10,000 Matching Annotations
  1. Oct 2025
    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this study, Ledamoisel et al. examined the evolution of visual and chemical signals in closely related Morpho butterfly species to understand their role in species coexistence. Using an integrative, state-of-the-art approach combining spectrophotometry, visual modeling, and behavioral mate choice experiments, they quantified differences in wing iridescence and assessed its influence on mate preference in allopatry and sympatry. They also performed chemical analyses to determine whether sympatric species exhibit divergent chemical cues that may facilitate species recognition and mate discrimination. The authors found iridescent coloration to be similar in sympatric Morpho species. Furthermore, male mate choice experiments revealed that in sympatry, males fail to discriminate conspecific females based on coloration, reinforcing the idea that visual signal convergence is primarily driven by predation pressure. In contrast, the divergence of chemical signals among sympatric species suggests their potential role in facilitating species recognition and mate discrimination. The authors conclude that interactions between ecological pressures and signal evolution may shape species coexistence.

      Strengths:

      The study is well-designed and integrates multiple methodological approaches to provide a thorough assessment of signal evolution in the studied species. I appreciate the authors' careful consideration of multiple selective pressures and their combined influence on signal divergence and convergence. Additionally, the inclusion of both visual and chemical signals adds an interesting and valuable dimension to the study, enhancing its importance. Beyond butterflies, this research broadens our understanding of multimodal communication and signal evolution in the context of species coexistence.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The broader significance of the findings needs to be better articulated. While the authors emphasize that comparing adaptive traits in sympatry and allopatry provides insights into selective processes shaping reproductive isolation and coexistence, it is unclear what key conceptual or theoretical questions are being addressed. Are these patterns expected under certain evolutionary scenarios? Have they been empirically demonstrated in other systems? The authors should explicitly state the overarching research question, incorporate some predictions, and better contextualize their findings within the existing literature. If the results challenge or support previous work, that should be highlighted to strengthen the study's importance in a broader context.

      We thank the reviewer for their valuable feedback. We understand that the framing of the results and the discussion may fail to convey the broader significance of our findings. In the first version of the manuscript, we framed our manuscript around the processes shaping reproductive isolation and co-existence in sympatry, but now realize that this question was too broad in regards to our results. We thus strictly focused on outlining the importance of ecological interactions in the evolution of traits in sympatric species. In the revised version of the manuscript, we rewrote the first paragraph of the introduction to introduce context regarding the effect of ecological interactions on trait evolution (lines 43-60). We then explicitly introduce the theoretical question investigated in our paper (i.e. “we investigate how ecological interactions in sympatry can constrain natural and sexual selection shaping trait evolution”, lines 62-63) and our predictions regarding the evolution of traits in sympatry vs. allopatry (lines 74-80). We also added predictions regarding our experiments on Morpho at the end of the introduction (lines 146-157). As a result, the discussion is now better aligned with the introduction, by discussing the putative effect of predation and mate choice on the evolution of wing iridescence in Morpho.

      (2) The motivation for studying visual signals and mate choice in allopatric populations (i.e., at the intraspecific level) is not well articulated, leaving their role in the broader narrative unclear. In particular, the rationale behind experiments 1, 2, and 3 is not well defined, as the authors have not made a strong case for the need for these intraspecific comparisons in the introduction. This issue is further compounded by the authors' primary focus on signal evolution in sympatry throughout both the results and the discussion. For instance, the divergence of iridescence in allopatry is a potentially interesting result. But the authors have not discussed its implications.

      We now clearly state in the introduction our motivation for studying visual signals and mate choice in allopatric populations (lines 74-80, lines 146-157). We argued that intraspecific comparisons help identify whether visual cues can be used in mate recognition between phylogenetically close subspecies, between whom visual resemblance is supposed to be higher than between closely-related species (tetrad experiment, and experiment 1). As M. h. bristowi and M. h. theodorus have different wing pattern, we also used this comparison to identify the traits involved in male mate preference within a species, testing the importance of iridescent color (experiment 2) or iridescent patterning (experiment 3). The results of those experiments can then be used to assess whether these traits are used in species recognition between sympatric species. See also our answers to recommendations 11 and 15 from reviewer #1.

      Overall, given that the primary conclusions are based on results and analyses in sympatry, the role of allopatric populations in shaping these conclusions needs to be better integrated and justified. Without a stronger link between the comparative framework and the study's key takeaways, the use of allopatric populations feels somewhat peripheral rather than central to the study's aim. Since the primary conclusions remain valid even without the allopatric comparisons, their inclusion requires a clearer rationale.

      To make a stronger case for the use of the allopatric population in our manuscript, we strengthened the justification behind the study of intraspecific allopatric populations vs. interspecific sympatric populations, as the iridescence measurements and the mate choice experiments in allopatric populations can serve as a baseline in studying how species interactions can shape the evolution of traits and mate recognition when compared to sympatric populations. Following your major comment #1, we rewrote the introduction to include a justification to the need for studying allopatric vs. sympatric populations (lines 74-80), and also further highlighted the need to study iridescence in sympatric species to fully understand the trait evolution of sympatric species in the discussion (339-343).

      (3) While the authors demonstrate that iridescence is indistinguishable to predators in sympatry, they overstate the role of predation in driving convergence. The present study does not experimentally demonstrate that iridescence in this species has a confusion effect or contributes to evasive mimicry. Alternatively, convergence could result from other selective forces, such as signal efficacy due to environmental conditions, rather than being solely driven by predation.

      We acknowledge that our study does not directly demonstrate that iridescence contributes to evasive mimicry. We did tone down the interpretation of the results in the discussion and state that predation is not the only selective pressure that could have promoted a convergent evolution of iridescence in sympatric species, as iridescence is a trait that could be involved in thermoregulation (lines 346-353) and camouflage (lines 363-369) for example. We made sure to mention that convergence in iridescent signals in sympatry is only an indirect support to the evasive mimicry hypothesis, and that further research is still needed, including direct predation experiments, to show that this convergence is indeed triggered by predation (lines 391-396).  

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      This study presents an investigation of the visual and chemical properties and mating behaviour in Morpho butterflies, aimed at addressing the nature of divergence between closely related species in sympatry. The study species consists of three subspecies of Morpho helenor (bristowi, theodorus, and helenor), and the conspecific Morpho achilles achilles. The authors postulate that whereas the iridescent blue signals of all (sub)species should function as a predator reduction signal (similar to aposematism) and therefore exhibit convergence, the same signals should indicate divergence if used as a mating signal, particularly in sympatric populations. They also assess chemical profiles among the species to assess the potential utility of scent in mediating species/sex discrimination.

      The authors first used reflectance spectrometry to calculate hue, brightness, and chroma, plus two measures of "iridescence" (perhaps better phrased as angular dependence) in each (sub)species. This indicated the ubiquitous presence of sexual dimorphism in brightness (males brighter), which also appears to be the case for iridescence (Figure 3A-B). Analysis of these data also indicated that whereas there is evidence for divergence among subspecies in allopatry, the same evidence is lacking for species in sympatry (P = 0.084). This was supported further by visual modelling, which showed that both conspecifics and birds should be (theoretically) capable of perceiving the colour difference among allopatric populations of M. helenor, whereas the same is not true for the sympatric species.

      The authors then conducted mate choice trials, first using live individuals and second using female dummies. The live experiments indicated the presence of assortative mating among the two subspecies of M. helenor (bristowi and theodorus). The dummy presentations indicated (a) bristowi males prefer conspecific wings, whereas theodorus have no preference, (b) bristowi males prefer the con(sub)specific colour pattern, (c) theodorus prefer the con(sub)specific iridescence when the pattern is manipulated to be similar among female dummies. A fourth experiment, using sympatric M. achilles and M. helenor, indicated no preference for conspecific female dummies. Finally, chemical analysis indicated substantial differences between these two species in putative pheromone compounds, and especially so in the males.

      The authors conclude that the similarity of iridescence among species in sympatry is suggestive of convergence upon a common anti-predation signal. Despite some behavioural evidence in favourof colour (iridescence)-based mate discrimination, chemical differences between Achilles and Helenor are posed as more likely to function for species isolation than visual differences.

      Overall, I enjoyed reading this manuscript, which presents a valiant attempt at studying visual, chemical and behavioural divergence in this iconic group of butterflies.

      Major comments

      My only major comment concerns the authors' favoured explanation for aposematism (or evasive mimicry) for convergence among species, which is based upon the you-can't-catch-me hypothesis first presented by Young 1971. Although there is supporting work showing that iridescent-like stimuli are more difficult to precisely localize by a range of viewers, most of the evidence as applied to the Morpho system is circumstantial, and I'm not certain that there is widespread acceptance of this hypothesis. Given that the present study deals with closely-related  (sub)species, one alternative explanation - a "null" hypothesis of sorts - is for a lack of divergence (from a common starting point) as opposed to evolutionary convergence per se. in other words, two subspecies are likely to retain ancestral character states unless there is selection that causes them to diverge. I feel that the manuscript would benefit from a discussion of this alternative, if not others. Signalling to predators could very well be involved in constraining the extent of convergence, but this seems a little premature to state as an up-front conclusion of this work. There is also the result of a *dorsal* wing manipulation by Vieira-Silva et al. 2024 which seems difficult to reconcile in light of this explanation. Whereas this paper is cited by the authors, a more nuanced discussion of their experimental results would seem appropriate here.

      We thank the reviewer for their constructive comments on our manuscript. We appreciate the reviewer’s concern regarding the way iridescence convergence between sympatric species is discussed in our manuscript, which align with similar concerns raised by Reviewer 1. Indeed, the you-can't-catch-me hypothesis has not been yet empirically tested in Morpho, this is currently a working hypothesis only supported by indirect lines of evidence.

      Among the 30 known Morpho species, iridescence is most likely the ancestral character, notably because iridescence is a trait shared by a majority of Morpho (we now mention this in the introduction lines 108-110). In this paper, we thus did not aim to identify the evolutionary forces involved in the appearance of iridescence in this group, but rather wanted to understand to what extent ecological interactions can impact the diversification (or not) of this trait. As such, the dorsal manipulations performed in Vieira-Silva et al 2024 showing that iridescence in Morpho may have a similar effect than crypsis does not impact our working hypothesis. Instead, we use VieraSilva et al 2024 to discuss the potential anti-predator effect of iridescence, that could potentially promote convergent evolution of iridescent patterns.

      In the main text, we now clearly mention our null hypothesis: under a scenario of neutral evolution of iridescence, we would expect that the divergence in wing coloration between two M. helenor subspecies would be lower than between two different Morpho species (M. helenor and M. achilles) and showed that our results sharply differ from this null expectation.

      We then improved the discussion by adding alternative hypotheses potentially explaining the convergent iridescent signal detected in sympatric species: we discussed the expected effect under neutral evolution (lines 339-343), but also added alternative hypotheses regarding the diversification of iridescence due to camouflage (lines 363-369), predator evasion (lines 373-377) and thermoregulation (lines 346-353).

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      The authors investigated differences in iridescence wing colouration of allopatric (geographically separated) and sympatric (coexisting) Morpho butterfly (sub)species. Their aim was to assess if iridescence wing colouration of Morpho (sub)species converged or diverged depending on coexistence and if iridescence wing colouration was involved in mating behaviour and reproductive isolation. The authors hypothesize that iridescence wing colouration of different (sub)species should converge in sympatry and diverge in allopatry. In sympatry, iridescence wing colouration can act as an effective antipredator defence with shared benefits if multiple (sub)species share the same colouration. However, shared wing colouration can have potential costs in terms of reproductive interference since wing colouration is often involved in mate recognition. If the benefits of a shared antipredator defence outweigh the costs of reproductive interference, iridescence wing colouration will show convergence and alternative mate recognition strategies might evolve, such as chemical mate recognition. In allopatry, iridescence wing colouration is expected to diverge due to adaptation to different local conditions and no alternative mate recognition is expected.

      Strengths:

      (1) Using allopatric and sympatric (sub)species that are closely related is a powerful way to test evolutionary hypotheses

      (2) By clearly defining iridescence and measuring colour spectra from a variety of angles, applying different methods, a very comprehensive dataset of iridescence wing colouration is achieved.

      (3) By experimentally manipulating wing coloration patterns, the authors show visual mate recognition for M. h. bristowi and could, in theory, separate different visual aspects of colouration (patterns VS iridescence strength).

      (4) Measurements of chemical profiles to investigate alternative mate recognition strategies in case of convergence of visual signals.

      Weaknesses:

      In my opinion, studies should be judged on the methods and data included, and not on additional measurements that could have been taken or additional treatments/species that should be included, since in most ecological and evolutionary studies, more measurements or treatments/species can always be included. However, studies do need to ensure appropriate replication and appropriate measurements to test their hypothesis AND support their conclusions. The current study failed to ensure appropriate replication, and in various cases, the results do not support the conclusions.

      First, when using allopatric and sympatric (sub)species pairs to test evolutionary hypotheses, replication is important. Ideally, multiple allopatric and sympatric (sub)species pairs are compared to avoid outlier (sub)species or pairs that lead to biased conclusions. Unfortunately, the current study compares 1 allopatric and 1 sympatric (sub)species pair, hence having poor (no) replication on the level of allopatric and sympatric (sub)species pairs,

      We would like to thank the reviewer for their constructive feedback. We agree that replication is important to test evolutionary hypotheses and that our study lacks replication for allopatric and sympatric Morpho populations. Ideally, one would require several allopatric and sympatric replicates to conclude on the effect of species interaction in trait evolution. Our study is a preliminary attempt at answering this question, covering a few Morpho populations but proposing a broad assessment of iridescence and mate preference for those populations. We clearly mentioned in the discussion that investigating multiple populations is needed to test whether the trend we observed in this paper can be generalized (line 388-392).

      Second, chemical profiles were only measured for sympatric species and not for allopatric (sub)species, which limits the interpretation of this data. The allopatric (sub)species could have been measured as non-coexistence "control". If coexistence and convergence in wing colouration drives the evolution of alternative mate recognition signals, such alternative signals should not evolve/diverge for allopatric (sub)species where wing colouration is still a reliable mate recognition cue. More importantly, no details are provided on the quantification of butterfly chemical profiles, which is essential to understand such data. It is unclear how the chemical profiles were quantified and what data (concentrations, ratios, proportions) were used to perform NDMS and generate Figure 5 and the associated statistical tests.

      We recognize that having the chemical profiles of the genitalia of the Morpho from the allopatric populations would have made a stronger case in favor of reinforcement acting on the divergence of the chemical compounds found on the genitalia of the sympatric Morpho species. Due to limited access to the biological material needed at the time of the chromatography, we could not test for lower divergence in the chemical profiles of allopatric Morpho butterflies. We made sure to mention this limitation in the discussion (lines 457-461). 

      We already stated in the methods that we compiled the area under the peak of each components found in the chromatograms of our samples and that we performed all the statistical analyses on this dataset. To make it clearer, we mention in the new version of the manuscript that the area under the peak of each component allows to measure the concentration of the components (in the methods lines 720, 723, 733). We also added some precisions in the legend of Figure 5.

      Third, throughout the discussion, the authors mention that their results support natural selection by predators on iridescent wing colouration, without measuring natural selection by predators or any other measure related to predation. It is unclear by what predators any of the butterfly species are predated on at this point

      We made sure to mention in the introduction (line 132-136) and in the discussion (line 373-377) that previous predation experiments performed on Morpho and other butterflies showed evidence that birds are likely predators for these species. These observations lead us to test for the putative effect of predation on the evolution of their color pattern, without directly testing predatory rates. We made sure this information is transparent in the revised manuscript, and now precise that assessing wing convergence is only an indirect way of testing the escape mimicry hypothesis (line 393-396).

      To continue on the interpretation of the data related to selection on specific traits by specific selection agents: This study did not measure any form of selection or any selection agent. Hence, it is not known if iridescent wing colouration is actually under selection by predators and/or mates, if maybe other selection agents are involved or if these traits converge due to genetic correlations with other traits under selection. For example, Iridescent colouration in ground beetles has functions as antipredator defence but also thermo- and water regulation. None of these issues are recognized or discussed.

      The lack of discussion of alternative selective pressures involved in the evolution of iridescence was pointed out by all reviewers. We thus modified the text to account for this comment, and no longer limit our discussion to the putative effects of predation. We now specifically discuss alternative hypotheses, including crypsis (362-369) and thermoregulation (line 346-353).

      Finally, some of the results are weakly supported by statistics or questionable methodology.

      Most notably, the perception of the iridescence coloration of allopatric subspecies by bird visual systems. Although for females, means and errors (not indicated what exactly, SD, SE or CI) are clearly above the 1 JND line, for males, means are only slightly above this line and errors or CIs clearly overlap with the 1 JND line. Since there is no additional statistical support, higher means but overlap of SD, SE or CI with the baseline provides weak statistical support for differences.

      We thank the reviewer for bringing interpretation issues concerning the chromatic distances of allopatric Morpho species measured with a bird vision model. We made sure to be nuanced in the description of this graph in the results section (line 208-212). Note that this addition does not change our main conclusion stating that Morpho and predator visual models better discriminate iridescence differences between allopatric subspecies than between sympatric species.

      We now also clearly mention in the figure’s legend that the error bars represent the confidence intervals obtained after performing a bootstrap analysis, in addition to the mention of the nature of the error bars already mentioned in the methods (line 580).

      Regarding the assortative mating experiment, the results are clearly driven by M. bristowi. For M. theodorus, females mate equally often with conspecifics (6 times) as with M. bristowi (5 times). For males, the ratio is slightly better (6 vs 3), but with such low numbers, I doubt this is statistically testable. Overall low mating for M. bristowi could indicate suboptimal experimental conditions, and hence results should be interpreted with care.

      We recognize that the tetrad experiment results are mainly driven by M. bristowi’s behavior as already mentioned in the results (line 231-232) but we now also mention it in the discussion (lines 401-402). This experiment would have benefited from more replicates, but the limited access to live males and virgin females for both subspecies was a limiting factor. Fisher’s exact test used to assess assortative mating is specifically appropriate to small sample sizes. We recognize that the sampling size is not ideal, however it is still statistically testable.

      Regarding the wing manipulation experiment, M. theodorus does not show a preference when dummies with non-modified wings are presented and prefers non-modified dummies over modified dummies. This is acknowledged by the authors but not further discussed. Certainly, some control treatment for wing modification could have been added.

      The use of controls to consider the effect of wing modification and odor by the permanent marker were already mentioned in the methods (lines 636-639). Following your recommendation and comments from the other reviewers, we now mention the use of this control in the results (lines 278283). We also address a potential issue that would have resulted in the rejection of these modified dummies by live males: we cannot be sure whether butterflies perceive these modifications as equivalent to natural coloration (lines 281-282). An additional control could have been used, adding black ink on the black dorsal parts of the pattern to assess its potential visual effect. The constraints on sampling unfortunately did not allow to add another treatment.

      Overall, the fact that certain measurements only provide evidence for 1 of the 2 (sub)species (assortative mating, wing manipulation) or one sex of one of the species (bird visual systems) means overall interpretation and overgeneralization of the results to both allopatric or sympatric species should be done with care, and such nuances should ideally be discussed.

      The aim of the authors, "to investigate the antagonistic effects of selective pressures generated by mate recognition and shared predation" has not been achieved, and the conclusions regarding this aim are not supported by the results. Nevertheless, the iridescence colour measurements are solid, and some of the behavioural experiments and chemical profile measurements seem to yield interesting results. The study would benefit from less overinterpretation of the results in the framework of predation and more careful consideration of methodological difficulties, statistical insecurities, and nuances in the results.

      Overall, we would like to thank all reviewers for their thorough assessment of our work. We understand that the imbalance between mate choice data, visual model data and chemical data only gives us a partial assessment of species recognition in Morpho butterflies, thus requiring more precision in the interpretation and the discussion of our results. We made sure to add balanced interpretations in our discussion, by mentioning the lack of replicates for allopatric and sympatric populations (lines 391-392), and the lack of chemical characterization of allopatric species (lines 458361, see previous comments) and by being more transparent on methodological limitations that we failed to convey in the first version of our manuscript. We brought nuance to our discussion and also discussed alternative hypotheses to predation to explain the convergence of iridescence found in sympatry.

      Reviewing Editor Comments:

      While all reviewers acknowledge the value of your data, they converge in their recommendations to tone down the evolutionary interpretations. Ideally, to test your main hypothesis, you would need several species pairs, or if only one, as in your case, replicated sympatric and allopatric sites for both species. Furthermore, your more specific hypotheses about convergence (vs. nondivergence), response to predators (vs. other environmental variables), and avoiding interspecific mating in sympatry (vs. not avoiding it in allopatry) would require appropriate alternative treatments/controls. We therefore recommend that you focus on those statements that you can support with your experiments and data, and introduce these statements in the introduction with reference to the appropriate literature.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Line 25: This stated aim seems a bit off. The authors did not sensu stricto quantify 'how shared adaptive traits may shape genetic divergence' in this study. I suggest rewriting or deleting this whole sentence altogether. The study's aim is already clear in lines 29-34.

      We deleted the mention of the characterization of genetic divergence, since this study did not focus on any genetic analysis.

      (2) Line 34: The authors here state that they compared allopatric vs sympatric populations. This is strictly not true for M. Achilles. Further, the results after this sentence focus solely ondivergence/convergence in sympatry, nothing at the intraspecific level and implications of the findings

      We now mention that we tested allopatric vs. sympatric species of M. helenor only (lines 28-29). We also mention that the behavioral experiments were based on intraspecific comparisons, and discuss the implications of this result in the discussion.

      (3) Line 35: 'convergence driven by predation': this is a strong statement and cannot be directly inferred from the present set of experiments. Consider toning it down.

      We added nuance to this statement by rephrasing it “suggesting that predation may favors local resemblance” (lines 32-33)

      (4) Line 36: Replace 'behavioral results' with 'behavioral experiments' or something similar.

      Corrected

      (5) Line 45-49: These opening statements need some citations.

      We provided references for the first few lines, by citing terHorst et al 2018 (line 44) underlining the importance of species interactions in trait evolution, and Blomberg et al 2003 (line 45) showing that closely-related species tend to resemble each other by quantifying the phylogenetic signal of various traits.

      (6) Line 83, 165: 'visual effect', not sure what the authors are referring to. Please rewrite.

      We defined “visual effect” as the way wing color patterns could be perceived by predators or mates. We removed mentions of “visual effect” and directly used its definition instead.

      (7) Line 105 onwards: This section of the introduction could benefit from more concise writing. The authors might consider reducing the number of specific examples and instead offering broader general statements, supported by citations from multiple studies.

      We reduced the number of examples given in this paragraph and used general statements supported by multiple citations as examples. (lines 102-119).

      (8) Line 108-110: This sentence seems to be redundant with the previous one.

      We merged this sentence with the previous one to improve clarity. (lines 103-105)

      (9) Line 140: 'with chemical defenses': include citations here.

      We added citations of Joron et al 1999 and Merrill et al 2014, which document the evolution of convergent wing patterns (mimicry) in butterfly species with chemical-defenses.

      (10) Line 149: This is a bit of a stretch. Note that genetic divergence could be influenced by many other things, not only the processes that the authors examined.

      We agree with the reviewer that the study of the convergent vs. divergent evolution of visual cues is not enough to fully understand the mechanisms allowing genetic divergence between species. Because this paper does not focus on characterizing genetic divergence, we removed it from the manuscript to avoid oversimplification.

      (11) Line 151: Again. Here, the author's primary focus seems to be at an interspecific level. One is left to wonder about the need for comparisons at the intraspecific level in M.helenor and the implications. Please clarify

      In the end of the introduction (lines 146-157), we specifically highlighted the importance of intraspecific comparisons. While studying the effect of sympatry on the evolution of the iridescent color pattern, we use this intraspecific comparison as a baseline to account for convergence or divergence of iridescence in a sympatric interspecific pair of Morpho, because under neutral evolution two subspecies are expected to be more similar than two different species (this assumption has been clarified line 147-148). We also used intraspecific mate choice to test for the use of visual cues in mate recognition (experiment 1) and to test what type of signal could be perceived by Morphos (the iridescent coloration or the iridescent pattern, experiment 2 and 3). These results help contextualize the interspecific mate choice, focused on determining whether visual cues could also be used in species recognition. Since we show that iridescent coloration is important in mate recognition at the intraspecific scale, it helps understand why species recognition is low at the interspecific scale because of wing color convergence between M. helenor and M. achilles.

      (12) Line 154: 'signals on mate preferences'.

      Corrected.

      (13) Line 189: 'At the intraspecific level', maybe in the brackets include 'allopatric populations' just so the results are in a similar format as in the color contrast section below.

      We added details to make clearer that the intraspecific level is studied between allopatric Morpho populations (line 189).

      (14) Line 189-192: Please rearrange the figure (current B as A and vice versa) or present the results in order as in the figure (interspecific first and then intraspecific level).

      We rearranged Figure 3 so that the intraspecific comparison (allopatric population) appears as A and the interspecific level (sympatric population) appears as B, to follow the order of presentation in the main text.

      (15) Line 232: The motivation behind experiments 1, 2, and 3 is unclear. The authors have not made a strong point in the introduction about the need for these comparisons at an intraspecific level. Given that the authors are focused on divergence/convergence at an interspecific level, this set of experiments seems to be irrelevant to the present study. The implications of these findings are also not discussed.

      We added motivation to the use of experiment 1, 2, and 3 in the introduction (lines 151-154) by stating that those experiments were used to assess whether blue color could indeed be used as a mating cue in Morpho helenor (experiment 1) and to try to understand what part of the visual signal is important in mate choice in Morpho helenor: the wing pattern (experiment 2) or the iridescent coloration (experiment 3). Although motivation for these experiments was not detailed in our manuscript, we already discussed the implications of the results of experiments 1, 2 and 3 in the discussion by stating that visual cues can take many forms and that considering both color AND pattern is important in understanding visual cues (lines 408-416). We carefully reworked this new version to make it more straightforward.

      (16) Line 260: Insert 'wild-type' before model to ensure similar wording as in the previous section.

      Corrected.

      (17) Line 286: Insert 'sympatric' after mimetic.

      Corrected.

      (18) Line 307: Include a reference to the figures or table where these results are presented.

      We now mention in the main text that the different proportions of beta-ocimene found between males M. helenor and M. achilles are shown in Table S2.

      (19) Line 343: These inferences are speculative. Add a line here, something like 'although this warrants further research in this species'.

      We detailed what additional experiments are needed lines 388-396.

      (20) Line 357: The authors have not discussed their results on iridescence divergence in allopatric populations (line 190) and its implications.

      We now made clear in the beginning of the discussion that the divergence of iridescence in allopatric populations is used as a baseline to test for convergent iridescence between species (lines 339-343).

      (21) Line 361 onwards: This first paragraph is a bit confusing, as the results mainly focus on allopatry, while the title refers to sympatry.

      To avoid confusion between the title and the content of the discussion, we divided the last part of the discussion into two different parts. As the first paragraph mainly focus on allopatry, we isolated it and titled it “Iridescent color patterns can be used as mate recognition cues in M. helenor” (line 498). The next paragraph of the discussion, focusing on the sympatric Morpho populations, has been titled “Evolution of visual and olfactory cues in mimetic sister-species living in sympatry” (line 418).

      (21)  Line 383: visual cues 'as' poor species.

      Corrected.

      (23) Line 405: Why females here and not males? This is again confusing since the authors tested for male mate choice in the main experiments. Some background information on sex-specific mate choice in the methods might help.

      In this specific sentence, we talk about performing mate choice experiments to test for the discrimination of olfactory cues by females (and not males) because we found a high divergence in the chemical compounds found on male genitalia. Although female chemical compounds could also be used as a cue by males in mate recognition, olfactive mate choice is often driven by female choice in butterflies. We recognize that this perspective does not line up with the mate choice presented in our results section which focused on male mate choice based on visual cues, because of ecological reasons (Morpho males tend to be attracted to bright blue colorations but not females) and technical reasons (in cages, females tend to hide away from the males or male dummies, and this behavior is not compatible with experiments involving flying around false males). In the discussion, we made sure to precise that the perspective we cite here is about testing the implications of divergence in male olfactory cues (line 454). We also added motivation to why we chose to investigate male (and not female) mate choice based on visual cues in the methods (lines 613-618) and in the results (219-223).

      (24) Line 417: This inference is speculative. Consider toning it down.

      We rewrote the sentence: “We find evidence of converging iridescent patterns in sympatry suggesting that predation could play a major role in the evolution of iridescence. Further work is nevertheless needed to directly test this hypothesis and establish the important of evasive mimicry in Morpho” (lines 465-468).

      (25) Line 429: 'Convergent trait evolution leads to mutualistic interactions enhancing coexistence'. Careful here. It is not very evident how convergent trait evolution (iridescence) is mutualistic in this case, as there is no experimental evidence for evasive mimicry yet. Consider rewording or toning this sentence down.

      We agree with the reviewer and removed this statement, only keeping the end of the sentence: “Altogether, this study addresses how convergence in one trait as a result of biotic interactions may alter selection on traits in other sensory modalities, resulting in a complex mosaic of biodiversity. (lines 479-481).

      (26) Line 442: Since the samples come from a breeding farm, I have a few questions. How are the authors sure about the location where the specimens were collected? How long have they been kept in captivity? Have they been subjected to any artificial selection? More details are needed here.

      Since M. helenor bristowi and M. helenor theodorus are only found in the wild in West and East Ecuador respectively, those M. helenor subspecies can only be collected in those two allopatric populations. Their phenotype is directly linked to their geographic repartition, this is how we made sure about their collect location. M. h. theodorus we used in this study were caught in East Ecuador in Tena, and M. h. bristowi were caught in West Ecuador in Pedro Vincente Madonado. We received pupae from the breeding farm, meaning that the Morpho used for the experiments were raised in captivity since their date of emergence. Upon emergence, they were transferred into cages for 4 to 5 days to wait for sexual maturity before performing the tetrad and mate choice experiments. This information was added to the method (lines 490-496).

      (27) Line 476: Include some citations supporting this statement.

      We now cite Bennett and Théry (2007), reviewing avian color vision, and Briscoe (2008), characterizing the sensitivity of the photoreceptors found in the eyes of butterflies. Both citations show that the 300-700nm range is seen by avian and butterfly visual systems.

      (28) Line 480 onwards: Please clarify if the analysis used only one value (mean?) per species, sex, angle of measurement, and locality or included data from multiple individuals.

      The analyses of both colorimetric variables and global iridescence were performed using iridescence data from multiple individuals (10 males and 10 females from M. h. bristowi, M. h. theodorus, M. h. helenor and M. a. achilles), for which we measured iridescence at 21 angles of illumination. Sampling size are mentioned lines 507, 515, 540-542.

      (29) Line 510: Is there a specific reason that authors did not investigate achromatic contrasts? Provide some justification here. Or include the results of achromatic contrasts in the supplement.

      We added the achromatic results in the supplement and in the results (lines 200-204). For both the avian visual model and the Morpho visual model, the confidence intervals always overlapped with the JND threshold, showing that neither birds nor butterflies could theoretically discriminate the wing reflectance brightness in allopatric and sympatric populations.

      (30) Line 552 onwards: I may have missed it. It is not entirely clear why the authors focused on male mate choice rather than female preference for visual cues. The authors should explicitly justify this choice and cite previous studies demonstrating that male mate choice, rather than female preference, is important in this species. This should be stated in the results section as well.

      We added a paragraph in the method (lines 613-618) to describe the ecological and technical reasons leading to testing only male mate choice using visual cues (also see our response to recommendation #23).

      (31) Line 537 onwards: What was the criterion used to score that mating had occurred? Why first mating and not how long they were mating? Please add these details.

      We stopped the experiment as soon as a male/female pair was formed by joining their genitalia (we added this information in the method lines 599-600). Since the tetrad experiment involves the interaction of two males and two females from different subspecies, we considered that mate choice happened before the formation of any couple, and is not necessarily dependent on how long they mate by observing their mating behavior. For instance, we witnessed avoidance behaviors from females that systematically hide their genitalia and refused to join their abdomen to some males, while being very ‘open’ to others (but did not quantify it).  

      (32) Line 571: The authors used a black permanent marker to modify wing patterns but did not validate whether butterflies perceive these modifications as equivalent to natural coloration. It is possible that the alterations introduced unintended visual cues and may explain why most males rejected the dummies (line 267). The authors should acknowledge this limitation here.

      We now acknowledge this limitation in the method (lines 638-639) and in the results section (lines 278-283).

      (33) Line 591: Insert 'above' after protocol.

      Corrected.

      (34) Line 605: If the authors included random effects in their model, then it should be generalized linear mixed model (GLMM) and not GLM as they wrote.

      We indeed included a random effect in our model accounting for male ID and trial number, we thus replaced “GLM” by “GLMM” in the manuscript.

      (35) Line 615: This set of analyses does not seem to account for pseudo-replication, as the data were recorded from the same male more than once (Line 583). Please clarify and redo the analysis with the GLMM framework

      We run new analyses using the GLMM framework: we used a binomial GLMM to test whether individuals preferentially interacted with dummy 1 vs. dummy 2 while accounting for pseudoreplication. The previously detected tendencies hold true with these new analyses, except for the visual mate discrimination of M. achilles: we now find statistical evidence that M. achilles tend to approach more their conspecifics during the mate choice experiment, although the signal is weak (line 297-307). Indeed, while we previously concluded that both species in sympatry (M. helenor and M. achilles) could not discriminate their conspecific mates, we now emphasize that M. achilles is somewhat sensitive to some visual signals. However, its estimated probability of approaching a conspecific is only 0.54, which is low compared to the estimated probability of approaching (0.61) or touching (0.84) a con-subspecific for M. bristowi. We thus concluded that even though some visual cues could be relevant for mate recognition, they are less reliable for male choice in sympatric populations were color patterns are more convergent, compared to allopatric populations. We thus updated Figure 4 and Figure S8 and S9, which are now picturing the probability of approaching or touching a conspecific or con-subspecific with the updated pvalues retrieved from the GLMM analyses. We also updated the results (line 297-307) and the discussion (lines 430-438) to bring nuance to our previous results.  

      (36) Line 963: Figure 3D. Is there a particular reason for comparing allopatric populations only within Ecuador rather than between Ecuador and French Guiana for M. helenor? Please clarify.

      We aimed at comparing the putative discrimination of blue coloration using visual models vs. what the butterflies actually discriminate using mate choice experiments. Since we only performed mate choice experiments involving M. h. bristowi x M. h. theodorus (allopatric populations within Ecuador) and M. h. helenor x M. a. achilles (sympatric population from Ecuador), we only looked at those comparisons using visual models. We added this precision lines (559-560).

      (37) Line 980: Are these predicted probabilities or just mean proportions as written in line 614? Then the label should be changed to 'Proportion of approaches' or something similar.

      Following our answer to recommendation #35, the points now represent the probability of touching a conspecific in the graph for each male, for every trial of every male tested. We corrected the legend of the figure. 

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Line 25: "...therefore facilitating co-existence in sympathy".

      Corrected.

      (2) Line 28: "contrasting" instead of contrasted.

      Corrected.

      (3) Line 33: begin a new sentence at the colon.

      Corrected.

      (4) Line 49: the phrase "habitat filtering" is unclear and should perhaps be defined or qualified.

      We replaced “habitat filtering” by its definition and cited Keddy (1992), describing the community assembly rules and defining habitat filtering (line 46)

      (5) Line 52: remove "even".

      Corrected.

      (6) Line 53: divergent suites may also result because traits are often constrained by genetic architecture (multivariate genetic covariances). This is discussed at length and specifically in relation to ornamental coloration by Kemp et al. 2023

      We rewrote the introduction and focused on only reviewing the ecological interactions promoting trait divergence in sympatric species, and did not mention genetics in this paper.

      (7) Line 87: (and throughout) refer to "colouration" or "colour pattern" rather than "colourations".

      Corrected.

      (8) Line 151: Remove "To do so,".

      Corrected.

      (9) Line 191: I would like to see the degrees of freedom for this test.

      We added the F-statistic=2.09 and the degrees of freedom df=1 of this test, and for all the following tests.

      (10) Line 201: (and throughout) replace "on" with "of".

      Corrected.

      (11) Line 205: modelling the visual properties of the wings allows one to infer what is theoretically visible/distinguishable. The modelling is useful but not necessarily definitive of vision/behaviour per se under different conditions in the wild. I therefore think it is appropriate to phrase the wording around the modelling approach more carefully. Perhaps refer to "theoretical" or "inferred" discriminability, or state (e.g.) that species should/should not be capable of perceiving differences based on the modelling data. You do this well in your wording of lines 207-209. This need not apply in the discussion because you're then dealing with the combination of modelling results and behaviour (mating trials).

      We agree with the reviewer that visual modelling only allows to infer what is theoretically discriminated by the butterflies, and that the wording of our sentence is confusing. We therefore modified the sentence to account for those precisions: “Morpho butterflies and predators can theoretically visually perceive the difference in the blue coloration between different subspecies of M. helenor…… using both bird and Morpho visual models” (line 206-209).

      (12) Line 222: Either the chi-square test or Fisher's exact test should be sufficient (why report both?)

      Chi-square test relies on large-sample assumptions (expected counts>5) whereas Fischer’s exact test does not and is valid even with small or unbalanced sample sizes. Since the M. bristowi female/M. h. theodorus male paring only occurred 3 times, we do not meet the primary assumptions to apply a Chi-square test, although it is significant. We used a Fischer’s test to confirm the results. Using both and finding that both tests are significant shows that the results are robust, although they may appear redundant. To simplify, we remove the results of the Chisquare test and only keep the Fisher’s test in the methodology and the results.

      (13) Line 224 (and throughout): Degrees of freedom should be provided for statistical tests.

      We reported the statistic value and the degrees of freedom for all mentions of the statistical tests in the main text, except for the Fischer test which does not rely on an asymptotic distribution like the Chi-squared distribution as it is an exact test.

      (14) Lines 266-267: This sentence has interest, but it is rather vague at present. Wouldn't your controls account for the effect of manipulation? This could be explained further.

      During our mate choice experiments, all Morpho female dummies used for the experiments were painted with black markers, either on their dorsal blue band to modify their blue iridescent phenotype, or on their ventral side, thus controlling for the effect of manipulation. However, we cannot rule out that the modification of the dorsal blue iridescence could have had a “repulsive” effect for males for several reasons. For example, depending on the visual discrimination of darker colors by Morphos, the painted black band could have a slightly different color compared to the dark “brown” usually surrounding their blue iridescent patterns. We now explain this in the results (lines 278-283) and in the methodology (lines 638-639)  

      (15) Line 316: I'm not certain that the similarity is best described as "striking", given a P-value of 0.084 for this contrast

      We agree with the reviewer and removed this adjective for this line.

      (16) Lines 387-390: This sentence is puzzling because, theoretically speaking, we should expect selection on visual preference to be heightened (not relaxed) in sympatry if colouration isincluded among the traits used in mate selection. I'm not certain I have understood the meaning here.

      We would like to thank the reviewer for pointing out this typo. If shared predatory pressures favors convergent evolution of color pattern, then the visual signals become less reliable for species recognition. As a result, sexual selection on visual preference is heightened and becomes stronger, favoring the evolution of alternative cues used to discriminate conspecific mates. We changed the sentence and now write “the convergent evolution of iridescent wing patterns… may have negatively impact visual discrimination and favored the evolution of divergent olfactory cues” (lines 457-458).

      (17) Line 529: Mating experiments. Given that these are quite large butterflies, I wondered whether a 3x3x2m cage would be sufficient in size to allow the expression of male courtship. A brief description of the courtship behaviour in these species or Morphos generally would be a useful addition to the paper.

      A cage this size was enough for the males to express a flight behavior similar to what can be seen in nature, while also being able to see the females (live females or dummies). We tried to perform mate experiments in a larger cage (7m x 5m x 3m) but the trials were not conclusive because male did not find the dummies depending on where they were flying in the cage. A 3mx3mx2m cage is a good compromise maximizing interactions while still allowing enough space to fly. We now describe Morpho male behavior and female behavior in the methods (lines 613-618).

      (18) Line 546: Why are both tests needed (chi-square AND Fisher's exact)?

      Similarly to our answer on recommendations #12, were used both tests to show robustness in the statistical results. We only kept the Fisher’s test results to simplify the results.

    1. Finalemen

      intégrer la proposition de Chloé : Là où l'entrée du numérique au musée s'est articulé à un idéal de participation des visiteurs à travers une offre interactive (Vidal, 2021), le projet ethno, proche dans sa structure du CD-ROM, ne s'inscrit pas encore tout à fait dans cette logique.

    2. (“Décret n° 2013-157 Du 21 Février 2013 Portant Création de l’Etablissement Public Du Musée Des Civilisations de l’Europe Et de La Méditerranée (MuCEM)” n.d.)

      rectifier le format zotero

    3. Ce « musée-laboratoire », comme l’a souhaité son fondateur Georges Henri Rivière, rassemblait chercheurs et conservateurs en ethnologie. Au départ essentiellement axé sur les cultures rurales préindustrielles, son champ d’action s’élargit au fil du temps aux problématiques plus contemporaines. Un de ses modes d’action privilégié est l’enquête-collecte8. Au-delà des objets, le musée déploie donc un très riche fonds d’archives photographiques et orales, et inclut sur une bibliothèque spécialisée. Il fut un des musées pionniers en matière d’informatique appliquée à la gestion et l’étude des collections dès le début des années 1970.

      si on doit gagner des caractères ça peut sauter

    1. Once out of the water, fish’s gills often collapse and their swim bladders can even rupture because of the sudden change in pressure.

      Where did the author find this information?

    2. Fish have nerves, just like cats, dogs, and humans, so they can feel pain. Hooked fish endure not only physical pain but also terror.

      Do fish actually have feelings?

    1. ემოციური ინტელექტი ეს არის ადამიანის უნარი გააცნობიეროს საკუთარი ემოციები, მართოს ეს ემოციები,გაიგოს სხვების ემოციები,მოახდინოს ემპათია და ეფექტურად მართოს ურთიერთობები. ემოციური ინტელექტი წარმოადგენს ადამიანის უნარს, გამოიცნოს, დაასახელოს, გააკონტროლოს და მართოს როგორც საკუთარი, ისე სხვისი ემოციები. ეს არის ფსიქოლოგიური კონცეპტი, რომელიც ბოლო ათწლეულების განმავლობაში განსაკუთრებით მნიშვნელოვანი გახდა როგორც პირად ცხოვრებაში, ისე სოციალურ და პროფესიულ ურთიერთობებში. როგორც სხვადასხვა წყაროები ამბობენ, ემოციური ინტელექტი შედგება ხუთი ძირითადი კომპონენტისგან: თვითშეცნობა, თვითრეგულაცია, შინაგანი მოტივაცია, ემპათია და სოციალური უნარები.თვითშეცნობა გულისხმობს საკუთარი ემოციების გაცნობიერებას და იმის გააზრებას, თუ როგორ მოქმედებენ ეს ემოციები ჩვენს ქცევაზე და ურთიერთობებზე, თვითრეგულაცია ნიშნავს იმპულსების მართვას,შინაგანი მოტივაცია არის ის ძალა, რომელიც ადამიანს შიგნიდან უბიძგებს მოქმედებისკენ, მიზნებისკენ სწრაფვისა და საკუთარი თავის გაუმჯობესებისკენ,ემპათია ემოციური ინტელექტის ერთ-ერთი ყველაზე ღირებული და რთული ასპექტი. ეს არის უნარი, იგრძნო სხვების ემოციები და გაუგო მათ მდგომარეობას ისე, თითქოს თვითონ გადიოდე იმავე გზას. შესაძლებელია ითქვას, რომ ემოციური ინტელექტი განსხვავდება იმ დოზით, რა დოზითაც განსხვავდება ადამიანების ფსიქოლოგია, შესაბამისად აღნიშნული ეფუძნება პიროვნების ფსიქოლოგიურ ამინდს, რისი შესწავლის მეთოდებიც დღეს აქტიურად ვითარდება.

    1. Saussure provides a critique of treating language as a “naming process — a list of words, each corresponding to the thing it names” (Saussure 65). The linguistic sign, according to him, does not unite a name and a thing but a concept and the sound image. We have to be careful that the sound image here doesn’t mean physical sound but the psychological imprint of the sound upon our senses. He coins the word signifier for the sound image and signified for the concept. A linguistic sign hence consists of signifier and signified. Here is an example. Take the word ‘dog’, it is the sign. The concept i.e. the signified is: ‘a four-legged animal that barks ’. The signifier is the sound image: ‘/dɒɡ/’.

      semiotics/semiology

      he provides a critique of treating language as a "naming process"

      signifier (sound image) x signified (the concept) = sign (naming)

      example;

      • dog = is the sign/naming
      • "a four-legged animal that barks" is signified concept (picture) in our minds
      • ‘/dɒɡ/’ is the sound image/ signifier
    2. It can also be added that the constituent parts derive their relevance within the structure. But this relevance is not derived out of any intrinsic or innate characteristic that they possess, but because of the relation of one to another, among themselves.

      the concept of "arbitrariness"

    1. Weialala leia 290 Wallala leialala

      Line 266 says, "The river sweats / Oil and tar." This line is contradictory in obvious ways; for one, a healthy river should not contain industrial waste like "oil and tar." The polluted river is another example of how nature is disrupted by modern life and industrialization. Also, a river shouldn't "sweat;" it's supposed to flow. The following line, as well as line 273, describes how "the barges drift" and "wash" rather than sail with purpose. There are hints of rhythm in this section, but they are inconsistent and sluggish, mimicking "the turning tide." Further down, lines 277-78 state "Weialala leia / Wallala leialala," which, in the context of Wagner, refers to the Rhinemaidens' song, a sorrowful song of lost gold and a pure, natural world despoiled by greed. By placing it here, Eliot transplants this lament to the polluted Thames. The modern world has its own stolen gold (industrial wealth), which has similarly cursed it, leading to spiritual decay. The song becomes a ghostly echo of a lost purity, now meaningless in its new context, just a sound the river makes. The scene of the poem then shifts to a ghostly memory: "Elizabeth and Leicester / Beating oars / The stern was formed / A gilded shell / Red and gold." This seems like a moment of romance and pageantry. However, Froude reveals the reality beneath the "gilded shell." Froude's history details the political intrigue, the suspected murder of Leicester's wife, and Elizabeth's calculated use of marriage as a political tool. Their relationship was not pure passion but a "fatal affection" entangled with power, suspicion, and death. The "brisk swell" in line 284 that they create is not just water, but the ripple of historical consequence and personal sin. The same Rhinemaidens' lament follows them, subtly linking their political greed to Alberich's curse on the ring.

    1. controlled capitalist market economies.

      A social democratic with the three branches of government executive, judicial, and legislative branches and also has a national parliament.

    1. Peeling, stripping my skin, causing a hemorrhage that left congealed black blood all over my body

      μου θύμισε λίγο την ταινία sorry to bother you https://www.imdb.com/video/vi1982642201/?playlistId=tt5688932&ref_=tt_ov_pr_ov_vi . Στην οποία ο πρωταγωνιστής δουλεύει σε ένα τηλεφωνικό κέντρο. Οι πελάτες τον αναγνωρίζουν από τη "μαύρη του φωνή" και του το κλείνουν. Για αυτό το λόγο αρχίζει να μιλάει με "λευκή φωνή" έτσι ώστε να κλείνει τις συμφωνίες. Με ένα τρόπο δηλαδή να ξεριζώνει τη μαύρη του φωνή, ακόμα και αν πονάει βάναυσα, ώστε να αντιμετωπίσει τον λευκό άνθρωπο. Σαν να φοράει δηλαδή τη λευκή μάσκα του, η οποία όμως βιάζει ολοένα και περισσότερο το δέρμα του, τη φωνή του.

      Το δέρμα, επίσης, εμφανίζεται πολύ σημαντική ύλη σε όλο το κείμενο (από τον τίτλο είναι εμφανές...). Πως διαχειρίζεσαι την κατάσταση ενός δέρματος που έχεις αλλά για αυτό σε αντιμετωπίζουν με τόση βία;

    2. suffocating

      πως μπορούμε να εξετάσουμε την ασφυξία ως μία αναλυτική έννοια, η οποία συνδέεται άμεσα με τη μαύρη σκέψη και θα μπορούσε να αποτελεί αναλυτική κατηγορία για να εξεταστούν άλλες καταστάσεις, άλλα γεγονότα; ή πως μπορούμε να εξετάσουμε το συναίσθημα τησ ασφυξίας ως ένα συναίσθημα το οποίο έχει παραχθεί σε/από μαύρα σώματα;

    1. Any surname ending with 'son' can be traced back to the Vikings as they brought in the 'son of' name structure. In Ireland and Scotland, the ‘Mc’ is the equivalent. Many of Britain’s towns and place names are derived from the Old Norse language, because the Vikings settled there – especially in northern and eastern England. Towns and places ending in ‘thorpe’ (an ‘outlying farm’ or ‘torp’ in Swedish today), ‘by’ (‘farm’ in Old English and ‘village’ in modern Swedish) and 'kirk' (meaning ‘church’ or ‘assembly’) are common. From Dunthorpe in Oxfordshire to Kirkby in Liverpool, the Viking presence is everywhere in England.1 / 4Show image on full screenPrevious imageNext imageAifur Restaurant & Bar, StockholmAifur is named after a Viking ship and offers a historical dining concept. On the small stage, musicians often sit and play music with historical instruments.Photo: Cheyenne Olander/AifurShow image informationi

      That’s amazing. Vikings influenced the English language and engineering principles that made society more advanced.

    2. Creative in spirit, Vikings had a way with wood and metal, fashioning these materials into beautiful objects. Silversmiths kept busy – Vikings of both genders loved adorning themselves with jewellery such as arm rings and amulets. But the masterpiece above all was the Viking longship – a precisely engineered vessel built to travel fast and go ashore with ease.

      I wonder what fashion trends developed over time?

    3. Alongside growing crops and keeping animals, Vikings were inventive and craft-focused. They were expert silversmiths and enjoyed wearing jewellery and ornaments made of silver, gold and other metals. Highly resourceful, they used every scrap of natural materials to create textiles and objects such as the Vikings’ famous ‘drinking horn’, made from the horns of their farm animals. This craft-focused, 'back to basics' attitude is reflected in Swedish society today, with sustainability and resourcefulness becoming ever more important

      More and more I read, the more and more i'm intrigued by the culture to learn more behind the scenes especially when art is involved

    4. tend to dominate popular culture today, most Vikings led peaceful, farmer-style lives close to nature with their families, with strong women at the fore

      I always wondered if they had a soft side! I knew deep down the Vikings had a soft side. No one is truly that violent or the whole group, especially when children are involved.

    5. Free-spirited, courageous and innovative, they were true explorers and travelled the world as far as north America and Asia

      Sounds like my kind of people, minus the violence

    6. Leaving their homelands in Scandinavia – Sweden, Norway and Denmark – these fearless seafarers set off in expertly engineered longships to trade and raid – and find better places to live. They traversed the coastlines of Europe, staking claims in countries such as Britain, France, Spain, Italy and modern-day Ukraine, Belarus and Russia. One well-documented raid, which has contributed to the Vikings' coldblooded reputation, played out in AD793 when the Vikings attacked a monastery at Lindisfarne in northeast England's Northumbria. Armed with axes and swords, the Vikings burned down buildings and raided monasteries for treasure.

      From what I can gather from this, the Vikings were the "bad apples" of the bunch, setting out to make their own lives by traveling to find a new home and stealing resources to sustain themselves.

    1. The city was sacked by Gauls in 390 BCE, but recovered and expanded. A Latin League helped protect the city until 340 BCE, when Rome defeated its former allies and dissolved the alliance.

      Wow, this is interesting — even though Rome was destroyed by the Gauls in 390 BCE, it didn’t give up and actually came back stronger! It’s kind of like a comeback story after a major setback. The Latin League was like a team-up for protection, but once Rome got powerful enough, it defeated its own allies and took full control — kind of like when a smaller group grows so strong that it doesn’t need help anymore and decides to lead on its own.

    2. Zeno saw the universe as a divine entity, with humans sharing its rational spark. His Republic, written as a response to Plato's book of the same name, imagined a stateless community, which shocked Athenian elites

      Wow, this is so cool — Zeno saw the whole universe as divine, and believed everyone shares a bit of its rational spark! It’s kind of like saying we’re all connected through the same energy or wisdom, like how people today talk about being “one with the universe.” His idea of a stateless community is also wild — it’s almost like an ancient version of trying to imagine a world with no governments or borders, just people living by shared reason and virtue.

    1. The association of various patient- and disease characteristics and post-aSAH depression have been investigated in a wide range of studies with varying results. Occasionally, female sex has been associated with post-aSAH depression8,9, along with younger age10, ethnicity and non-fluency in the majority language3. Multiple studies have shown association between prior psychiatric conditions and post-aSAH depression3,8,11, along with certain somatic comorbidities such as diabetes and chronic obstructive pulmonary disease being weakly associated12. Outcome tools such as the Modified Rankin Scale (mRS) and the Glasgow Outcome Scale (GOS) have been consistently demonstrated to be associated with depression9,10,12, as has post-aSAH epilepsy10. Certain evidence also indicates an association between depression and aSAH-related infarcts and infarct or aneurysm location3,13,14, and at least two studies show a correlation between cortisol levels and depression15,16. Finally, indicators of more severe disease (e.g. increased Hunt-Hess score) have shown an association with post-aSAH depression9,10.

      Ska kortas ned men får stå fn.

    2. SAH. Importantly, we also show that this increased risk is approximately 40% higher than what would be expected as a consequence of intensive care alone, supporting an inherent pathophysiologic effect of aSAH on the development om mood disorders. Additionally, we are able to translate this increased risk into a statistically significant increase in sick leave, highlighting the socio-economic impact of the disease.

      Har inte sett supplement men jag tror generellt att denna studie är värd att submitta rätt högt. Den är bra! Men då krävs ofta lite lull-lull i supplement. Tydligt vad som är inkluderat i SAH, kanske nån sensitivitetsanalys. Kanske kolla det du resonerar om i discussion som jag nämnde förut. Är det bara så att SAH-patienter följs upp så sjukt bra och får SSRI insatt därför. Kolla kanske om de får annat insatt i mkt högra utsträckning som INTE har koppling till depression som jag snackade om där uppe nånstans. Vi kan snacka mer om det vb /JE

    3. ore pronounced in patients treated with AD after aSAH. Since we have elected to view SA and DP as outcomes resulting from mood disorders, we have chosen to evaluate a time frame beginning one year after the onset of disease. While this approach allows for a correct temporal analysis, we are still unable to assess causality and our results should therefore be interpreted with caution. Regardless, our findings do highlight the association between mood disorders and socio-economic consequences with profound patient-centred implications.

      Kan ha missat i Methods men VET vi att SA/DP kommer efter insättning av antidepp? Kan det vara så att temporalt kommer först SA/DP, det blir man deprimerad av och får därför AD insatt? Alltså är det kodat så att SA/DP är tvingat att hända efter uthämtade AD? Också competing risks /JE --> <!--# Jag tycker att ett tillägg i diskussionen bör ha med SSRI som neuromodulator per se. Det kommer data från djur och humanstudier kring hjärnplasticitet och SSRI-effekten av detta. Kan vi sen ökning av SSRI senaste åren dvs 2020-2024 jmf 2014-2019? De senaste 5 åren har mer data talat för SSRI som en stroke-regenerator.../PA

    4. he significant differences between the study and control cohorts, especially with regards to markers of disease severity, a propensity score matching process was undertaken to evaluate the marginal effect of aSAH on the risk of post-ICU AD-treatment versus the risk conferred by exposure to intensive care alone. Using a the Random Fores

      Saknar en tabell, med antal patienter, som visar matchade SAH mot icke SAH. Denna matchning ligger till grund för pekets huvudbudskap och är därför jätteviktig. /AO

    5. a cohort consisting of all ICU-patients with non-neurovascular primary causes of admission in Sweden between 2012-2018. A comparison of baseline demographics, comorbidities, and ICU-variables is shown in Table 2. Of note, patients in the control cohort were predominantly male, and both older and more comorbid tha

      Skulle vara kanske mer intressant att se tabell 2 för de kontroller som de facto jämfördes. Alltså de matchade kontrollerna. Åtminstone båda varianterna (239,570 kontroller samt de 4*3637 kontrollerna som användes i analysen). /JE Man kanske kan lägga till en tredje kolumn som visar matchade populationen? / RFJ-

    6. bootstrap resample and the resulting metrics presented as risk ratios along with their bootstrap distribution.

      Kan man tänka sig att SAH-patienterna följs upp mycket bättre än en generell IVA-kohort. Dvs att det finns en diagnostik-bias eller vad det nu kan heta. SAH får oftare uppföljning hos dr Alpqvist, Lundberg, Svensson som är inkännande och sätter in AD? Gubben med dille får uppföljning vid behov? Om ja så kanske man kan gräva i det och jämföra med en annan omhuldad grupp. Alternativt testa med “negativ kontroll”. Välj ett LM som INTE är kopplat till depression. Typ Omeprazol. Eller nåt vanligt blodtrycksläkemedel. Om SAH också har en ökad förskrivning av detta icke-depressionskopplade läkemedel så indikerar det på att de bara följs upp mer och bättre och inte givet har mer depressioner. /JE Fundera och regionala skillnader??? /LH

      Jag vet inte hur generella IVA-pat följs upp men SAH-pat har en enormt standard uppföljning och kontroll-schema sedan 2005. Samtliga inkluderas i neurorehab-kedja, samtliga har åt 3 mån + 1år hos ansv neurokir. Samtliga har en uppföljning via neurorehab i öppen-vård, efter utskrivning från inneliggande neurorehab och därefter går remissen till husläkare. ALLA aSAH pat som är yrkesföra (dvs inte pensionärer) får en sjuksrivningsperiod om minst 100% i 3 mån /PA

    7. rtion of AD-treated patients requiring immediate invasive ventilation and having a poorer initial consciousness level.

      14% previous ASAH låter mkt /PA Oklarheter hos flera medförfattare ang vad "ASAH" är. JO påpekade också risken att fånga upp nuvarande Dx i denna grupp. Kolla upp detta! -->

    8. In total, 78985 unique patients were extracted from the NPR (Figure 1). Following the selection strategy described in the Methods section, a study cohort of 7378 patients with a presumed aSAH was identified. Of these patients, 1423 had dispensed a prescription of antidepressants in the 180 days prior to ICU-admission for aSAH and were thus excluded from final analysis, along with 1652 patients who died within 1 year following ICU-admission or where the ICU admission occurred less than 1 year prior to the end of the study. The remai

      Här vill jag att studiepopulationen beskrivs bättre. Är siffror samma som aSAH? Ålder? Incidens? Är det rimligt att ha med 2024 års kohort, läkemedel för depression sätts ofta in senare i förloppet är min erfarenhet./PA

    9. Propensity scores denoting the probability of cohort membership (study or control cohorts) were subsequently calculated for each admission in the combined data set by employing a Random Forests classification model. First, 20% of the data was allocated to a holdout set while the model was trained on the remaining 80%. Model preprocessing included a log-transformation of the ICU length of stay variable along with dummy coding of nominal variables and normalization of covariates . Hyperparameter tuning across a 10-fold cross-validation resample of the training subset was performed by using a space-filling grid search with the Latin Hypercube method and a grid size of 20. The hyperparameter settings maximizing the Area Under the Receiver Operating Characteristic curve (AUROC) were selected and the final model trained. Subsequent model testing on the 20% holdout set was performed, analysing accuracy, Brier score, and AUROC, with further validation performed using a 10-fold cross-validation resample and plotting the ROC for each fold individually. Finally, the propensity scores were predicted on the entire data set.
    10. Raw register data were thoroughly preprocessed in order to create continuo

      Skulle ta bort. Om det står kvar vill man (jag) gärna veta hur. Tänker att det är underförstått att du gjort det. Du nämner ju nedan viktiga preprocessningar som du gjort.

    11. mitigate the competing risk

      Här kanske man får en fråga om vad denna exklusion innebär för resultaten. Ev göra en sensitivitetsanalys med non-random dropouts due to death. /JE Kolla Dufoil et al Statist Med 2004 el Scharfstein et al Am Stat Assoc 1999 samt Eriks opioidpek i CCM

    12. 180

      Är det inte rimligare att räkna med 1 år innan? Om frågeställningen är om SAH inducerar egentlig depression så känns 3 mån innan lite väl snålt. En del pat kommer då ha förskrivning hemma och vara under diagnosen depression, men inte under behandling. Rimligare och jmf med diagnoskod ICD för depression? /PA

    13. Using the nationwide study cohort, selection of aSAH cases were subsequently performed using the steps seen in Figure 1 (SQL source code available in Supplementary Materials).

      Här tycker jag det finns utrymme för att betona selektionen. I60-I60.9 inkluderar även blödning pga fistlar, AVM med blödning utan parenkymblödning samt dissektionsorsakad blödning. I text tycker jag det ska beskrivas även och inte bara ref till supplementary. Räknar jag grovt blir detta incidens om 7/100000/år, vilket är i överkant mtp att vi idag snarare har sjukhusbehandlad incidens omkring 3/100000/år sedan 2010.

    1. In the door-yard fronting an old farm-house, near the white-wash’d palings, Stands the lilac bush, tall-growing, with heart-shaped leaves of rich green, With many a pointed blossom, rising, delicate, with the perfume strong I love, With every leaf a miracle......and from this bush in the door-yard, With delicate-color’d blossoms, and heart-shaped leaves of rich green, A sprig, with its flower, I break.

      The nature imagery here is showing the beauty in nature as things grow, and is also a symbol for death and life, because in nature plants have to die at some point in the winter to come back to life in the spring. This connects to lincolns death because although witman is mourning his loss, he tries to remember his life in a good and positive light.

    1. There are no differences at all — both species look and act the same everywhere.

      What it means: Either they’re responding identically to the environment, or competition is so weak that no differentiation happens.

    Annotators

    1. THREE GUIDING PRINCIPLES OF RECOMMENDATIONS

      Very expansive and thoughtful. This report was to recommend an implementation of social insurance right after the war ended. It classifed a broad range of people that would recieve these benefits. I believe it touched on where the money would come from.

  2. social-media-ethics-automation.github.io social-media-ethics-automation.github.io
    1. Matt Binder. The majority of traffic from Elon Musk's X may have been fake during the Super Bowl, report suggests. February 2024. Section: Tech. URL: https://mashable.com/article/x-twitter-elon-musk-bots-fake-traffic (visited on 2024-03-31).

      This article is a great example of the downsides when it comes to the simplification of data. To briefly summarize, it basically explains that during the 2024 Superbowl, 75.85% of traffic from X's advertising was fake. Advertising companies learned that while their viewership seemed high, their website clicks/content engagement was staggeringly low in comparison. This revealed that X had a huge bot problem. A real example talked about in the article was how a small business owner, despite having 29,000 views on his ad posted to X, Google Analytics reported that X wasn't the source of any of the traffic during that time. This shows that simplifying certain data, such as social media traffic, can blur the line between what is human vs. bot. However, bots cannot be entirely to blame, as the article talks about how this could be an Elon Musk problem as well. The 2023 Superbowl was only a few months after Musk had acquired twitter and had 72% less fake traffic. So, it’s possible that money hungry billionaires like him could be purposely exploiting data simplification, and allowing more bots onto his site in order to increase profits made from advertisers.

    2. Anna Lytical [@theannalytical]. Send me the worst gender selection forms you've seen, I'll start. January 2021. URL: https://twitter.com/theannalytical/status/1349392166716657664 (visited on 2023-11-24).

      The image from this tweet shows the gender selection options from what is assumed to be an online tax form. Hilariously though, besides "Male" and "Female", it also displays the options "N/A", "Unknown", and "Tax Entity". Aside from this being a funny yet really bad mistake on the website's part, it also ironically displays the difficulties people within the LGBTQ community may face when it comes to data collection. Since gender can be considered a spectrum, it can become extremely complicated to accurately create a way to gather data on ALL the genders, besides the main two (male and female).

    3. Kurt Wagner. Musk's Dispute With Twitter Over Bots Continues to Dog Deal. Bloomberg, July 2022. Section: Math. URL: https://www.bloomberg.com/news/articles/2022-07-07/twitter-reiterates-that-spam-bots-are-well-under-5-of-users#xj4y7vzkg (visited on 2023-11-24).

      This article is about Elon's query of the percentage of fake accounts and his plan of buying Twitter. Currently, twitter is already under the name of Elon Musk, but the question about the fake accounts on social media is still a worth-considering question. Data on the internet is simplified and it comes with some bad impacts, so when using internet, we should be able to realize what is real and what is fake. And when we post data on the internet, we should try to use the most helpful way to represent the reality we want to show to others.

    4. Anna Lauren Hoffmann. Data Violence and How Bad Engineering Choices Can Damage Society. Medium, April 2018. URL: {https://medium.com/@annaeveryday/data-violence-and-how-bad-engineering-choices-can-damage-society-39e44150e1d4} (visited on 2023-11-24).

      [d28] Hoffman, "Data Violence" (2018): Harm arises not only from biased outcomes but also from categorization/interface choices that force people to "adapt." Key takeaway: If the affected community doesn't shape the architecture, technical fairness improvements can still perpetuate violence. This reframes the formal failure in §4.3: a flawed drop-down menu isn't a failure; it's a governance failure.

    5. Elon Musk [@elonmusk]. @PPathole Exactly. I have yet to see *any* analysis that has fake/spam/duplicates at \textless 5%. May 2022. URL:

      It’s interesting that Elon Musk is so concerned about bots on Twitter. Not all bots are harmful, and it’s hard to know the exact number of bots on the platform. Claiming there are less than 5% might oversimplify a complex situation. Even if some accounts are fake or spam, that doesn’t change the fact that bots exist and can have different impacts.

    6. Shannon Sullivan and Nancy Tuana. Race and Epistemologies of Ignorance. State University of New York Press, Albany, 2007. ISBN 978-0-7914-8003-8. URL: https://muse.jhu.edu/pub/163/monograph/book/5200 (visited on 2024-04-01).

      This article introduces a book that generally talks about the connection between ignorance and racism. The gap for knowledge is not the only reason causes the racism, and the main reason is the accidental result of epistemological oversight.

    7. Shannon Bond. Twitter takes Elon Musk to court, accusing him of bad faith and hypocrisy. NPR, July 2022. URL: https://www.npr.org/transcripts/1111032233 (visited on 2023-11-24).

      In July 2022, Tesla CEO Elon Musk announced the termination of the $44 billion agreement to acquire Twitter. This decision stemmed from the company's misrepresentation of fake account data and refusal to provide critical information. According to the merger agreement, if Twitter prevails in court, it can compel Musk to complete the transaction; otherwise, he must pay a $1 billion termination fee. Legal experts believe Twitter has a high probability of winning the case.

    1. , people are inherently creative, at least within the bounds of their experience,

      I agree with this idea because I believe everyone can be creative in their own way. Creativity doesn’t have to mean inventing something completely new, but it can come from noticing problems or ways things could be improved. I realize that I’ve sometimes doubted my ideas because they felt small or “obvious,” but this reading helped me see that even those ideas have value.

    1. instructions were duly sent to the recruiting agent in Madeira to discontinue sending emigrants to Guyana after March 1, 1842

      Who was the recruiting agent?

    1. How do I write a chrome extension that applies a content script to a Single Page Application?

      4.

      • /single Page Aplication
      • SPA
      • content script
      • chrome extension
  3. www.planalto.gov.br www.planalto.gov.br
    1. prazo
      • Edição Extraordinária nº 8
      • Direito Público
      • 17 de janeiro de 2023
      • Processo: AgInt no AREsp 2.118.653-SP, Rel. Ministro Humberto Martins, Segunda Turma, por unanimidade, julgado em 28/11/2022, DJe 30/11/2022.

      Ramo do Direito DIREITO PROCESSUAL CIVIL

      TemaPaz, Justiça e Instituições Eficazes <br /> Recurso especial e agravo em recurso especial. Tempestividade recursal. Aferição. Prazos em curso no Tribunal de origem.

      Destaque - A tempestividade do recurso especial e do respectivo agravo em recurso especial deve ser aferida de acordo com os prazos em curso na Corte de <u>origem</u>.

      Informações do Inteiro Teor - A tempestividade do recurso especial e do agravo respectivo deve ser aferida de acordo com os prazos em curso na Corte de origem, e não no STJ.

      • Esta é a jurisprudência do Superior Tribunal de Justiça, no sentido de que "O prazo dos recursos interpostos perante a Corte de origem ainda que estejam endereçados a este Tribunal - obedece ao calendário de funcionamento do Tribunal de origem, sendo irrelevante, para a verificação da tempestividade do recurso, a existência de recesso forense, no STJ" (AgInt no AREsp 1.149.576/SP, rel. Ministra Assusete Magalhães, Segunda Turma, julgado em 3/4/2018, DJe 10/4/2018).
    2. § 4º
      • Edição Extraordinária nº 8
      • Direito Público
      • 17 de janeiro de 2023
      • Processo: REsp 1.934.881-SP, Rel. Ministra Assusete Magalhães, Segunda Turma, por unanimidade, julgado em 27/9/2022, DJe 30/9/2022.

      Ramo do Direito DIREITO PROCESSUAL CIVIL

      TemaPaz, Justiça e Instituições Eficazes <br /> Cumprimento de sentença. Apresentação de memória do cálculo. Impugnação. Contador judicial. Homologação de cálculo em valor superior ao postulado pelo exequente. Decisão ultra petita. Não configuração.

      Destaque - Não configura julgamento ultra petita o acolhimento dos cálculos elaborados por contador Judicial em valor superior ao apresentado pelo exequente.

      Informações do Inteiro Teor - Cinge-se a controvérsia a determinar se extrapola os limites do pedido decisão que homologa os cálculos apresentados pela contadoria judicial em valor superior àquele apresentado pela parte exequente em sua memória de cálculo.

      • Conforme a jurisprudência consolidada no Superior Tribunal de Justiça, "o acolhimento dos cálculos elaborados por Contador Judicial em valor superior ao apresentado pelo exequente não configura julgamento ultra petita, uma vez que, ao adequar os cálculos aos parâmetros da sentença exequenda, garante a perfeita execução do julgado" (STJ, AgRg no Ag 1.088.328/SP, Rel. Ministro Napoleão Nunes Maia Filho, Quinta Turma, DJe de 16/8/2010).

      • Como apontado, "os cálculos da liquidação devem ser fiéis ao título exequendo, sem que isso configure decisão ultra ou extra petita, <u>caso se homologue valor maior que o apresentado pelas partes</u>. Dessa forma, considerando as explanações lançadas pela Perícia Contábil, deve-se observar a regra da fidelidade ao título executivo em sede de cumprimento e liquidação de sentença, conforme disposto no art. 509, § 4º, do CPC/2015".

    1. públicos
      • Edição Extraordinária nº 8
      • Direito Público
      • 17 de janeiro de 2023
      • Processo: REsp 1.986.143-DF, Rel. Ministro Herman Benjamin, Segunda Turma, por unanimidade, julgado em 6/12/2022, DJe 19/12/2022.

      Ramo do Direito DIREITO ADMINISTRATIVO

      TemaPaz, Justiça e Instituições Eficazes <br /> Imóvel público. Ocupação por particular. Ausência de prévia formalização de ato ou negócio jurídico administrativo. Taxa de ocupação. Cobrança. Cabimento.

      Destaque - É cabível a cobrança de taxa de ocupação de imóvel público, ainda que não haja prévia formalização de ato ou negócio jurídico administrativo.

      Informações do Inteiro Teor - Com relação à taxa de ocupação, o art. 24 da Lei n. 4.545/1964 prescreve, tão somente, que a ocupação de imóvel público por particular tem caráter precário e submete-se à conveniência e à oportunidade da Administração Pública. Nada mais. Do dispositivo não se extrai o entendimento de que a taxa de ocupação só é devida caso exista prévia autorização formal do Governador do Distrito Federal.

      • A interpretação de que a taxa de ocupação de imóvel público só é devida caso haja prévia formalização de ato ou negócio jurídico administrativo contraria o princípio da boa-fé objetiva. <u>O ocupante irregular de bem público não pode se beneficiar da sua própria ilegalidade para deixar de cumprir obrigação a todos imposta, o pagamento da taxa de ocupação</u>.

      • Nos termos do art. 884 do Código Civil, caracteriza enriquecimento sem causa ocupar, usar, fruir ou explorar ilicitamente a totalidade ou parte do patrimônio público, material e imaterial.

      • Quem ocupa, usa ou explora indevidamente bem público deve, em primeiro lugar, restituir tudo o que contra legem se apropriou, inclusive rendimentos com ele auferidos; em segundo lugar, deve pagar tributos incidentes, além de indenização tanto pela restrição em si do domínio público, como por eventuais danos causados, sem prejuízo da restauração das funções ecológicas, paisagísticas e outras, tangíveis ou intangíveis, acaso afetadas.

      • Sendo o particular detentor de má-fé de bem público, haverá de responder, entre outras obrigações, por todos os frutos que o Estado deixou de perceber, na forma do art. 1.216 do Código Civil, cujas disposições a respeito do possuidor se aplicam também, com mais razão, ao simples detentor ou a quem se apossa irregularmente daquilo que a todos pertence. Acrescente-se que, no caso do patrimônio público, a má-fé do detentor se constitui ipso facto, a partir da ocupação sem título válido, dispensada qualquer forma de notificação ou formalidade.

      • À luz do princípio da indisponibilidade do interesse público, eventual omissão do Estado no exercício do seu poder de polícia - ao deixar de fiscalizar e adotar medidas cabíveis para se opor ou reagir à apropriação irregular de bem público - <u>não transforma o errado em certo</u>. Irrelevante, ademais, que a injuricidade ocorra às vistas do Administrador ou com a sua inércia, conivência ou mesmo (inconcebível) aceitação tácita.

      • É certo que as prerrogativas do Estado encarnam obrigações, na fórmula de deveres-poderes. Essa constatação, contudo, não implica defender ou aceitar que quando falhar o Estado em prevenir, reparar ou reprimir o apossamento indevido do patrimônio público, venha-se, sob o pretexto de não exercício do poder de polícia e de falha no dever de agir, a abrir as portas para premiar o infrator e, dessa maneira, punir a sociedade mais uma vez, já <u>agravada com a inépcia e desídia daqueles que são pagos para protegê-la</u>.

      • Finalmente, desimportante a antiguidade da ilicitude, diante da imprescritibilidade da natureza do bem público, que deságua no veto a usucapião (art. 102 do Código Civil). Outrossim, a cobrança e o pagamento da taxa de ocupação não se prestam quer como argumento para validar o que, por ventura, não seja passível de legalização, quer para infirmar a precariedade de anterior autorização ou ato da Administração, quer para embaraçar o exercício pelo Estado de medidas de garantia eficaz da integridade e inviolabilidade do patrimônio público.

  4. www.planalto.gov.br www.planalto.gov.br
    1. não exime

      A existência de plano de desenvolvimento integrado para a região metropolitana/ aglomeração urbana não dispensa a criação, pelos Municípios, de Plano Diretor.

    1. eLife Assessment

      This study presents important information about the role of mu opioid receptors in neurotransmission between the medial habenula and the interpeduncular nucleus. The authors provide convincing evidence that mu opioid receptor activation has differential effects on transmission from substance P neurons and cholinergic neurons, and that blockade of potassium channels can unmask a nicotinic cholinergic synaptic response. This work will be of high interest to those studying this brain region, and potentially to the larger neuroscience community studying motivated behavior.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, the authors demonstrate for the first time that opioid signaling has opposing effects on the same target neuron depending on the source of the input. Further, the authors provide evidence to support the role of potassium channels in regulating a brake on glutamatergic and cholinergic signaling, with the latter finding being developmentally regulated and responsive to opioid treatment. This evidence solves a conundrum regarding cholinergic signaling in the interpeduncular nucleus that evaded elucidation for many years.

      Strengths:

      This manuscript provides 3 novel and important findings that significantly advance our understanding of the medial habenula-interpeduncular circuitry:

      (1) Mu opioid receptor activation (mOR) reduces postsynaptic glutamatergic currents elicited from substance P neurons while simultaneously enhancing postsynaptic glutamatergic currents from cholinergic neurons, with the latter being developmentally regulated.

      (2) Substance P neurons from the Mhb provide functional input to the rostral nucleus of the IPN, in addition to the previously characterized lateral nuclei.

      (3) Potassium channels (Kv1.2) provide a break on neurotransmission in the IPN,

      The findings here suggest that the authors have identified a novel mechanism for the normal function of neurotransmission in the IPN, so it would be expected to be observable in almost any animal. In the revised manuscript, the authors put forth significant effort to increase the n, thus increasing the confidence in the observations.

      There are also significant sex differences in nAChR expression in the IPN that might not be functionally apparent using the low n presented here. In the revised manuscript, the authors increased the n, and provided data to the reviewers that no significant sex differences were apparent, although there was a trend. Future studies should examine sex differences in detail.

      There are also some particularly novel observations that are presented but not followed up on, and this creates a somewhat disjointed story. For example, in Figure 2, the authors identify neurons in which no response is elicited by light stimulation of ChAT-neurons, but application of DAMGO (mOR agonist) un-silences these neurons. Are there baseline differences in the electrophysiological or morphological properties of these "silent" neurons compared to the responsive neurons? In the revised manuscript, the authors directly tested this with new experiments in SST+ neurons in the IPN, demonstrating convincingly that mOR activation unsilences these neurons.

      With the revisions, the authors have addressed the reviewers' concerns and significantly improved the manuscript. I find no further weaknesses.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this paper, Chittajallu and colleagues present compelling evidence that mu opioid receptor (MOR) activation can potentiate synaptic neurotransmission in a medial habenula to interpeduncular nucleus (mHb-IPN) subcircuit. While, projections from mHb tachykinin 1 (Tac1) neurons onto lateral IPN neurons show a canonical opioid-induced synaptic depression in glutamate release, excitatory neurotransmission in mHb choline acetyltransferase (ChAT) projections to the rostral IPN is potentiated by opioids. This function emerges around age P27 in mice, when MOR expression in the IPN peaks.

      Strengths:

      Carefully executed electrophysiological experiments with appropriate controls. Interesting description of a neurodevelopmental change in the effects of opioids on mHb-IPN signaling.

      Weaknesses:

      A minor concern is that the genetic strategy used to target the mHb-IPN pathway (constitutive ChR2 expression in all ChAT+ and Tac1+ neurons) might not specifically target this projection. Future studies are needed to examine the precise mechanism whereby MOR signaling can potentiate glutamatergic neurotransmission in ChAT+ MHb-IPN projections."

    4. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Here the authors describe the role of mORs in synaptic glutamate release from substance P and cholinergic neurons in the medial habenula to interpeduncular nucleus (IPN) circuit in adult mice. They show that mOR activation reduces evoked glutamate release from substance P neurons yet increases evoked glutamate release and Ach release from cholinergic neurons. Unlike glutamate release, Ach release is only detected when potassium channels are blocked with 4-AP or dendrotoxin. The authors also report a previously unidentified glutamatergic input to IPR from SP neurons and describe the developmental timing of mOR- facilitation in adolescent mice.

      Strengths:

      - The experiments provide new insight into the role of mORs in controlling evoked glutamate release in a circuit with high levels of mORs and established roles in relevant behaviors.

      - The experiments are rigorous, and the results are clear cut. The conclusions are supported by the data.

      - The findings will be of interest to those working in the field of synaptic transmission and those interested in the function of the medial habenula or interpeduncular nucleus, as well as those seeking to understand the role of opioids on normal and pathological behaviors.

      Weaknesses:

      - The mechanistic underpinnings of these interesting and novel results are not pursued.

    5. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public Reviews):

      Weaknesses: 

      Overall I find the data presented compelling, but I feel that the number of observations is quite low (typically n=3-7 neurons, typically one per animal). While I understand that only a few slices can be obtained for the IPN from each animal, the strength of the novel findings would be more convincing with more frequent observations (larger n, more than one per animal). The findings here suggest that the authors have identified a novel mechanism for the normal function of neurotransmission in the IPN, so it would be expected to be observable in almost any animal. Thus,  it is not clear to me why the authors investigated so few neurons per slice and chose to combine different treatments into one group (e.g. Figure 2f), even if the treatments have the same expected effect.  

      This is a well taken suggestion. However, we must  point out that we do perform statistical analyses on the original datasets and we believe that our conclusions are justified as acknowledged by the Reviewer. As the Reviewer is aware,  the IPN is a small nucleus and with the slicing protocol used, we typically attain 1-2 slices per mouse that are suitable for recordings. Since most of the experiments in the manuscript deals with some form of pharmacological interrogation, we were reticent to use slices that are not naïve and therefore in general did not perform more than 1 cell recording per slice. Having said this, to comply with the Reviewer’s suggestion we have now performed additional experiments to increase the n number for certain experiments. We have amended all figures and legends to incorporate the additional data. We must point out that during the replotting of the data in the summary Figure 8i (previously Figure 7i) we noticed an error with the data representation of the TAC IPL data and have now corrected this oversight  

      Figure 2b,c. 

      500nM DAMGO effect on TAC IPL AMPAR EPSC – n increased from 5 to 9

      Figure 3g. 

      500nM DAMGO effect on CHAT IPR AMPAR EPSC – n increased from 8 to 16 Effect of CTAP on DAMGO on CHAT IPR AMPAR EPSC – n increased from 4 to 7

      Figure 3i. 

      500nm DAMGO or Met-enk effect in “silent” CHAT IPR AMPAR EPSC – n increased    from 7 to 9

      Figure 4e. 

      500nM DAMGO effect on ES coupling – Note: in the original version the n number was 5 and not 7 as written in the figure legend. We have now increased the n from 5 – 9.

      Figure 5e,f. 

      500nM DAMGO effect on TAC IPR AMPAR EPSC – n increased from 5 to 9

      Figure 7f.

      Effect of DHE on EPSC amplitude after application of DNQX/APV/4-AP or DTX-α – n increased from 7-9.

      Figure 7g.

      Emergence of nAChR EPSC after DTX – n increased from 4 to 7

      Figure 7i. 

      Effect of ambenonium on nAChR amplitude and charge – n increased from 4 to 7

      Supplementary Figure 3c and h

      Effect of DAMGO after DNQX – n increased from 4 to 7

      Effect of DNQX after DAMGO mediated potentiation – n increased from 3 to 5.

      Throughout the study (Figs. 3i, 7f and 8h in the revised manuscript)  we do indeed pool datasets that were amassed from different conditions since we were not directly investigating the possibility of any deviation in the extent of response between said treatments. For example, and as pointed out by the Reviewer, in Fig. 2F (now Fig. 3i) the use of DAMGO and met-ENK were merely employed to ascertain whether light-evoked synaptic transmission (ChATCre:ai32 mice) in cells that had no measurable EPSC could be pharmacologically “unsilenced” by mOR activation. Thus, the means by which mOR receptor was activated was not relevant to this specific question. Note: 2 more recordings are now added to this dataset (Fig. 3i) that were taken from ChATChR2/SSTCre:ai9 mice in response to the comment by this Reviewer below (“Are there baseline differences in the electrophysiological or morphological properties of these "silent" neurons compared to the responsive neurons?”).  Similarly, in the revised Fig.7f we pooled data investigating the pharmacological block of the EPSC that emerged following application of either DNQX/APV/4-AP or DNQX/APV/DTX. Low concentrations 4-AP or DTX were interchangeably employed to reveal the DNQX-insensitive EPSC that we go on to show is indeed the nAChR response. Finally, in Fig. 8h, we pooled data demonstrating a  lack of effect of DAMGO in potentiating  both the glutamatergic and cholinergic arms of synaptic transmission in the OPRM1 KO mice. Again, here we were only interested in determining whether removal of mOR expression prevented potentiation of transmission mediated by mHB ChAT neurons irrespective of neurotransmitter modality.  Thus, overall we were careful to only pool data in those instances where it  would not change the interpretation and hence conclusions reached. 

      There are also significant sex differences in nAChR expression in the IPN that might not be functionally apparent using the low n presented here. It would be helpful to know which of the recorded neurons came from each sex, rather than presenting only the pooled data.  

      As the reviewer correctly states there are veins of literature concerning a divergence, based on sex, of not only nicotinic receptor expression but also behaviors associated with nicotine addiction. However, we have reanalyzed our datasets focusing on the extent of the mOR potentiation of glutamatergic and cholinergic transmission mediated by mHB ChAT neurons in IPR  between male and female mice. Please refer to the Author response image 1 below. Although there is a possible trend towards a higher potentiation of nAChR in female mice, this was not found to be of statistical significance (see Author response image 1 below). We therefore chose not to split our data in the manuscript based on gender.

      Author response image 1.

      Comparison of the mOR (500nM DAMGO) mediated potentiation on evoked (a) AMPAR and (b) nAChR  EPSCs in IPR between male and female mice.  

      There are also some particularly novel observations that are presented but not followed up on, and this creates a somewhat disjointed story. For example, in Figure 2, the authors identify neurons in which no response is elicited by light stimulation of ChAT-neurons, but the application of DAMGO (mOR agonist) un-silences these neurons. Are there baseline differences in the electrophysiological or morphological properties of these "silent" neurons compared to the responsive neurons?  

      Unfortunately, we did not routinely measure intrinsic properties of the recorded postsynaptic neurons nor systematically recovered biocytin fills to assess morphology. Therefore, it remains unclear whether the  neurons in which there were none or minimal AMPAR-mediated EPSCs are distinct to the ones displaying measurable responses. The IPR is resident to GABAergic SST neurons that comprise the most numerous neuron type in this IPN subdivision. Although heavily outnumbered by the SST neurons there are additionally VGluT3+ glutamatergic neurons in IPN. The Reviewer is likely referring to a recent study investigating synaptic transmission specifically onto  SST+ and VGluT3+ neurons in IPN demonstrating that mHB cholinergic mediated glutamatergic input is “weaker” onto the glutamatergic neurons. Furthermore, in some instances synaptic transmission onto this latter population can be “unsilenced” by GABAB receptor activation in a similar manner to that seen with mOR activation in this manuscript when IPR neurons are blindly targeted(Stinson & Ninan, 2025).  Using a similar strategy as in this recent study(Stinson & Ninan, 2025), we now include experiments in which the ChATChR2 mouse was crossed with  a SSTCre:Ai14. This allowed for recording of postsynaptic EPSCs in directly identified SST IPR neurons. We demonstrate that DAMGO can indeed increase glutamatergic EPSCs and in 2 of the cells where light activation demonstrated no appreciable AMPAR EPSC upon maximal LED light activation, DAMGO clearly “unsilenced” transmission.  Thus, our additional analyses directly demonstrate that our original observations concerning mOR modulation extend to the mHb cholinergic AMPAR mediated input onto IPR SST neurons. This additional data is in the revised manuscript (Figure 3D-F, I). Future experimentation will be required to determine if the propensity of encountering a  “silent” input that can be converted to robust synaptic transmission by mOR differs between these two cell types. Furthermore, it will be of interest to investigate if any differences exist in the magnitude of the cholinergic input or the mOR mediated potentiation of co-transmission between postsynaptic SST GABA and glutamatergic neuronal subtypes. 

      Reviewer #2 (Public review)

      Weaknesses: 

      The genetic strategy used to target the mHb-IPN pathway (constitutive expression in all ChAT+ and Tac1+ neurons) is not specific to this projection.  

      This is an important point made. We are acutely aware that the source of the synaptic input in IPN mediated by conditional expression of ChR2 employing  using transgenic cre driver lines does not confer specificity to mHB. This is particularly relevant considering one of the novel observations here relates to  a previously unidentified functional input from TAC1 neurons to the IPR. At this juncture we would like to point the Reviewer to the publicly available Connectivity Atlas provided by the Allen Brain Institute (https://connectivity.brain-map.org/). With reference to mHB TAC1 neuronal output, targeted viral injection into the habenula of Tac1Cre mice allows conditional expression of EGFP to SP neurons as evidenced by the predominant expression of reported fluorescence in dorsal mHB (see Author response image 2 a,b below). Tracing the axonal projections to the IPN clearly demonstrates dense fibers in IPL as expected but also arborization in  IPR (Author response image 2 a,c) . This pattern is reminiscent of that seen in the transgenic Tac1Cre:ai9 or ai32 mice used in the current study (Figs. 1c, 2a, 5c). Closer inspection of the fibers in the IPR reveals putative synaptic bouton like structures as we have shown in Fig. 5a,b (Author response image 2 d below).

      Author response image 2.

      Sterotaxic viral injection into mHB pf Tac1Cre mice taken from Allen Brain connectivity atlas (Link to Connectivity Atlas for mHb SP neuronal projection pattern)

      These anatomical data suggest that part of the synaptic input to the IPR originates from mHB TAC1 neurons although we cannot fully discount additional synaptic input from other brain areas that may impinge on the IPR. Indeed, as the Reviewer points out, it is evident that other regions including the nucleus incertus send outputs to the IPN(Bueno et al., 2019; Liang et al., 2024; Lima et al., 2017). However, it is unclear if neuronal inputs from these alternate sources {Liang, 2024 #123;Lima, 2017 #33}{Bueno, 2019 #178} are glutamatergic in nature AND mediated by a TAC1/OPRM1-expressing neuronal population. Nevertheless, we have now modified text in the discussion to highlight the limitations of using a transgenic strategy (pg 12, para 1).

      In addition, a braking mechanism involving Kv1.2 has not been identified.

      It is unclear to what the Reviewer is referring to here. Although most of our experiments pertaining to the brake on cholinergic  transmission by potassium channels use low concentrations of 4-AP (50100M) which have been used to block Shaker Kv1 channels there although at these concentrations there are additional action at other K+-channels such as Kv3, for instance. However, we essentially demonstrate that a selective Kv1.1 and Kv1.2 antagonist dendrotoxin replicates the 4-AP effects. We have now also included RNAseq data demonstrating the relative expression levels of Kv1 channel mRNA in mHb ChAT neurons (KCNA1 through KCNA6; Figure 6b). The complete absence of KCNA1 yet a high expression level of KCNA2 transcripts highly suggests a central role of Kv1.2 in unmasking nAChR mediated synaptic transmission. 

      Reviewer #3 (Public review)

      Weaknesses:  

      The significance of the ratio of AMPA versus nACh EPSCs shown in Figure 6 is unclear since nAChR EPSCs measured in the K+ channel blockers are compared to AMPA EPSCs in control (presumably 4-AP would also increase AMPA EPSCs). 

      We understand the Reviewer’s concern regarding the calculation of nicotinic/AMPA ratios since they are measured under differing conditions i.e. absence and presence of 4-AP, respectively. As the reviewer correctly points point 4-AP likely increases the amplitude of the AMPA receptor mediated EPSC. However, our intention of calculating this ratio was not to ascertain a measure of relative strengths of fast glutamatergic vs cholinergic transmission onto a given postsynaptic IPN neuron per se. Rather, we used the ratio as a means to normalize the size of the nicotinic receptor EPSC to the strength of the light stimulation (using the AMPA EPSC as the normalizing factor) in each individual recording. This permits a more meaningful comparison across cells/slices/mice . We apologize for the confusion and have amended the text in the results section to reflect this (pg 9; para2).

      The mechanistic underpinnings of the most now  results are not pursued. For example, the experiments do not provide new insight into the differential effects of evoked and spontaneous glutamate/Ach release by Gi/o coupled mORs, nor the differential threshold for glutamate versus Ach release. 

      Our major goal of the current manuscript was to provide a much-needed roadmap outlining the effects of opioids in the habenulo-interpeduncular axis. Of course, a full understanding of the mechanisms underlying such complex opioid actions at the molecular level will be of great value. We feel that this is beyond the scope of this already quite result dense manuscript but will be essential if directed manipulation of the circuit is to be leveraged to alter maladaptive behaviors associated with addiction/emotion during adolescence and in adult. 

      The authors note that blocking Kv1 channels typically enhances transmitter release by slowing action potential repolarization. The idea that Kv1 channels serve as a brake for Ach release in this system would be strengthened by showing that these channels are the target of neuromodulators or that they contribute to activity-dependent regulation that allows the brake to be released. 

      The exact mechanistic underpinnings that can potentially titer Kv1.2 availability and hence nAChR transmission would be essential to shed light on potential in vivo conditions under which this arm of neurotransmission can be modulated. However, we feel that detailed mechanistic interrogation constitutes significant work but one that future studies should aim to achieve. Thus, it presently remains unclear under what physiological or pathological scenarios result in attenuation of Kv1.2 to subsequently promote nAChR mediated transmission but as mentioned in the existing discussion future work to decipher such mechanisms would be of great value.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      Overall I find this to be a very interesting and exciting paper, presenting novel findings that provide clarity for a problem that has persisted in the IPN field: that of the conundrum that light-evoked cholinergic signaling was challenging to observe despite the abundance of nAChRs in the IPN. 

      Major concerns: 

      (1) The n is quite low in most cases, and in many instances, data from one figure are replotted in another figure. Given that the findings presented here are expected in the normal condition, it should not be difficult to increase the n. A more robust number of observations would strengthen the novel findings presented here. 

      Please refer to the response to the public review above.

      (2) In general, I find the organization of the figures somewhat disjointed. Sometimes it feels as if parts of the information presented in the results are split between figures, where it would make more sense to be together in a figure. For example, all the histology for each of the lines is in Figure 1, but only ephys data for one line is included there. It would be more logical to include the histology and ephys data for each line in its own figure. It would also be helpful to show the overlap of mOR expression with Tac1-Cre and ChAT-Cre terminals in the IPN. Likewise, the summarized Tac1Cre:Ai32 IPR data is in Figure 4, but the individual data is in Figure 5. 

      We introduce both ChAT and TAC1 cre lines in Figure 1 as an overview particularly for those readers who are not entirely familiar with the distinct afferent systems operating with the habenulointerpeduncular pathway.  However, in compliance with the Reviewer’s suggestion we have now restructured the Figures. In the revised manuscript, the functional data pertaining to the various transmission modalities mediated by the distinct afferent systems impinging on the subdivision of the IPN tested are now split into their own dedicated figure as follows:

      Figure 2. 

      mOR effect on TAC1neuronal glutamatergic output in IPL.

      Figure 3. 

      mOR effect on CHAT neuronal glutamatergic output in IPR.

      Figure 5. 

      mOR effect on TAC1neuronal glutamatergic output in IPR.

      Figure 8.

      mOR effect on CHAT neuronal cholinergic output in IPC.

      Supp. Fig. 1 mOR effect on CHAT neuronal glutamatergic output in IPC.

      We thank the Reviewer for their suggestions regarding the style of the manuscript. The restructuring has now resulted in a much better flow of the presented data.

      (3) The discussion is largely satisfactory. However, a little more discussion of the integrative function of the IPN is warranted given the opposing effects of MOR activation in the Tac vs ChAT terminals, particularly in the context of both opioids and natural rewards. 

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. However, we feel the discussion is rather lengthy as is and therefore we refrained from including additional text.  

      Minor concerns: 

      (1)  The methods are missing key details. For example, the stock numbers of each of the strains of mice appear to have been left out. This is of particular importance for this paper as there are key differences between the ChAT-Cre lines that are available that would affect observed electrophysiological properties. As the authors indicate, the ChAT-ChR2 mice overexpress VAChT, while the ChAT-IRES-Cre mice do not have this problem. However, as presented it is unclear which mice are being used. 

      We apologize for the omission - the catalog numbers of the mice employed have now been included in the methods section.

      We have now clearly included in each figure panel (single trace examples and pooled data) from which mice the data are taken from – in some instances the pooled data are from the two CHAT mouse strains employed. Despite the tendency of the ChATChR2 mice to demonstrate more pronounced nAChR mediated transmission (Fig. 7h),  we justify pooling the data since we see no statistical significance in the effect of mOR activation on either potentiating AMPA or nAChR EPSCs (Please refer to response to Reviewer 2, Minor Concern point 2)

      (2) Likewise, antibody dilutions used for staining are presented as both dilution and concentration, which is not typical. 

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out this inconsistency. We have amended the text in the methods to include only the working dilution for all antibodies employed in the study.

      (3) There are minor typos throughout the manuscript. 

      All typos have been corrected.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      The authors provide a thorough investigation into the subregion, and cell-type effect of mu opioid receptor (MOR) signaling on neurotransmission in the medial habenula to interpeduncular nucleus circuit (mHb-IPN). This circuit largely comprises two distinct populations of neurons: mHb substance P (Tac1+) and cholinergic (ChAT+) neurons. Corroborating prior work, the authors report that Tac1+ neurons preferentially innervate the lateral IPN (IPL) and rostral IPN (IPR), while ChAT+ neurons preferentially innervate the central IPN (IPC) and IPR. The densest expression of MOR is observed in the IPL and MOR agonists produce a canonical presynaptic depression of glutamatergic neurotransmission in this region. Interestingly, MOR signaling in the ChAT+ mHb projection to the IPR potentiates light-evoked glutamate and acetylcholine-mediated currents (EPSC), and this effect is mediated by a MOR-induced inhibition of Kv2.1 channels. 

      Major concerns: 

      (1) The method used for expressing channelrhodopsin (ChR2) into cholinergic and neurokinin neurons in the mHb (Ai32 mice crossed with Cre-driver lines) has limitations because all Tac1+/ChAT+ inputs to the IPN express ChR2 in this mouse. Importantly, the IPN receives inputs from multiple brain regions besides the IPN-containing neurons capable of releasing these neurotransmitters (PMID: 39270652). Thus, it would be important to isolate the contributions of the mHb-IPN pathway using virally expressed ChR2 in the mHb of Cre driver mice. 

      Please refer to the response to the public review above. 

      (2) Figure 4: The authors conclude that the sEPSC recorded from IPR originate from Tac1+ mHbIPR projections. However, this cannot be stated conclusively without additional experimentation. For instance, an optogenetic asynchronous release experiment. For these experiments it would also be important to express ChR2 virus in the mHb in Tac1- and ChAT-Cre mice since glutamate originating from other brain regions could contribute to a change in asynchronous EPSCs induced by DAMGO. 

      This is a well taken point. The incongruent effect of DAMGO on evoked CHAT neuronal EPSC amplitude and sEPSC frequency prompted us  to consider the the possibility of differing effect of DAMGO on a  secondary input. We agree that we do not show directly if the sEPSCs originate from a TAC1 neuronal population. Therefore, we have tempered our wording with regards the origin of the sEPSCs and  have also restructured the Figure in question moving the sEPSC data into supplemental data (Supplemental Fig. 2) 

      (3) Figure 5D: lt would be useful to provide a quantitative measure in a few mice of mOR fluorescence across development (e.g. integrated density of fluorescence in IPR). 

      We have now included mOR expression density across development  (Fig. 6). Interestingly, the adult expression levels of mOR in the IPR are essentially reached at a very early developmental age (P10) yet we see stark differences in the role of mOR activation in modulating glutamatergic transmission mediated by mHB cholinergic neurons. Note: since we processed adult tissue (i.e. >p40) for these developmental analyses we utilized these slices to also include an analysis of the relative mOR expression density specifically in adults between the subdivisions of IPN in Fig. 1.

      (4) Figure 6B: It would be useful to quantify the expression of Kcna2 in ChAT and Tac1 neurons (e.g. using FISH). 

      We thank the Reviewer for this suggestion. We have now included mRNA expression levels available from publicly available 10X RNA sequencing dataset provided by the Allen Brain Institute (Figure 7b).  

      (5) It would be informative to examine what the effects of MOR activation are on mHb projections to the (central) . 

      In response to this suggestion, we now have included  additional data in the manuscript in putative IPC cells that clearly demonstrate a similar DAMGO elicited potentiation of AMPAR EPSC to that  seen in IPR. These data are now included in the revised manuscript  (Supplemental Fig. 1; Fig. 8i). 

      (6) What is the proposed link between MOR activation and the inhibition of Kv1.2 (e.g. beta-Arrestin signaling, G beta-gamma interaction with Kv1.2, PKA inhibition?) 

      We apologize for any confusion. We do not directly test whether the potentiation of EPSCs upon mOR activation occurs via inhibition of Kv1.2.Although we have not directly tested this possibility we find it an unlikely underlying cellular mechanism, especially for the potentiation of the cholinergic arm of neurotransmission since in the presence of DNQX/APV, the activation of mOR does not result in any emergence of any nAChR EPSC (see Supplementary Fig. 3a-c)

      Minor concerns: 

      (1) Methods: Jackson lab ID# for used mouse strains is missing. 

      We apologize for this omission and have now included the mouse strain catalog numbers.

      (2) The authors use data from both ChAT-Cre x Ai32 and ChAT-ChR2 mice. It would be helpful to show some comparisons between the lines to justify merging data sets for some of the analyses as there appear to be differences between the lines (e.g. Figure 6G). 

      This is a well taken point. We have now provided a figure for the Reviewer (see below) that illustrates the lack of  significant difference between the mOR mediated potentiation of both mHB CHAT neuronal AMPAR and nAChR transmission between the two mouse lines employed despite a divergence in the extent of glutamatergic vs cholinergic transmission shown in Fig. 7g (previously Figure 6g). We have chosen not to include this data in the revised manuscript.

      Author response image 3.

      Comparison of the mOR (500nM DAMGO) mediated potentiation on evoked AMPAR (a) and nAChR (b)EPSCs in IPR between ChATCre:Ai32  and ChATChR2 mice.

      (3)  Line 154: How was it determined that the EPSC is glutamatergic? 

      We apologize for any confusion. In the revised manuscript we now clearly point to the relevant figures (see Supplementary Figs. 2a and 3) in the Results section (pg. 4, para 2; pg 7, para 1; pg 8, para2) where we determine that both the sEPSCs and ChAT mediated light evoked EPSCs recorded under baseline conditions are totally blocked by DNQX and hence are exclusively AMPAR events 

      (4) It would be helpful to discuss the differences between GABA-B mediated potentiation of mHbIPN signaling and the current data in more detail. 

      We are unclear as to what differences the Reviewer is referring to. At least from the perspective of ChAT neuronal mediated synaptic transmission, other groups (and in the current study; Fig. 7h) have clearly shown that GABA<sub>B</sub> activation markedly potentiates synaptic transmission like mOR activation. Nevertheless, based on our novel findings it would be of interest to determine whether the influence of GABA<sub>B</sub> is inhibitory onto the TAC mediated input in IPR and whether there is a developmental regulation of this effect as we demonstrate upon mOR activation. These additional comparisons between the effect of the two Gi-linked receptors may shed light onto the similarity, or lack thereof, regarding the underlying cellular mechanisms. We now have included a few sentences in the discussion to highlight this (pg 11, para 1).

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      The abstract was confusing at first read due to the complex language, particularly the sentence starting with... Further, specific potassium channels... 

      The authors might want to consider simplifying the description of the experiments and the results to clarify the content of the manuscript for readers who many only read the abstract. 

      We have altered the wording of the abstract and hope it is now more reader friendly.

      The opposite effect of mOR activation on spontaneous EPSCs versus electrical or ChR2-evoked EPSCs is very interesting and raises the issue of which measure is most physiologically relevant. For example, it is unclear whether sEPSCs arise primarily from cholinergic neurons (that are spontaneously active in the slice, Figure 3), and if so, does mOR activation suppress or enhance cholinergic neuron excitability and/or recruitment by ChR2? While a full analysis of this question is beyond the scope of this manuscript, the assumption that glutamate release assayed by electrical/ChR2 evoked transmission is the most physiologically relevant might merit some discussion since sEPSCs presumably also reflect action-potential dependent glutamate release. One wonders whether mORs hyperpolarize cholinergic neurons to reduce spontaneous spiking yet enhance fiber recruitment by ChR2 or an electrical stimulus (i.e. by removing Na channel inactivation). The authors have clearly stated that they do not know where the mORs are located, and that the effects arising from disinhibition are likely complex. But they also might discuss whether glutamate release following synchronous activation of a fiber pathway by ChR2 or electrode is more or less physiologically relevant than glutamate release assayed during spontaneous activity. It seems likely that an equivalent experiment to Figure 3D, E using spontaneous spiking of IPR neurons would show that spiking is reduced by mOR activation. 

      We thank the Reviewer for this comment. As pointed it would be of interest to dissect the “network” effect of mOR activation but as the Reviewer acknowledges this is beyond the scope of the current manuscript. The Reviewer is correct in postulating that mOR activation results in hyperpolarization of mHB ChAT neurons.  A recent study(Singhal et al 2025) demonstrate that a subpopulation of ChAT neurons undergoes a reduction in firing frequency following DAMGO application. This is corroborated by our own observations although we chose not to include this data in our current manuscript (but see below).

      Additionally, the Reviewer questions whether ChR2/electrical stimulation is physiological. This is a well taken point and of course the simultaneous activation of potentially all possible axonal release sites is not the mode under which the circuit operates. Nevertheless, our data clearly demonstrates the ability of mORs to modulate release under these circumstances that must reflect an impact on spontaneous action potential driven evoked release.  Although the suggested experiment  could shed light on the synaptic outcomes of mOR receptor activation on ES coupling of downstream IPN neurons. Interpretation of the outcome would be confounded by the fact that postsynaptic IPN neurons also express mORs . Thus,  we would not be able to isolate the effects of presynaptic changes in modulating ES coupling from any direct postsynaptic effect on the recorded cell when in current clamp. 

      Together these additional sites of action of mOR (i.e. mHB ChAT somatodendritic and postsynaptic IPN neuron) only serve to further highlight the complex nature of the actions of opioids on the habenulo-interpeduncular axis warranting  future work to fully understand the physiological and pathological effects on the habenulo-interpeduncular axis as a whole.

      The idea that Kv2.1 channels serve as a brake raises the question of whether they contribute to activity-dependent action potential broadening to facilitate Ach release during trains of stimuli. 

      This is an interesting suggestion and one that we had considered ourselves. Indeed, as the Reviewer is likely aware and as mentioned in the manuscript, previous studies have shown nAChR signaling can be revealed under conditions of multiple stimulations given at relatively high frequencies.  We therefore attempted to perform high frequency stimulation (20 stimulations at 25Hz and 50Hz) in the presence of ionotropic glutamatergic receptor antagonists DNQX and APV. We have now included this data in the revised manuscript (Supplementary Fig 3b). As shown, this failed to engage nAChR mediated synaptic transmission in our hands. Interestingly there is evidence from reduced expression systems demonstrating that Kv1.2 channels undergo use-dependent potentiation(Baronas et al., 2015) in contrast to that seen with other K+-channels. Whether this is the case for the axonal Kv1.2 channels on mHB axonal terminals in situ is not known but this may explain the inability to reveal nAChR EPSCs upon delivery of such stimulation paradigms.  

      References 

      Baronas, V. A., McGuinness, B. R., Brigidi, G. S., Gomm Kolisko, R. N., Vilin, Y. Y., Kim, R. Y., … Kurata, H. T. (2015). Use-dependent activation of neuronal Kv1.2 channel complexes. J Neurosci, 35(8), 3515-3524. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4518-13.2015

      Bueno, D., Lima, L. B., Souza, R., Goncalves, L., Leite, F., Souza, S., … Metzger, M. (2019). Connections of the laterodorsal tegmental nucleus with the habenular-interpeduncular-raphe system. J Comp Neurol, 527(18), 3046-3072. doi:10.1002/cne.24729

      Liang, J., Zhou, Y., Feng, Q., Zhou, Y., Jiang, T., Ren, M., … Luo, M. (2024). A brainstem circuit amplifies aversion. Neuron. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2024.08.010

      Lima, L. B., Bueno, D., Leite, F., Souza, S., Goncalves, L., Furigo, I. C., … Metzger, M. (2017). Afferent and efferent connections of the interpeduncular nucleus with special reference to circuits involving the habenula and raphe nuclei. J Comp Neurol, 525(10), 2411-2442. doi:10.1002/cne.24217

      Singhal, S. M., Szlaga, A., Chen, Y. C., Conrad, W. S., & Hnasko, T. S. (2025). Mu-opioid receptor activation potentiates excitatory transmission at the habenulo-peduncular synapse. Cell Rep, 44(7), 115874. doi:10.1016/j.celrep.2025.115874

      Stinson, H.E., & Ninan, I. (2025). GABA(B) receptor-mediated potentiation of ventral medial habenula glutamatergic transmission in GABAergic and glutamatergic interpeduncular nucleus neurons. bioRxiv doi.10.1101/2025.01.03.631193

    1. poluidor
      • Edição Extraordinária nº 8
      • Direito Público
      • 17 de janeiro de 2023
      • Edição com notas inéditas, de julgados do segundo semestre de 2022, dos órgãos julgadores do Superior Tribunal de Justiça na área de direito público.
      • SEGUNDA TURMA
      • Processo: REsp 1.845.200-SC, Rel. Ministro Og Fernandes, Segunda Turma, julgado em 16/8/2022, DJe 6/9/2022.

      Ramo do Direito DIREITO AMBIENTAL

      TemaCidades e comunidades sustentáveis Paz, Justiça e Instituições Eficazes Dano ambiental. Indenização por dano intercorrente. Compensação pelo período em que o ambiente natural degradado deixa de estar a serviço do homem e do ecossistema. Enriquecimento ilícito. Reparação in natura ou mediante indenização. Obrigações distintas. Restauração que não afasta aquela obrigação.

      Destaque - O cumprimento da obrigação de reparar integralmente o dano ambiental (in natura ou pecuniariamente) não afasta a obrigação de indenizar os danos ambientais interinos.

      Informações do Inteiro Teor - Cinge-se a controvérsia a determinar se a reparação do dano ambiental afasta a obrigação de reparar o dano intercorrente.

      • Este Tribunal distingue ao menos três espécies de danos ambientais, considerados no tempo: i) o <u>dano em si</u>, reparável preferencialmente pela restauração do ambiente ao estado anterior; ii) o <u>dano remanescente</u> (residual, perene, definitivo, permanente), que se protrai no tempo mesmo após os esforços de recuperação in natura, em regra indenizável; e iii) o <u>dano interino</u> (intercorrente, intermediário, temporário, provisório), que ocorre entre a ocorrência da lesão em si e a reparação integral, haja ou não dano remanescente.

      • É perfeitamente possível e lógico que haja dano interino mesmo com a integral reparação do meio ambiente degradado, seja essa reparação in natura ou mediante indenização. As parcelas e suas causas não se confundem.

      • No caso do dano interino, sua causa é a lesão experimentada pelo meio ambiente desde o momento da lesão (tempo passado) até sua reparação (tempo futuro). Nessas hipóteses, pode perfeitamente haver dano interino indenizável, ainda que não se vislumbre dano remanescente.

      • Ele se configura pela diminuição temporária do valor do bem ambiental (nas diversas manifestações desse valor de uso). É uma compensação da sociedade pelo período que deixou de gozar dos serviços e recursos ecológicos, inclusive a título de reserva e precaução.

      • Desse modo, se o dano residual pode vir a não incidir, pela reparação integral primária in natura, dificilmente se vislumbrará hipótese de não incidência do dano ambiental intercorrente. Em regra, ele existirá, pela própria lógica das coisas: i) havendo o dano ambiental, ele deve ser integralmente reparado; ii) a reparação, ainda que in natura e in loco, somente ocorrerá em certo momento futuro; iii) no ínterim, o dano já existiu e permanecerá existindo até a reparação integral; iv) como resultado, o dano transitório deverá ser reparado pelo período da degradação ambiental até o da futura e antevista reparação (in natura ou pecuniária).

      • Em outra perspectiva, a reparação dos danos transitórios não é senão uma forma de internalização, temporalmente diferida, dos custos dos serviços e recursos ambientais ilegalmente expropriados da coletividade pelo particular, que deles usufruiu sem qualquer direito. Trata-se de compensação pelo período em que o ambiente natural degradado deixará de estar a serviço não só do homem, mas do ecossistema.

      • Configurada a degradação ambiental, impõe-se a reparação integral do dano, tanto daquele já experimentado quanto dos que são certos de existirem até o momento da reparação integral, dê-se esta na forma primária ou complementar. Não reste dúvida: o dano intercorrente é certo e atual.

      • Anote-se ser possível cumular os danos interinos e os remanescentes; nessa situação, as parcelas devem ser aferidas separadamente, na medida em que, repita-se, seus fatos ensejadores são diversos. O simples fato de afirmar que a restauração será completa não é suficiente para afastar a indenização. Para isso, a restauração deve ser completa e imediata. Isso deve ser refletido sobre dois aspectos. Primeiro, para o dano residual, esse termo deve ser aferido em relação às medidas de restauração. Isto é: concluída a implementação das medidas, o dano residual será indenizável se a restauração não for imediata após esse marco.

      • Para o dano intercorrente, porém, esse marco não é o do término da restauração, mas sim o da ocorrência do dano. Portanto, o parâmetro "imediato", neste caso, de dano ambiental intercorrente, é aquele medido entre o evento degradante e o <u>término das medidas restaurativas</u>.

      • Verificada a lesão ambiental, o dano intercorrente somente pode ser afastado se for <u>temporalmente irrelevante</u>, mediante justificativa expressa do julgador fundada na prova dos autos. A reparabilidade imediata (após as medidas de recuperação) e mesmo que completa da lesão não afasta o dano já experimentado no período entre a degradação e sua restauração.

    1. As you can see in the apple example, any time we turn something into data, we are making a simplification.[1] If we are counting the number of something, like apples, we are deciding that each one is equivalent. If we are writing down what someone said, we are losing their tone of voice, accent, etc. If we are taking a photograph, it is only from one perspective, etc.

      I totally agree with this. All data is a simplification of reality, but in the process of simplification, some information is lost. So when we are choosing the way of simplification, we should consider the pros and cons, then decide which simplification would be the best to satisfy the need of different users.

    2. 4.2.5. Revisiting Twitter Users vs. Bots# Let’s go back to the question of whether less than 5% of Twitter users are spam bots. In this claim are several places where there are simplifications being made, particularly in the definitions of “Twitter users” and “spam bots.”

      While reading §4.2 and the debate over Twitter bots, I was struck by how definitions can quietly gain influence. For a class project, I modified a spam heuristic (number of URLs + account age), and my bot estimate went from 3% to 14%—the same data, just simplified. This convinced me that the "<5%" number wasn't a fact, but a governance decision about what's considered criticaI. Platforms should publish ranges based on uncertain scenarios and disclose the underlying assumptions. Question: Why don't we require confidence intervals and other definitions for platform metrics, as we do in epidemioIogy?

    1. There is agreement that LoRA underperforms in settings that resemble pre-training,LoRA Learns Less and Forgets Less (Biderman et al, 2024) namely those with very large datasets that exceed the storage limits of LoRA parameters. But for dataset sizes that are typical in post-training, LoRA has sufficient capacity to store the essential information. However, this fact makes no guarantees regarding sample efficiency and compute efficiency. The question is: can LoRA match the performance of full fine-tuning, and if so, under which conditions?

      Feels like this can be explained by the Linear Direction Hypothesis, each direction encodes a "latent" or some form of "concept". Thus, when we use LoRa its like doing PCA and identifying the most relevant directions for this dataset and encoding them in the model, making the model more "sensitive" to them. When the dataset size is larger than the rank then we cant quite encode all necessary information.

    Tags

    Annotators

    1. eLife Assessment

      This important study elucidates the role of the exocyst component EXOC6A at distinct stages of ciliogenesis, which advances our understanding of ciliary membrane remodeling and cilium formation. The authors provide solid evidence that EXOC6A interacts with myosin-Va and is dynamically recruited via dynein-, microtubule-, and actin-dependent mechanisms, to support proper formation of the ciliary membrane. The study will be of interest to cell biologists and other researchers interested in vesicular trafficking, organellar membrane dynamics, and ciliogenesis.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The study by Lin et al. studies the role of EXOC6A in ciliogenesis and its relationship with the interactor myosin-Va using a range of approaches based on the RPE1 cell line model. They establish its spatio-temporal organization at centrioles, the forming ciliary vesicle and ciliary sheath using ExM, various super-resolution techniques, and EM, including correlative light and electron microscopy. They also perform live imaging analyses and functional studies using RNAi and knockout. They establish a role of EXOC6A together with myosin-Va in Golgi-derived, microtubule- and actin-based vesicle trafficking to and from the ciliary vesicle and sheath membranes. Defects in these functions impair robust ciliary shaft and axoneme formation due to defective transition zone assembly.

      Strengths:

      The study provides very high-quality data that support the conclusions. In particular, the imaging data is compelling. It also integrates all findings in a model that shows how EXOC6A participates in multiple stages of ciliogenesis and how it cooperates with other factors.

      Weaknesses:

      The precise role of EXOC6A remains somewhat unclear. While it is described as a component of the exocyst, the authors do not address its molecular functions and whether it indeed works as part of the exocyst complex during ciliogenesis.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The molecular mechanisms underlying ciliogenesis are not well understood. Previously, work from the same group (Wu et al., 2018) identified myosin-Va as an important protein in transporting preciliary vesicles to the mother vesicles, allowing for initiation of ciliogenesis. The exocyst complex has previously been implicated in ciliogenesis and protein trafficking to cilia. Here, Lin et al. investigate the role of exocyst complex protein EXOC6A in cilia formation. The authors find that EXOC6A localizes to preciliary vesicles, ciliary vesicles, and the ciliary sheath. EXOC6A colocalizes with Myo-Va in the ciliary vesicle and the ciliary sheath, and both proteins are removed from fully assembled cilia. EXOC6A is not required for Myo-Va localization, but Myo-VA and EHD1 are required for EXOC6A to localize in ciliary vesicles. The authors propose that EXOC6A vesicles continually remodel the cilium: FRAP analysis demonstrates that EXOC6A is a dynamic protein, and live imaging shows that EXOC6A fuses with and buds off from the ciliary membrane. Loss of EXOC6A reduces, but does not eliminate, the number of cilia formed in cells. Any cilia that are still present are structurally abnormal, with either bent morphologies or the absence of some transition zone proteins. Overall, the analyses and imaging are well done, and the conclusions are well supported by the data. The work will be of interest to cell biologists, especially those interested in centrosomes and cilia.

      Strengths:

      The TEM micrographs are of excellent quality. The quality of the imaging overall is very good, especially considering that these are dynamic processes occurring in a small region of the cell. The data analysis is well done and the quantifications are very helpful. The manuscript is well-written and the final figure is especially helpful in understanding the model.

      Weaknesses:

      Additional information about the functional and mechanistic roles of EXOC6A would improve the manuscript greatly.

    4. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Lin et al report on the dynamic localization of EXOC6A and Myo-Va at pre-ciliary vesicles, ciliary vesicles, and ciliary sheath membrane during ciliogenesis using three-dimensional structured illumination microscopy and ultrastructure expansion microscopy. The authors further confirm the interaction of EXOC6A and Myo-Va by co-immunoprecipitation experiments and demonstrated the requirement of EHD1 for the EXOC6A-labeled ciliary vesicles formation. Additional experiments using gene-silencing by siRNA and pharmacological tools identified the involvement of dynein-, microtubule-, and actin in the transport mechanism of EXOC6A-labeled vesicles to the centriole, as they have previously reported for Myo-Va. Notably, loss of EXOC6A severely disrupts ciliogenesis, with the majority of cells becoming arrested at the ciliary vesicle (CV) stage, highlighting the involvement of EXOC6A at later stages of ciliogenesis. As the authors observe dynamic EXOC6A-positive vesicle release and fusion with the ciliary sheath, this suggests a role in membrane and potentially membrane protein delivery to the growing cilium past the ciliary vesicle stage. While CEP290 localization at the forming cilium appears normal, the recruitment of other transition zone components, exemplified by several MKS and NPHP module components, was also impaired in EXOC6A-deficient cells.

      Strengths:

      (1) By applying different microscopy approaches, the study provides deeper insight into the spatial and temporal localization of EXOC6A and Myo-Va during ciliogenesis.

      (2) The combination of complementary siRNA and pharmacological tools targeting different components strengthens the conclusions.

      (3) This study reveals a new function of EXOC6A in delivering membrane and membrane proteins during ciliogenesis, both to the ciliary vesicle as well as to the ciliary sheath.

      (4) The overall data quality is high. The investigation of EXOC6A at different time points during ciliogenesis is well schematized and explained.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) Since many conclusions are based on EXOC6A immunostaining, it would strengthen the study to validate antibody specificity by demonstrating the absence of staining in EXOC6A-deficient cells.

      (2) While the authors generated an EXOC6A-deficient cell line, off-target effects can be clone-specific. Validating key experiments in a second independent knockout clone or rescuing the phenotype of the existing clone by re-expressing EXOC6A would ensure that the observed phenotypes are due to EXOC6A loss rather than unintended off-target effects.

      (3) Some experimental details are lacking from the materials and methods section. No information on how the co-immunoprecipitation experiments have been performed can be found. The concentrations of pharmacological agents should be provided to allow proper interpretation of the results, as higher or lower doses can produce nonspecific effects. For example, the concentrations of ciliobrevin and nocodazole used to treat RPE1 cells are not specified and should be included. More precise settings for the FRAP experiments would help others reproduce the presented data. Some details for the siRNA-based knockdowns, such as incubation times, can only be found in the figure legends.

      Taken together, the authors achieved their goal of elucidating the role of EXOC6A in ciliogenesis, demonstrating its involvement in vesicle trafficking and membrane remodeling in both early and late stages of ciliogenesis. Their findings are supported by experimental evidence. This work is likely to have an impact on the field by expanding our understanding of the molecular machinery underlying cilia biogenesis, particularly the coordination between the exocyst complex and cytoskeletal transport systems. The methods and data presented offer valuable tools for dissecting vesicle dynamics and cilium formation, providing a foundation for future research into ciliary dysfunction and related diseases. By connecting vesicle trafficking to structural maturation of an organelle, the study adds important context to the broader description of cellular architecture and organelle biogenesis.

    1. eLife Assessment

      This valuable study investigates the role of HIF1a signaling in epicardial activation and neonatal heart regeneration in mice. Using a combination of genetic and pharmacological approaches, the authors demonstrate that stabilization of HIF1a enhances epicardial activation and extends the regenerative capacity of the heart beyond the typical neonatal window following myocardial infarction. The main conclusion is well supported by solid data, although some minor concerns regarding experimental interpretation require further clarification to ensure accuracy.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript by Gamen et al. analyzed the functional role of HIF signaling in the epicardium providing evidence that stabilization of the hypoxia signaling pathway might contribute to neonatal heart regeneration. By generating different conditionally mouse mutants and performing pharmacological interventions, the authors demonstrate that stabilizing HIF signaling enhances cardiac regeneration after MI in P7 neonatal hearts.

      Strengths:

      The study presents convincing genetic and pharmacological approaches on the role of hypoxia signaling enhance the regenerative potential of the epicardium

      Weaknesses:

      The major weakness remains the lack of convincing evidence demonstrating the role of hypoxia signaling in EMT modulation in the epicardial cells. The authors claimed that EMT assays adopted in this study are based on similar previous studies. Surprisingly, two of the references provided correspond to their own research group (PMID: 17108969, PMID: 19235142), limiting the credit for such claims, and the other two (PMID: 27023710, PMID: 12297106) assessment of cell migration but not EMT is reported. Thus, EMT remains to be convincingly demonstrated.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this study, Gamen et al. investigated the roles of hypoxia and HIF1a signaling in regulating epicardial function during cardiac development and neonatal heart regeneration. The authors identified hypoxic regions in the epicardium during development and demonstrated that genetic and pharmacological stabilization of HIF1a during neonatal heart injury prolonged epicardial activation, preserved myocardium, enhanced infarct resolution, and maintained cardiac function beyond the normal postnatal regenerative window.

      Strengths:

      HIF1a signaling was manipulated in an epicardium-specific manner using appropriate genetic tools.

      Weaknesses:

      Some conclusions still need clarification.

      Comments on revisions:

      (1) The authors' comment on the partial overlap of HP1 and HIF1a IF signals (HIF1a is highly unstable ... broader regions of hypoxia) is reasonable and would help readers interpret the data if included in the text describing Fig. 1.

      (2) The conclusion regarding WT1+ cells in Fig. 2a and b remains unclear. Both panels display larger and smaller magenta cells, and when all are taken into account, the overall number does not appear substantially different. Additional clarification is needed on how the quantification was performed.

      (3) Regarding Figure 6-figure supplement 1c, it seems difficult to conclude the endothelial identity of WT1+ cells based on EMCN staining, as the markers do not overlap. The authors note that WT1 is upregulated in endothelial cells, but this has been reported in the context of injury, which differs from the context of the present study involving Molidustat.

    4. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The author's research here was to understand the role of hypoxia and hypoxia-induced transcription factors Hif-1a in the epicardium. The authors noted that hypoxia was prevalent in the embryonic heart and this persisted into neonatal stages until post natal day 7 (P7). Hypoxic regions in the heart were noted in the outer layer of the heart and expression of Hif-1a coincided with the epicardial gene WT1. It has been documented that at P7, the mouse heart cannot regenerate after myocardial infarction and the authors speculated that the change in epicardial hypoxic conditions could play a role in regeneration. The authors then used genetic and pharmacological tools to increase the activity of Hif genes in the heart and noted that there was a significant improvement in cardiac function when Hif-1a was active in the epicardium. The authors speculated that the presence of Hif-1a improved cell survival.

      Strengths:

      A focus on hypoxia and its effects on the epicardium in development and after myocardial infraction. This study outlines a potential to extend the regenerative time window in neonatal mammalian hearts.

      Weaknesses:

      While the observations of improved cardiac function is clear, the exact mechanism of how increased Hif-1a activity causes these effects is not completely revealed. The authors mention improved myocardium survival, but do not include studies to demonstrate this.

      There is an indication that fibrosis is decreased in hearts where Hif activity is prolonged, but there are no studies to link hypoxia and fibrosis.

      Comments on revisions:

      In the manuscript revision, the authors address my comments. They outline differences between genetic disruption of Phd2 and chemical inactivation could be due to dosing and drug half-life of Molidustat. The other comments are addressed by explaining that they have analyzed enough heart sections and hearts to come to their conclusions. The authors also state they cannot generate more numbers for this study, therefore I accept their conclusions as stated.

    5. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      eLife Assessment

      This valuable study investigates the role of HIF1a signalling in epicardial activation and neonatal heart regeneration in mice. Through a combination of genetic and pharmacological approaches, the authors show that stabilization of HIF1a enhances epicardial activation and extends the regenerative capacity of the heart beyond the typical neonatal window following myocardial infarction (MI). However, several aspects of the study remain incomplete and would benefit from further clarification and additional experimental support to solidify the conclusions.

      We reveal herein prolonged epicardial activation following myocardial infarction (MI) beyond post-natal days 1-7 (P1-P7) by genetic or pharmacological stabilisation of HIF-signalling. This extends the so-called “regenerative window” during an adult-like response to injury, leading to enhanced survived myocardium and functional improvement of the heart, even against a backdrop of persistent, albeit reduced, fibrosis. The epicardium is known to enhance cardiomyocyte proliferation and myocardial growth during heart development via trophic growth factor (for example, IGF-1, FGF, VEGF, TGFβ and BMP) signalling (reviewed in PMID:29592950) and epicardium-derived cell-conditioned medium reduces infarct size and improves heart function (PMID: 21505261). Further experiments, outside of the scope of the current study, are required to determine whether activated neonatal epicardium elicits similar paracrine support to sustain the myocardium and heart function after injury beyond P7 into adulthood.

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript by Gamen et al. analyzed the functional role of HIF signaling in the epicardium, providing evidence that stabilization of the hypoxia signaling pathway might contribute to neonatal heart regeneration. By generating different conditionally mouse mutants and performing pharmacological interventions, the authors demonstrate that stabilizing HIF signaling enhances cardiac regeneration after MI in P7 neonatal hearts.

      Strengths:

      The study presents convincing genetic and pharmacological approaches to the role of hypoxia signaling in enhancing the regenerative potential of the epicardium.

      Weaknesses:

      The major weakness is the lack of convincing evidence demonstrating the role of hypoxia signaling in EMT modulation in epicardial cells. Additionally, novel experimental approaches should be performed to allow for the translation of these findings to the clinical arena.

      We respectfully disagree that we have not convincingly demonstrated a role for HIF-signalling in promoting epicardial EMT. We adopt epicardial explant assays utilising a well characterised ex vivo protocol previously described for studying EMT in embryonic, neonatal and adult epicardium (PMID: 27023710, PMID: 12297106; PMID: 17108969, PMID: 19235142). These assays demonstrate in WT1<sup>CreERT2</sup>;Phd2<sup>fl/fl</sup> explants enhanced cobblestone to spindle-like change in cell morphology, increased cell migration, appearance of stress fibres and an up-regulation of the mesenchymal marker alpha-smooth muscle actin (αSMA); all parameters associated with EMT. In addition, our in vivo analyses of Wt1<sup>CreERT2</sup>;Phd2<sup>fl/fl</sup> hearts, in response to neonatal injury, reveal elevated numbers of WT1+ epicardial cells within the sub-epicardial region and underlying myocardium as is associated with active EMT and subsequent migration from the epicardium.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this study, Gamen et al. investigated the roles of hypoxia and HIF1a signaling in regulating epicardial function during cardiac development and neonatal heart regeneration. They found that WT1<sup>+</sup> epicardial cells become hypoxic and begin expressing HIF1a from mid-gestation onward. During development, epicardial HIF1a signaling regulates WT1 expression and promotes coronary vasculature formation. In the postnatal heart, genetic and pharmacological upregulation of HIF1a sustained epicardial activation and improved regenerative outcomes.

      Strengths:

      HIF1a signaling was manipulated in an epicardium-specific manner using appropriate genetic tools.

      Weaknesses:

      There appears to be a discrepancy between some of the conclusions and the provided histological data. Additionally, the study does not offer mechanistic insight into the functional recovery observed.

      We respectfully disagree with the comment that our histological data does not support our conclusions and expand on this in the response to specific reviewer comments. We agree that further mechanistic experiments outside of the scope of the current study are required to identify precisely how activated neonatal epicardium results in increased healthy myocardium after injury beyond post-natal day 7 (P7).

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors' research here was to understand the role of hypoxia and hypoxia-induced transcription factor Hif-1a in the epicardium. The authors noted that hypoxia was prevalent in the embryonic heart, and this persisted into neonatal stages until postnatal day 7 (P7). Hypoxic regions in the heart were noted in the outer layer of the heart, and expression of Hif-1a coincided with the epicardial gene WT1. It has been documented that at P7, the mouse heart cannot regenerate after myocardial infarction, and the authors speculated that the change in epicardial hypoxic conditions could play a role in regeneration. The authors then used genetic and pharmacological tools to increase the activity of Hif genes in the heart and noted that there was a significant improvement in cardiac function when Hif-1a was active in the epicardium. The authors speculated that the presence of Hif-1a improved cell survival.

      Strengths:

      A focus on hypoxia and its effects on the epicardium in development and after myocardial infarction. This study outlines the potential to extend the regenerative time window in neonatal mammalian hearts.

      We thank the reviewer for this positive endorsement and recognition of the importance of mechanistic insight into how to extend the window of neonatal heart regeneration.

      Weaknesses:

      While the observations of improved cardiac function are clear, the exact mechanism of how increased Hif-1a activity causes these effects is not completely revealed. The authors mention improved myocardium survival, but do not include studies to demonstrate this.

      We report an increase in healthy myocardium arising from prolonged activation of the epicardium during the neonatal window and following injury at post-natal day 7 (P7). We speculate this recapitulates the role of the epicardium during heart development which is known to be a source of trophic growth factors that can enhance myocardial growth. Further experiments are required, out-of-scope of this study, to define a mechanistic link between HIF-signalling, epicardial activation and myocardial survival in the setting of prolonged neonatal heart regeneration.

      There is an indication that fibrosis is decreased in hearts where Hif activity is prolonged, but there are no studies to link hypoxia and fibrosis.

      We believe the decreased fibrosis is a natural consequence of the increase in survived myocardium arising from the activated epicardium. There is strong precedent here following injury at post-natal day 1 (P1) in which fibrosis is evident early-on but is resolved over time with growth of the myocardium in the regenerating heart (PMID: 23248315).

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewing Editor Comments:

      (1) Address issues related to image quality, colocalization, sample labeling, appropriate controls, and quantification - particularly in Figures 1, 2, 6, and Supplementary Figure 9. Increase sample size as noted by reviewers.

      The issues of co-localisation and sample labelling have been addressed under response to reviewers. We are unable to increase sample numbers but have clarified the number of regions per section and numbers of sections per heart analysed where appropriate.

      (2) Clarify the effects of epicardial HIF1a activation on neovascularization.

      We have removed reference in the abstract to an effect on neovascularisation.

      (3) Extend assessments of epicardial hypoxia and HIF1a expression to earlier embryonic stages, when epicardial EMT is more active.

      Our earliest timepoint of E12.5 marks the onset of epicardial EMT and E13.5 is the stage with the most significant mobilisation of epicardium-derived cells (EPDCs) into the sub-epicardial region and underlying myocardium (PMID: 32359445). In the same study, E11.5 lineage tracing of epicardial cells is restricted to outer layer of the heart; thus, our timepoints are representative in capturing both the onset and progression of in vivo EMT.

      (4) Strengthen EMT assays and mechanistic modeling. Provide evidence from physiologically relevant models, as current 2D culture assays do not adequately support conclusions about EMT. Include additional EMT markers and quantification where appropriate.

      We respectfully disagree that epicardial explants are not a valid assay for assessing EMT. As noted under responses to reviewers, such primary explants have been widely described elsewhere (PMID: 27023710, PMID: 12297106; PMID: 17108969, PMID: 19235142) and enable documentation of multiple parameters that are associated with active EMT, including an assessment of the extent of cell migration, cobblestone (epithelial) to spindle-like (mesenchymal) cell morphologies, stress fibre formation and expression of alpha-smooth muscle actin as a mesenchymal marker. We support our findings in explants by revealing reduced WT1+ epicardium-derived cells (EPDCs) in the sub-epicardial region and underlying myocardium of WT1<sup>CreERT2/+</sup>;Hif1a<sup>fl/fl</sup> embryonic hearts (data in Figure 2) indicative of impaired epicardial EMT and migration of EPDCs and in vivo following neonatal MI with pharmacological inhibition of PHD2, where we observe the reciprocal phenotype of increased numbers of epicardium-derived cells emerging from the outer epicardial layer (data in Figure 6).

      (5) Strengthen mechanistic insights into the role of epicardial cells in the functional recovery observed in MI hearts.

      We agree that further experiments are required, out-of-scope of this study, to define a mechanistic link between HIF-signalling, epicardial activation and myocardial survival in the setting of prolonged neonatal heart regeneration.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The manuscript by Gamen et al. analyzed the functional role of HIF signaling in the epicardium, providing evidence that stabilization of the hypoxia signaling pathway might contribute to neonatal heart regeneration. By generating different conditionally mouse mutants and performing pharmacological interventions, the authors demonstrate that stabilizing HIF signaling enhances cardiac regeneration after MI in P7 neonatal hearts. The study is potentially interesting, but it presents several major caveats.

      (1) One of the critical points reported in the early stages of this study is the early co-localization of Wt1, the hypoxic report (HP1), and HIF signaling pathways master regulators (i.e., HIF1a and HIF1b) during embryonic development. Figure 1 is meant to report such findings. However, unfortunately, I hardly see any co-localization at all in the Wt1+ epicardial cells for HP1, with some colocalization is seen for HIF1 and 2 alpha, although none of these data are quantified. Thus, it is hard to believe such co-localization.

      We respectfully disagree with this comment. We highlight cells in Figure 1 that are co-stained for WT1+ and HP1. In addition, we identify HIF1-α and HIF2- α positive cells which either reside within the epicardium, as the outer cell layer, or within the underlying sub-epicardial region, respectfully.

      (2) The authors claimed that they have analyzed the expression of the hypoxic report, as well as Wt1 and the HIF signaling pathways master regulators (i.e., HIF1a and HIF1b) in the AV groove, as compared to the apex, in embryonic heart ranging from E12.5 to E18.5 (Figure 1). Unfortunately, all images provided that are tagged as AV groove are rather misleading. They do not represent the AV groove but part of the right ventricular free wall. If the authors want to refer to the AV groove, AV cushions should be visible underneath.

      We have removed specific reference to the AV groove and refer to the highlighted regions as the “Base” of the heart.

      (3) The authors analyzed the hypoxic condition of the developing heart from E12.5 to E18.5. However, it remains unclear why the authors only explored the hypoxic conditions from E12.5 onwards, since epicardial EMT mainly occurs earlier than this time point, i.e., E10.5 onwards. Therefore, it would be needed to explore it already at this earlier time point.

      We respectfully disagree with the reviewer and refer to the comment above regarding the fact that E12.5 marks the onset of epicardial EMT and E13.5 is the stage with the most significant mobilisation of epicardium-derived cells (EPDCs) into the sub-epicardial region and underlying myocardium (PMID: 32359445).

      (4) The authors reported a conditional mouse model of HIF1alpha deletion by using the Wt1CreERT2 driver. Curiously, Wt1 is dependent on hypoxia signaling (i.e., HIF1a). Therefore, it is unclear whether there is a negative feedback loop between the deletion of Hif1alpha and the activation of the Cre driver might have functional consequences. Convincing evidence should be provided that such crosstalk does not interfere with Hif1alpha inactivation, and therefore, appropriate controls should be run in parallel.

      We discount a negative feedback loop in this instance based on the fact we have utilised heterozygous mice for the WT1<sup>CreERT2/+</sup> line and observe a consistent and reproducible phenotype for the developing hearts on a Wt1<sup>CreERT2/+</sup>;Hif1a<sup>fl/fl</sup> background and following injury in Wt1<sup>CreERT2/+</sup>;Phd2<sup>fl/fl</sup> mice. Collectively this indicates that the WT1-CreERT2 driver is active in the context of diminishing HIF-1α and Phd2, respectively. In addition, have carried out parallel experiments using epicardial explants derived from R26R-CreERT2;Phd2<sup>fl/fl</sup> (Figure 3) to circumvent any potential confounding issues; the results of which are consistent with increased epicardial EMT in support of our overall hypothesis.

      (5) On Figure 2a-f the authors reported that epicardial cells are diminished in Wt1CreERT2Hif1alpha mice as compared to controls. I am very sorry, but I do not see any difference. Furthermore, it is unclear to me how the authors quantified such differences, i.e., what marker signal did they use and how it was performed (Figure 2c and d)?

      We respectfully disagree with the reviewer and draw attention to the single channel panels of WT1+ staining in Figure 2, which show clear differences between numbers of epicardial cells in the mutant mice compared to controls (comparing magenta cells in panels a) versus b). Quantification was carried out for numbers of WT1+ cells residing within the PDPN-positive epicardium (and underlying PDPN-negative myocardium) across multiple images from multiple sections and multiple hearts.

      (6) On Figure 2g, the authors reported differences in total vessel length. Are they referring to impaired microvasculature development? Or is this analysis also including major coronary vessels? What about the major coronary vessels and trees, is there any affection?

      This analysis refers to the microvasculature and not the major coronary arteries or coronary trees.

      (7) The authors reported that there might be some differences in EMT markers, but unfortunately, all of them are analyzed on 2D cultures, where no substrate for EMT is present, i.e., an underlying ECM bed. Thus, the authors cannot claim that EMT is altered. Additional experiments using either collagen substrate and/or Matrigel are required to fully demonstrate that EMT is impaired. Furthermore, quantitative analyses of such differences should be provided.

      The 2D cultures are epicardial explants from mutant versus wild type hearts and represent a widely adopted previously published ex-vivo assay for investigating epicardial EMT across embryonic to adult stages (PMID: 27023710, PMID: 12297106; PMID: 17108969, PMID: 19235142); including an assessment of the extent of migration and cobblestone (epithelial) to spindle-like (mesenchymal) cell morphologies, stress fibre formation and expression of alpha-smooth muscle actin as a mesenchymal marker. We do not understand the comment regarding an “underlying ECM bed” as the cells exhibit EMT routinely on tissue culture plastic and will deposit their own ECM during the culture time course and in response to EMT/cell migration. In terms of quantification this was carried out for scratch assay experiments, as a proxy for EMT and emergent mesenchymal cell migration, as presented in Figure 3i, j with significant enhanced scratch closure and cell migration following Molidustat treatment.

      (8) The description of data provided on Supplementary Figure 5 is spurious and should be removed. A note in the discussion might be sufficient.

      We respectfully disagree. The ChIP-seq data, in what is now Figure 2- figure supplement 3, highlights a HIF-1 α binding site within the Wt1 locus suggesting putative upstream regulation of WT1 by HIF-1α. Thus this provides a potential explanation as to how HIF-1α may activate the epicardium through up-regulation of Wt1/WT1.

      (9) On Figure 3, the authors further illustrate the change of EMT markers using ex vivo cardiac explants. They reported increased expression of Snai2 that, although statistically significant, is most likely of no biological relevance (increase of only 20% at transcript level). What about Snai1, Prrx1, and other EMT promoters? Are they also induced? As previously stated, these 2D cultures do not provide supporting evidence that EMT is occurring, thus 3D gel assays should be performed in which Z-axis analyses will provide evidence on the different migratory behaviour of those cells.

      We respectfully suggest that a 20% change in snai2 expression is biologically meaningful with respect to EMT. This in-turn is supported by associated cell migration, reduced ZO-1 expression, increased stress fibres and increased alpha-SMA as a mesenchymal marker; all properties associated with active EMT. Other suggested markers have not been validated as formally required for EMT, for example Snai1 (PMID: 23097346). The migratory capacity of targeted versus epicardial cells was assessed by combined explant and scratch assay experiments.

      (10) The description of single-cell analyses is very incomplete. Which mice were used for these analyses, wildtype control, or hypoxic mice? Please provide a clearer description of the samples used. Additionally, the entire rationale of these analyses is dubious. Doing single-cell analyses to analyze a couple or three markers in a very small cell population is rather ridiculous. qPCR might be far more appropriate and convincing, or a bulk RNAseq analysis of isolated epicardial cells.

      The single-cell analyses represent an unbiased assessment of different pathways in epicardial cells (identified bioinformatically) between intact P1 and P7 stages in wild type (control) hearts, with a focus on hypoxia-related gene expression and HIF-dependent pathways. It was not designed to analyse a small number of genes, rather global differences in the hypoxic states between P1 and P7 hearts. Selected genes (Vegfa, Pdk3, Egln 1 (Phd2)) were analysed to highlight the key differences in hypoxic signalling across the regenerative window. The fact the hearts were uninjured/intact is clarified in the text and legends for Figure 4 and now Figure 4-figure supplement 1.

      (11) The analyses provided in Figure 5 are very interesting and their findings are very relevant. However, I would think that the complementary experimental approach should also be done, i.e, MI followed by activation with tamoxifen, since that situation would be more realistic in the clinical setting.

      Tamoxifen causes respiratory failure in neonates with MI, so the two cannot be combined at the same time or soon after surgery. Moreover, tamoxifen takes significant time to take effect on targeted gene down-regulation which may negate sufficient activation of the epicardium following injury.

      The experiments in Figure 5 were designed to demonstrate that prolonged heart regeneration could be elicited in a cell-specific (epicardial-specific) manner via a genetic approach. The pharmacological experiments in Figure 6 are complementary in this regard by demonstrating equivalent effects with drug (Molidustat) delivery to reduce PHD2 and stabilise HIF post-MI.

      (12) In Figure 6, expression of Wt1 is highly prominent in P7 controls, mainly restricted to the epicardial lining while in the experimental setting, such Wt1 expression is broadly distributed on the subepicardial space, nicely demonstrating epicardial activation. However, it is very surprising to see such Wt1 expression in controls, something that is not expected, as compared to the data reported in Figure 4g. Could the authors please reconcile these findings?

      Figure 6 represents the injury setting and Figure 4g the intact setting (as clarified above, in the text and revised figure legends). Hence in the latter WT1 expression is significantly reduced in the P7 heart, as anticipated. With injury at P7 we anticipate activation of WT1 in control hearts, albeit restricted to the epicardial layer (as occurs in adult hearts, PMID: 21505261). In contrast, following Molidustat-treatment of P7 hearts post-MI we observe extensive epicardial expansion into the sub-epicardial region and EPDC migration into the underlying myocardium (Figure 6b).

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The role of hypoxia and HIF1a signaling in epicardial activation is an important topic, and the genetic approaches employed in this study are appropriate. However, several aspects of the study remain unclear and would benefit from further clarification or explanation by the authors:

      (1) The authors detected hypoxic regions using an anti-pimonidazole fluorescence-conjugated monoclonal antibody (HP1). The data would become more compelling if negative and positive controls were provided.

      We believe the HP1 staining is compelling in the images shown and is consistent with hypoxic regions of the developing heart. We reveal HP1 staining at cellular resolution with neighbouring cells positive and negative for the HP1 signal in the apex of the heart and within the epicardium and sub-epicardial regions at E12.5 (Figure 1a) and diminished/altered hypoxic/HP1 regional signal through subsequent developmental stages at E14.5-18.5 (Figure 1a-d).

      (2) Many HIF1a-positive cells in the AV groove region do not appear to overlap with HP1 staining (Figure 1a). Providing a low-magnification image of HIF1α expression would be helpful to better assess the extent of overlap with HP1 staining

      HIF-1 is highly unstable and hence detection of HIF-1+ cells will likely only sample of cells compared to HP1 which is a surrogate for broader regions of hypoxia.

      (3) Although the authors conclude that epicardial HIF1a deletion results in a significant reduction of WT1⁺ cells in both the epicardium and myocardium (Figure 2a-d), the provided images are not sufficiently clear to fully support this interpretation. Providing additional evidence to support this conclusion would be helpful.

      We respectfully disagree with the reviewer and draw attention to the single channel panels of WT1+ staining which show clear differences between numbers of epicardial cells in the mutant mice compared to controls (Figure 2a versus 2b; magenta WT1+ staining).

      (4) Similar to the point raised above, the authors' conclusion regarding the increased expression of WT1 following Molidustat treatment does not appear to be fully supported by the provided images (Figure 6b-f). Immunofluorescence staining for WT1 does not clearly demonstrate epicardial expression in the remote zone of either the control or Molidustat-treated hearts. In addition, while an increase of WT1<sup>+</sup> cells is observed in the infarct zone of the Molidustat-treated heart, it is somewhat unexpected that such expansion is not evident in the corresponding region of the control heart, given that epicardial cells typically expand near the infarct area. Clarification on these points would be helpful.

      Figure 6b reveals WT1 expression in controls (upper panel set) that is reactivated proximal to the infarct region, given WT1 is not expressed in adult epicardium but restricted to the epicardial layer (as occurs in injured adult mouse hearts PMID: 21505261). This contrasts with what is observed in the Molidustat-treated P7 hearts post-MI, where we observe epicardial expansion and migration of WT1+ cells into the underlying myocardium (Figure 6b, lower panel set, infarct zone).

      (5) The authors conclude that WT1<sup>+</sup> cells in the myocardial tissue exhibit endothelial identity based on the colocalization of WT1 and EMCN signals (Supplementary Figure 9c). However, this interpretation is difficult to assess, as WT1 is a nuclear marker and EMCN is a membrane protein, which makes precise colocalization challenging to confirm with confidence. Additional supporting evidence may be necessary to substantiate this conclusion.

      WT1 is known to be up regulated in endothelial cells in response to injury as shown previously in several studies (for example, PMID: 25681586). Here we show clear co-localisation of nuclear WT1 and cytoplasmic Endomucin (EMCN) in what is now Figure 6- figure supplement 1c and would encourage the reviewer and readers to magnify the image by zooming-in on the relevant co-stained panel.

      (6) The authors conclude that activation of epicardial HIF1a signaling has no effect on neovascularization in postnatal MI hearts (Figure 5c). However, the abstract states: "Finally, a combination of genetic and pharmacological stabilisation of HIF ... increased vascularisation, augmented infarct resolution and preserved function beyond the 7-day regenerative window" (Lines 38-41). Clarification regarding this apparent discrepancy would be appreciated.

      The abstract has been altered to remove the statement of increased vascularisation.

      (7) The study appears somewhat incomplete, as it lacks mechanistic insight into the functional recovery observed following epicardial Phd2 deletion and Molidustat treatment in postnatal MI hearts. Although the authors suggest a potential paracrine role of the epicardium in protecting cardiomyocytes from apoptosis, this hypothesis has not been experimentally addressed. Incorporating such analysis would help to reinforce the study's conclusions.

      Further experiments are required, which are out-of-scope of this study, to define a mechanistic link between the genetic or pharmacological stabilisation of HIF-signalling, epicardial activation and myocardial survival in the setting of prolonged neonatal heart regeneration.

      Other points:

      (1) Providing single-channel images for Figures 1a-d and 6g would be helpful for clarity and interpretation.

      We believe the combined channel views of co-staining for two markers on a background of DAPI staining to pin-point cell nuclei, are informative and support our conclusions.

      (2) Have the authors considered using AngioTool to quantify the number of vessels in Figure 5b-c?

      AngioToolTM was used to quantify the vessels, as we have used previously (PMID: 33462113) and this is now added to the methods and legend of Figure 2.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      There are several areas where the manuscript can be improved, such that its conclusions can be solidified.

      (1) The authors highlight a point where blocking Phd2 can enhance survival of cardiac tissue, but did not report on survival markers. They surmised that apoptosis could be decreased in Phd2 mutant or Molidustat treatment but did not show this. The authors should determine if apoptosis is decreased in the myocardium and epicardium.

      We show evidence of increased levels of healthy myocardium in the genetic and pharmacological models of stabilised HIF-signalling. We exclude increased cardiac hypertrophy or increased cardiomyocyte proliferation as causative, so suggest as a reasonable alternative enhanced survival, albeit this need not necessarily be via an apoptotic pathway given the incidence of necrotic cell death during MI. We are unable to generate new surgeries and mutant/treated heart samples to analyse for apoptotic markers at this stage.

      (2) There appears to be no difference in cardiomyocyte proliferation in Molidustat-treated animals, but the experiment was only performed on 2 to 3 animals. This is too small a sample size to conclude from these results. The authors should increase the sample size to make this assertion.

      We respectfully disagree that we are unable to conclude no effect on cardiomyocyte proliferation. We analysed multiple heart regions per section, for EdU+/cTnT+ colocalised signals across several sections per heart, set against a consistency of effect on other parameters in hearts treated with Molidustat. We are unable to generate more P7 heart surgeries +/- Molidustat and +/- EdU at this stage.

      (3) It is curious as to how, after myocardial infarction, the fibrotic scar tissue is decreased in the Phd2 deletion but not as profound in Molidustat-treated mice at d21. Can the authors speculate why the difference exists and how this decrease arises? For example, are there decreased pro-inflammatory signals in Phd2 deleted mice? Is there decreased collagen deposition and ECM gene expression? Do macrophage recruitment into the infarct zone differ between mutant/treated vs WT?

      The representative images in Figure 6k reveal a trend towards reduced fibrosis with Molidistat treatment (Figure 6l), but across all hearts analysed this was not as significant as observed in the epicardial-specific deletion injured hearts (Figure 5g, h). This may be due to the relatively short half-life of Molidustat (approximately 4-10 hours, PMID: 32248614), the dosing regimen for the drug and/or the fact that it was not specifically delivered/targeted to the epicardium.

      (4) The magnified images in Figure 1 do not match the boxes in the whole heart images. It is unclear what the white boxes signify.

      The white boxes have been removed from Figure 1. The magnified image panels are from serial heart sections and this is now clarified in the Figure 1 legend.

    1. solution.

      Dans la solution du 3ème point, à la dernière du fichier .css, est-ce grâce à "display: flex;" que la div "Voir la photo" s'affiche lors du survol de l'image avec la souris ?

    1. Not doing “la bise,” the kiss on the cheek that many French and Europeans use to greet each other, was on his list.

      not doing anything french related is an attack on french values

    2. religious neutrality of state institutions. In 2010, it outlawed the fully face-covering niqab and burqa everywhere in public, arguing that those garments threaten public safety and represent a rejection of a society of equal citizens.

      seems a criminizalition and marginalization of the other

    3. “If you are Muslim and you hide your face for religious reasons, you are liable to a fine and a citizenship course where you will be taught what it is to be ‘a good citizen,’ ” said Fatima Khemilat, a fellow at the Political Science Institute of Aix-en-Provence. “But if you are a non-Muslim citizen in the pandemic, you are encouraged and forced as a ‘good citizen’ to adopt ‘barrier gestures’ to protect the national community.”

      hypocritical to allow masks for coronovrius but not for religious reasons

    1. “What we must attack is Islamist separatism,” he told the nation, saying extremists preyed upon desperate Muslims in desolate neighborhoods, basically creating anti-French enclaves by spreading their radical Islamic “ideology” and “project.” He also made some sweeping, incendiary generalizations, such as that “Islam is a religion that is in crisis today, all over the world.”

      orientalist, treats islam as a monolith of hate

    2. The problem with that is French Muslims may feel extremely targeted by what Macron’s government is doing. After all, Holocaust denial is criminalized, which means some forms of expression are outlawed in France. But when it comes to images of the prophet, Macron says that’s fair play.

      same idea of why jews get protections but not muslims

    1. Publications such as Charlie Hebdo and Jyllands-Posten aim to incite the very passions that they simultaneously criticize Muslims for harboring. Theology and issues of semiotic representation are thus not of primary importance here. Rather, the performative political power of mass media, exercised from a hegemonic position against a vulnerable minority, defines events such as the Charlie Hebdo attack, the Jyllands-Posten cartoon controversy, and the Rushdie Affair.

      muslims were not offended because it insulted muhammed but because it betittled their beliefs in muhammed

    2. Besides showing insensitivity to their target, caricatures like Muhammad with a bomb instead of turban on his head contribute to entrenching the mindless Islamophobia that sees all Muslims as enemies of the West and its freedoms. Not a wise move if one is concerned either with integrating immigrants from the Maghreb in French society, or with avoiding the “clash of civilizations” which Islamophobes seem so eager to bring on.

      should not be branded muslims are religious freaks especiallt when trying to assimilate said muslims

    3. First, Muslims, like Jews, are not counseled to “turn the other cheek” when attacked. But this acceptance of violence in self-defense does not entail violent aggression against the defenseless. Thus, the Charlie Hebdo murders cannot be called Muslim: what looks like a Muslim proclivity for violence may actually be more accurately described as the absence of a Muslim pacifism. Religions come in different shapes: unarmed prophets, like Jesus or the Buddha, occupy a different moral universe than armed prophets, like Muhammad or Moses. But, just because a prophet is armed doesn’t mean he will attack the defenseless.

      the attackers are not a representation of muslims

    4. This mess is ours and it needs no “outsiders” to provoke it. Where we err, is when we take them as the embodiment of our malfunctioning and conflicted category of religion. When they are provoked, we can select from this homegrown mess to simultaneously assert our foundational right to attack the gods—with all the aggression that a nothing deserve

      by taking the claim that free speech attacking religion is fine because its not real makes the complexity of blasphemy in islam to not be accounted for

    5. Blasphemy as a category invokes the specter of religion, not ethnicity, which is why it is safe. Western modernity constructs itself on the assumption that the gods do not exist—or that they do not exist for all, which is the same thing.

      the reason why we allow free speech that makes fun of religion but not race is because of western belief that religion is fake

    6. Charlie Hebdo published many images of Muslim-like characters, some of them acting violently. Are such images meant to represent the generality of Muslims? In the way that, for example, the 1920s Nazi magazine, Der Sturmer, would have a cartoon of a Jewish financier, the undesirable qualities of whom—heartless, exploitative, greedy and so on—was meant to be about Jews as such. Yet if it is meant to lampoon jihadists, it is inoffensive and embodies a form of political defiance against terrorism.

      if we wouldn't make fun of jews then why would we do so for muslims

    7. We are Charlie because we are Malala; je suis Charlie because I Am Malala.

      the idea that we must respect an individuals commitment to break the norms, but also recognize that we must not support it in order to mantain the guise of respecting free speech

    8. For French authorities to demand that Muslims now publicly proclaim that they “are” Charlie—that they effectively endorse the content of the cartoons—is not to defend free speech. It is to enforce compulsory, official speech—the very opposite of free speech.

      supporting the contents of Hebedo as universal values is not supporting free speech but enforcing several kinds of speech

    9. Though it doesn’t cause violence, it is often the excuse for it. But religion can’t “do” anything—motivate actions or sway thinking—by itself. It is not a disembodied thing that has power of its own. It is simply a part of culture, something that people can use and abuse, for good or for ill. And lately, much of it has been for ill indeed.

      religion is just an excuse, not the cause

    10. When right-wing patriots almost literally wrap themselves in flags as they plot to assassinate the President of the United States—which they have—few people blame nationalism itself. Rather, they look at the mixture of psychological and political motives that may have brought the conspirators to their savage plans. When a whole group or culture adopts a vicious form of extreme nationalism—Nazism comes to mind—again it is not nationalism itself that we blame, but a perverted form of it crafted to buttress the power-hungry designs of a political junta.

      it hypocritical to blame islam for the attacks, when attacks by those carrying the american flag arent used as examples of nationalism being the cause

    11. Because this “us versus them” is very accessible to young Muslims everywhere through the Internet and other social media, it is no surprise that this rhetoric resonates with their daily experience in European societies and therefore make some of them easy recruits for the global jihad.

      strict secularism is a problem because it makes jihadist groups look more presentable when they claim the west is out to get them

    12. This rhetoric presents Islam as an external religion that threatens the core liberties of European democracies and therefore needs to be limited or circumvented, following the argument made famous by the French Revolutionary Saint-Just: “No freedom for the enemies of freedom”

      they are an another

    13. Muslims claiming that they could be protected by existing legislations across Europe (including France) that actually limit freedom of speech in cases of inciting racial hatred or denying the Holocaust.

      if jews are protected under hate speech laws, why not muslisms

    14. In the French context as well as all over Europe, we have witnessed in the last 20 years an increasing political resistance against the practices of Islam and their visibility in public spaces: from the ban on hijab (head covering) and niqab (full face covering) to the limitations on mosque-building, halal slaughtering, and even circumcision. Muslims have the feeling that being or looking like a practitioner of the Islamic faith will ostracize them, not to mention that this hostility goes hand in hand with concrete discriminations against the practice of the religion: women barred from entering public buildings because they wear hijabs, discrimination on the job market, in the workplace, etc.

      can fit within the orientalist context of trying to modernize those who are stuck with traditions and not with the modern west of secularism

    15. These measures are, in fact, part of a climate in which laïcité and republican values, while never clearly defined, have been used as a justification to scrutinize and interfere with the bodies, sensibilities, and practices of Muslim citizens (particularly women).

      strict secularism has been used when it pertains to muslims by treating them as others that need to be assimilated

    16. In no country is freedom of expression absolute, and the risk of turning the libertarian-anarchist soixante-huisards of Charlie Hebdo into martyrs in a liberal free speech pantheon through liberal media megaphones, is to feed the flames of stigmatization and polarization

      should not champion the magazine because it would only heighten tension between groups

    17. There is no single cause of terrorism—whether right-wing extremist or salafi-jihadist. The Paris terrorists are dead and do not speak. But we can be reasonably sure that their hatred and resentment did not relate exclusively to caricatures and cartoonists. Through the rituals of mourning and commemoration, these have been turned into core postmortem symbols of French elitist liberal and secular culture. The terror and violence seem also to have spoken about the longue durée of French-Arab-North African relations; its legacies of extreme brutality and violence on all sides; and its long afterlife in everyday lives marked by segregation, exclusion, marginalization and discriminatory policing in the French banlieues.

      the killings were not only a response to the caricturates, but also a sentiment burned from a history of french and muslims relations

    18. . But it is not unlikely that the rush by many in the United States and Europe to identify with the publication had something to do with the fact that the killers were Muslims and the cartoons were of Muhammad. By conflating form and content, Je suis Charlie lent a patina of liberal respectability to anti-Islamicism: “I disapprove of you, and I will defend to the death my right to say so.”

      arguing that people only showed support to this obscure magazine because it was an attack commited by muslims

    19. Muhammad deeply offensive, and critics have called the magazine racist and demeaning of the country’s cultural minorities, especially its sizable populations of North African origin. Notably, its offices were firebombed after the magazine named Muhammad its “editor in chief” for an issue on sharia in 2011.

      the magazine is very provacative with its liberal use of Muhammed

    1. Use assessment as a teaching tool to extend

      I really like the idea of on-going assessment and modifying learning strategies for students. Assessments are not necessarily quizzes or tests, but a mere tool for measuring where a student is struggling. But this can be a time burden for teachers to grade.

    2. modifying instructional approaches to meet student needs

      I really like teachers assess their students and then modify their instructional approaches based on the needs and learning readiness.

    Annotators

    1. Mr. Paty was a strong believer in laïcité, the strict secularism that separates religion from the state in France. Ms. Davoust recalled Mr. Paty once asking a young girl wearing a cross around her neck in school to take it off.

      the teacher embodies the strict sense of assimilation that placed on immigrant communities

    2. In a country guided by strict secularism, such actions are a violation of French law and regarded as signs of radicalization by the authorities — and they have led to many sports clubs being placed under surveillance.

      strict sense of secularism in French society

    3. Located in a public facility, the club was investigated by the local authorities because some members prayed in the locker room and asked women to cover their arms and legs, according to the French news media.

      could have been a source of his religious radicalism that clashed with the French ideals of secularism and freedom of expression

    4. Jean-Pierre Obin, a former senior national education official, said that public schools played a leading role in “the cultural assimilation and political integration” of immigrant children who “were turned into good little French” and no longer felt “Italian, Spanish, Portuguese or Polish.” Other institutions that also played this role — the Catholic church, unions and political parties — have been weakened, leaving only the schools, he said.

      goal of the French model of education was assimilating different groups of people under French identity

    5. Offended by cartoons of the Prophet Muhammad shown in a class on free speech given by the teacher, Samuel Paty, 47, the teenager beheaded him a week ago with a long knife before being gunned down by the police.

      this is a case related to blasphemy in Islam

    1. Images are created by defining a grid of dots, called pixels. Each pixel has three numbers that define the color (red, green, and blue), and the grid is created as a list (rows) of lists (columns).

      I find it interesting how much work goes into programming a computer to know the steps necessary to do a task. Its fascinating how a complicated technical process comes down to minimal details, and how simply a small error can cause many issues.

    1. in all likelihood, the reasonwhy we face challenges in shifting ourhealth care system to a focus on prevention

      There are a lot of pre-existing opinions, advice and strategies for health care and promotion that are deep-rooted into many layers of current policies and understanding of health across all different groups of people. Many of these messages and strategies are actually harmful to health promotion and health which has been redefined through the Ottawa Charter and WHO among others. I believe that generational differences in opinions about all aspects of life also contribute to the lack of people actually trying to understand new concepts and the importance of health promotion on a population level. People can become "stuck in their old ways and understandings", perhaps.

    2. The gradientin health suggests that we need populationwide universalist strategies as well as thosetargeted at the most disadvantaged.

      It is arguably more important to target disadvantage groups when discussing health promotion strategies, as currently, they are much less likely to receive and understand information about the underlying causes of health, as more of their basic prerequisites for health are unlikely to be met

    1. Whether written into the margins of texts, integratedinto the print, or digitally superimposed, in what contexts have you encoun-tered annotations written by another person? In what ways have you sharedyour annotations with other readers?

      When I use to go to used bookstores to buy books I could sometimes find books with annotations in them it was interesting reading what people were thinking as I was thinking the same thing!

    1. So we can make a list of the numbers from 1 to 10: 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 10

      List is very convenient for the programers. We can store as many number we want into the list. When we want to use the specific number inside the list, we can just state the location of that number in the list. Also, if you want to add more into the list, it is also flexible for programmers.