution and plate counting techniques. To address this, the authors introduce REPOP, a new tool designed to improve the quantification of microbial populations by explicitly accounting for the inherent stochasticity in these methods. They test REPOP using both simulated and experimental datasets, showing how it can help recover meaningful trends.

      Strengths:

      Overall, this paper is a good contribution to the field. The motivation is clear: improving our ability to quantify microbial populations is crucial for many research areas. The authors make a strong case that ignoring experimental noise is no longer acceptable, and they offer a well-argued solution. The manuscript is well-written and easy to follow, and the logic behind REPOP is convincingly laid out. The use of simulated data is especially valuable, as it allows the authors to test whether the method can recover known inputs, an important validation step. Even with experimental data, where true values are unknown, the method seems to behave in a reasonable and expected way, which is reassuring. All in all, this is an important step forward in how we quantify microbial populations.

      Weaknesses:

      While the study is promising, there are a few areas where the paper could be strengthened to increase its impact and usability. First, the extent to which dilution and plating introduce noise is not fully explored. Could this noise significantly affect experimental conclusions? And under what conditions does it matter most? Does it depend on experimental design or specific parameter values? Clarifying this would help readers appreciate when and why REPOP should be used. Second, more practical details about the tool itself would be very helpful. Simply stating that it is available on GitHub may not be enough. Readers will want to know what programming language it uses, what the input data should look like, and ideally, see a step-by-step diagram of the workflow. Packaging the tool as an easy-to-use resource, perhaps even submitting it to CRAN or including example scripts, would go a long way, especially since microbiologists tend to favor user-friendly, recipe-like solutions. Third, it would be great to see the method tested on existing datasets, such as those from Nic Vega and Jeff Gore (2017), which explore how colonization frequency impacts abundance fluctuation distributions. Even if the general conclusions remain unchanged, showing that REPOP can better match observed patterns would strengthen the paper's real-world relevance. Lastly, it would be helpful for the authors to briefly discuss the limitations of their method, as no approach is without its constraints. Acknowledging these would provide a more balanced and transparent perspective.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Microbial population abundances are regularly estimated by multiplying plate counts by dilution factors, with inferences made about sample heterogeneity without taking into account heterogeneity generated through dilution and plating methods. The authors have developed REPOP, a method for disentangling methodological stochasticity from ecological heterogeneity using a Bayesian framework. They present three models: a unimodal distribution, a multimodal distribution, and a multimodal distribution that incorporates a colony count cutoff. They use a combination of simulated and experimental data to show the effectiveness of the REPOP method in resolving true microbial population distributions.

      Strengths:

      Overall, this paper addresses a significant issue in microbial ecology and reliably demonstrates that the REPOP method improves upon current methods of estimating microbial population heterogeneity, particularly with simulation data. The three models presented build upon each other and are discussed in a way that is fairly accessible to a broad audience. The authors also show that leveraging the information provided by non-countable plates is important. Additionally, the authors address the potential for extending this method to other sources of methodological stochasticity that may occur in microbial plating. However, it does seem that they could extend this further by discussing ways that this method could be applied to non-microbial systems, allowing this work to appeal to a broader audience.

      Weaknesses:

      A more thorough discussion of when and by how much estimated microbial population abundance distributions differ from the ground truth would be helpful in determining the best practices for applying this method. Not only would this allow researchers to understand the sampling effort necessary to achieve the results presented here, but it would also contextualize the experimental results presented in the paper. Particularly, there is a disconnect between the discussion of the large sample sizes necessary to achieve accurate multimodal distribution estimates and the small sample sizes used in both experiments.

    3. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors developed a novel theoretical/computational procedure to count bacterial populations without introducing artificial randomness effects due to dilution. Surprisingly, this very important aspect of studies of bacterial systems has been overlooked. The proposed method provides a simple and transparent approach to eliminate the randomness of bacterial accounting procedures, allowing now to fully concentrate on the intrinsic effects of the studied systems.

      Strengths:

      A very simple and clear procedure is introduced and explained in full detail. This elegant approach finds an excellent compromise between mathematical rigor and computational efficiency, which is important for practical applications. The provided examples are convincing beyond a doubt, clearly indicating the potential strong impact of the proposed framework. Various complications and possible issues are also discussed and analyzed. This seems to be a very powerful novel method that should significantly advance the analysis of complex biological systems.

      Weaknesses:

      The only minor weakness that I found is the assumption of independence of bacterial species, which is expressed as the well-stirred approximation. One could imagine that bacterial species might cooperate, leading to non-uniform distributions that are real. How to distinguish such situations?

      I believe that this method can be extended to determine if this is the case or not before the application. For example, if the bacteria species are independent of each other and one can use the binomial distributions, then the Fano factor would be proportional to the overall relative fraction of bacterial species. Maybe a simple test can be added to test it before the application of REPOP. However, I believe that this is a minor issue.

    4. eLife Assessment

      This important study introduces a Bayesian method to determine bacterial counts that accounts for the experimental noise inherent to dilution and plating methods, and distinguishes it from biological uncertainty. The evidence supporting the conclusions is convincing, combining simulated data and experimental data. The method will be of interest to microbial ecologists, and potentially to the broader community interested in inference from biological data, even more so if the domain of application and the limitations are further clarified.

    5. Author response:

      Reviewer #1:

      The only minor weakness that I found is the assumption of independence of bacterial species, which is expressed as the well-stirred approximation. One could imagine that bacterial species might cooperate, leading to non-uniform distributions that are real. How to distinguish such situations? I believe that this method can be extended to determine if this is the case or not before the application. For example, if the bacteria species are independent of each other and one can use the binomial distributions, then the Fano factor would be proportional to the overall relative fraction of bacterial species. Maybe a simple test can be added to test it before the application of REPOP. However, I believe that this is a minor issue.

      This is an interesting point raised by the reviewer.

      First, we need to clarify an important point–we do not make a well-stirred assumption. Samples can be drawn and plated from any region of space however small and that region’s population can be quantified using our method. The stirring only occurs after we collect a sample in order to dilute the contents and pour the solution homogeneously over the plate.

      As such, learning multiple independent species is possible and not impacted by the dilution (“wellstirred” assumption). In the revised manuscript we will make it clear that this assumption concerns the dilution process. Any correlation between species arises in the initial sample and should be retained in the plating. Once given the sample, the dilution itself produces independent binomial draws from that point in space from which cultures were harvested. REPOP is designed to recover the true underlying heterogeneity in species abundance (even from limited data) by leveraging a Bayesian framework that remains valid regardless of whether species are independent or correlated.

      If one applies the method for multiple species as is, REPOP can recover the marginal distribution of each species in each plate if they are selectively cultured or many species at once if the colonies are sufficiently distinct. To demonstrate this, we will add a synthetic example with two species whose populations in a sample are correlated to the manuscript.

      However, in order to learn the joint distribution and capture correlations between species within samples, the method would need to be extended. At present, in Eq. 5 we sum the likelihood over all values of n, using a data-driven cutoff (twice the na¨ıvely estimated count times the dilution factor). Extending this to multiple species adding up to (n1,n2), while retain the generality of the method, would require quadratically scaling memory with this cutoff in the population number. For this reason while we will comment on this in the next version of the manuscript, it will not be implemented as part of REPOP.

      Reviewer #2:

      A more thorough discussion of when and by how much estimated microbial population abundance distributions differ from the ground truth would be helpful in determining the best practices for applying this method. Not only would this allow researchers to understand the sampling effort necessary to achieve the results presented here, but it would also contextualize the experimental results presented in the paper. Particularly, there is a disconnect between the discussion of the large sample sizes necessary to achieve accurate multimodal distribution estimates and the small sample sizes used in both experiments.

      That is a great suggestion from the reviewer. To address it, we will expand Appendix B, which currently presents the relative error between the means for the experimental results in Fig. 3, to also include a comparable evaluation for the synthetic data example in Fig. 2.

      Specifically, for each example, we will report (1) the relative error in the estimated means (as already done for Fig. 3), and (2) the Kullback-Leibler (KL) divergence between the reconstructed and ground truth distributions. These metrics will be shown as a function of the size of the dataset, enabling a direct assessment of how the sampling effort affects the precision of the inference.

      That said, we highlight that by explicitly modeling the dilution process within a Bayesian framework, REPOP extracts the mathematically optimal amount of information from each individual sample no matter the sample size. Our strategy therefore leads to better inference with fewer measurements, which is particularly important in applications such as plate counting, where data acquisition is laborintensive.

      Reviewer #3:

      While the study is promising, there are a few areas where the paper could be strengthened to increase its impact and usability. First, the extent to which dilution and plating introduce noise is not fully explored. Could this noise significantly affect experimental conclusions? And under what conditions does it matter most? Does it depend on experimental design or specific parameter values? Clarifying this would help readers appreciate when and why REPOP should be used.

      We agree with the reviewer that this is an important point, and we will expand Appendix B to include a quantitative analysis using simulated data (Fig. 2), reporting both relative error and KL divergence as a function of dataset size. This complements our response to Reviewer #2 clarifying when REPOP offers the greatest benefit.

      In addition, we will expand the discussion on how modeling dilution noise becomes essential when learning population dynamics. In particular, we will emphasize the role of Model 3, especially relevant when working with multiple plates and approaching the asymptotic regime—an aspect that was alluded to in Fig. 3 but not fully explored.

      Second, more practical details about the tool itself would be very helpful. Simply stating that it is available on GitHub may not be enough. Readers will want to know what programming language it uses, what the input data should look like, and ideally, see a step-by-step diagram of the workflow. Packaging the tool as an easy-to-use resource, perhaps even submitting it to CRAN or including example scripts, would go a long way, especially since microbiologists tend to favor user-friendly, recipe-like solutions.

      We will update the introduction to reinforce that REPOP is written in Python(PyTorch), installable via pip, and designed for ease of use. We are also expanding the tutorials to include clearer guidance on data formatting and common workflows. Author response image 1 will be added in the revised manuscript to better illustrate the full application process.

      Author response image 1.

      Third, it would be great to see the method tested on existing datasets, such as those from Nic Vega and Jeff Gore (2017), which explore how colonization frequency impacts abundance fluctuation distributions. Even if the general conclusions remain unchanged, showing that REPOP can better match observed patterns would strengthen the paper’s real-world relevance.

      That is a great suggestion from the reviewer. We will demonstrate the application of REPOP to datasets such as that of Vega and Gore (Ref. 27 in the manuscript), as well as other publicly available datasets, in the revised version.

      Lastly, it would be helpful for the authors to briefly discuss the limitations of their method, as no approach is without its constraints. Acknowledging these would provide a more balanced and transparent perspective.

      We agree with the reviewer on that. A new subsection will explicitly address the assumptions of our method, and therefore its limitations, including assumptions about species classification, computational cost of joint inference, and dependence on accurate dilution modeling. This discussion will synthesize points raised throughout our response to all reviewers.

    1. eLife Assessment

      This valuable study addresses a gap in our understanding of how the size of the attentional field is represented within the visual cortex. The evidence supporting the role of visual cortical activity is convincing, based on a novel modeling analysis of fMRI data. The results will be of interest to psychologists and cognitive neuroscientists.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      The authors conducted an fMRI study to investigate the neural effects of sustaining attention to areas of different sizes. Participants were instructed to attend to alphanumeric characters arranged in a circular array. The size of attention field was manipulated in four levels, ranging from small (18 deg) to large (162 deg). They used a model-based method to visualize attentional modulation in early visual cortex V1 to V3, and found spatially congruent modulations of the BOLD response, i.e., as the attended area increased in size, the neural modulation also increased in size in the visual cortex. They suggest that this result is a neural manifestation of the zoom-lens model of attention and that the model-based method can effectively reconstruct the neural modulation in the cortical space.

      The study is well-designed with sophisticated and comprehensive data analysis. The results are robust and show strong support for a well-known model of spatial attention, the zoom-lens model. Overall, I find the results interesting and useful for the field of visual attention research.

      Comments on revisions:

      The authors have addressed my previous comments satisfactorily. I would encourage the authors to make data and code publicly available, which appears to be the custom in this era.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The study in question utilizes functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to dynamically estimate the locus and extent of covert spatial attention from visuocortical activity. The authors aim to address an important gap in our understanding of how the size of the attentional field is represented within the visual cortex. They present a novel paradigm that allows for the estimation of the spatial tuning of the attentional field and demonstrate the ability to reliably recover both the location and width of the attentional field based on BOLD responses.

      Strengths:

      (1) Innovative Paradigm: The development of a new approach to estimate the spatial tuning of the attentional field is a significant strength of this study. It provides a fresh perspective on how spatial attention modulates visual perception.

      (2) Refined fMRI Analysis: The use of fMRI to track the spatial tuning of the attentional field across different visual regions is methodologically rigorous and provides valuable insights into the neural mechanisms underlying attentional modulation.

      (3) Clear Presentation: The manuscript is well-organized, and the results are presented clearly, which aids in the reader's comprehension of the complex data and analyses involved.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) Lack of Neutral Cue Condition: The study does not include a neutral cue condition where the cue width spans 360{degree sign}, which could serve as a valuable baseline for assessing the BOLD response enhancements and diminishments in both attended and non-attended areas.

      (2) Clarity on Task Difficulty Ratios: The explicit reasoning for the chosen letter-to-number ratios for various cue widths is not detailed. Ensuring clarity on these ratios is crucial, as it affects the task difficulty and the comparability of behavioral performance across different cue widths. It is essential that observed differences in behavior and BOLD signals are attributable solely to changes in cue width and not confounded by variations in task difficulty.

      Comments on revisions:

      (1) Please standardize the naming of error metrics across Figures 4-6 to improve clarity (e.g., "angular error" (Figure 4), "|angular error|" (Figure 5), and "absolute error" (Figure 6) appear to refer to the same measure). This inconsistency is also present in the main text.

      (2) Consider briefly mentioning the baseline offset in Lines 179-186. It is included in Figures 4-7 and serves as a reference for interpreting attentional modulation alongside gain. Introducing it with other model parameters would improve clarity.

      (3) It may be valuable to examine BOLD responses in unattended visual regions. As shown in Figure 2a, suppression patterns (e.g., the most negative responses) appear to vary in extent and distribution with attentional cue width. Analyzing these unattended regions may offer a more complete view of how attention shapes the spatial profile of cortical activity.

    4. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      We thank the three reviewers for their insightful feedback. We look forward to addressing the raised concerns in a revised version of the manuscript. There were a few common themes among the reviews that we will briefly touch upon now, and we will provide more details in the revised manuscript. 

      First, the reviewers asked for the reasoning behind the task ratios we implemented for the different attentional width conditions. The different ratios were selected to be as similar as possible given the size and spacing of our stimuli (aside from the narrowest cue width of one bin, the ratios for the others were 0.66, .6 and .66). As Figure 1b shows, while the ratios were similar, task difficulty is not constant across cue widths: spreading attention makes the task more difficult generally. But, while the modeled width of the spatial distribution of attention changes monotonically with cue width, task difficulty does not. Furthermore, prior work has indicated that there is a relationship between task difficulty and the overall magnitude of the BOLD response, however we don’t suspect that this will influence the width of the modulation. How task difficulty influences the BOLD response is an important topic, and we hope that future work will investigate this relationship more directly.   

      Second, reviewers raised interest in the distribution of spatial attention in higher visual areas. In our study we focus only on early visual regions (V1-V3). This was primarily driven by pragmatic considerations, in that we only have retinotopic estimates for our participants in these early visual areas. Our modeling approach is dependent on having access to the population receptive field estimates for all voxels, and while the main experiment was scanned using whole brain coverage, retinotopy was measured in a separate session using a field of view only covering the occipital cortex.  

      Lastly, we appreciate the opportunity to clarify the purpose of the temporal interval analysis. The reviewer is correct in assuming we set out to test how much data is needed to recover the cortical modulation and how dynamic a signal the method can capture. This analysis does show that more data provides more reliable estimates, though the model was still able to recover the location and width of the attentional cue at shorter timescales of as few as two TRs. This has implications for future studies that may involve more dynamic tracking of the attentional field.

      Public Reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review): 

      The authors conducted an fMRI study to investigate the neural effects of sustaining attention to areas of different sizes. Participants were instructed to attend to alphanumeric characters arranged in a circular array. The size of attention field was manipulated in four levels, ranging from small (18 deg) to large (162 deg). They used a model-based method to visualize attentional modulation in early visual cortex V1 to V3, and found spatially congruent modulations of the BOLD response, i.e., as the attended area increased in size, the neural modulation also increased in size in the visual cortex. They suggest that this result is a neural manifestation of the zoomlens model of attention and that the model-based method can effectively reconstruct the neural modulation in the cortical space. 

      The study is well-designed with sophisticated and comprehensive data analysis. The results are robust and show strong support for a well-known model of spatial attention, the zoom-lens model. Overall, I find the results interesting and useful for the field of visual attention research. I have questions about some aspects of the results and analysis as well as the bigger picture. 

      (1) It appears that the modulation in V1 is weaker than V2 and V3 (Fig 2). In particular, the width modulation in V1 is not statistically significant (Fig 5). This result seems a bit unexpected. Given the known RF properties of neurons in these areas, in particular, smaller RF in V1, one might expect more spatially sensitive modulation in V1 than V2/V3. Some explanations and discussions would be helpful. Relatedly, one would also naturally wonder if this method can be applied to other extrastriate visual areas such as V4 and what the results look like. 

      We agree with the reviewer. It’s very interesting how the spatial resolution within different visual regions contributes to the overall modulation of the attentional field, and how this in turn would influence perception. Our data showed that fits in V1 appeared to be less precise than in V2 and V3. This can be seen in the goodness of fit of the model as well as the gain and absolute angular error estimates. The goodness of fit and gain were lowest in V1 and the absolute angular error was largest in V1 (see Figure 5). We speculate that the finer spatial granularity of V1 RFs was countered by a lower amplitude and SNR of attention-related modulation in V1, resulting in overall lower sensitivity to variation in attentional field width. Prior findings concur that the magnitude of covert spatial attention increases when moving from striate to extrastriate cortex (Bressler & Silver (2010); Buracas & Boynton (2007)). Notably, in our perception condition, V1 showed more spatially sensitive modulation (see Figure 7), consistent with the known RF properties of V1 neurons.

      Regarding the second point: unfortunately, our dataset did not allow us to explore higherorder cortical regions with the model-based approach. While the main experiment was scanned using a sequence with whole brain coverage, the pRF estimates came from a separate scanning session which only had limited occipital coverage. Our modeling approach is dependent on the polar angle estimates from this pRF session. We now explicitly state this limitation in the methods (lines 87-89):

      “In this session, the field of view was restricted to the occipital cortex to maximize SNR, thereby limiting the brain regions for which we had pRF estimates to V1, V2, and V3.”

      (2) I'm a bit confused about the angular error result. Fig 4 shows that the mean angular error is close to zero, but Fig 5 reports these values to be about 30-40 deg. Why the big discrepancy? Is it due to the latter reporting absolute errors? It seems reporting the overall bias is more useful than absolute value. 

      The reviewer’s inference here is exactly right: Figure 4 shows signed error, whereas Figure 5 shows absolute error. We show the signed error for the example participant because, (1) by presenting the full distribution of model estimates for one participant, readers have access to a more direct representation of the data, and (2) at the individual level it is possible to examine potential directional biases in the location estimates (which do not appear to be present). As we don’t suspect a consistent directional bias across the group, we believe the absolute error in location estimates is more informative in depicting the precision in location estimates using the model-based approach. In the revised manuscript, we modified Figure 5 to make the example participant’s data visually distinct for easy comparison. We have clarified this reasoning in the text (results lines 59-64):

      “The angular error distribution across blocks, separated by width condition, is shown in Figure 4 for one example participant to display block-to-block variation. The model reliably captured the location of the attentional field with low angular error and with no systematic directional bias. This result was observed across participants. We next examined the absolute angular error to assess the overall accuracy of our estimates.”

      (3) A significant effect is reported for amplitude in V3 (line 78), but the graph in Fig 5 shows hardly any difference. Please confirm the finding and also explain the directionality of the effect if there is indeed one. 

      We realize that the y-axis scale of Figure 5 was making it difficult to see that gain decreases with cue width in area V3. Instead of keeping the y-axis limits the same across visual regions, we now adapt the y-axis scale of each subplot to the range of data values:  

      We now also add the direction of the effect in the text (results lines 83-86):

      “We observed no significant relationship between gain and cue width in V1 and V2 (V1 t(7)=.54, p=.605; V2 t(7)=-2.19, p=.065), though we did find a significant effect in V3 illustrating that gain decreases with cue width (t(7)=-3.12, p=.017).”

      (4) The purpose of the temporal interval analysis is rather unclear. I assume it has to do with how much data is needed to recover the cortical modulation and hence how dynamic a signal the method can capture. While the results make sense (i.e., more data is better), there is no obvious conclusion and/or interpretation of its meaning. 

      We apologize for not making our reasoning clear. We now emphasize our reasoning in the revised manuscript (results lines 110-112). Our objective was to quantify how much data was needed to recover the dynamic signal. As expected, we found that including more data reduces noise (averaging helps), but importantly, we found that we still obtained meaningful model fits even with limited data. We believe this has important implications for future paradigms that explore more dynamic deployment of spatial attention, where one would not want to average over multiple repetitions of a condition.

      The first paragraph of the Temporal Interval Analysis section in the results now reads: 

      “In the previous analyses, we leveraged the fact that the attentional cue remained constant for 5-trial blocks (spatial profiles were computed by averaging BOLD measurements across a block of 10 TRs). We next examined the degree to which we were able to recover the attentional field on a moment-by-moment (TR-by-TR) basis. To do this, we systematically adjusted the number of TRs that contributed to the averaged spatial response profile. To maintain a constant number of observations across the temporal interval conditions, we randomly sampled a subset of TRs from each block. This allowed us to determine the amount of data needed to recover the attentional field, with a goal of examining the usability of our modeling approach in future paradigms involving more dynamic deployment of spatial attention.”

      (5) I think it would be useful for the authors to make a more explicit connection to previous studies in this literature. In particular, two studies seem particularly relevant. First, how do the present results relate to those in Muller et al (2003, reference 37), which also found a zoom-lens type of neural effects. Second, how does the present method compare with spatial encoding model in Sprague & Serences (2013, reference 56), which also reconstructs the neural modulation of spatial attention. More discussions of these studies will help put the current study in the larger context.

      We now make a more explicit connection to prior work in the discussion section (lines 34-54). 

      “We introduced a novel modeling approach that recovered the location and the size of the attentional field. Our data show that the estimated spatial spread of attentional modulation (as indicated by the recovered FWHM) consistently broadened with the cue width, replicating prior work (Müller et al., 2003; Herrmann et al., 2010). Our results go beyond prior work by linking the spatial profiles to pRF estimates, allowing us to quantify the spread of both attentional and perceptual modulation in degrees of polar angle. Interestingly, the FWHM estimates for the attentional and perceptual spatial profiles were highly similar. Additionally, for area V3 we replicate that the population response magnitude decreased with cue width (Müller et al., 2003; Feldmann-Wüstefeld and Awh, 2020). One innovation of our method is that it directly reconstructs attention-driven modulations of responses in visual cortex, setting it apart from other methods, such as inverted encoding models (e.g. Sprague & Serences, 2013). Finally, we demonstrated that our method has potential to be used in more dynamic settings, in which changes in the attentional field need to be tracked on a shorter timescale.”

      (6) Fig 4b, referenced on line 123, does not exist. 

      We have corrected the text to reference the appropriate figure (Figure 5, results line 136).

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary: 

      The study in question utilizes functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to dynamically estimate the locus and extent of covert spatial attention from visuocortical activity. The authors aim to address an important gap in our understanding of how the size of the attentional field is represented within the visual cortex. They present a novel paradigm that allows for the estimation of the spatial tuning of the attentional field and demonstrate the ability to reliably recover both the location and width of the attentional field based on BOLD responses. 

      Strengths: 

      (1) Innovative Paradigm: The development of a new approach to estimate the spatial tuning of the attentional field is a significant strength of this study. It provides a fresh perspective on how spatial attention modulates visual perception. 

      (2) Refined fMRI Analysis: The use of fMRI to track the spatial tuning of the attentional field across different visual regions is methodologically rigorous and provides valuable insights into the neural mechanisms underlying attentional modulation. 

      (3) Clear Presentation: The manuscript is well-organized, and the results are presented clearly, which aids in the reader's comprehension of the complex data and analyses involved. 

      We thank the reviewer for summarizing the strengths in our work. 

      Weaknesses: 

      (1) Lack of Neutral Cue Condition: The study does not include a neutral cue condition where the cue width spans 360°, which could serve as a valuable baseline for assessing the BOLD response enhancements and diminishments in both attended and non-attended areas. 

      We do not think that the lack of a neutral cue condition substantially limits our ability to address the core questions of interest in the present work. We set out to estimate the locus and the spread of covert spatial attention. By definition, a neutral cue does not have a focus of attention as the whole annulus becomes task relevant. We agree with the reviewer that how spatial attention influences the magnitude of the BOLD response is still not well defined; i.e., does attending a location multiplicatively enhance responses at an attended location or does it instead act to suppress responses outside the focus of attention? A neutral cue condition would be necessary to be able to explore these types of questions. However, our findings don’t rest on any assumptions about this. Instead, we quantify the attentional modulation with a model-based approach and show that we can reliably recover its locus, and reveal a broadening in the attentional modulation with wider cues. 

      We realize that throughout the original manuscript we often used the term ‘attentional enhancement,’ which might inadvertently specify an increase with respect to a neutral condition. To be more agnostic to the directionality of the effect, we have changed this to ‘attentional modulation’ and ‘attentional gain’ throughout the manuscript. Additionally, we have added results and visualizations for the baseline parameter to all results figures (Figures 4-7) to help readers further interpret our findings.  

      (2) Clarity on Task Difficulty Ratios: The explicit reasoning for the chosen letter-to-number ratios for various cue widths is not detailed. Ensuring clarity on these ratios is crucial, as it affects the task difficulty and the comparability of behavioral performance across different cue widths. It is essential that observed differences in behavior and BOLD signals are attributable solely to changes in cue width and not confounded by variations in task difficulty.  

      The ratios were selected to be as similar as possible given the size and spacing of our stimuli (aside from the narrowest cue width of one bin, the proportions for the others were 0.67, 0.60, and 0.67). We have updated the methods section to state this explicitly (methods lines 36-38): 

      “The ratios were selected to be as similar as possible given the size and spacing of our stimuli (aside from the one-bin cue, the proportions for the other cues were 0.67, 0.60, 0.67).”

      As Figure 1b shows, task accuracy showed small and non-monotonic changes across the three larger cue widths, dissociable from the monotonic pattern seen for the modelestimated width of the attentional field. Furthermore, as prior work has indicated that there is a relationship between task difficulty and the overall magnitude of the BOLD response (e.g., Ress, Backus & Heeger, 2000), we would primarily expect effects of task difficulty on the gain or baseline rather than the width. How exactly task difficulty influences the BOLD response and whether this would, in fact, interact with the width of the attentional field is an important topic, and we hope that future work will investigate this relationship more directly.  

      We have clarified these points within the text, and now explicitly motivate future work looking at these important interactions (discussion lines 57-67):

      “The observed effects of attentional field width were unlikely to be directly attributable to variation in task difficulty. Participants' task in our study was to discriminate whether more numbers or more letters were presented within a cued region of an iso-eccentric annulus of white noise. For our different cue widths, the ratios of numbers and letters were selected to be as similar as possible given the size and spacing of our stimuli. Changes in accuracy across the three larger cue widths were small and non-monotonic, implying task difficulty was dissociable from width per se. This dissociation bolsters the interpretability of our model fits; nevertheless, future work should further investigate how task difficulty interacts with the spread of the attentional field and the amplitude of attention-related BOLD effects (cf. Ress, Backus & Heeger, 2000).”

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary: 

      In this report, the authors tested how manipulating the contiguous set of stimuli on the screen that should be used to guide behavior - that is, the scope of visual spatial attention - impacts the magnitude and profile of well-established attentional enhancements in visual retinotopic cortex. During fMRI scanning, participants attended to a cued section of the screen for blocks of trials and performed a letter vs digit discrimination task at each attended location (and judged whether the majority of characters were letters/digits). Importantly, the visual stimulus was identical across attention conditions, so any observed response modulations are due to topdown task demands rather than visual input. The authors employ population receptive field (pRF) models, which are used to sort voxel activation with respect to the location and scope of spatial attention and fit a Gaussian-like function to the profile of attentional enhancement from each region and condition. The authors find that attending to a broader region of space expands the profile of attentional enhancement across the cortex (with a larger effect in higher visual areas), but does not strongly impact the magnitude of this enhancement, such that each attended stimulus is enhanced to a similar degree. Interestingly, these modulations, overall, mimic changes in response properties caused by changes to the stimulus itself (increase in contrast matching the attended location in the primary experiment). The finding that attentional enhancement primarily broadens, but does not substantially weaken in most regions, is an important addition to our understanding of the impact of distributed attention on neural responses, and will provide meaningful constraints to neural models of attentional enhancement. 

      Strengths: 

      (1) Well-designed manipulations (changing location and scope of spatial attention), and careful retinotopic/pRF mapping, allow for a robust assay of the spatial profile of attentional enhancement, which has not been carefully measured in previous studies.

      (2) Results are overall clear, especially concerning width of the spatial region of attentional enhancement, and lack of clear and consistent evidence for reduction in the amplitude of enhancement profile.

      (3) Model-fitting to characterize spatial scope of enhancement improves interpretability of findings.

      We thank the reviewer for highlighting the strengths of our study. 

      Weaknesses: 

      (1) Task difficulty seems to vary as a function of spatial scope of attention, with varying ratios of letters/digits across spatial scope conditions, which may complicate interpretations of neural modulation results  

      The reviewer is correct in observing that task accuracy varied across cue widths. Though we selected the task ratios to be as similar as possible given the size and spacing of our stimuli (aside from the narrowest cue width of one bin, the proportions for the others were 0.67, 0.60, and 0.67), behavioral accuracy across the three larger cue widths was not identical. Prior research has shown that there is a relationship between task difficulty and the overall magnitude of the BOLD response (e.g., Ress, Backus & Heeger, 2000). Thus, we would primarily expect effects of task difficulty on gain rather than width. How task difficulty influences the BOLD response and whether this would, in fact, interact with the width of the attentional field is an important topic, and we hope that future work will investigate this relationship more directly.  

      To clarify these points and highlight the potential for future work looking at these important interactions, we added the following text to the discussion section (discussion lines 57-67):

      “The observed effects of attentional field width were unlikely to be directly attributable to variation in task difficulty. Participants' task in our study was to discriminate whether more numbers or more letters were presented within a cued region of an iso-eccentric annulus of white noise. For our different cue widths, the ratios of numbers and letters were selected to be as similar as possible given the size and spacing of our stimuli. Changes in accuracy across the three larger cue widths were small and non-monotonic, implying task difficulty was dissociable from width per se. This dissociation bolsters the interpretability of our model fits; nevertheless, future work should further investigate how task difficulty interacts with the spread of the attentional field and the amplitude of attention-related BOLD effects (cf. Ress, Backus and Heeger, 2000).”

      (2) Some aspects of analysis/data sorting are unclear (e.g., how are voxels selected for analyses?) 

      We apologize for not describing our voxel selection in sufficient detail. Some of the questions raised in the private comments are closely related to this point, we therefore aim to clarify all concerns below:

      - Voxel selection: To select voxels that contribute to the 1D spatial profiles, we relied on the independent pRF dataset. We first defined some general requirements that needed to be met. Specifically, 1) the goodness of fit (R<sup>2</sup>) of the pRF fits needed to be greater than 10%; 2) the estimated eccentricity had to fall within [0.7 9.1] degree eccentricity (to exclude voxels in the fovea and voxels with estimated eccentricities larger than the pRF mapping stimulus); 3) the estimated size must be greater than 0.01 degree visual angle. 

      Next, we included only voxels whose pRF overlapped with the white noise annulus. Estimated eccentricity was used to select all voxels whose eccentricity estimate fell within the annulus bounds. However, here it is also important to take the size of the pRF into account. Some voxels’ estimated eccentricity might fall just outside the annulus, but will still have substantial overlap due to the size of their pRF. Therefore, we further included all voxels whose estimated pRF size resulted in overlap with the annulus. 

      This implies that some voxels with greater eccentricities and larger pRF sizes contribute to the 1D profile, which will influence the spatial specificity of the 1D profiles. However, we want to emphasize that in our view, the exact FWHM value is not so much of interest, as this will always be dependent on the voxel selection and many other data processing steps. Instead, we focus on the relative differences of the FWHM driven by the parametric attentional cue width manipulation. 

      - Data sorting and binning. The reviewer raises an important point about how the FWHM value should be interpreted considering the data processing steps. To generate the 1D spatial profile, we binned voxels based on their estimated polar angle preference into 6degree bins and applied a moving average of 18 degrees to smooth the 1D profiles. Both of these processing steps will influence the spatial specificity of the profile. The binning step facilitates recentering based on cue center and combining across trials.

      To explore the extent to which the moving average substantially impacted our results, we reran our analyses without that smoothing step. The vast majority of the results held. In V1, we found a significant effect of cue width on FWHM where the result was not significant previously (t(7)=2.52, p\=.040). Additionally, when looking at the minimum number of TRs needed to see a significant effect of cue width on FWHM, without the smoothing step in V1 it took 10 TRs (not significant at 10 TRs previously), in V2 it took 5 TRs (10 previously), and in V3 it took 3 TRs (2 previously). The other notable difference is that FWHM was generally a bit larger when the moving average smoothing was performed. We have visualized the group results for the FWHM estimates below to help with comparison. 

      Author response image 1.

      No moving average smoothing:

      Voxel selection methods have been clarified in methods section lines 132-139:

      “Within each ROI, pRF modeling results were used to constrain voxel selection used in the main experiment. We excluded voxels with a preferred eccentricity outside the bounds of the pRF stimulus (<0.7° and >9.1°), with a pRF size smaller than 0.01°, or with poor spatial selectivity as indicated by the pRF model fit (R2 < 10%). Following our 2D visualizations (see below), we further constrained voxel selection by only including voxels whose pRF overlapped with the white noise annulus. We included all voxels with an estimated eccentricity within the annulus bounds, as well as voxels with an estimated pRF size that would overlap the annulus.”

      Data binning methods have been clarified in methods section lines 154-159: 

      “Voxels with pRFs overlapping the white noise annulus were grouped into 60 bins according to their pRF polar angle estimate (6° polar angle bin width). We computed a median BOLD response within each bin. This facilitated the recentering of each profile to align all cue centers for subsequent combining across trials. To improve the signal-to-noise ratio, the resulting profile was smoothed with a moving average filter (width 18° polar angle; see Figure 2b).”

      (3) While the focus of this report is on modulations of visual cortex responses due to attention, the lack of inclusion of results from other retinotopic areas (e.g. V3AB, hV4, IPS regions like IPS0/1) is a weakness 

      We agree with the reviewer that using this approach in other retinotopic areas would be of significant interest. In this case, population receptive field mapping occurred in a separate session with a field of view only covering the occipital cortex (in contrast to the experimental session, which had whole-brain coverage). Because our modeling approach relies on these pRF estimates, we were unable to explore higher visual areas. However, we hope future work will follow up on this.

      We have added the following text to the methods section describing the pRF mapping session (lines 87-89):

      “In this session, the field of view was restricted to the occipital cortex to maximize SNR, thereby limiting the brain regions for which we had pRF estimates to V1, V2, and V3.”

      (4) Additional analyses comparing model fits across amounts of data analyzed suggest the model fitting procedure is biased, with some parameters (e.g., FWHM, error, gain) scaling with noise. 

      In this analysis, we sought to test how much data was needed to recover the attentional field, in view of the need for additional fMRI-based tools for use in tasks that involve more rapid dynamic adaptation of attention. Though we did find that more data reduced noise (and accordingly decreased absolute error and amplitude while increasing FWHM and R<sup>2</sup>), absolute angular error remained low across different temporal intervals (well below the chance level of 90°). With regard to FWHM, we believe that the more important finding is that the model-estimated FWHM was modulated by cue width at shorter timescales of as few as two TRs while maintaining relatively low angular error. We refrain from drawing conclusions here on the basis of the exact FWHM values, both because we don’t have a ground truth for the attentional field and because various processing pipeline steps can impact the values as well. Rather, we are looking at relative value and overall patterns in the estimates. The observed patterns imply that the model recovers meaningful modulation of the attentional field even at shorter time scales.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Additional data reporting and discussion of results are needed as outlined in the public review. 

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) The current experimental design effectively captured the impact of varying cue widths on the BOLD response in the visual cortex. However, the inclusion of a neutral cue condition, where the cue width spans 360{degree sign} and all peripheral stimuli are attended, could serve as a valuable baseline. This would enable a quantitative assessment of how much the BOLD response is enhanced in specific spatial regions due to focused cues and, conversely, how much it is diminished in non-attended areas, along with the spatial extent of these effects. 

      Please refer to our response in the public review. 

      (2) While the study provides valuable insights into BOLD signal changes in visual areas corresponding to the focus of attention, it does not extend its analysis to the impact on regions outside the focus of attention. It would be beneficial to explore whether there is a corresponding decrease in BOLD signal in non-attended regions, and if identified, to describe the spatial extent and position of this effect relative to the attended area. Such an analysis could yield deeper insights into how attention influences activity across the visual cortex. 

      We agree with the reviewer that it is very interesting to examine the spread of attention across the whole visual field. Our experiment was designed to focus on width modulations at a fixed eccentricity, but future work should explore how the attentional field changes with eccentricity and interacts with spatial variations across the visual field. This is highlighted in our discussion section (lines 76-81): 

      “Future work can help provide a better understanding of the contribution of spatial attention by considering how the attentional field interacts with these well described spatial variations across the visual field. Measuring the full spatial distribution of the attentional field (across both eccentricity and polar angle) will shed light on how spatial attention guides perception by interacting with the non-uniformity of spatial representations.”

      The addition of figure panels for the estimated baseline parameter in Figures 4-7 provides further information about BOLD effects in unattended regions of the annulus.  

      (3) The rationale behind the selection of task difficulty ratios for different cue widths, specifically the letter-to-number ratios of 1:0, 1:2, 2:3, and 3:6 (or vice versa) for cue widths of 18{degree sign}, 54{degree sign}, 90{degree sign}, and 162{degree sign} respectively, was not explicitly discussed. It would be beneficial to clarify the basis for these ratios, as they may influence the perceived difficulty of the task and thus the comparability of behavioral performance across different cue widths. Ensuring that the task difficulty is consistent across conditions is crucial for attributing differences in behavior and BOLD signals solely to changes in cue width and not confounded by variations in task difficulty. 

      Please refer to our response in the public review. We now clarify why we selected these ratios, and acknowledge more explicitly that behavioral performance differed across width conditions. See also our reply to private comment 1 from Reviewer 3 for some additional analyses examining task related influences.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Task difficulty: the task seems exceptionally challenging. Stimuli are presented at a relativelyeccentric position for a very brief duration, and a large number of comparisons must be made across a broad region of space. This is reflected in the behavioral performance, which decreases rapidly as the scope of attention increases (Fig. 1). Because trials are blocked, does this change in task difficulty across conditions impact the degree to which neural responses are modulated? How should we consider differences in task difficulty in interpreting the conclusions (especially with respect to the amplitude parameter)? Also, note that the difficulty scales both with number of stimuli - as more need to be compared - but also with the ratio, which differs nonmonotonically across task conditions. One way to dissociate these might be RT: for 54/162, which both employ the same ratio of letter/digits and have similar accuracy, is RT longer for 162, which requires attending more stimuli? 

      In addition to our comments in response to the public review, we emphasize that the reviewer makes an important point that there are differences in task difficulty, though the ratios are as close as they can be given the size and spacing of our stimuli. Behavioral performance varied non-monotonically with cue width, bolstering our confidence that our monotonically increasing model-estimated width is likely not entirely driven by task difficulty. There nevertheless remain open questions related to how task difficulty does impact BOLD attentional modulation, which we hope future work will more directly investigate.

      The reviewer's comments identify two ways our data might preliminarily speak to questions about BOLD attentional modulation and task difficulty. First: how might the amplitude parameter reflect task difficulty? This is an apt question as we agree with the reviewer that it would be a likely candidate in which to observe effects of task difficulty. We do find a small effect of cue width on our amplitude estimates (amplitude decreases with width) in V3. Using the same analysis technique to look at the relationship between task difficulty and amplitude, we find no clear relationship in any of the visual areas (all p >= 0.165, testing whether the slopes differed from zero at the group level using a one-sample t-test). We believe future work using other experimental manipulations should look more systematically at the relationship between task difficulty and amplitude of the attentional BOLD enhancement.

      Second: Does the same ratio at different widths elicit different behavioral responses (namely accuracy and RT)? We followed the reviewer’s suggestion to compare performance between cue widths of three and nine (identical ratios, different widths; see Author response image 2 and Figure 5). We found that, using a paired t-test, behavioral accuracy differed between the two cue widths (mean accuracy of 0.73 versus 0.69, p = 0.008), with better performance for cue width three. RT did not differ significantly between the two conditions (paired t-test, p = 0.729). This could be due to the fact that participants were not incentivized to respond as quickly as possible, they merely needed to respond before the end of the response window (1.25 s) following the stimulus presentation (0.5 s). The comparisons for accuracy and RT (calculated from time of stimulus appearance) are plotted below:

      Author response image 2.

      In summary, with matched stimulus ratios, the wider cue was associated with worse (though not slower) performance. This could be due to the fact that more elements are involved and/or that tasks become more difficult when attending to a broader swath of space. Given these results, we believe that future studies targeting difficulty effects should use direct and independent manipulations of task difficulty and attentional width. 

      (2) Eye movements: while the authors do a good job addressing the average eccentricity of fixation, I'm not sure this fully addresses concerns with eye movements, especially for the character-discrimination task which surely benefits from foveation (and requires a great deal of control to minimize saccades!). Can the authors additionally provide data on, e.g., # of fixations within the attended stimulus annulus, or fixation heatmap, or # of saccades, or some other indicator of likelihood of fixating the letter stimuli for each condition? 

      We agree with the reviewer that this task is surely much easier if one foveated the stimuli, and it did indeed require control to minimize saccades to the annulus. (We appreciate the effort and motivation of our participants!) We are happy to provide additional data to address these reasonable concerns about eye movements. Below, we have visualized the number of fixations to the annulus, separated by participant and width. Though there is variability across participants, there are at most 16 instances of fixations to the annulus for a given participant, combined across all width conditions. The median number of fixations to the annulus per width is zero (shown in red). Considering the amount of time participants engaged in the task (between 8 and 12 runs of the task, each run with 100 trials), this indicates participants were generally successful at maintaining central fixation while the stimuli were presented.

      Author response image 3.

      We added the results of this analysis to the methods section (lines 205-208):

      “Additionally, we examined the number of fixations to the white noise annulus itself. No participant had more than 16 fixations (out of 800-1200 trials) to the annulus during the task, further suggesting that participants successfully maintained fixation.”

      (3) pRF sorting and smoothing: Throughout, the authors are analyzing data binned based on pRF properties with respect to the attended location ("voxels with pRFs overlapping with the white noise annulus", line 243-244) First, what does this mean? Does the pRF center need to be within the annulus? Or is there a threshold based on the pRF size? If so, how is this implemented? Additionally, considering the methods text in lines 242-247, the authors mention that they bin across 6 deg-wide bins and smooth with a moving average (18 deg), which I think will lead to further expansion of the profile of attentional enhancement (see also below) 

      We provide a detailed response in the public review. Furthermore, we have clarified the voxel selection procedure in the Methods (lines 132–139 & 154–159).

      (4) FWHM values: The authors interpret the larger FWHMs estimated from their model-fitting than the actual size of the attended region as a meaningful result. However, depending on details of the sorting procedure above, this may just be due to the data processing itself. One way to identify how much expansion of FWHM occurs due to analysis is by simulating data given estimates of pRF properties for a 'known' shape of modulation (e.g., square wave exactly spanning the attended aperture) and compare the resulting FWHM to that observed for attention and perception conditions (e.g., Fig. 7c). 

      We provide a detailed response in the public review. The essence of our response is to refrain from interpreting the precise recovered FWHM values, which will be influenced by multiple processing steps, and instead to focus on relative differences as a function of the attentional cue width. Accordingly, we did not add simulations to the revised manuscript, although we agree with the reviewer that such simulations could shed light on the underlying spatial resolution, and how binning and smoothing influences the estimated FWHM. We have clarified our interpretation of FWHM results in the manuscript as follows:

      Results lines 137-141:

      “One possibility is that the BOLD-derived FWHM might tend to overestimate the retinotopic extent of the modulation, perhaps driven by binning and smoothing processing steps to create the 1D spatial profiles. If this were the case, we would expect to obtain similar FWHM estimates when modeling the perceptual modulations as well.”

      Results lines 169-175:

      “Mirroring the results from the attentional manipulation, FWHM estimates systematically exceeded the nominal size of the perceptually modulated region of the visual field. Comparing the estimated FWHMs of the perceptual and attentional spatial profiles (Figure 7c) revealed that the estimated widths were highly comparable (Pearson correlation r=0.664 across width conditions and visual regions). Importantly, the relative differences in FWHM show meaningful effects of both cue and contrast width in a similar manner for both attentional and perceptual forms of modulation.”

      Discussion lines 16-22:

      “We also found that the estimated spatial spread of the attentional modulation (as indicated by the recovered FWHM) was consistently wider than the cued region itself. We therefore compared the spread of the attention field with the spatial profile of a perceptually induced width manipulation. The results were comparable in both the attentional and perceptual versions of the task, suggesting that cueing attention to a region results in a similar 1D spatial profile to when the stimulus contrast is simply increased in that region.”

      (5) Baseline parameter: looking at the 'raw' response profiles shown in Fig. 2b, it looks, at first, like the wider attentional window shows substantially lower enhancement. However, this seems to be mitigated by the shift of the curve downwards. Can the authors analyze the baseline parameter in a similar manner as their amplitude analyses throughout? This is especially interesting in contrast to the perception results (Fig. 7), for which the baseline does not seem to scale in a similar way. 

      We agree with the reviewer that the baseline parameter is worth examining, and have therefore added panels displaying the baseline parameter into all results figures (Figures 4-7). There was no significant association between cue width and baseline offset in any of the three visual regions.

      (6) Outlier: Fig. 5, V2, Amplitude result seems to have a substantial outlier - is there any notable difference in e.g. retinotopy in this participant? 

      One participant indeed has a notably larger median amplitude estimate in V2. Below, we plot the spatial coverage from the pRF data for this participant (022), as well as all other participants.

      Author response image 4.

      Each subplot represents a participant's 2D histogram of included voxels for the 1D spatial profiles; the colors indicate the proportion of voxels that fell within a specific x,y coordinate bin. Note that this visualization only shows x and y estimates and does not take into account size of the pRF. While there is variation across participants in the visual field coverage, the overall similarity of the maps indicates that retinotopy is unlikely to be the explanation. 

      To further explore whether this participant might be an outlier, we additionally looked at behavioral performance, angular error and FWHM parameters as well as the goodness of fit of the model. On all these criteria this participant did not appear to be an outlier. We therefore see no reason to exclude this participant from the analyses.  

      (7) Fig. 4 vs Fig. 5: I understand that Fig. 4 shows results from a single participant, showing variability across blocks, while Fig. 5 shows aggregate results across participants. However, the Angular Error figure shows complementary results - Fig. 4 shows the variability of best-fit angular error, while Fig. 5 shows the average deviation (approximately the width of the error distribution). This makes sense I think, but perhaps the abs(error) for the single participant shown in Fig. 4 should be included in the caption so we can easily compare between figures. 

      That's right: the Figure 4 results show the signed error, whereas the Figure 5 results show the absolute error. We agree that reporting the absolute error values for the example participant would facilitate comparison. Rather than add the values to the text, we have made the example participant’s data visually distinct within Figure 5 for easy comparison.  

      (8) Bias in model fits: the analysis shown in Fig. 6 compares the estimated parameters across amounts of data used to compute attentional modulation profiles for fitting those parameters. If the model-fitting procedure were unbiased, my sense is we would likely see no impact of the number of TRs on the parameters (R^2 should improve, abs(error) should improve, but FWHM, amplitude, baseline, etc should be approximately stable, if noisier). However, instead, it looks like more/less data leads to biased estimates, such that FWHM is biased to be smaller with more noise, and amplitude is biased to be larger. This suggests (to me) that the fit is landing on a spiky function that captures a noise wiggle in the profile. I don't think this is a problem for the primary results across the whole block of 10 TRs, which is the main point of the paper. Indeed, I'm not sure what this figure is really adding, since the single-TR result isn't pursued further (see below). 

      Please refer to our response in the public review, comment 4. 

      (9) 'Dynamics': The paper, starting in the title, claims to get at the 'dynamics' of attention fields. At least to me, that word implies something that changes over time (rather than across trials). Maybe I'm misinterpreting the intent of the authors, but at present, I'm not sure the use of the word is justified. That said, if the authors could analyze the temporal evolution of the attention field through each block of trials at 1- or 2-TR resolution, I think that could be a neat addition to the paper and would support the claim that the study assays dynamic attention fields. 

      We thank the reviewer for giving us a chance to speak more directly to the dynamic aspect of our approach. Here, we specifically use the word “dynamic” to refer to trial-to-trial dynamics.  Importantly, our temporal interval analysis suggests that we can recover information about the attentional field at a relatively fine-grained temporal resolution (a few seconds, or 2 TRs). Following this methodological proof-of-concept to dynamically track the attentional field, we are excited about future work that can more directly investigate the manner in which the attentional field evolves through time, especially in comparison to other methods that first require training on large amounts of data.

      (10) Correction for multiple comparisons across ROIs: it seems that it may be necessary to correct statistical tests for multiple comparisons across each ROI (e.g., Fig. 5 regression tests). If this isn't necessary, the authors should include some justification. I'm not sure this changes any conclusions, but is worth considering. 

      We appreciate the opportunity to explain our reasoning regarding multiple comparisons. We thought it appropriate not to correct as we are not comparing across regions and are not treating tests of V1, V2, and V3 as multiple opportunities to support a common hypothesis. Rather, the presence or absence of an effect in each visual region is a separate question. We would typically perform correction for multiple comparisons to control the familywise error rate when conducting a family of tests addressing a common hypothesis. We have added this to the Methods section (lines 192-195): 

      “No multiple comparison correction was applied, as the different tests for each region are treated as separate questions. However, using a threshold of 0.017 for p-values would correct for comparisons across the three brain regions.”

      However, we are happy to provide corrected results. If we use Bonferroni correction across ROIs (i.e. multiply p-values by three), there are some small changes from significant to only trending towards significance, but these changes don’t affect any core results. The changes that go from significant to trending are:

      Associated with Figure 5 – In V3, the relationship of cue width to amplitude goes from a p-value of 0.017 to 0.051.

      Associated with Figure 6 –

      V1: the effect of cue width on FWHM goes from p = 0.043 to 0.128.

      V2: the effect of TR on both FWHM and R2 goes from p = ~0.02 to ~0.06. 

      V3: the effect of cue width on amplitude goes from p = 0.024 to 0.073.

    1. eLife Assessment

      This important work offers a fresh perspective central to merozoite surface biology and potential implications on vaccine design, challenging the dogma that MSPs are indispensable invasion engines. Although the authors only deleted bp 132-819, the data based on Western blot, IFA, and RNA‐seq provide compelling evidence that while MSP2 is dispensable for growth, it serves as an immune modulator for AMA1. This work will be of particular interest to scientists working on different aspects of Plasmodium biology and vaccinology.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Henshall et al. delete the highly abundant merozoite surface protein PfMSP2 from two Plasmodium falciparum laboratory lines (3D7 and Dd2) using CRISPR-Cas9. Parasites lacking MSP2 replicate and invade red cells normally, opposing the experimental history that suggests MSP2 is essential. Unexpectedly, the knock-outs become more susceptible to several inhibitory antibodies - most strikingly those that target the apical antigen AMA1-while antibodies to other surface or secreted proteins are largely unaffected. Recombinant MSP2 added in vitro can dampen AMA1-antibody binding, supporting a "conformational masking" model. The reported data suggest that MSP2 helps shield key invasion ligands from host antibodies and may itself be a double-edged vaccine target.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors were trying to establish the role of Plasmodium falciparum surface protein 2 in merozoite biology, specifically the process of erythrocyte invasion.

      Strengths:

      The major strengths of the manuscript are in the Plasmodium falciparum genetic and phenotyping approaches. PfMSP2 knockouts are made in two different strains, which is important as it is known that invasion pathways can vary between strains, but is a level of comprehensiveness that is not always delivered in P. falciparum genetic studies. The knockout strains are characterised very thoroughly using multiple different assays, and the authors should be commended for publishing a good deal of negative data, where no phenotype was detected. This is not always done, but is very helpful for the field and reduces the potential for experimental redundancy, i.e., others repeating work that has already been performed but never published. The quality of the writing, referencing, and figures is also generally strong, although a few minor typos and technical comments on presentation have been communicated to the authors.

      Weaknesses:

      There are, however, some areas that are weaker.

      (1) The section describing Laverania and avian Plasmodium MSP2 comparison is a lengthy section and could be told much more concisely for clarity in delivering the key message, i.e., that conservation in distantly related Plasmodium species could indicate an important function. The identification of MSP2-like genes in avian Plasmodium species was highlighted previously in the referenced Escalante paper, so it is not entirely novel, although this paper goes into more detailed characterisation of the extent of conservation. Overall, this section takes up much more space in the manuscript than is merited by the novelty and significance of the findings.

      (2) Characterisation of the knockout strains is generally thorough, though relatively few interactions were followed by live microscopy (Figures 3E-H). A minimum of 30 merozoites were followed in each assay (although the precise number is not specified in the figure or legend), but there are intriguing trends in the data that could potentially have become significant if n was increased.

      (3) The comparative RNAseq data is interesting, but is not followed up to any significant degree. Multiple transcripts are up-regulated in the absence of PfMSP2, but they are largely dismissed because they are genes of unknown function, not previously linked to invasion, or lack an obvious membrane anchor. Having gone to the lengths of exploring potentially compensatory changes in gene expression, it is disappointing not to validate or explore the hits that result.

      (4) Given the abundance of PfMSP2 on the merozoite surface, it would have been interesting to see whether the knockout lines have any noticeable difference in surface composition, as viewed by electron microscopy, although, of course, this experiment relies on access to the appropriate facilities.

      (5) One of the key findings is that deletion of PfMSP2 increases inhibition by some antibodies/nanobodies (some anti-CSS2, some anti-AMA1) but not others (anti-EBA/RH, anti-EBA175, anti-Rh5, anti-TRAMP, some anti-CSS2, some anti-AMA1). The data supporting these changes in inhibition are solid, but the selectivity of the effect (only a few antibodies, and generally those targeting later stages in invasion) is not really discussed in any detail. Do the authors have a hypothesis for this selectivity? The authors make attempts to explore the mechanisms for this antibody-masking (Figure 7), but the data is less solid. Surface Plasmon Resonance was non-conclusive, while an ELISA approach co-incubating MSP2 and anti-AMA1 antibodies to wells coated with AMA1 lacks appropriate controls (eg, including other merozoite proteins in similar experiments).

      Overall, the claim that PfMSP2 is non-essential for in vitro growth is well justified and is an important contribution to the field. The impact of PfMSP2 deletion on antibody inhibition (which is highlighted in the title of the manuscript) and the mechanism behind it is much less definitive, but does open up an interesting area for further investigation, with more work to be done.

    1. eLife Assessment

      This valuable work explores how synaptic activity encodes information during memory tasks. All reviewers agree that the work is of very high quality and that the methodological approach is praiseworthy. Although the experimental data support the possibility that phospholipase diacylglycerol signaling and synaptotagmin 7 (Syt7) dynamically regulate the vesicle pool required for presynaptic release, concerns remain that the central finding of paired-pulse depression at very short intervals may be more likely due to Ca²⁺ channel inactivation rather than vesicle pool depletion. Overall, this is a solid study although the results warrant consideration of alternative interpretations.

    2. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      The central issue for evaluating the overfilling hypothesis is the identity of the mechanism that causes the very potent (>80% when inter pulse is 20 ms), but very quickly reverting (< 50 ms) paired pulse depression (Fig 1G, I). To summarize: the logic for overfilling at local cortical L2/3 synapses depends critically on the premise that probability of release (pv) for docked and fully primed vesicles is already close to 100%. If so, the reasoning goes, the only way to account for the potent short-term enhancement seen when stimulation is extended beyond 2 pulses would be by concluding that the readily releasable pool overfills. However, the conclusion that pv is close to 100% depends on the premise that the quickly reverting depression is caused by exocytosis dependent depletion of release sites, and the evidence for this is not strong in my opinion. Caution is especially reasonable given that similarly quickly reverting depression at Schaffer collateral synapses, which are morphologically similar, was previously shown to NOT depend on exocytosis (Dobrunz and Stevens 1997). Note that the authors of the 1997 study speculated that Ca2+-channel inactivation might be the cause, but did not rule out a wide variety of other types of mechanisms that have been discovered since, including the transient vesicle undocking/re-docking (and subsequent re-priming) reported by Kusick et al (2020), which seems to have the correct timing.

      In an earlier round of review, I suggested raising extracellular Ca2+, to see if this would increase synaptic strength. This is a strong test of the authors' model because there is essentially no room for an increase in synaptic strength. The authors have now done experiments along these lines, but the result is not clear cut. On one hand, the new results suggest an increase in synaptic strength that is not compatible with the authors' model; technically the increase does not reach statistical significance, but, likely, this is only because the data set is small and the variation between experiments is large. Moreover, a more granular analysis of the individual experiments seems to raise more serious problems, even supporting the depletion-independent counter hypothesis to some extent. On the other hand, the increase in synaptic strength that is seen in the newly added experiments does seem to be less at local L2/3 cortical synapses compared to other types of synapses, measured by other groups, which goes in the general direction of supporting the critical premise that pv is unusually high at L2/3 cortical synapses. Overall, I am left wishing that the new data set were larger, and that reversal experiments had been included as explained in the specific points below.

      Specific Points:

      (1) One of the standard methods for distinguishing between depletion-dependent and depletion-independent depression mechanisms is by analyzing failures during paired pulses of minimal stimulation. The current study includes experiments along these lines showing that pv would have to be extremely close to 1 when Ca2+ is 1.25 mM to preserve the authors' model (Section "High double failure rate ..."). Lower values for pv are not compatible with their model because the k1 parameter already had to be pushed a bit beyond boundaries established by other types of experiments. The authors now report a mean increase in synaptic strength of 23% after raising Ca to 2.5 mM. The mean increase is not quite statistically significant, but this is likely because of the small sample size. I extracted a 95% confidence interval of [-4%, +60%] from their numbers, with a 92% probability that the mean value of the increase in the full population is > 5%. I used the 5% value as the greatest increase that the model could bear because 5% implies pv < 0.9 using the equation from Dodge and Rahamimoff referenced in the rebuttal. My conclusion from this is that the mean result, rather than supporting the model, actually undermines it to some extent. It would have likely taken 1 or 2 more experiments to get above the 95% confidence threshold for statistical significance, but this is ultimately an arbitrary cut off.

      (2) The variation between experiments seems to be even more problematic, at least as currently reported. The plot in Figure 3-figure supplement 3 (left) suggests that the variation reflects true variation between synapses, not measurement error. And yet, synaptic strength increased almost 2-fold in 2 of the 8 experiments, which back extrapolates to pv < 0.2. If all of the depression is caused by depletion as assumed, these individuals would exhibit paired pulse facilitation, not depression. And yet, from what I can tell, the individuals depressed, possibly as much as the synapses with low sensitivity to Ca2+, arguing against the critical premise that depression equals depletion, and even arguing - to some extent - for the counter hypothesis that a component of the depression is caused by a mechanism that is independent of depletion. I would strongly recommend adding an additional plot that documents the relationship between the amount of increase in synaptic strength after increasing extracellular Ca2+ and the paired pulse ratio as this seems central.

      (3) Decrease in PPR. The authors recognize that the decrease in the paired-pulse ratio after increasing Ca2+ seems problematic for the overfilling hypothesis by stating: "Although a reduction in PPR is often interpreted as an increase in pv, under conditions where pv is already high, it more likely reflects a slight increase in pocc or in the number of TS vesicles, consistent with the previous estimates (Lin et al., 2025)." I looked quickly, but did not immediately find an explanation in Lin et al 2025 involving an increase in pocc or number of TS vesicles, much less a reason to prefer this over the standard explanation that reduced PPR indicates an increase in pv. The authors should explain why the most straightforward interpretation is not the correct one in this particular case to avoid the appearance of cherry picking explanations to fit the hypothesis.

      (4) The authors concede in the rebuttal that mean pv must be < 0.7, but I couldn't find any mention of this within the manuscript itself, nor any explanation for how the new estimate could be compatible with the value of > 0.99 in the section about failures.

      (5) Although not the main point, comparisons to synapses in other brain regions reported in other studies might not be accurate without directly matching experiments. As it is, 2 of 8 synapses got weaker instead of stronger, hinting at possible rundown, but this cannot be assessed because reversibility was not evaluated. In addition, comparing axons with and without channel rhodopsins might be problematic because the channel rhodopsins might widen action potentials.

      (6) Perhaps authors could double check with Schotten et al about whether PDBu does/does not decrease the latency between osmotic shock and transmitter release. This might be an interesting discrepancy, but my understanding is that Schotten et al didn't acquire information about latency because of how the experiments were designed.

      (7) The authors state: "These data are difficult to reconcile with a model in which facilitation is mediated by Ca2+-dependent increases in pv." However, I believe that discarding the premise that depression is always caused by depletion would open up wide range of viable possibilities.

    3. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Shin et al. conduct extensive electrophysiological and behavioral experiments to study the mechanisms of short-term synaptic plasticity at excitatory synapses in layer 2/3 of the rat medial prefrontal cortex. The authors interestingly find that short-term facilitation is driven by progressive overfilling of the readily releasable pool, and that this process is mediated by phospholipase C/diacylglycerol signaling and synaptotagmin-7 (Syt7). Specifically, knockdown of Syt7 not only abolishes the refilling rate of vesicles with high fusion probability, but it also impairs the acquisition of trace fear memory.

      Overall, the authors offer novel insight to the field of synaptic plasticity through well-designed experiments that incorporate a range of techniques.

      Comments on revisions:

      The authors have adequately addressed my earlier comments and questions.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      All the comments from Reviewer #2 are the same as her/his comments to our original manuscript. Therefore, we have already responded to all the following comments in the first revision. Here we described our additional responses to the same comments.

      Summary:

      Shin et al aim to identify in a very extensive piece of work a mechanism that contributes to dynamic regulation of synaptic output in the rat cortex at the second time scale. This mechanism is related to a new powerful model and is well versed to test if the pool of SV ready for fusion is dynamically scaled to adjust supply demand aspects. The methods applied are state-of-the-art and both address quantitative aspects with high signal to noise. In addition, the authors examine both excitatory output onto glutamatergic and GABAergic neurons, which provides important information on how general the observed signals are in neural networks. The results are compellingly clear and show that pool regulation may be predominantly responsible. Their results suggests that a regulation of release probability, the alternative contender for regulation, is unlikely to be involved in the observed short term plasticity behavior (but see below). Besides providing a clear analysis of the underlying physiology, they test two molecular contenders for the observed mechanism by showing that loss of Synaptotagmin7 function and the role of the Ca dependent phospholipase activity seems critical for the short term plasticity behavior. The authors go on to test the in vivo role of the mechanism by modulating Syt7 function and examining working memory tasks as well as overall changes in network activity using immediate early gene activity. Finally, they model their data, providing strong support for their interpretation of TS pool occupancy regulation.

      Strengths:

      This is a very thorough study, addressing the research question from many different angles and the experimental execution is superb. The impact of the work is high, as it applies recent models of short term plasticity behavior to in vivo circuits further providing insights how synapses provide dynamic control to enable working memory related behavior through non-permanent changes in synaptic output.

      Weaknesses:

      While this work is carefully examined and the results are presented and discussed in a detailed manner, the reviewer is still not fully convinced that regulation of release probability is not a putative contributor to the observed behavior. No additional work is needed, but in the moment, I am not convinced that changes in release probability are not in play. One solution may be to extend the discussion of changes in rules probability as an alternative.

      As the Reviewer #3 suggested, we examined the dependence of EPSC amplitude on extracellular [Ca<sup>2+</sup>] ([Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>o</sub>) in order to test our assertion that vesicular release probability (p<sub>v</sub>) is already saturated in resting conditions at L2/3 recurrent synapses. A three-fold increase is expected according to Dodge and Rahamimoff (1967), if resting p<sub>v</sub> has enough room to increase, when [Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>o</sub> is elevated from 1.3 to 2.5 mM. We found an increase in the baseline EPSC amplitude only by 23%, and this change was not statistically significant, supporting our assertion.

      Fig 3. I am confused about the interpretation of the Mean Variance analysis outcome. Since the data points follow the curve during induction of short term plasticity, doesn't these suggests that release probability and not the pool size increases?

      We separated the conventional release probability into a multiplication of p<sub>v</sub> and p<sub>occ</sub>, in which p<sub>v</sub> = probability of TS vesicles and p<sub>occ</sub> = occupancy of release sites by TS vesicles. In this regard, the abscissa of V-M plot represents the conventional release probability. Because p<sub>v</sub> is close to unity, we interpreted a change along the abscissa as a change of p<sub>occ</sub>.

      Related, to measure the absolute release probability and failure rate using the optogenetic stimulation technique is not trivial as the experimental paradigm bias the experiment to a given output strength, and therefore a change in release probability cannot be excluded.

      We agree to this concern. Because EPSC data were obtained by optogenetic stimulation, it cannot be ruled out a possibility that optogenetic stimulation biased the release probability. Although we found that STP obtained by dual patch experiment was not different from that by optogenetic stimulation, it needs to confirm our conclusion using dual patch or other methods.

      Fig. 4B interprets the phorbol ester stimulation to be the result of pool overfilling, however, phorbol ester stimulation has also been shown to increase release probability without changing the size of the readily releasable pool. The high frequency of stimulation may occlude an increased paired pulse depression in presence of OAG, that others have interpreted in mammalian synapses as an increase in release probability.

      Provided that pv of TS vesicles is very high, the OAG-induced increase in EPSC1 and low STF and PTA are consistent with higher baseline p<sub>occ</sub> in PDBu conditions, while the number of docking sites is limited. It should be noted that previous PDBu-induced invariance of the RRP size is based on measuring the RRP size using hypertonic solution (Basu et al., 2007). Given that this sucrose method releases not only TS but also LS vesicles, the sucrose-based RRP size may not be affected by PDBu or OAG at L2/3 synapses too. Therefore, PDBu or OAG-induced increase in p<sub>occ</sub> (proportion of TS vesicles over LS+TS vesicles) would result in an increase in release probability without a change in the RRP size.

      The literature on Syt7 function is still quite controversial. An observation in the literature that loss of Syt7 function in the fly synapse leads to an increase of release probability. Thus the observed changes in short term plasticity characteristics in the Syt7 KD experiments may contain a release probability component. Can the authors really exclude this possibility? Figure 5 shows for the Syt7 KD group a very prominent depression of the EPSC/IPSC with the second stimulus, particularly for the short interpulse intervals, usually a strong sign of increased release probability, as lack of pool refilling can unlikely explain the strong drop in synaptic output.

      Comments on revisions:

      I am satisfied with the reply of the authors and I do not have any further points of concern.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      The results are consistent with the main claim that facilitation is caused by overfilling a readily releasable pool, but alternative interpretations continue to seem more likely, especially when the current results are taken together with previous studies. Key doubts could be resolved with a single straightforward experiment (see below).

      The central issue is the interpretation of paired pulse depression that occurs when the interval between action potentials is 25 ms, but not when 50. To summarize: a similar phenomenon was observed at Schaffer collateral synapses (Dobrunz and Stevens, 1997), but was interpreted as evidence for a decrease in pv. Ca2+-channel inactivation was proposed as the mechanism, but this was not proven. The key point for evaluating the current study is that Dobrunz and Stevens specifically ruled out the kind of decrease in pocc that is the keystone premise of the current study because the depression occurred independently of whether or not the first action potential elicited exocytosis. Of course, the mechanism might be different at layer 2/3 cortical synapses. But, it seems reasonable to hope that the older hypothesis would be ruled out for the cortical synapses before concluding that the new hypothesis must be correct.

      The old and new hypotheses could be distinguished from each other cleanly with a straightforward experiment. Most/maybe all central synapses strengthen a great amount when extracellular Ca2+ is increased from 1.3 to 2 mM, even when intracellular Ca2+ is buffered with EGTA. According to the authors' model, this is only possible when pv is low, and so could not occur at synapses between layer 2/3 neurons. Because of this, confirmation that increasing extracellular Ca2+ does not change synaptic strength would support the hypothesis that baseline pv is high, as the authors claim, and the support would be impressive because large changes have been seen at every other type of synapse where this has been studied (to my knowledge at least). In contrast, the Ca2+ imaging experiment that has been added to the new version of the manuscript does not address the central issue because a wide range of mechanisms could, in principle, decrease release without involving prior exocytosis or altering bulk Ca2+ signals, including: a small decrease in nano-domain Ca2+, which wouldn't be detected because nano-domains contribute a minuscule amount to the bulk signal during Ca2+-imaging; or even very fast activity-dependent undocking of synaptic vesicles, which was reported in the same Kusick et al, 2020 study that is central to the LS/TS terminology adopted by the authors.

      Additional points:

      (1) A new section in the Discussion (lines 458-475) suggests that previous techniques employed to show that augmentation and facilitation are caused by increases in pv did not have the resolution to distinguish between pv and pocc, but this is misleading. The confusion might be because the terminology has changed, but this is all the more reason to clarify this section. The previous evidence for increases in pv - and against increases in pocc - is as follows: The residual Ca2+ that drives augmentation decreases the latency between the onset of hypertonic solution and onset of the postsynaptic response by about 150 ms, which is large compared to the rise time of the response. The decrease indicates that the residual Ca2+ drives a decrease in the energy barrier that must be overcome before readily releasable vesicles can undergo exocytosis, which is precisely the type of mechanism that would enhance pv. In contrast, an increase in pocc could change the rise time, but not the latency. There is a small change in the rise time, but this could be caused by changes in either pv or pocc, and one of the studies (Garcia-Perez and Wesseling, 2008) showed that augmentation occluded facilitation, even at times when pocc was reduced by a factor of 3, which would seem to argue against parallel increases in both pv and pocc.

      We greatly appreciate for pointing out our mis-understanding. We acknowledge that the post-tetanic acceleration of the latency in the hypertonicity-induced vesicle release may reflect a decrease in the activation energy barrier (ΔEa) for vesicle fusion resulting in an increase in fusion probability of TS vesicles (Stevens and Wesseling, 1999; Garcia-Perez and Wesseling, 2008). We agree that such latency changes are not easily explained by increases in p<sub>occ</sub> alone. Indeed, Taschenberger et al (2016) concluded that PTP is similar to the PDBu-induced increase in baseline EPSCs. Subsequently, Lin et al (2025) estimated PDBu-induced changes of TS vesicle pool size and p_fusion of TS vesicles (these correspond to p<sub>occ</sub> and p<sub>v</sub> in this study, respectively), and found that PDBu increases majorly the former (2 folds) and minorly the latter (1.3 folds). Although it has not been directly tested, it is possible that PTP increases p<sub>v</sub>. Accordingly, we corrected the first statement of the paragraph, and mentioned the possibility for a post-tetanic increase in p<sub>v</sub> of TS vesicles.

      It should be noted, however, it is still puzzling what is represented by the acceleration of the latency in the hypertonicity-induced vesicle release. Schotten et al (2015) simulated how vesicle release rate is affected by reducing ΔEa for vesicle fusion. They found that a reduction of ΔEa resulted in increases in the peak amplitude and shorter time-to-peak of vesicle fusion, but did not accelerate the latency. Therefore, it remains to be clarified whether shorter latency can be regarded as lower activation barrier.  Moreover, the sucrose-induced release rate is comparable with the vesicle recruitment rate (1-2/s; Neher, Neuron, 2008). This slowness of sucrose-induced vesicle release rate makes it difficult to distinguish the vesicle fusion rate from their priming rate.

      (2) Similar evidence from hypertonic stimulation indicates that Phorbol esters increase pv, but I am not aware of evidence ruling out a parallel increase in pocc.

      As noted above, none of known mechanisms can clearly explain the PDBu-induced shorter latency to hypertonicity-induced vesicle fusion (Schotten et al, 2015). Even if shorter latency reflects higher p<sub>v</sub>, it does not rule out a concurrent change in p<sub>occ</sub>. Supporting this notion, Lin et al. (2025) showed in the framework of the two state vesicle fusion model that PDBu application leads to a substantial increase in the number of TS vesicles (vesicles having high fusion propensity), with a moderate change in fusion probability (p<sub>fusion</sub>). In light of previous observation that high tonicity (500 or 1000 mOsm) did not alter the RRP size (Basu et al., 2007), the results of Lin et al. (2025) can be interpreted as an increase of ‘p<sub>occ</sub>’ in terms of the present study.

      Reference:

      Schotten et al. (2015). Additive effects on the energy barrier for synaptic vesicle fusion cause supralinear effects on the vesicle fusion rate. eLife 4:e05531.

      Lin, K.-H., Ranjan, M., Lipstein, N., Brose, N., Neher, E., & Taschenberger, H. (2025). Number and relative abundance of synaptic vesicles in functionally distinct priming states determine synaptic strength and short-term plasticity. J. Physiology.

      Comments on revisions:

      There are at least two straightforward ways to address the main concern.

      The first would be experiments analogous to those in Dobrunz and Stevens that show that - unlike at Schaffer collateral synapses - paired pulse depression at L2/3 synapses requires neurotransmitter release. I proposed this in the first round, but realized since that a simpler and more powerful strategy would be to test directly that pv is/is-not near 1.0 in 1.2 mM Ca2+ simply by increasing to 2 mM Ca2+ (and showing that synaptic strength does-not/does change). This would be powerful because the increase in Ca2+ greatly increases synaptic strength at Schaffer collaterals by about 2.5-fold. Concerns about a confounding elevation in the basal intracellular Ca2+ concentration could be easily neutralized by pre-treating with EGTA-AM, which the authors have already done for other experiments.

      We thank to Reviewer #3 for suggesting an experiment for testing our assertion that the vesicular release probability (p<sub>v</sub>) is very high at layer 2/3 recurrent excitatory synapses. As the Reviewer recommended, we assessed EPSC changes induced by an increase in extracellular calcium concentration ([Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>o</sub>). The results are added as Figure 3—figure supplement 3 to the revised manuscript.

      Dodge and Rahamimoff (1967) discovered a fourth-power relationship between end-plate potential (EPP) and [Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>o</sub> at a neuromuscular junction. More specifically they found

      EPP amplitude µ  ([Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>o</sub> / (1 + [Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>o</sub> /1.1 mM + [Ma<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>o</sub> /2.97 mM))<sup>4</sup>.

      This equation nicely predicts the effects of high external calcium on EPSC amplitudes observed at the calyx synapses: a 2.6-fold increase of EPSC by changing [Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>o</sub> from 1.25 to 2 mM  (Thanawala and Regehr, 2013; predicted as 2.57);  a 2.36-fold increase by changing [Ca<sup>2+</sup>] from 1.5 to 2 mM (Lin and Taschenberger, 2025; predicted as 2.16). In the framework of two-step priming model, Lin et al. (2015) estimated a 1.9-fold increase (from 0.22 to 0.42) in p<sub>v</sub> of TS vesicles and a 1.23-fold increase in the number of TS vesicles. It is clear that the increase in p<sub>v</sub> would be possible only if p<sub>v</sub> is not saturated, while the increase in the number of TS vesicles is still possible regardless of baseline p<sub>v</sub> of TS vesicles.

      The Dodge and Rahamimoff’s equation predicts a 3.24-fold increase in baseline EPSC amplitude by elevating [Ca Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>o</sub> from 1.3 mM to 2.5 mM at L2/3 synapses. Contrary to this prediction, our recordings revealed a 1.23 fold increase in baseline EPSC amplitude, and this change was not statistically significant.

      Given the steep dependence of vesicle release on [Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>o</sub>, this minimal increase strongly suggests that p<sub>v</sub> at L2/3 recurrent synapses is already near maximal at rest, limiting the dynamic range for further enhancement through increased calcium influx. Accordingly, we observed a small but statistically significant decrease in the paired-pulse ratio (PPR) at higher [Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>o</sub>. Although this reduction in PPR might be indicative of increased p<sub>v</sub>, it is more consistent with a slight increase in p<sub>occ</sub> rather than a substantive increase in p<sub>v</sub> under the context of very high p<sub>v</sub>. Accordingly, Lin et al. (2025) recently estimated an increase in the TS vesicle subpool size as 1.23-fold by elevating [Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>o</sub> under the framework of the two-step vesicle priming mode. Taken together, these findings suggest that an increase in the number of TS vesicles or p<sub>occ</sub> may contribute to both an increase in baseline EPSC amplitudes and a decrease in PPR.

      Overall, our central claim that baseline p<sub>v</sub> is near maximal at L2/3 recurrent synapses is supported by 1) high baseline PPR; 2) insensitivity to EGTA-AM; 3) high double failure rate; 4) insensitivity to elevating [Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>o</sub>. These data are difficult to reconcile with a model in which facilitation is mediated by Ca<sup>2+</sup>-dependent increases in p<sub>v</sub>. Instead, our results support a mechanism in which facilitation arises from changes in release site occupancy.

      References

      Dodge, F.A., & Rahamimoff, R. (1967). Co-operative action of calcium ions in transmitter release at the neuromuscular junction. J Physiol, 193(2), 419–432. 

      Thanawala, M.S., & Regehr, W.G. (2013). Presynaptic calcium influx controls neurotransmitter release in part by regulating the effective size of the readily releasable pool. J Neurosci, 33(11), 4625–4633.

      Lin, K.-H., Ranjan, M., Lipstein, N., Brose, N., Neher, E., & Taschenberger, H. (2025). Number and relative abundance of synaptic vesicles in functionally distinct priming states determine synaptic strength and short-term plasticity. J. Physiology.

      Neher E, Sakaba T (2008) Multiple Roles of Calcium Ions in the Regulation of Neurotransmitter Release. Neuron 59:861-872.

    1. eLife Assessment

      This important study presents an evaluation of several tools used for detecting Identity-By-Descent (IBD) segments in highly recombining genomes, using simulated data to replicate the high recombination and low marker density of Plasmodium falciparum, the parasite responsible for malaria. The evidence presented by the authors is convincing demonstrating that users should be cautious calling IBD when SNP density is low and recombination rate is high. This study will be of interest to scientists working in the field of genome evolution and infectious diseases

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Authors benchmarked five IBD detection methods (hmmIBD, isoRelate, hap-IBD, phasedIBD, and Refined IBD) in Plasmodium falciparum using simulated and empirical data. Plasmodium falciparum has a mutation rate similar to that of humans but a much higher recombination rate and lower SNP density. Thus, the authors evaluated how recombination rate and marker density affect IBD segment detection. Next, they performed parameter optimization for Plasmodium falciparum and benchmarked the robustness of downstream analyses (selection detection and Ne inference) using IBD segments detected by each method. They also tracked the computational efficiency of these methods. The authors' work is valuable for the tested species, and the analyses presented support their claim that users should be cautious when calling IBD in contexts of low SNP density and high recombination rate.

      Strengths:

      The study design is convincing and well-structured. The authors chose to use P. falciparum, which presents an interesting case due to its high recombination rate and a mutation rate similar to that of humans. The authors note that despite the widespread use of IBD for genomic surveillance, comprehensive evaluation of these methods in high-recombination, low-marker-density contexts has been lacking. Furthermore, they also examined the performance of IBD detection methods developed specifically for P. falciparum, and evaluated it with phased data which broadened the applicability of the work.

      Weaknesses:

      The authors thoughtfully addressed our prior concerns by 1) expanding the simulations; 2) updating figures and methods for clarity; and 3) more clearly framing the broader utility of their benchmarking effort. These updates strengthen the manuscript and make the relevance of their findings beyond Plasmodium falciparum more apparent.

      More specifically:

      The authors added three full replicates per simulation scenario and updated figures to reflect uncertainty at relevant levels, which addresses earlier concerns about reproducibility. The limited number of replicates is due to computational intensity. In the future, broader generalizability and deeper exploration of variance in benchmarking accuracy across parameter space would further strengthen the conclusions/generalizability. The author's also emphasized that, while the study is centered on Plasmodium falciparum, the benchmarking framework, not the parameters, are broadly applicable to other sexually recombining species. Lastly, they extensively updated multiple figures to include simulation models, results from simulation replicates, and improved the figures from the previous version of the manuscript.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Guo et al. benchmarked and optimized methods for detecting Identity-By-Descent (IBD) segments in Plasmodium falciparum (Pf) genomes, which are characterized by high recombination rates and low marker density. Their goal was to address the limitations of existing IBD detection tools, which were primarily developed for human genomes and do not perform well in the genomic context of highly recombinant genomes. They first analysed various existing IBD callers, such as hmmIBD, isoRelate, hap-IBD, phased-IBD, and refinedIBD. They focused on the impact of recombination on the accuracy, which was calculated based on two metrics, the false negative rate and the false positive rate. The results suggest that high recombination rates significantly reduce marker density, leading to higher false negative rates for short IBD segments. This effect compromises the reliability of IBD-based downstream analyses, such as effective population size (Ne) estimation.<br /> They showed that the best tool for IBD detection in Pf is hmmIBD, because it has relatively low FN/FP error rates and is less biased for relatedness estimates. However, this method is less computationally efficient.<br /> Their suggestion is to optimize human-oriented IBD methods and use hmmIBD only for the estimation of Ne.

      Strengths:

      Although I am not an expert on Plasmodium falciparum genetics, I believe the authors have developed a valuable benchmarking framework tailored to the unique genomic characteristics of this species. Their framework enables a thorough evaluation of various IBD detection tools for non-human data, such as high recombination rates and low marker density, addressing a key gap in the field.

      This study provides a comparison of multiple IBD detection methods, including probabilistic approaches (hmmIBD, isoRelate) and IBS-based methods (hap-IBD, Refined IBD, phased IBD). This comprehensive analysis offers researchers valuable guidance on the strengths and limitations of each tool, allowing them to make informed choices based on specific use cases. I think this is important beyond the study of Pf.

      The authors highlight how optimized IBD detection can help identify signals of positive selection, infer effective population size (Ne), and uncover population structure.

      They demonstrate the critical importance of tailoring analytical tools to suit the unique characteristics of a species. Moreover, the authors provide practical recommendations, such as employing hmmIBD for quality-sensitive analyses and fine-tuning parameters for tools originally designed for non-P. falciparum datasets before applying them to malaria research.

      Overall, this study represents a meaningful contribution to both computational biology and malaria genomics, with its findings and recommendations likely to have an impact on the field.

      Weaknesses:

      One weakness of the study is the lack of emphasis on the broader importance of studying Plasmodium falciparum as a critical malaria-causing organism. Malaria remains a significant global health challenge, causing hundreds of thousands of deaths annually.

      While the study provides a thorough technical evaluation of IBD detection methods and their application to Pf, it does not adequately connect these findings to the broader implications for malaria research and control efforts. Additionally, the discussion on malaria and its global impact could have framed the study in a more accessible and compelling way, making the importance of these technical advances clearer to a broader audience, including researchers and policymakers in the fight against malaria. In the revised version, the authors have placed greater emphasis on this aspect, while still maintaining the methodological focus of the paper.

    4. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews: 

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary: 

      Authors benchmarked 5 IBD detection methods (hmmIBD, isoRelate, hap-IBD, phasedIBD, and Refined IBD) in Plasmodium falciparum using simulated and empirical data. Plasmodium falciparum has a mutation rate similar to humans but a much higher recombination rate and lower SNP density. Thus, the authors evaluated how recombination rate and marker density affect IBD segment detection. Next, they performed parameter optimization for Plasmodium falciparum and benchmarked the robustness of downstream analyses (selection detection and Ne inference) using IBD detected by each of the methods. They also tracked the computational efficiency of these methods. The authors work is valuable for the tested species and the analyses presented appear to support their claim that users should be cautious calling IBD when SNP density is low and recombination rate is high. 

      Strengths: 

      The study design was solid. The authors set up their reasoning for using P. falciparum very well. The high recombination rate and similar mutation rate to humans is indeed an interesting case. Further, they chose methods that were developed explicitly for each species. This was a strength of the work, as well as incorporating both simulated and empirical data to support their goal that IBD detection should be benchmarked in P. falciparum

      Weaknesses: 

      The scope of the optimization and application of results from the work are narrow, in that everything is finetuned for Plasmodium. Some of the results were not entirely unexpected for users of any of the tested software that was developed for humans. For example, it is known that Refined IBD is not going to do well with the combination of short IBD segments and low SNP density. Lastly, it appears the authors only did one largescale simulation (there are no reported SDs). 

      We thank the reviewer for highlighting the strengths and weaknesses of the study. 

      First, we would like to highlight that: (1) while we use Plasmodium as a model to investigate the impact of high recombination and low marker density on IBD detection and downstream analyses, our IBD benchmarking framework and strategies are widely applicable to IBD methods development for many sexually recombining species including both Plasmodium and non-Plasmodium species. (2) Although some results are not completely unexpected, such as the impact of low marker density on IBD detection, IBD-based methods have been increasingly used in malaria genomic surveillance research without comprehensive benchmarking for malaria parasites despite the high recombination rate. Due to the lack of benchmarking, researchers use a variety of different IBD callers for malaria research including those that are only benchmarked in human genomes, such as refined-ibd. Our work not only confirmed that low marker density (related to high recombination rate) can affect the accuracy of IBD detection, but also demonstrated the importance of proper parameter optimization and tool prioritization for specific downstream analyses in malaria research. We believe our work significantly contributes to the robustness of IBD segment detection and the enhancement of IBDbased malaria genomic surveillance.

      Second, we agree that there is a lack of clarity regarding simulation replicates and the uncertainty of reported estimates. We have made the following improvements, including (1) running n = 3 full sets of simulations for each analysis purpose, which is in addition to the large sample sizes and chromosomal-level replications already presented in our initial submission, and (2) updating data and figures to reflect the uncertainty at relevant levels (segment level, genome-pair level or simulation set level).   

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary: 

      Guo et al. benchmarked and optimized methods for detecting Identity-By-Descent (IBD) segments in Plasmodium falciparum (Pf) genomes, which are characterized by high recombination rates and low marker density. Their goal was to address the limitations of existing IBD detection tools, which were primarily developed for human genomes and do not perform well in the genomic context of highly recombinant genomes. They first analysed various existing IBD callers, such as hmmIBD, isoRelate, hap-IBD, phased-IBD, refinedIBD. They focused on the impact of recombination on the accuracy, which was calculated based on two metrics, the false negative rate and the false positive rate. The results suggest that high recombination rates significantly reduce marker density, leading to higher false negative rates for short IBD segments. This effect compromises the reliability of IBD-based downstream analyses, such as effective population size (Ne) estimation. They showed that the best tool for IBD detection in Pf is hmmIBD, because it has relatively low FN/FP error rates and is less biased for relatedness estimates. However, this method is less computationally efficient. Their suggestion is to optimize human-oriented IBD methods and use hmmIBD only for the estimation of Ne. 

      Strengths: 

      Although I am not an expert on Plasmodium falciparum genetics, I believe the authors have developed a valuable benchmarking framework tailored to the unique genomic characteristics of this species. Their framework enables a thorough evaluation of various IBD detection tools for non-human data, such as high recombination rates and low marker density, addressing a key gap in the field. This study provides a

      comparison of multiple IBD detection methods, including probabilistic approaches (hmmIBD, isoRelate) and IBS-based methods (hap-IBD, Refined IBD, phased IBD). This comprehensive analysis offers researchers valuable guidance on the strengths and limitations of each tool, allowing them to make informed choices based on specific use cases. I think this is important beyond the study of Pf. The authors highlight how optimized IBD detection can help identify signals of positive selection, infer effective population size (Ne), and uncover population structure. They demonstrate the critical importance of tailoring analytical tools to suit the unique characteristics of a species. Moreover, the authors provide practical recommendations, such as employing hmmIBD for quality-sensitive analyses and fine-tuning parameters for tools originally designed for non-P. falciparum datasets before applying them to malaria research. 

      Overall, this study represents a meaningful contribution to both computational biology and malaria genomics, with its findings and recommendations likely to have an impact on the field. 

      Weaknesses: 

      One weakness of the study is the lack of emphasis on the broader importance of studying Plasmodium falciparum as a critical malaria-causing organism. Malaria remains a significant global health challenge, causing hundreds of thousands of deaths annually. The authors could have introduced better the topic, even though I understand this is a methodological paper. While the study provides a thorough technical evaluation of IBD detection methods and their application to Pf, it does not adequately connect these findings to the broader implications for malaria research and control efforts. Additionally, the discussion on malaria and its global impact could have framed the study in a more accessible and compelling way, making the importance of these technical advances clearer to a broader audience, including researchers and policymakers in the fight against malaria. 

      We thank the reviewer for highlighting the need to better contextualize the work and emphasize its relevance to malaria control and elimination efforts. We have edited the introduction and discussion sections to highlight the importance of studying Plasmodium as malaria-causing organisms and why IBD-based analysis is important to malaria researchers and policymakers. We believe the changes will better emphasize the public health relevance of the work and improve clarity for a general audience.  

      We would like to clarify that we are not recommending that researchers “optimize human-oriented IBD methods and use hmmIBD only for the estimation of Ne.” We recommended hmmIBD for Ne analysis; however, hmmIBD can be utilized for other applications, including population structure and selection detection. Thus, we generally recommend using hmmIBD for Plasmodium when phased genotypes are available. To avoid potential misunderstandings, we have revised relevant sentences in the abstract, introduction, and discussion. One reason to consider human-oriented IBD detection methods in Plasmodium research is that hmmIBD currently has limitations in handling large genomic datasets. Our ongoing research focuses on improving hmmIBD to reduce its computational runtime, making it scalable for large Plasmodium wholegenome sequence datasets.

      Recommendations for the authors

      Reviewer #1:

      (1) Additional experiments 

      (i) More simulation replicates would be valuable here. The way that results are presented, it appears as though there are no replicates. Apologies if I am incorrect, but when looking through the authors code the --num_reps defaults to one simulation and there are no SDs reported for any figure. Perhaps the authors are bypass replicates by taking a random sample of lineages? Some clarification here would be great. 

      We agree with the reviewer’s constructive suggestions. We have increased the number of simulation sets to (n = 3) in addition to the existing replicates at the chromosomal level. We did not use a larger n for full sets of simulation replicates for two reasons: (1) full replication is quite computationally intensive (n=3 simulation sets already require a week to run on our computer cluster with hundreds of CPU cores). (2), the results from different simulation sets are highly consistent with each other, likely due to our large sample size (n= 1000 haploid genomes for each parameter combination).  The consistency across simulation sets can be exemplified by the following figures (Author response image 1 and 2) based on simulation sets different from Figures and Supplementary Figures included in the manuscript. 

      Author response image 1.

      Additional simulation sets repeating experiments shown in Fig 2.

      Author response image 2.

      Post-optimization Ne estimates based on three independent simulation sets (Fig 5 shows data simulation set 1).

      In our updated figures, we address the uncertainty of measurements as follows:

      (1) For IBD accuracy based on overlapping IBD segments, we present the mean ± standard deviation (SD) at the segment level (IBD segment false positives and false negatives for each length bin) or genome-pair level (IBD error rates at the genome-wide level). Figures in the revised manuscript show results from one of the three simulation set replicates. The SD of IBD segment accuracy is included in all relevant figures. In the S2 Data file, we chose not to show SDs to avoid text overcrowding in the heatmaps; however, a detailed version, including SD plotting on the heatmap and across three simulation set replicates, is available on our GitHub repository at https://github.com/bguo068/bmibdcaller_simulations/tree/main/simulations/ext_data

      (2) For IBD-based genetic relatedness, the uncertainty is depicted in scatterplots.

      (3) For IBD-based selection signal scans, we provide the mean ± SD of the number of true selection signals and false selection signals. The SD is calculated at the simulation set level (n=3). 

      (4) For IBD network community detection, the mean ± SD of the adjusted Rand index is reported at the simulation set level (n=3). A representative simulation set is randomly chosen for visualization purposes.

      (5) For IBD-based Ne estimates, each simulation set provides confidence intervals via bootstrapping. We found Ne estimates across n=3 simulation sets to be highly consistent and decided to display Ne from one of the simulation sets.

      (6) For the measurement of computational efficiency and memory usage, the mean ± SD was calculated across chromosomes from the same simulation sets.

      We have included a paragraph titled "Replications and Uncertainty of Measures" in the methods section to clarify simulation replications. Additionally, a table of simulation replicates is provided in the new S1 Data file under the sheet named “02_simulation_replicates.”

      (ii) I might also recommend a table or illustrative figure with all the simulation parameters for the readers rather than them having to go to and through a previous paper to get a sense of the tested parameters. 

      We have now generated tables containing full lists of simulation/IBD calling parameters. We have organized the tables into two sections: simulation parameters and IBD calling parameters. For the simulations, we are using three demographic models: the single-population (SP) model, the multiple-population (MP) model, and the human population demography in the UK (UK) model, each with different sets of parameters. Parameters and their values are listed separately for each demographic model (SP, MP and UK). For the IBD calling, we have five different IBD callers, each with different parameters. We have provided lists of the parameters and their values separately for each caller. In total, there are 15 different combinations of 3 demographic models in simulation and five callers in IBD detection (Author response image 3). We provide a table for each of the 15 combinations. We also provide a single large table by concatenating all 15 tables. In the combined table, demographic model-specific or IBD caller-specific parameters are displayed in their own columns, with NA values (empty cells) appearing in rows where these parameters are not applied (see S2 Data file).

      Author response image 3.

      Schematic of combined parameters from simulations and IBD detection (also included in the S2 Data file)

      (2) Recommendations for improving the writing and presentation 

      Overall, the writing was great, especially the introduction. 

      Three thoughts: 

      (i) It would be great if the authors included a few sentences with guidance on the approach one would take if their organism was not human or P. falciparum

      We have updated our discussion with the following statement: “Beyond Plasmodium parasites, there are many other high-recombining organisms such as Apicomplexan species like Theileria, insects like Apis mellifera (honeybee), and fungi like Saccharomyces cerevisiae (Baker's yeast). For these species, our optimized parameters may not be directly applicable, but the benchmarking framework established in this study can be utilized to prioritize and optimize IBD detection methods in a context-specific manner.”

      (ii) I think there was a lot of confusion about the simulations as they were presented between the co-reviewer and I. Clarification on whether there were replicates and how sampling of lineages occurred would be helpful for a reader. 

      We have added a paragraph with heading “Replications and uncertainty of measures” under the method section to clarify simulation replicates.  Please also refer to our response above for more details (Reviewer #1 (1) Additional experiments).

      (iii) Maybe we missed it, but could the authors add a sentence or two about why isoRelate performed so poorly (e.g. lines 206-207) considering it was developed for Plasmodium? This result seems important. 

      IsoRelate assumes non-phased genotypes as input; therefore, even if phased genotypes are provided, the HMM model used in isoRelate (distinct from the hmmIBD model) may not utilize them. Below, we present examples of IBD segments between true sets and inferred sets from both isoRelate and hmmIBD, where many small IBD segments identified by tskibd (ground truth) and hmmIBD (inferred) are not detected by isoRelate (inferred), although isoRelate still captures very long IBD segments. These patterns are also illustrated in Fig. 3 and S3 Fig. We acknowledge that isoRelate may outperform other methods in the context of unphased genotypes. However, we chose not to benchmark IBD calling methods using unphased genotypes in simulations, as the results may be significantly influenced by the quality of genotype phasing for all other IBD detection methods. The characterization of deconvolution methods is beyond the scope of this paper. We have added a paragraph in the discussion to reflect the above explanation.

      Author response image 4.

      Example IBD segments inferred by isoRelate and hmmIBD compared to true IBD segments calculated by tskibd.

      (3) Minor corrections to the text and figures 

      Lines 105-110 feel like introduction because the authors are defining IBD and goals of work 

      We have shortened these sentences and retained only relevant information for transition purposes. 

      Line 121-122 The definition of false positive is incorrect, it appears to be the exact text from false negative 

      We apologize for the typo and have corrected the definition, so that  it is consistent with that in the methods section. 

      Lines 177-180 feels more like discussion than results 

      We have removed this sentence for brevity. 

      Figure 1: 

      Remove plot titles from the figure 

      Write out number in a 

      The legend in b overlaps the data so moving that inset to the right would be helpful 

      We have removed the titles from Figure 1. In Figure 1a, we have changed the format of  the y-axis tick labels from scientific notation to integers.  In Figure 1b, we have adjusted the size and location of the legend so that it does not overlap with the data points.

      Figure 2-3 & S4-5: 

      It was hard to tell the difference between [3-4) and [10-18) because the colors and shapes are similar. It might be worth using a different color or shape for one of them? 

      We have changed the color for the [10-18) group so that the two groups are easier to distinguish.

      Figure 3 & S3-5: 

      Biggest suggestion is that when an axis is logged it should not only be mentioned in the caption but also should be shown in the figure as well. 

      We have updated all relevant figures so that the log scale is noted in the figure captions (legends) as well as in the figures (in the x and/or y axis labels).

      Supplementary Figure S2 

      (i) It would be nice to either combine it with the main text Figure 1 (I don't believe it would be overwhelming) or add in the other two methods for comparison 

      We have now plotted data for all five IBD callers in S1 Fig for better comparison. 

      (ii) the legend overlaps the data so relocating it to the top or bottom would be helpful 

      We have moved the legend to the bottom of the figure to avoid overlap with the data.

      Reviewer #2:

      I don't have any major comments on the paper. It is well-written, although perhaps a bit long and repetitive in some sections. Make sure not to repeat the same concepts too many times. 

      We have consolidated and removed several paragraphs to reduce repetition of the same concepts.

      I am not a methodological developer, but it seems you have addressed several challenges regarding IBD detection in P. falciparum. You have also acknowledged the study's caveats, which I agree with. 

      Thank you for the positive comments.

      Minor comments: 

      -In my opinion, the paper would benefit from including the workflow figure in the main text rather than keeping it in the supplementary materials. This would make it more accessible and useful for readers. 

      We have moved the original S1 Fig to be Fig 1 in the main text.

      -Some of the figures (e.g. Fig. 2, 4) should be larger for better clarity and interpretation. 

      We have updated Fig 2 and Fig 4 (now labeled as Figure 3 and 5) to make them larger for improved clarity and interpretation.

      -While the focus on P. falciparum is understandable, it would have been valuable to include examples of other species and discuss the broader implications of the findings for a broader field. 

      We have updated the third-to-last paragraph to discuss implications for other species, such as Apicomplexan species like Theileria, insects like Apis mellifera (honeybee), and fungi like Saccharomyces cerevisiae (Baker's Yeast). We acknowledge that optimal parameters and tool choices may vary among species due to differences in demographic history and evolutionary parameters. However, we emphasize that the methods outlined are adaptable for prioritizing and optimizing IBD detection methods in a context-specific manner across different species.

      -Figure 6 is somewhat confusing and could use clearer labeling or additional explanation to improve comprehension. 

      We have updated the labels and titles in the figure to improve clarity. We also edited the figure caption for better clarity.

      -Although hmmIBD outperformed other tools in accuracy, its computational inefficiency due to single-threaded execution poses a significant challenge for scaling to large datasets. The trade-off between accuracy and computational cost could be discussed in more detail. 

      We have added a paragraph in the discussion section to highlight the trade-off between accuracy and computation cost. We noted that we are developing an adapted tool to enhance the hmmIBD model and significantly reduce the runtime via parallelizing the IBD inference process.

    1. eLife Assessment

      This important study investigates the role of Drp1 in early embryo development. The authors have addressed most of the original comments and the work now presents convincing evidence on how this protein influences mitochondrial localization and partitioning during the first embryonic divisions. The research employs the Trim-Away technique to eliminate Drp1 in zygotes, revealing critical insights into mitochondrial clustering, spindle formation, and embryonic development.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Gekko, Nomura et al., show that Drp1 elimination in zygotes using the Trim-Away technique leads to mitochondrial clustering and uneven mitochondrial partitioning during the first embryonic cleavage, resulting in embryonic arrest. They monitor organellar localization and partitioning using specific targeted fluorophores. They also describe the effects of mitochondrial clustering in spindle formation and the detrimental effect of uneven mitochondrial partitioning to daughter cells.

      Strengths:

      The authors have gathered solid evidence for the uneven segregation of mitochondria upon Drp1 depletion through different means: mitochondrial labelling, ATP labelling and mtDNA copy number assessment in each daughter cell. Authors have also characterised the defects in cleavage mitotic spindles upon Drp1 loss

      Weaknesses:

      This study convincingly describes the phenotype seen upon Drp1 loss. Further studies should be conducted to elucidate the mechanism by which Drp1 ensures even mitochondrial partitioning during the first embryonic cleavage.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Gekko et al investigate the impact of perturbing mitochondrial during early embryo development, through modulation of the mitochondrial fission protein Drp1 using Trim-Away technology. They aimed to validate a role for mitochondrial dynamics in modulating chromosomal segregation, mitochondrial inheritance and embryo development and achieve this through the examination of mitochondrial and endoplasmic reticulum distribution, as well as actin filament involvement, using targeted plasmids, molecular probes and TEM in pronuclear stage embryos through the first cleavages divisions. Drp1 deletion perturbed mitochondrial distribution, leading to asymmetric partitioning of mitochondria to the 2-cell stage embryo, prevented appropriate chromosomal segregation and culminated in embryo arrest. Resultant 2-cell embryos displayed altered ATP, mtDNA and calcium levels. Microinjection of Drp1 mRNA partially rescued embryo development. A role for actin filaments in mitochondrial inheritance is described, however the actin-based motor Myo19 does not appear to contribute.

      Overall, this study builds upon their previous work and provides further support for a role of mitochondrial dynamics in mediating chromosomal segregation and mitochondrial inheritance. In particular, Drp1 is required for the redistribution of mitochondria to support symmetric partitioning and ongoing development.

      Strengths:

      The study is well designed, the methods are appropriate and the results are clearly presented. The findings are nicely summarised in a schematic.

      The addition of further quantification, including mitochondrial cluster size, elongation/aspect ratio and ROS, as requested by the reviewers, has provided further evidence for the impact of Drp1 depletion on mitochondrial morphology and function.

      Understanding the role of mitochondria in binucleation and mitochondrial inheritance is of clinical relevance for patients undergoing infertility treatment, particularly those undergoing mitochondrial replacement therapy.

      Weaknesses:

      The only remaining weakness is that the authors have not undertaken additional experiments to clarify any role for mitochondrial transport following Drp1 depletion.

    4. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Why mitochondria are finely maintained in the female germ cell (oocyte), zygotes, and preimplantation embryos? Mitochondrial fusion seems beneficial in somatic cells to compensate for unhealthy mitochondria, for example, mitochondria with mutated mtDNA that potentially defuel the respiratory activity if accumulated above a certain threshold. However, in the germ cells, it may rather increase the risk of transmitting mutated mtDNA to the next generation. Also, finely maintained mitochondria would also be beneficial for efficient removal when damaged, as the authors briefly discussed. Due in part to the limited suitable model, physiological role of mitochondrial fission in embryos were obscure. In this study, authors demonstrated that mitochondrial fission prevents multiple adverse outcomes, especially including the aberrant demixing of parental genome (a clinical phenotype of human embryos) in zygotic stage. Thus, this study would be also of clinical importance that could contribute by proposing a novel mechanism.

      The authors have adequately indicated the limitations at each of the specific points. The revisions the authors made have consolidated their conclusion, thus still, making this study an excellent one.

    5. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The authors have taken into consideration and addressed all my previous comments.

      This referee has one major concern remaining: although the authors have refined their analysis of mitochondrial morphology, my concern regarding the characterization of mitochondria in Drp1-depleted zygotes as "elongated" persists.

      Taking into account this reviewers' comment, the following description has been changed. Line 256-257: “Quantification of the aspect ratio (major axis/minor axis) suggests that mitochondria are significantly elongated in Drp1-depleted embryos" to “The mean aspect ratio (major axis/minor axis) increased slightly from 1.36 in control to 1.66 in Drp1-depleted embryos ."

      (1) The morphological analysis of mitochondria reveals that both axes increase in length. Yet, the aspect ratio it is virtually unchanged, at least in biologically relevant terms, if not statistically.

      - Please calculate and represent mitochondrial aspect ratio as major axis/minor axis in fig 2M.

      - Could the authors also display individual data points in the graphs of Figure 2 K, L and M?

      We have revised the graph display format in accordance with the reviewer's suggestions.

      (2) The authors provide PMID: 25264261 as an example, yet mitochondria in PMID: 35704569 are apparently elongated. Judging by the authors discussion about the differences between these two studies, it would be enriching to comment, in the discussion of the manuscript, on the differences in morphology and to the reason why these might arise

      This referee believes that the unconventional mitochondrial morphology upon fission inhibition, reported here, enhances the relevance of the study and raises questions that could promote novel research lines, if thoroughly discussed in the manuscript.

      Thank you for your insightful suggestion. However, since the latter paper (PMID: 35704569) lacks EM images, it would be difficult to accurately assess the elongation. Thus, we would like to reconsider the mitochondrial morphological changes in zygotes caused by Drp1 deletion levels based on the results of future research.

      Minor

      (1) Labels for the staining used are missing in figure 1-figure supplement 1

      (2) Line 218. Could the intended sentence be:

      "Live imaging of mitochondria (mt-GFP) and chromosomes (H2B-mCherry) in Myo19 depleted zygotes shows symmetric distribution and partitioning of mitochondria during the first embryonic cleavage (Figure 1-figure supplement 2A, 2B; Figure 1-Video 2)."

      (3) Figure 2M: Please calculate and represent mitochondrial aspect ratio as major axis/minor axis.

      (4) Include a label with the experimental condition in figure 1 fig supp 2.

      (5) Line 592: missing reference.

      Thank you for your careful correction. We have corrected all the points the reviewer pointed out in the revised version.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The authors have sufficiently revised the manuscript to accommodate the majority of suggestions provided by myself and the other reviewers. While it would have been useful to see further clarity around mitochondrial transport, the data presented provide valuable insight into the role of a mitochondrial dynamics regulator in mediating the first mitosis event in embryo development.

      We thank again reviewer 2 for the helpful comment. We would like to address the issue of (aggregated) mitochondrial transport, including analysis methods, as a future challenge.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      After reading through the comments of other reviewers, what authors could potentially improve their manuscript had been largely summarized in three following points.

      (1) Authors would better clarify whether a loss of Drp1 contributes to the chromosome segregation defects directly (e.g. checking SAC-like activity) or indirectly (aggregated mitochondria became physically obstacle; maybe in part getting the cytoskeleton involved).

      (2) Although the level of Myo19 may not be so high (given the low level of TRAK2 in oocytes: Lee et al. PNAS 2024, PMID 38917013), authors would better further clarify the effect of Myo19-Trim with timelapse (e.g. EB3-GFP/Mt-DsRed) and EM analysis (detailed mitochondrial architecture).

      (3) Authors would better clarify phenotypic heterogeneity/variety regarding the degree of alteration in mitochondrial morphology/ architecture dependent on the levels of Drp1 loss with detailed quantification of EM images to address why aggregation of mitochondria in Drp1-/- parthenote (possibly, more likely Drp1 protein-free) looks different/weaker than Trim-awayed one. Employment of the parthenotes of Trim-awayed MII oocytes might also complement the further discussion.

      The revised preprinted have addressed all the points described above. Authors have also adequately indicated the limitations at each of the specific points. Revisions authors made have consolidated their conclusion, thus still, making this study an excellent one.The only remaining weakness is that the authors have not undertaken additional experiments to clarify any role for mitochondrial transport following Drp1 depletion.

      We thank again reviewer 3 for the insightful comments. We would like to address the comments you have raised (points that were unclear in this study) as issues for future study.

    1. eLife Assessment

      This valuable manuscript describes cryo-EM structures of archaeal proteasomes that reveal insights into how occupancy of binding pockets on the 20S proteasome regulates proteasome gating. The evidence supporting these claims is convincing, although the extrapolation of these findings to the more complex eukaryotic proteasome may prove challenging. This work will be of high interest to researchers interested in proteasome structure and regulation.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, Chua, Daugherty, and Smith analyze a new set of archaeal 20S proteasomes obtained by cryo-EM that illustrate how the occupancy of the HbYX binding pocket induces gate opening. They do so primarily through a V24Y mutation in the α-subunit. These results are supported by a limited set of mutations in K66 in the α subunit, bringing new emphasis to this unit.

      Strengths:

      The new structure's analysis is comprehensive, occupying the entire manuscript. As such, the scope of this manuscript is very narrow, but the strength of the data are solid, and they offer an interesting and important new piece to the gate-opening literature.

      Weaknesses:

      Extrapolating from the core HbYX activating motif shared by Archae and Eukaryotes to the specific operations of gate opening, which is more elaborate in eukaryotes, may prove challenging.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript by Chuah et al. reports the experimental results that suggest the occupancy of the HbYX pockets suffices for proteasome gate opening. The authors conducted cryo-EM reconstructions of two mutant archaeal proteasomes. The work is technically sound and may be of special interest in the field of structural biology of the proteasomes.

      Strengths:

      Overall, the work incrementally deepens our understanding of the proteasome activation and expands the structural foundation for therapeutic intervention of proteasome function. The evidence presented appears to be well aligned with the existing literature, which adds confidence in the presentation.

      Comments on revisions:

      The authors have addressed all my questions.

    4. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, Chua, Daugherty, and Smith analyze a new set of archaeal 20S proteasomes obtained by cryo-EM that illustrate how the occupancy of the HbYX binding pocket induces gate opening. They do so primarily through a V24Y mutation in the αsubunit. These results are supported by a limited set of mutations in K66 in the α subunit, bringing new emphasis to this unit.

      Strengths:

      The new structure's analysis is comprehensive, occupying the entire manuscript. As such, the scope of this manuscript is very narrow, but the strength of the data is solid, and they offer an interesting and important new piece to the gate-opening literature.

      Weaknesses:

      Major Concerns

      (1) This manuscript rests on one new cryo-EM structure, leading to a single (albeit convincing) experiment demonstrating the importance of occupying the pocket and moving K66. Could a corresponding bulky mutation at K66 not activate the 20S proteasome?

      Thank you for this insightful question. We believe such a mutation would likely not activate the proteasome, and would likely  be detrimental to gate opening. Our previous work (Smith et al., Molecular Cell, 2007), and data presented in this manuscript, demonstrate that a K66A mutation, which removes the side chain, blocks 20S gate opening. Furthermore, our new αV24Y T20S structure reveals that Lys66 forms specific hydrogen bonds with surrounding residues that are crucial for stabilizing the open gate conformation (Fig. 5). An aromatic or bulky hydrophobic mutation at this position would be unable to form these essential hydrogen bonds and would likely disrupt the necessary stabilizing interactions.  

      (2) To emphasize the importance of this work, the authors highlight the importance of gateopening to human 20S proteasomes. However, the key distinctions between these proteasomes are not given sufficient weight.

      (a) As the authors note, the six distinct Rpt C-termini can occupy seven different pickets. However, how these differences would impact activation is not thoroughly discussed.

      We appreciate the reviewer's point regarding the complexities of eukaryotic 26S proteasome activation. While our manuscript discusses some aspects of this, we agree that a detailed mechanistic extrapolation from our archaeal T20S model to the diverse interactions within the human 26S proteasome is challenging. As we elaborate in our response to Reviewer #2 (Recommendation #3), the significant differences in α-ring composition (homoheptameric vs. heteroheptameric) and the multifactorial nature of Rpt C-termini binding make direct, wide-reaching speculations about specific pocket contributions in the eukaryotic system difficult at this stage. Our aim was to focus on the conserved fundamental role of the HbYX hydrophobic pocket itself. 

      (b) With those other sites, the relative importance of various pockets, such as the one controlling the α3 N-terminus, should be discussed more thoroughly as a potential critical difference.

      The reviewer raises an excellent point about the regulation of specific α-subunits, like the α3 N-terminus, which acts as a lynchpin in gating. Understanding its precise regulation in the eukaryotic 26S proteasome is indeed a key goal in the field. However, determining which specific HbYX binding events (e.g., in the α2-α3 pocket, the α3-α4 pocket, or cooperative binding across multiple pockets) control the α3 subunit's conformation is beyond the scope of what our current T20S structural data can definitively inform. The cooperative nature of HbYX binding and its precise allosteric consequences across the heteroheptameric α-ring are complex questions that remain to be fully elucidated in the eukaryotic system. Our study focuses on demonstrating the sufficiency of hydrophobic pocket occupancy for activation in a conserved manner, which we propose is a fundamental aspect of HbYX action. Identifying which of the seven distinct eukaryotic hydrophobic pockets must be engaged for full activation remains an important area for future research.

      (c) These differences can lead to eukaryote 20S gates shifting between closed and open and having a partially opened state. This becomes relevant if the goal is to lead to an activated 20S. It would have been interesting to have archaea 20S with a mix of WT and V24Y α-subunits. However, one might imagine the subclassification problem would be challenging and require an extraordinary number of particles.

      We agree with the reviewer that exploring mixed subunit populations is an interesting idea, particularly given the dynamic and potentially partially open states of eukaryotic proteasomes. We have previously considered co-expressing WT and V24Y α-subunits. However, the interpretation of such experiments would be challenging. With 14 potential sites for mutant incorporation across the two homoheptameric α-rings, a heterogeneous population of proteasomes with varying numbers and arrangements of V24Y subunits would be generated. Correlating any observed changes in activity or structure (e.g. via cryoEM subclassification, would be exceedingly difficult) to specific stoichiometries or arrangements of mutant subunits would be highly complex and likely inconclusive for deriving clear mechanistic insights.

      (d) Furthermore, the conservation of the amino acids around the binding pocket was not addressed. This seems particularly important in the relative contribution of a residue analogous to K66 or V24.

      We apologize for the mislabeled figure title in the previous submission, which may have made this information less accessible. We have now corrected the title for Supplemental Figure S10 (previously S9). This figure presents the sequence alignment showing the conservation of residues in and around the HbYX hydrophobic pocket, including those analogous to T20S αV24, αL21, and αA154. As discussed in the manuscript, key residues that form this pocket, such as those corresponding to and surrounding T20S L21 and A154, are indeed well conserved in human α-subunits. This conservation supports the relevance of our findings to eukaryotic proteasomes.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript by Chuah et al. reports the experimental results that suggest the occupancy of the HbYX pockets suffices for proteasome gate opening. The authors conducted cryo-EM reconstructions of two mutant archaeal proteasomes. The work is technically sound and may be of special interest in the field of structural biology of the proteasomes.

      Strengths:

      Overall, the work incrementally deepens our understanding of the proteasome activation and expands the structural foundation for therapeutic intervention of proteasome function. The evidence presented appears to be well aligned with the existing literature, which adds confidence in the presentation.

      Weaknesses:

      The paper may benefit from some minor revision by making improvements on the figures and necessary quantitative comparative studies.

      We appreciate the reviewers thoughtful critique of our manuscript and have made the requested changes and provided further perspectives mentioned below.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Line 467: Mammalian should be replaced with eukaryotic.

      Done.  

      (2) Figure 1 Caption: The descriptions of the blue and green boxes should be described in panel A's caption rather than waiting until panel C.

      Done.

      (3) Figure 2 A: For greater clarity, the asterisks should be replaced with the numbers H4, H5, and H6.

      Done.

      (4) Figure 7 caption: The panels are misannotated. What is listed as E should become D, and what is listed as F should become E.

      Done.

      (5) The title for Figure S9, "αV24Y T20S validation," is inappropriate. A better title should discuss the sequence conservation of those amino acids. Why is the arrow drawing attention to L21 when the paper is about V24? There should be a corresponding alignment that includes K66.

      Thank you for pointing out the title issue for Figure S10 (previously S9); this has now been corrected to reflect its focus on sequence conservation. The arrow highlighting L21 (and its eukaryotic analogues) is intended to draw attention to a key residue that, along with A154, forms part of the hydrophobic pocket occupied by V24Y. As detailed in the main text and shown in Figures 3C, 3D, and 4G, measurements involving L21 were used to demonstrate the widening of this pocket upon V24Y mutation or ZYA binding.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The authors might consider improving the manuscript by addressing the following minor issues:

      (1) Figure 1: it might be easier for readers to understand what the authors meant to show by superimposing the atomic model of the mutated sidechain with the density map. In this case, the density map could be rendered half-transparent, or it could be represented by mesh.

      We appreciate this suggestion for enhancing Figure 1. While we agree that showing the model fit within the density is valuable, we found that incorporating this directly into the comparative overlay panels of Figure 1 (which already depict multiple aligned density maps) made the figure overly complex and visually detracted from its primary message of comparing overall conformational states. However, we do provide a clear illustration of the model-to-map fit for the αV24Y T20S structure in Supplemental Figure S3, where the atomic model is shown within the transparent map surface. Furthermore, all our maps and models are publicly available, and we encourage interested readers to perform detailed comparisons. We believe this approach balances clarity in the main figure with the provision of detailed validation data.  

      (2) What is the solvent-inaccessible surface area of the mutated side-chain buried by its hydrophobic interaction with the HbYX pockets? How is this buried surface area compared to the solvent-accessible surface area of the HbYX pocket without the mutation?

      We appreciate the idea of another visual to answer the question and provide the reader with a better perception of this pocket in the WT versus V24Y T20S. To address this we added a new Supplemental Figure 7 with surfaces showing this comparison including each separate pocket and an overlay with solid and mesh surfaces. We also added this line to the text: “Moreover, molecular surface representations of the hydrophobic pocket clearly show occupancy by the mutant tyrosine’s side chain (Fig. S7)”.

      (3) Based on the data of the buried surface area of the mutated side-chain (requested above), can the authors make some quantitative comparison with the activated eukaryotic proteasome (either human or yeast 26S) with the alpha-pocket occupied with HbYX motifs from Rpt subunits? How similar are they?

      This is a thoughtful suggestion, and we understand the interest in directly comparing pocket occupancy across systems. While we draw general parallels regarding HbYXdependent activation in the discussion, we believe a direct quantitative extrapolation of specific surface area occupancies from our T20S V24Y mutant to the eukaryotic system would be overly speculative and unlikely to yield further definitive insights into the eukaryotic gate-opening mechanism at this time. The primary reason for this is the significant disparity in complexity between the archaeal T20S and eukaryotic 26S proteasomes. The eukaryotic α-ring is a heteroheptamer, composed of seven distinct αsubunits, which creates seven non-identical inter-subunit pockets. In contrast, our study utilizes the homoheptameric archaeal T20S. Furthermore, eukaryotic 26S proteasome activation involves the intricate binding of multiple C-terminal tails from the six different Rpt ATPase subunits of the 19S regulatory particle. These C-termini include various HbYX motifs as well as non-HbYX tails, and they interact with the diverse α-subunit pockets in a highly complex, multifactorial manner that drives what appears to be an allosteric mechanism for gate regulation.

      Crucially, the precise number of C-termini required for 20S gate-opening in the eukaryotic system, the specific combination of these Rpt C-termini, and even the exact inter-subunit pockets that must be occupied to induce robust gate opening are still areas of active investigation and are not resolved (as discussed in our manuscript). Therefore, attempting to extrapolate nuances, such as the precise degree of hydrophobic pocket occupancy from our single, engineered αV24Y side-chain (which models one specific type of Hb-pocket interaction in a simplified system) to each of the potentially five or more different Rpt Ctermini interactions within the various 20S inter-subunit pockets in the eukaryotic 26S proteasome, would involve too many assumptions and would not provide reliable predictive power to understand mechanism.

      However, regarding the fundamental question of how a hydrophobic group occupies the HbYX pocket in our archaeal model system, we believe Figure 4D provides relevant insight that may address the reviewer's underlying curiosity. This figure carefully illustrates the spatial overlap, showing that the engineered αV24Y side-chain and the hydrophobic 'Z' group of the ZYA HbYX-mimetic occupy the same region within the T20S inter-subunit hydrophobic pocket. This provides a clear visual comparison of this key 'Hb' interaction in our defined and structurally characterized system.

      (4) It may be helpful that at the end of the discussion, the authors make some comments on how the current results might offer insights into the eukaryotic proteasome activation, and on what the limitations of the current study are.

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. We agree that discussing the implications for eukaryotic proteasome activation and the study's limitations is important.

      Insights into Eukaryotic Proteasome Activation:

      We have indeed discussed how our current findings with the αV24Y T20S mutant offer insights into eukaryotic proteasome activation in the Discussion section. To briefly summarize:

      (1) Conservation of the Target Site: Our study highlights that the key residues forming the hydrophobic pocket targeted by the αV24Y mutation (αL21 and αA154 in T20S) are well-conserved in the human 20S α-subunits (as shown in Fig. S9). This suggests that the mechanism of inducing gate opening through occupancy of this specific hydrophobic 'Hb' pocket by an aromatic residue is a plausible strategy for activating eukaryotic proteasomes.

      (2) Relevance of the IT Switch: The αV24Y mutation, by occupying the Hb-pocket, allosterically affects the conserved IT switch, promoting an open-gate conformation. As detailed in our previous work (Chuah et al., Commun. Biol. 2023; Ref. 31 in the current manuscript), this IT switch mechanism is also functionally conserved in most human α-subunits. The current study reinforces that direct manipulation of the Hb-pocket is sufficient to trigger this conserved downstream gating machinery.

      (3) Therapeutic Implications: These findings further pinpoint the HbYX hydrophobic pocket as a specific and promising target for the design of small molecule proteasome activators aimed at human proteasomes.

      While these parallels are informative, we reiterate our caution (as also mentioned in response to comment #3 and in the manuscript regarding direct quantitative extrapolation due to the increased complexity of the heteroheptameric eukaryotic α-ring and the multifactorial nature of Rpt C-termini interactions.

      We also agree that we should add a statement regarding key limitation raised by the reviewer, to our manuscript. Below is the key limitations paragraph that has been added to the penultimate paragraph of the discussion: 

      “While this study provides significant insights, it is important to acknowledge certain limitations. A key limitation stems from using the homoheptameric archaeal T20S as our model. Although this simpler system allows for more reliable dissection of fundamental mechanisms, and core elements like HbYX-induced gate opening are conserved at the intersubunit pocket level, the overall T20S and eukaryotic 20S/26S proteasomes differ significantly in their complexity. Specifically, our engineered αV24Y mutation results in a tyrosine constitutively occupying all seven identical hydrophobic pockets. This contrasts with the eukaryotic proteasome, which possesses seven distinct α-subunit pockets that interact with various Rpt C-termini through dynamic binding. Moreover, the specific Rpt Ctermini interactions—whether acting individually or cooperatively—that are essential to drive gate opening in the eukaryotic system remain incompletely understood. Therefore, while insights from our archaeal system are valuable for understanding general principles, direct comparisons and extrapolations to the intricate allostery and interaction complexities of the eukaryotic 26S proteasome must be made with caution.”

    1. eLife Assessment

      This useful study describes expression profiling by scRNA-seq of thousands of cells of recombinant yeast genotypes from a system that models natural genetic variation. The rigorous new method presented here shows promise for improving the efficiency of genotype-to-phenotype mapping in yeast, providing convincing evidence for its efficacy. This manuscript focuses on overcoming technical challenges with this approach and identifies several new biological insights that build upon the field of genotype-to-phenotype mapping, a central question of interest to geneticists and evolutionary biologists.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      N'Guessan et al have improved the report of their study of expression QTL (eQTL) mapping in yeast using single cells. The authors make use of advances in single cell RNAseq (scRNAseq) in yeast to increase the efficiency with which this type of analysis can be undertaken. Building on prior research led by the senior author that entailed genotyping and fitness profiling of almost 100,000 cells derived from a cross between two yeast strains (BY and RM) they performed scRNAseq on a subset of ~5% (n = 4,489) individual cells. To address the sparsity of genotype data in the expression profiling they used a Hidden Markov Model (HMM) to infer genotypes and then identify the most likely known lineage genotype from the original dataset. To address the relationship between variance in fitness and gene expression the authors partition the variance to investigate the sources of variation. They then perform eQTL mapping and study the relationship between eQTL and fitness QTL identified in the earlier study.

      This paper seeks to address the question of how quantitative trait variation and expression variation are related. scRNAseq represents an appealing approach to eQTL mapping as it is possible to simultaneously genotype individual cells and measure expression in the same cell. As eQTL mapping requires large sample sizes to identify statistical relationships, the use of scRNAseq is likely to dramatically increase the statistical power of such studies. However, there are several technical challenges associated with scRNAseq and the authors' study is focused on addressing those challenges. The authors have successfully demonstrated their stated goal of developing, and illustrating the benefit of, a one-pot scRNA-seq experiment and analysis for eQTL mapping.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      This work describes the single-cell expression profiling of thousands of cells of recombinant genotypes from a model natural-variation system, a cross between two divergent yeast strains.

      I appreciate the addition of lines 282-291, which now makes the authors' point about one advantage of the single-cell technique for eQTL mapping clearly: the authors don't need to normalize for culture-to-culture variation the way standard bulk methods do (e.g. in Albert et al., 2018 for the current yeast cross), and without this normalization, they can integrate analyses of expression with those of estimates of growth behaviors from the abundance of a genotype in the pool. The main question the manuscript addresses with the latter, in Figure 3, is how much variation in growth appears to have nothing to do with expression, for which the answer the authors given is 30%. I agree that this represents a novel finding. The caveats are (1) the particular point will perhaps only be interesting to a small slice of the eQTL research community; (2) the authors provide no statistical controls/error estimate or independent validation of the variance partitioning analysis in Figure 3, and (3) the authors don't seem to use the single-cell growth/fitness estimates for anything else, as Figure 4 uses loci mapped to growth from a previously published, standard culture-by-culture approach.

    4. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Reviewer #2:

      Minor reviews:

      The caveats are (1) the particular point will perhaps only be interesting to a small slice of the eQTL research community; (2) the authors provide no statistical controls/error estimate or independent validation of the variance partitioning analysis in Figure 3, and (3) the authors don't seem to use the single-cell growth/fitness estimates for anything else, as Figure 4 uses loci mapped to growth from a previously published, standard culture-by-culture approach. It would be appropriate for the manuscript to mention these caveats.

      We have added two small mention of these caveats – mainly that the study may not generalize, and that the study does not attempt to try the variance partitioning on other traits or other system where the values of the partitions are better established.

      I also think it is not appropriate for the manuscript to avoid a comparison between the current work and Boocock et al., which reports single-cell eQTL mapping in the same yeast system. I recommend a citation and statement of the similarities and differences between the papers.

      We have added this reference and a clear statement of similarities between the two studies. It was not our intention to avoid this; we had simply not seen that study in the initial submission.

    1. eLife Assessment

      This important study provides convincing evidence that the Kinesin protein family member KIF7 regulates the development of the cerebral cortex and its connectivity and the specificity of Sonic Hedgehog signaling by controlling the details of Gli repressor vs activator functions. This study provides new insights into general aspects of cortical development.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This is an interesting follow-up to a paper published in Human Molecular Genetics reporting novel roles in corticogenesis of the Kif7 motor protein that can regulate the activator as well as the repressor functions of the Gli transcription factors in Shh signalling. This new work investigates how a null mutation in the Kif7 gene affects the formation of corticofugal and thalamocortical axon tracts and the migration of cortical interneurons. It demonstrates that Kif7 null mutant embryos present with ventriculomegaly and heterotopias as observed in patients carrying KIF7 mutations. The Kif7 mutation also disrupts the connectivity between cortex and thalamus and leads to an abnormal projection of thalamocortical axons. Moreover, cortical interneurons show migratory defects that are mirrored in cortical slices treated with the Shh inhibitor cyclopamine suggesting that the Kif7 mutation results in a down-regulation of Shh signalling. Interestingly, these defects are much less severe at later stages of corticogenesis.

      Strengths/weaknesses:

      The findings of this manuscript are clearly presented and are based on detailed analyses. Using a compelling set of experiments, especially the live imaging to monitor interneuron migration, the authors convincingly investigate Kif7's roles and their results support their major claims. The migratory defects in interneurons and the potential role of Shh signalling present novel findings and provide some mechanistic insights but rescue experiments would further support Kif7's role in interneuron migration. Similarly, the mechanism underlying the misprojection which has previously been reported in other cilia mutants remains unexplored. Taken together, this manuscript makes novel contributions to our understanding of the role of primary cilia in forebrain development and to the aetiology of the neural symptons in ciliopathy patients.

      Comments on revisions:

      The authors addressed most of the points I raised in my original review. However, I am not convinced by the figures the authors present on Shh protein expression. The "bright tiny dots" of Shh protein in the cortex are not visible on the images in Figure 7. I wonder whether the authors could present higher magnification and/or black and white images with increased contrast.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study investigates the role of KIF7, a ciliary kinesin involved in the Sonic Hedgehog (SHH) signaling pathway, in cortical development using Kif7 knockout mice. The researchers examined embryonic cortex development (mainly at E14.5), focusing on structural changes and neuronal migration abnormalities.

      Strengths:

      (1) The phenotype observed is interesting, and the findings provide neurodevelopmental insight into some of the symptoms and malformations seen in patients with KIF7 mutations.

      (2) The authors assess several features of cortical development, including structural changes in layers of the developing cortex, connectivity of the cortex with thalamus, as well as migration of cINs from CGE and MGE to cortex.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The Kif7 null does have phenotype differences from individual mutations seen in patients. It would be interesting to add more thoughts about how the null differs from these mutants in ciliary structure and SHH signaling via the cilium.

      (2) The description of altered cortex development at E14.5 is perhaps rather descriptive. It would be useful to assess more closely the changes occurring in different cell types and stages. For this it seems very important to have a time course of cortical development and how the structural organization changes over time. This would be easy to assess with the addition of serial sections from the same mice. It might also be interesting to see how SHH signaling is altered in different cortical cell types over time with a SHH signaling reporter mouse.

      (3) Abnormal neurodevelopmental phenotypes have been widely reported in the absence of other key genes affecting primary cilia function (Willaredt et al., J Neurosci 2008; Guo et al., Nat Commun 2015). It would be interesting to have more discussion of how the Kif7 null phenotype compares to some of these other mutants.

      (4) The authors see alterations in cIN migration to the cortex and observe distinct differences in the pattern of expression of Cxcl12 as well as suggest cell intrinsic differences within cIN in their ability to migrate. The slice culture experiments though make it a little difficult to interpret the cell intrinsic effects on cIN of loss of Kif7, as the differences in Cxcl12 patterns still exist presumably in the slice cultures. It would be useful to assess their motility in an assay where they were isolated, as well as assess transcriptional changes in cINs in vivo lacking KIF7 for expression patterns that may affect motility or other aspects of migration.

      Comments on revisions:

      The authors have made significant and thoughtful responses as well as experimental additions to the authors comments. Their efforts are appreciated and the manuscript is much improved.

    4. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This is an interesting follow-up to a paper published in Human Molecular Genetics reporting novel roles in corticogenesis of the Kif7 motor protein that can regulate the activator as well as the repressor functions of the Gli transcription factors in Shh signalling. This new work investigates how a null mutation in the Kif7 gene affects the formation of corticofugal and thalamocortical axon tracts and the migration of cortical interneurons. It demonstrates that the Kif7 null mutant embryos present with ventriculomegaly and heterotopias as observed in patients carrying KIF7 mutations. The Kif7 mutation also disrupts the connectivity between the cortex and thalamus and leads to an abnormal projection of thalamocortical axons. Moreover, cortical interneurons show migratory defects that are mirrored in cortical slices treated with the Shh inhibitor cyclopamine suggesting that the Kif7 mutation results in a down-regulation of Shh signalling. Interestingly, these defects are much less severe at later stages of corticogenesis.

      Strengths/weaknesses:

      The findings of this manuscript are clearly presented and are based on detailed analyses. Using a compelling set of experiments, especially the live imaging to monitor interneuron migration, the authors convincingly investigate Kif7's roles and their results support their major claims. The migratory defects in interneurons and the potential role of Shh signalling present novel findings and provide some mechanistic insights but rescue experiments would further support Kif7's role in interneuron migration. Similarly, the mechanism underlying the misprojection which has previously been reported in other cilia mutants remains unexplored. Taken together, this manuscript makes novel contributions to our understanding of the role of primary cilia in forebrain development and to the aetiology of neural symptoms in ciliopathy patients.

      We again thank Reviewer 1 for her/his positive assessment of our article. We have addressed several weaknesses identified by the reviewer, supplementing the initial results with new data, and correcting or clarifying the text where necessary. Our detailed responses to the reviewer’s recommendations appear at the end of each comment.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) The authors report remarkable phenotypic changes in E14.5 embryos in the projection patterns of corticofugal/thalamocortical axons and in interneuron migration, but some of those phenotypes appear much less severe at E16.5. This might be indicative of a delay in development. Does the migration of interneurons to more dorsal regions correspond to an extended Cxcl12 expression? Do interneuorons still show migratory defects at E16.5? To address a potential delay, the authors could, if feasible, repeat Tbr2/Tomato and L1 or neurofilament stainings in E18.5 embryos?

      The question of a possible developmental delay in Kif7 -/- embryos is important. To document this topic, we have extended our study initially focused on embryonic stage E14.5 to earlier (E12.5) and later (E16.5, E18.5/P0) developmental stages. We added new data on E12.5 (Fig. 1, Fig. 3, Fig. S4) and E18.5 (Fig. 3, Fig. 4) embryos in the main figures, and considerably extended the data on E16.5 embryos (Fig. 1, Fig. 3). The legends of figures and the text of the result section (p5-p6) have been modified accordingly. We now describe developmental defects in Kif7 -/- embryos, which are not simple developmental delays. The sequences of thalamic axon development and cIN migration are representative of this complexity.

      Thalamic axons: the pioneer projection is misrouted to the amygdala at E14.5 (Fig. 4B) whereas most Kif7 -/- thalamic axons extend to the cortex at E16.5, with a slight delay compared to WT axons (Fig. 4D). At E18.5, the Kif7 -/- thalamo-cortical projection appears rather normal in the rostral forebrain but is drastically reduced in the median and caudal forebrain (Fig. 4E). This strong decrease is confirmed by neurofilament staining performed at E18.5 which identifies a major loss of corticofugal and thalamo-cortical projections in Kif7 -/- brains (Fig. 4F). 

      Migrating cIN: During normal development, CXCL12 maintains cIN in their tangential pathways as they start to colonize the cortical wall (E13.5/E14.5). Then CXCL12 drops in the SVZ (Tiveron et al., 2006; Caronia-Brown and Grove, 2011) allowing wild type cIN to invade the cortical plate (Stumm et al., 2003; Li et al., 2008; Atkins et al., 2023). In Kif7 -/- embryos, CXCL12 is never expressed in the SVZ of the dorsal cortex. Therefore Kif7 -/- cIN migrate radially in the dorsal cortex instead of tangentially. We have improved our text in the result section to clarify this transient defect (p8-9).

      (2) Figure 1D: The overview of the Gsh2 and Tbr2 stainings does not allow us to see details of the PSPB. The lines indicating the position of the PSPB are not helpful either. Higher magnifications are required to see whether there are subtle differences at these boundaries as observed for other cilia mutants.

      We thank the reviewer for her/his question that allowed us to identify a mild default of patterning at the PSB, illustrated by high magnification pictures in the Fig. 1D and described in the result section (p5). This subtle defect of PSB patterning is consistent with previous observations in Kif7 -/- embryos (Putoux et al, 2019) and appears milder than the PSB defect in hypomorphic Gli3 Pdn mutants (PSB shifted dorsally and less well defined as illustrated in Kuschel et al, 2003 and Magnani et al., 2010).

      (3) Figure 3: The authors report an interesting mis-projection of thalamocortical axons towards the amygdala. A very similar pattern has been described in Gli3 hypomorphic Pdn mutants (Magnani et al., 2010), in Rfx3, and in Inpp5e null mutant embryos (Magnani et al., 2015). These papers lend further support that this Kif7 phenotype is Gli3 dependent and should be cited in the manuscript. Moreover, the mechanism(s) underlying this mis-projection remain unexplored. Is this phenotype rescued in the previously reported Kif7/ Gli3D699 double mutants? Is there an abnormal expression of axon guidance molecules?

      We deeply thank the reviewer for drawing our attention to the abnormal projection of thalamic axons to the amygdala described in the Gli3 Pdn mutant and in two ciliary mutants, Rfx3 -/- and Lnpp5e -/-. We cite these two papers (Magnani et al., 2010, 2015) in the revised manuscript (p7). In the Gli3 Pdn mutant, transplantation experiments show that a patterning defect of the ventral telencephalon (VT) underlies the mis-projection of the thalamus to the amygdala (Magnani et al, 2010). In the Rfx3 ciliary mutant, two possible mechanisms are proposed: pre-thalamus patterning defect and ectopic Netrin and Slit1 expression in the VT (Magnani et al, 2015). We do agree that understanding the mechanism of the thalamic misprojection in the Kif7 mutant would be of great interest. However, given the complexity of the putative mechanisms described in the Gli3 Pdn and Rfx3 mutants, we believe that this question deserves further investigation in a future study. Finally, the possibility that the thalamic projection defect observed in Kif7 -/- embryos could be rescued in Kif7/Gli3699 (double mutants in which Gli3R is overexpressed in the dorsal and ventral forebrain) is very unlikely. Our two main arguments are:

      (1) Magnani et al (2015) did not rescue the TCA pathfinding defect in the Rfx3 -/- ciliary mutant when they overexpressed GLI3-R (see TCA description in the Rfx3/ Gli3699 double mutant, last paragraph of the result section). The authors concluded “This finding could be explained by a requirement for Gli activator and not Gli repressor function in VT {ventral telencephalon} patterning and indeed, Gli3 western blots showed that the levels of Gli3R are not altered in the VT of Rfx3 -/- embryos”.

      (2) The GLI3-R/Gli3-FL ratio is decreased in the cortex of the Kif7 -/- embryos (dorsal telencephalon) as expected, whereas it is very low in the MGE of WT embryos (ventral telencephalon) and remains unaltered in the Kif7 -/- embryos (Fig. 2B).  

      Similarly, the analysis of Kif7 -/- cIN migratory defects leads us to conclude that Kif7 ablation impairs Gli activation function rather than Gli repressor function in the VT where cIN are generated.

      (4) Figure 4: The authors should discuss the difference between Tbr2 and Cxcl12 expression which does not extend into the dorsal-most cortical SVZ.

      We observed that the transient CXCL12 expression is lacking in the SVZ of the dorsal cortex of Kif7 -/- embryos at E14.5, in a region where TBR2 cells abnormally reach the cortical surface and intermingle with post-mitotic cells. A sentence in our previous version (lines 233-234) could suggest a link between the abnormal location of TBR2 expressing cells and the lack of CXCL12 expression. Having found no data in the literature to explain the absence of CXCL12 expression in the brain by an abnormal cellular environment or by a defect in transcription factor expression, we do not want to further elaborate on differences and similarities between TBR2 and CXCL12 expression patterns in the Kif7 -/- brain. We have modified our text accordingly in the result section of the revised manuscript (p8-9). 

      (5) Figure 5: The authors convincingly describe migratory defects of interneurons. The treatment with Shh agonist and antagonist provides some mechanistic insights but genetic or pharmacological rescue experiments would lend further support. For example, they could treat Kif7 mutant sections with Shh agonists or analyse Kif7/Gli3D699 double mutants.

      We thank the reviewer for her/his positive assessment of our analysis of the cIN migration. Unfortunately, the rescue experiments proposed by the reviewer should not help to further support our conclusions. First, Kif7 ablation in cIN prevents the processing of any SHH signal in the transcriptional pathway. Second, increasing GLI3R by crossing Kif7 -/- animals with Gli3D699 mice could possibly rescue the alterations of layering in the dorsal cortex where the GLI3R/GLI-FL ratio is strongly decreased and the SHH pathway activated. Such a rescue had been previously described for corpus callosum defects (Putoux et al., 2019). However, because cIN are generated in the ventral forebrain where SHH signaling predominantly activates the formation of GLI-A and where Kif7 ablation does not alter the GLI3 ratio, GLI3R re-introduction in the basal forebrain should rather increase the migratory defects of Kif7 -/- cIN instead of producing a rescue. To further support our conclusion, we analyzed the migratory behavior of Kif7 -/- cIN in a WT cortical environment. The results illustrated in the Fig. 6A and described in page 9 of the result section confirm that the migration defects of Kif7 -/-  cIN are reminiscent of an inhibition but not an activation of the  transcriptional SHH pathway (same phenotype as in Kif3a ciliary mutants described in Baudoin et al, 2012).

      (6) Figure 6: The authors describe the Shh mRNA and protein expression with relevance to interneuron migration. In contrast to the in situ hybridisation, the immunofluorescence analysis is not very convincing and requires further controls. The authors should at least show a no primary antibody control and, if available, could include a staining on Shh mutants. These additional controls are important as Shh protein expression in the developing cortex is highly controversial and a recent paper describes a different pattern (Manuel et al., 2022: https://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/article?id=10.1371/journal.pbio.3001563#). Moreover, it remains unclear whether the Shh protein expression is uniform within the cortex or follows lateral to medial or ventricular to pial gradients. A more thorough description and corresponding figures would be helpful. 

      Manuel et al. (2022) used the SHH KO (generated by Chiang et al., 1996) that develops a long proboscis to validate the rabbit anti-SHH antibodies (from Genetech) used in their study. They show a lack of SHH signal in the SHH KO. However, it is difficult to identify the cortex in this mouse line and the authors did not specify which part of the SHH protein was used to generate antibodies. We wished to use the SHH KO generated by Chiang and backcrossed on a C57B/6 line (Rash and Grove, 2007) that develops a layered neocortex at E17.5. However,

      (1) the SHH KO was obtained by replacing exon2 with a PGK-neo cassette and could express a 101 aa truncated protein comprising the N-ter part of the protein, and

      (2) the antibody we used, is a polyclonal N-ter antibody that targets the active SHH protein (Cys25-Gly198 part of SHH protein used as immunogen to produce the antibody). We thus thought that this labeling experiment will not give information on the specificity of the antibody, some epitopes being able to recognize the truncated protein produced in the SHH KO.

      To overcome the lack of a good mutant mice to validate the SHH N-ter antibodies, we analyzed the SHH immunostaining pattern at E12.5 and compared the expression profile with previously published SHH mRNA expression patterns. The border of the third ventricle and the ZLI were strongly immunostained by SHH-Nter antibodies and these regions were shown to express SHH mRNA at E12.5-E13.5 (Kicker et al. 2004, Loulier et al., 2005, Sahara et al., 2007 and Fig. 7B1). In brain sections at E14.5, only the choroid plexus was strongly labeled and some structures showed diffused labeling. We analyzed the distribution of SHH mRNAs in the cortex using a highly sensitive technique (RNAscop) at E14.5 and showed that very few cortical cells expressed SHH mRNA and at very low level. Anti-SHH-Nter antibodies immunostained numerous bright dots throughout the cortical neuropile, which is not surprising for a diffusible factor like SHH. However, the labeling was not homogeneous and showed a ventricle to pial gradient at E12.5 and aligned distributions in the different cortical layers at E14.5. We have described the expression pattern in more detail and modified the Fig. S4 by adding an image of immunostaining performed without SHH N-ter antibody.  

      (7) Figure S1: The Gli3 Western blot needs to be quantified. As the authors only show one control and one mutant sample, it remains unclear how representative this blot is. In addition to Gli3R and Gli3FL, the authors should also determine the ratio of both isoforms. Are there also differences in the MGE?

      We now produce results of Gli3 western blots in the cortex and MGE of several E14.5 Kif7 KO (n=4) and WT (n=4) embryos. The GLI3R/GLI3FL ratio has been determined in the cortex and in the MGE of WT and mutant embryos. Results are illustrated in the Fig. 2. 

      Minor points:

      The authors should carefully amend the literature on Gli genes and forebrain development. For example:

      (1) Line 85: Add Hasenpusch-Theil et al., 2018.

      We added this reference.

      (2) Line 141: Remove Magnani et al., 2010 (they characterized hypomorphic Gli3 Pdn mutants) and replace with Kuschel et al., 2003.

      Since our revised figure 2 illustrates GLI3 western blots and compare GLI3R/GLI3FL ratios in the cortex and MGE of WT and Kif7-/- embryos, we no longer cite these papers in the result section.

      (3) Line 380: Replace reference with Theil, 2005.

      We have replaced Magnani et al, 2014 by Theil 2005 in the sentence.

      (4) Line 414: Rallu et al is not an appropriate reference for this as this manuscript does not investigate the expression of a single cortical marker in Shh/Gli3 double mutants.

      We removed the reference Rallu et al. in the sentence.

      (5) Reference in line 355: do not use Vancouver style.

      We apologize for the mistake that was corrected.

      (6) Spelling: Line 447 it should read "choroid plexus"

      We again apologize for the mistake that has been corrected.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study investigates the role of KIF7, a ciliary kinesin involved in the Sonic Hedgehog (SHH) signaling pathway, in cortical development using Kif7 knockout mice. The researchers examined embryonic cortex development (mainly at E14.5), focusing on structural changes and neuronal migration abnormalities.

      Strengths:

      (1) The phenotype observed is interesting, and the findings provide neurodevelopmental insight into some of the symptoms and malformations seen in patients with KIF7 mutations.<br /> (2) The authors assess several features of cortical development, including structural changes in layers of the developing cortex, connectivity of the cortex with the thalamus, as well as migration of cINs from CGE and MGE to the cortex.

      We greatly thank Reviewer 2 for her/his positive assessment of our work that characterize the neurodevelopmental defects induced by KIF7 ablation. We have deeply reorganized and implemented data in the figures to show changes occurring in different cortical cell types and at different stages. We have moreover corrected and clarified the text where necessary. Our detailed responses to the reviewer’s recommendations appear at the end of each comment.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The Kif7 null does have phenotype differences from individual mutations seen in patients. It would be interesting to add more thoughts about how the null differs from these mutants in ciliary structure and SHH signaling via the cilium.

      We are grateful to the Reviewer for recalling that Kif7 ablation alters SHH signaling within primary cilium and has a strong effect on ciliary structure. In the revised version of the manuscript, we discuss data from the literature that describe these alterations in human (Putoux et al, 2011) and in murine KIF7 depleted cells (He et al, 2015; Cheung et al., 2009; Lai et al., 2021) (discussion p13).

      (2) The description of altered cortex development at E14.5 is perhaps rather descriptive. It would be useful to assess more closely the changes occurring in different cell types and stages. For this it seems very important to have a time course of cortical development and how the structural organization changes over time. This would be easy to assess with the addition of serial sections from the same. It might also be interesting to see how SHH signaling is altered in different cortical cell types over time with a SHH signaling reporter mouse.

      We thank the Reviewer for her/his request that helped us to improve our description of developmental defaults in the Kif7 -/- cortex.  In the revised manuscript, we have expanded our study initially focused on embryonic stage E14.5 to earlier (E12.5) and later (E16.5, E18.5 /P0) developmental stages. Instead of focusing on median forebrain sections, we have expanded our observations to rostral and caudal sections. Altogether, these new observations allow us to describe more precisely the complex developmental defects in the Kif7 -/- cortex over time, in specific cortical regions (dorsal versus lateral cortex, and rostral versus caudal levels). Figures 1, 3, 4, and S4 have been deeply edited to present new data on E12.5 (Fig. 1, Fig. 3, Fig. S4), E16.5 (Fig. 1, Fig. 3) and E18.5 (Fig. 3, Fig. 4) embryos. We have modified the legends and text in the result section (p5-6) accordingly. We agree with the Reviewer that deciphering how SHH signaling is altered in the different cortical cells over time should be highly interesting and relevant. Nevertheless, we anticipate complex analyses and consider that they should be retained for future studies.

      (3) Abnormal neurodevelopmental phenotypes have been widely reported in the absence of other key genes affecting primary cilia function (Willaredt et al., J Neurosci 2008; Guo et al., Nat Commun 2015). It would be interesting to have more discussion of how the Kif7 null phenotype compares to some of these other mutants.

      We agree with this Reviewer concern. In the revised manuscript, we discuss our results with regard to previous observations in other ciliary mutants. The murine cobblestone mutant described in Willaredt et al. (2008) indeed shows defects similar to those we describe in the Kif7 -/- mouse. We thank again the Reviewer for her/his helpful comment that allowed us to strengthen and better interpret our results. Guo et al (2015) did not conduct a study of ciliary mutants. Nevertheless, their characterization of cortical developmental defects following invalidation of genes involved in human ciliopathies identified cell autonomous defects in cortical progenitors and in differentiating cortical neurons, which corroborate our observations (p.15)

      (4) The authors see alterations in cIN migration to the cortex and observe distinct differences in the pattern of expression of Cxcl12 as well as suggest cell-intrinsic differences within cIN in their ability to migrate. The slice culture experiments though make it a little difficult to interpret the cell intrinsic effects on cIN of loss of Kif7, as the differences in Cxcl12 patterns still exist presumably in the slice cultures. It would be useful to assess their motility in an assay where they were isolated, as well as assess transcriptional changes in cINs in vivo lacking KIF7 for expression patterns that may affect motility or other aspects of migration.

      To circumvent the difference in the expression profile of CXCL12 in the dorsal cortex of WT and Kif7 -/- embryos on the migratory behavior of cIN, we compared the trajectories and dynamics of WT and Kif7 -/- cIN imaged in the lateral cortex where CXCL12 expression appears similar in WT and Kif7 -/- brains.

      We moreover followed the reviewer recommendation and analyzed the migratory behavior of Kif7 -/- cIN that migrate as isolated cells on a dissociated substrate of WT cortical cells. We sincerely thank the reviewer for her/his suggestion as the results revealed an interesting and relevant ciliary phenotype in migrating Kif7 -/- cIN. This additional experiment confirms that Kif7 -/- cIN exhibit the same migratory defects as those initially characterized in the Kif3a -/-  ciliary mutant.  The new results are illustrated in the Fig. 6A and described in the result section (p9). We agree with the reviewer that the analysis of transcriptional changes that could affect Kif7 -/- cIN motility and migration would be very interesting to study, but this study is beyond the scope of the present article.

    1. eLife Assessment

      This manuscript by Li, Lu et al., presents important findings on the role of cDC1 in atherosclerosis and their influence on the adaptive immune system. Using Xcr1Cre-Gfp Rosa26LSL-DTA ApoE-/- mouse models, these data convincingly reveal an unexpected, non-redundant role of the XCL1-XCR1 axis in mediating cDC1 contributions to atherosclerosis.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this study by Li et al., the authors re-investigated the role of cDC1 for atherosclerosis progression using the ApoE model. First, the authors confirmed the accumulation of cDC1 in atherosclerotic lesions in mice and humans. Then, in order to examine the functional relevance of this cell type, the authors developed a new mouse model to selectively target cDC1. Specifically, they inserted the Cre recombinase directly after the start codon of the endogenous XCR1 gene, thereby avoiding off-target activity. Following validation of this model, the authors crossed it with ApoE-deficient mice and found a striking reduction of aortic lesions (numbers and size) following a high-fat diet. The authors further characterized the impact of cDC1 depletion on lesional T cells and their activation state. Also, they provide in-depth transcriptomic analyses of lesional in comparison to splenic and nodal cDC1. These results imply cellular interactions between lesion T cells and cDC1. Finally, the authors show that the chemokine XCL1, which is produced by activated CD8 T cells (and NK cells), plays a key role in the interaction with XCR1-expressing cDC1 and particularly in the atherosclerotic disease progression.

      Strengths:

      The surprising results on XCL1 represent a very important gain in knowledge. The role of cDC1 is clarified with a new genetic mouse model.

      Weaknesses:

      My criticism is limited to the analysis of the scRNAseq data of the cDC1. I think it would be important to match these data with published data sets on cDC1. In particular, the data set by Sophie Janssen's group on splenic cDC1 might be helpful here (PMID: 37172103; https://www.single-cell.be/spleen_cDC_homeostatic_maturation/datasets/cdc1). It would be good to assign a cluster based on the categories used there (early/late, immature/mature, at least for splenic DC).

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      This study investigates the role of cDC1 in atherosclerosis progression using Xcr1Cre-Gfp Rosa26LSL-DTA ApoE-/- mice. The authors demonstrate that selective depletion of cDC1 reduces atherosclerotic lesions in hyperlipidemic mice. While cDC1 depletion did not alter macrophage populations, it suppressed T cell activation (both CD4+ and CD8+ subsets) within aortic plaques. Further, targeting the chemokine Xcl1 (ligand of Xcr1) effectively inhibits atherosclerosis. The manuscript is well-written, and the data are clearly presented. However, several points require clarification:

      (1) In Figure 1C (upper plot), it is not clear what the Xcr1 single-positive region in the aortic root represents, or whether this is caused by unspecific staining. So I wonder whether Xcr1 single-positive staining can reliably represent cDC1. For accurate cDC1 gating in Figure 1E, Xcr1+CD11c+ co-staining should be used instead.

      (2) Figure 4D suggests that cDC1 depletion does not affect CD4+/CD8+ T cells. However, only the proportion of these subsets within total T cells is shown. To fully interpret effects, the authors should provide:<br /> a) Absolute numbers of total T cells in aortas.<br /> b) Absolute counts of CD4+ and CD8+ T cells.

      (3) How does T cell activation mechanistically influence atherosclerosis progression? Why was CD69 selected as the sole activation marker? Were other markers (e.g., KLRG1, ICOS, CD44) examined to confirm activation status?

      (4) Figure 7B: Beyond cDC1/2 proportions within cDCs, please report absolute counts of: Total cDCs,cDC1, and cDC2 subsets. Figure 7D: In addition to CD4+/CD8+ T cell proportions, the following should be included:<br /> a) Total T cell numbers in aortas<br /> b) Absolute counts of CD4+ and CD8+ T cells.

      (5) cDC1 depletion reduced CD69+CD4+ and CD69+CD8+ T cells, whereas Xcl1 depletion decreased Xcr1+ cDC1 cells without altering activated T cells. How do the authors explain these different results? This discrepancy needs explanation.

    1. eLife Assessment

      This valuable study presents an innovative noninvasive immunotherapeutic strategy for hepatocellular carcinoma by combining ultrasound stimulation with calcium-loaded nanodroplets to activate splenic immune responses. The authors provide solid preclinical data, including single-cell transcriptomic analyses and evidence of tumor growth suppression, supported by a creative and well-executed methodology. Further validation of the calcium signaling mechanisms and assessment of long-term safety will strengthen the translational potential of this approach. The work will be of broad interest to researchers in oncology, immunotherapy, and biomedical engineering.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study presents a compelling strategy for ultrasound-mediated immunomodulation in HCC, supported by robust scRNA-Seq data. While the mechanistic depth and translational validation require further refinement, the work significantly advances the field of noninvasive cancer immunotherapy. Addressing the major concerns, particularly regarding calcium signaling specificity and STNDs@Ca²⁺ safety, will strengthen the manuscript's impact.

      Strengths:

      (1) Innovative Approach:<br /> The integration of noninvasive ultrasound with calcium-targeted nanotechnology (STNDs@Ca²⁺) represents a significant advancement in cancer immunotherapy. The dual mechanism - direct immunomodulation via FUS and calcium delivery via nanoparticles - is both novel and promising.

      (2) Comprehensive Mechanistic Insights:<br /> The use of scRNA-seq and flow cytometry provides a detailed map of immune cell dynamics, highlighting key pathways (TNF, NFκB, MAPK) and cellular transitions (e.g., MDSC suppression, CD8⁺/NK cell activation).

      (3) Robust Preclinical Validation:<br /> The study validates findings in two distinct HCC models (H22 and Hepal-6), demonstrating consistent tumor suppression (>70-90%) and prolonged survival, which strengthens translational relevance.

      Weaknesses:

      Major Issues:

      (1) Mechanistic Specificity of Calcium Influx:<br /> While the study attributes immunomodulation to ultrasound-induced calcium influx, the exact mechanism (e.g., involvement of mechanosensitive channels like Piezo1 or TRP families) remains underdiscussed. The qRT-PCR data shows no changes in TRP channels, but the upregulation of Piezo1 warrants deeper exploration.

      Suggestion: The authors should include experiments to inhibit Piezo1 or other calcium channels to confirm their role in FUS-mediated effects.

      (2) STNDs@Ca²⁺ Biodistribution and Safety:<br /> Although biodistribution data show splenic accumulation, potential off-target effects (e.g., liver/lung uptake) and long-term toxicity are not fully addressed. The serum biochemical analysis (Table 2) lacks critical markers like inflammatory cytokines or immune cell counts.

      Suggestion: The authors should provide longitudinal toxicity data (e.g., histopathology beyond 3 hours) and assess systemic immune activation/inflammation.

      (3) Statistical and Technical Clarifications:<br /> The statistical methods for multi-group comparisons (e.g., ANOVA vs. t-test) are inconsistently described. For instance, Figure 1 labels significance without specifying correction for multiple comparisons.

      Suggestion: the authors should clarify statistical methods in figure legends and the Methods section; apply Bonferroni or FDR correction where appropriate.

      (4) Interpretation of scRNA-seq Data:<br /> The clustering of MDSCs using surface markers (Itgam/Ly6c2/Ly6g) overlaps with conventional myeloid populations (Supplementary Figure 16), raising questions about subset specificity.

      Suggestion: The authors should validate MDSC identity using functional assays (e.g., T cell suppression) or additional markers (e.g., Arg1, iNOS).

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Wang et al. studied the therapeutic potential of focused ultrasound noninvasively stimulating the spleen (FUS sti. spleen) to modulate the splenic immunity, with an aim to exert anti-tumor effect. They found that the treatment enhanced antitumor capability in CD8 T/ NK cells and reduced the immunosuppression, facilitating the inhibition of HCC growth in vivo.

      Strengths:

      They have utilized bulk RNA sequencing, single cell RNA sequencing, and flow cytometry to investigate the immune and tumor cell profiling in the mouse models upon FUS sti. spleen. Moreover, they highlighted the importance of combining FUS with spleen-targeted nanodroplets encapsulating bioavailable calcium ions (STND@Ca2+), which facilitated the calcium influx into the murine spleen and further enhanced the therapeutic efficacy of FUS.

      Weaknesses:

      While the study is interesting and potentially clinically impactful, the mechanism of action of the therapy is not fully elucidated. It would benefit from more rigorous approaches. With the theoretical part strengthened, this paper would be of interest to cancer cell biologists and clinician scientists working on the oncology field.

    1. eLife Assessment

      This study presents a valuable finding on the neural representation of time from two distinct egocentric and allocentric reference frames. The presentation of evidence in the version of the original submission is incomplete, as further conceptual clarifications, methodological details, and addressing potential confounds would strengthen the study. The work will be of interest to cognitive neuroscientists working on the perception and memory of time.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this fMRI study, the authors wished to assess neural mechanisms supporting flexible "temporal construals". For this, human participants learned a story consisting of fifteen events. During fMRI, events were shown to them and they were instructed to consider the event from "an internal" or from "an external" perspective. The authors found opposite patterns of brain activity in the posterior parietal cortex and the anterior hippocampus for the internal and the external viewpoint. They conclude that allocentric sequences are stored in the hippocampus, whereas egocentric sequences are used in the parietal cortex. The claims align with previous fMRI work addressing this question.

      Strengths:

      The research topic is fascinating, and very few labs in the world are asking the question of how time is represented in the human brain. Working hypotheses have been recently formulated, and this work seems to want to tackle some of them.

      Weaknesses:

      The current writing is fuzzy both conceptually and experimentally. I cannot provide a sufficiently well-informed assessment of the quality of the experimental work because there is a paucity of details provided in the report. Any future revisions will likely improve transparency.

      (1) Improving writing and presentation:

      The abstract and the introduction make use of loaded terms such as "construals", "mental timeline", "panoramic views" in very metaphoric and unexplained ways. The authors do not provide a comprehensive and scholarly overview of these terms, which results in verbiage and keywords/name-dropping without a clear general framework being presented. Some of these terms are not metaphors. They do refer to computational concepts that the authors should didactically explain to their readership. This is all the more important that some statements in the Introduction are misattributed or factually incorrect; some statements lack attributions (uncited published work).

      Once the theory, the question, and the working hypothesis are clarified, the authors should carefully explain the task.

      (2) The experimental approach lacks sufficient details to be comprehensible to a general audience. In my opinion, the results are thus currently uninterpretable. I highlight only a couple of specific points (out of many). I recommend revision and clarification.

      a) No explanation of the narrative is being provided. The authors report a distribution of durations with no clear description of the actual sequence of events. The authors should provide the text that was used, how they controlled for low-level and high-level linguistic confounds.

      b) The authors state, "we randomly assigned 15 phrases to the events twice". It is impossible to comprehend what this means. Were these considered stimuli? Controls? IT is also not clear which event or stimulus is part of the "learning set" and whether these were indicated to be such to participants.

      c) The left/right counterbalancing is not being clearly explained. The authors state that there is counterbalancing, but do not sufficiently explain what it means concretely in the experiment. If a weak correlation exists between sequential position and distance, it also means that the position and the distance have not been equated within. How do the authors control for these?

      d) The authors used two tasks. In the "external perspective" one, the authors asked participants to report whether events were part of the same or a different part of the day. In the "internal perspective one", the authors asked participants to project themselves to the reference event and to determine whether the target event occurred before or after the projected viewpoint. The first task is a same/different recognition task. The second task is a temporal order task (e.g., Arzy et al. 2009). These two asks are radically different and do not require the same operationalization. The authors should minimally provide a comprehensive comparison of task requirements, their operationalization, and, more importantly, assess the behavioral biases inherent to each of these tasks that may confound brain activity observed with fMRI.

      e) The authors systematically report interpreted results, not factual data. For instance, while not showing the results on behavioral outcomes, the authors directly interpret them as symbolic distance effects.

      Crucially, the authors do not comment on the obvious differences in task difficulty in these two tasks, which demonstrates a substantial lack of control in the experimental design. The same/different task (task 1 called "external perspective") comes with known biases in psychophysics that are not present in the temporal order task (task 2 called " internal perspective"). The authors also did not discuss or try to match the performance level in these two tasks. Accordingly, the authors claim that participants had greater accuracy in the external (same/different) task than in the internal task, although no data are shown and provided to support this report. Further, the behavioral effect is trivialized by the report of a performance accuracy trade-off that further illustrates that there is a difference in the task requirements, preventing accurate comparison of the two tasks.

      All fMRI contrasts are also confounded by this experimental shortcoming, seeing as they are all reported at the interaction level across a task. For instance, in Figure 4, the authors report a significant beta difference between internal and external tasks. It is impossible to disentangle whether this effect is simply due to task difference or to an actual processing of the duration that differs across tasks, or to the nature of the representation (the most difficult to tackle, and the one chosen by the authors).

      Conclusion:

      In conclusion, the current experimental work is confounded and lacks controls. Any behavioral or fMRI contrasts between the two proposed tasks can be parsimoniously accounted for by difficulty or attentional differences, not the claim of representational differences being argued for here.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Xu et al. used fMRI to examine the neural correlates associated with retrieving temporal information from an external compared to internal perspective ('mental time watching' vs. 'mental time travel'). Participants first learned a fictional religious ritual composed of 15 sequential events of varying durations. They were then scanned while they either (1) judged whether a target event happened in the same part of the day as a reference event (external condition); or (2) imagined themselves carrying out the reference event and judged whether the target event occurred in the past or will occur in the future (internal condition). Behavioural data suggested that the perspective manipulation was successful: RT was positively correlated with sequential distance in the external perspective task, while a negative correlation was observed between RT and sequential distance for the internal perspective task. Neurally, the two tasks activated different regions, with the external task associated with greater activity in the supplementary motor area and supramarginal gyrus, and the internal condition with greater activity in default mode network regions. Of particular interest, only a cluster in the posterior parietal cortex demonstrated a significant interaction between perspective and sequential distance, with increased activity in this region for longer sequential distances in the external task, but increased activity for shorter sequential distances in the internal task. Only a main effect of sequential distance was observed in the hippocampus head, with activity being positively correlated with sequential distance in both tasks. No regions exhibited a significant interaction between perspective and duration, although there was a main effect of duration in the hippocampus body with greater activity for longer durations, which appeared to be driven by the internal perspective condition. On the basis of these findings, the authors suggest that the hippocampus may represent event sequences allocentrically, whereas the posterior parietal cortex may process event sequences egocentrically.

      Strengths:

      The topic of egocentric vs. allocentric processing has been relatively under-investigated with respect to time, having traditionally been studied in the domain of space. As such, the current study is timely and has the potential to be important for our understanding of how time is represented in the brain in the service of memory. The study is well thought out, and the behavioural paradigm is, in my opinion, a creative approach to tackling the authors' research question. A particular strength is the implementation of an imagination phase for the participants while learning the fictional religious ritual. This moves the paradigm beyond semantic/schema learning and is probably the best approach besides asking the participants to arduously enact and learn the different events with their exact timings in person. Importantly, the behavioural data point towards successful manipulation of internal vs. external perspective in participants, which is critical for the interpretation of the fMRI data. The use of syllable length as a sanity check for RT analyses, as well as neuroimaging analyses, is also much appreciated.

      Weaknesses/Suggestions:

      Although the design and analysis choices are generally solid, there are a few finer details/nuances that merit further clarification or consideration in order to strengthen the readers' confidence in the authors' interpretation of their data.

      (1) Given the known behavioural and neural effects of boundaries in sequence memory, I was wondering whether the number of traversed context boundaries (i.e., between morning-afternoon, and afternoon-evening) was controlled for across sequential length in the internal perspective condition? Or, was it the case that reference-target event pairs with higher sequential numbers were more likely to span across two parts of the day compared to lower sequential numbers? Similarly, did the authors examine any potential differences, whether behaviourally or neurally, for day part same vs. day part different external task trials?

      (2) I would appreciate further insight into the authors' decision to model their task trials as stick functions with duration 0 in their GLMs, as opposed to boxcar functions with varying durations, given the potential benefits of the latter (e.g., Grinband et al., 2008). I concur that in certain paradigms, RT is considered a potential confound and is taken into account as a nuisance covariate (as the authors have done here). However, given that RTs appear to be critical to the authors' interpretation of participant behavioural performance, it would imply that variations in RT actually reflect variations in cognitive processes of interest, and hence, it may be worth modelling trials as boxcar functions with varying durations.

      (3) The activity pattern across tasks and sequential distance in the posterior parietal cortex appears to parallel the RT data. Have the authors examined potential relationships between the two (e.g., individual participant slopes for RT across sequential distance vs. activity betas in the posterior parietal cortex)?

      (4) There were a few places in the manuscript where the writing/discussion of the wider literature could perhaps be tightened or expanded. For instance:

      i) On page 16, the authors state 'The negative correlation between the activation level in the right PPC and sequential distance has already been observed in a previous fMRI study (Gauthier & van Wassenhove, 2016b). The authors found a similar region (the reported MNI coordinate of the peak voxel was 42, -70, 40, and the MNI coordinate of the peak voxel in the present study was 39, -70, 35), of which the activation level went up when the target event got closer to the self-positioned event. This finding aligns with the evidence suggesting that the posterior parietal cortex implements egocentric representations.' Without providing a little more detail here about the Gauthier & van Wassenhove study and what participants were required to do (i.e., mentally position themselves at a temporal location and make 'occurred before' vs. 'occurred after' judgements of a target event), it could be a little tricky for readers to follow why this convergence in finding supports a role for the posterior parietal cortex in egocentric representations.

      ii) Although the authors discuss the Lee et al. (2020) review and related studies with respect to retrospective memory, it is critical to note that this work has also often used prospective paradigms, pointing towards sequential processing being the critical determinant of hippocampal involvement, rather than the distinction between retrospective vs. prospective processing.

      iii) The authors make an interesting suggestion with respect to hippocampal longitudinal differences in the representation of event sequences, and may wish to relate this to Montagrin et al. (2024), who make an argument for the representation of distant goals in the anterior hippocampus and immediate goals in the posterior hippocampus.

    1. eLife Assessment

      This important study describes a computational model of the rat spinal locomotor circuits and how they could be plastically reconfigured after lateral hemisection or contusion injuries to replicate gaits observed experimentally in vivo. Overall, the simulation results convincingly mirror the gait parameters observed experimentally. The model suggests the emergence of detour circuits after lateral hemisection, whereas after a midline contusion, the model suggests plasticity of left-right and sensory inputs below the injury.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This is a rigorous data-driven modeling study, extending the authors' previous model of spinal locomotor central pattern generator (CPG) circuits developed for the mouse spinal cord and adapted here to the rat to explore potential circuit-level changes underlying altered speed-dependent gaits, due to asymmetric (lateral) thoracic spinal hemisection and symmetric midline contusion. The model reproduces key features of the rat speed-dependent gait-related experimental data before injury and after recovery from these two different thoracic spinal cord injuries and suggests injury-specific mechanisms of circuit reorganization underlying functional recovery. There is much interest in the mechanisms of locomotor behavior recovery after spinal cord injury, and data-driven behaviorally relevant circuit modeling is an important approach. This study represents an important advance in the authors' previous experimental and modeling work on locomotor circuitry and in the motor control field.

      Strengths:

      (1) The authors use an advanced computational model of spinal locomotor circuitry to investigate potential reorganization of neural connectivity underlying locomotor control following recovery from symmetrical midline thoracic contusion and asymmetrical (lateral) hemisection injuries, based on an extensive dataset for the rat model of spinal cord injury.

      (2) The rat dataset used is from an in vivo experimental paradigm involving challenging animals to perform overground locomotion across the full range of speeds before and after the two distinct spinal cord injury models, enabling the authors to more completely reveal injury-specific deficits in speed-dependent interlimb coordination and locomotor gaits.

      (3) The model reproduces the rat gait-related experimental data before injury and after recovery from these two different thoracic spinal cord injuries, which exhibit roughly comparable functional recovery, and suggests injury-specific, compensatory mechanisms of circuit reorganization underlying recovery.

      (4) The model simulations suggest that recovery after lateral hemisection mechanistically involves partial functional restoration of descending drive and long propriospinal pathways. In contrast, recovery following midline contusion relies on reorganization of sublesional lumbar circuitry combined with altered descending control of cervical networks.

      (5) These observations suggest that symmetrical (contusion) and asymmetrical (lateral hemisection) injuries induce distinct types of plasticity in different spinal cord regions, suggesting that injury symmetry partly dictates the location and type of neural plasticity supporting recovery.

      (6) The authors suggest that therapeutic strategies may be more effective by targeting specific circuits according to injury symmetry.

      Weaknesses:

      The recovery mechanisms implemented in the model involve circuit connectivity/connection weights adjustment based on assumptions about the structures involved and compensatory responses to the injury. As the authors acknowledge, other factors affecting locomotor patterns and compensation, such as somatosensory afferent feedback, neurochemical modulator influences, and limb/body biomechanics, are not considered in the model.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this paper, the authors present a detailed computational model and experimental data concerning overground locomotion in rats before and after recovery from spinal cord injury. They are able to manually tune the parameters of this physiologically based, detailed model to reproduce many aspects of the observed animals' locomotion in the naive case and in two distinct injury cases.

      Strengths:

      The strengths are that the model is driven to closely match clean experimental data, and the model itself has detailed correspondence to proposed anatomical reality. As such, this makes the model more readily applicable to future experimental work. It can make useful suggestions. The model reproduces a large number of conditions across frequencies, and with the model structure changed by injury and recovery. The model is extensive and is driven by known structures, with links to genetic identities, and has been extensively validated across multiple experiments and manipulations over the years. It models a system of critical importance to the field, and the tight coupling to experimental data is a real strength.

      Weaknesses:

      A downside is that, scientifically, here, the only question tackled is one of sufficiency. By manually tuning parameters in a manner that aligns with the field's understanding from experimental work, the detailed model can accurately reproduce the experimental findings. One of the benefits of computational models is that the counterfactual can be tested to provide evidence against alternative hypotheses. That isn't really done here. I'm fairly certain that there are competing theories regarding what happens during recovery from a hemi-section injury and a contusion injury. The model could be used to make predictions for some alternative hypotheses, supporting or rejecting theories of recovery. This may be part of future plans. Here, the focus is on showing that the model is capable of reproducing the experimental results at all, for any set of parameters, however tuned.

    4. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study describes a computational model of the rat spinal locomotor circuit and how it could be reconfigured after lateral hemisection or contusion injuries to replicate gaits observed experimentally.

      The model suggests the emergence of detour circuits after lateral hemisection, whereas after a midline contusion, the model suggests plasticity of left-right and sensory inputs below the injury.

      Strengths:

      The model accurately models many known connections within and between forelimb and hindlimb spinal locomotor circuits.

      The simulation results mirror closely gait parameters observed experimentally. Many gait parameters were studied, as well as variability in these parameters in intact versus injured conditions.

      Weaknesses:

      The study could provide some sense of the relative importance of the various modified connectivities after injury in setting the changes in gait seen after the two types of injuries.

      Overall, the authors achieved their aims, and the model provides solid support for the changes in connectivity after the two types of injuries were modelled. This work emphasizes specific changes in connectivity after lateral hemisection or after contusion that could be investigated experimentally. The model is available for public use and could serve as a tool to analyze the relative importance of various highlighted or previously undiscovered changes in connectivity that may underlie the recovery of locomotor function in spinalized rats.