1. Jun 2024
    1. andere

      a enlever

    2. Milk drinks

      Melkdranken

    3. heetwateruitloop

      Heetwatertank

    4. melkachtige dranken

      melkdranken

    5. superieure ervaring is.

      ervaring van superieure kwaliteit is.

    6. uitmuntendheid met de

      de kwaliteitsvolle

    7. vast te houden.

      te maken.

    8. 500 gr koffie per keer

      telkens 500 gr koffie

    9. zonder problemen tot

      gemakkelijk tot

    1. We find that win rates for DITTO outperform methods like SFT (avg. 11% pt. increase),self-play methods like SPIN (20.2% pt.), and few-shot prompting (33.4% pt.) on Mistral 7B—evenwhen few-shot prompts are provided to a more powerful LLM (GPT-4, 18% pt.).

      Alignment by demonstration essentially does better than all prevalent tuning techniques.

    2. DITTO can be interpreted as an online imitation learning algorithm, where data sampled fromthe LLM is used to distinguish expert behavior.

      Since it involve s the LLM comparing demonstrations with its own intermediate outputs.

    3. we can achieve strong alignment with individuals by leveraging a small number ofuser-provided examples of desired behavior.

      As opposed to a large number of examples required for tuning

    4. How might weefficiently communicate preferences and align a language model to a new individual or task?

      Tuning for a small task can require hundres or thousands of examples

    5. LLM outputs feel unopinionated and generic because of this mismatch.

      Because before tuning they are designed to handle anything in the world.

    6. Across our benchmarks and userstudy, we find that win-rates for DITTO outperform few-shot prompting, supervisedfine-tuning, and other self-play methods by an average of 19% points

      This method of aligned produces better results than previous methods even without tuning.

    7. DITTO cheaply generatesonline comparison data by treating users’ demonstrations as preferred over outputfrom the LLM and its intermediate checkpoints.

      DITTO takes user examples and treats them as better than what the model generates. It uses these comparisons to help the model learn and improve.

    1. andere

      a enlever

    2. geinteresseerd

      geïnteresseerd

    3. Milk drinks

      Melkdranken

    4. heetwateruitloop

      Heetwatertank

    5. en combineert elegantie, prestaties en gebruiksgemak.

      aangezien deze elegantie, prestaties en gebruiksgemak combineert.

    6. Deze koffiemachine van Zwitserse makelij combineert een elegant design met topprestaties. Dankzij het kleurendisplay is de Jura X6 Dark Inox een koffiemachine die elke dag gemakkelijk te gebruiken is en je personeel zal plezieren.

      Deze Zwitserse merk combineert een elegant design met hoogwaardige prestaties. Dankzij het kleurenscherm is de Jura X6 Dark Inox een zeer gebruiksvriendelijke machnie die uw medewerkers dagelijks blij zal maken.

    7. Jura bean-to-cup machine die ideaal is

      bonenmachine van Jura, ideaal

    8. Descriptie

      Beschrijving

    9. vast te houden.

      te maken.

    10. 500 gr koffie per keer

      telkens 500 gr koffie

    11. zonder problemen

      gemakkelijk

    12. plug & play geleverd

      via plug&play geleverd

    13. reparatieservice

      herstellingservice

    1. in an argument is that it may be the only time you share your real feelings

      fighting is healthy, able to share opinions

    2. Fighting for your deeper needs

      couples have a hard tike to construct their feelings into words

    1. , with the lowest value of 0.0878 observed in LH R, and the highest value of 2.18 in BLH L (Fig. S6a). The Pielou evenness exhibited a minimum value of 0.0353 in LH R. and a maximum value of 0.761 in AK R (Fig. S6b). Based on beta diversity analysis (Fig. S6c), the cyanobacterial communities could be categorized into three groups. Group I comprised three reservoirs from Zhuhai City (NP R., FHS R., and QW R.), while group II included QCS R., MY R., YQ R., LH R., and others. The remaining reservoirs were classified into group III.

      这些数字都确认过吗?

    2. 254

      确认数字

    1. koffiemachine

      koffiemachines

    2. Ja

      --> sur le point d'interrogation juste derrière il faut changer "gerepareerd" par hersteld "reparatie" par herstelling.

    3. een

      a enlever

    4. veel opties

      meerdere mogelijkheden !

    5. geinteresseerd

      geïnteresseerd

    6. Milk drinks

      Melkdranken

    7. heetwateruitloop

      Heetwatertank

    8. , die een uitzonderlijk design combineert met een intuïtieve bediening.

      .Deze is zeer intuïtief en bovenop zeer design !

    9. Descriptie

      Beschrijving

    10. 500 gr koffie per keer bijvullen, genoeg om ongeveer 62 kopjes vast te houden

      telkens 500 gr koffie toevoegen, hiermee kun je ongeveer 62 kopjes serveren !

    11. zonder problemen tot

      gemakkelijk

    1. eLife assessment

      The study by Kim et al. is a valuable contribution to the topic of obtaining good channel conductance parameters from electrophysiological recordings. While promising in its ability to rapidly construct newly fitted models using generative adversarial networks, the approach is incompletely described and the generated models often substantially deviate from the dynamics observed empirically. The comparison with existing multi-objective optimization methods is also incomplete.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      The manuscript describes a GAN-based approach that generates parameters for HH-like channels for multiple C. Elengans neurons. The network is trained on generated data to produce parameter sets that, on the one hand, reproduce voltage responses and IV curves, and on the other hand, are indistinguishable from the ground truth parameters, as tested by the discriminator. It is then shown that these generated parameter sets lead to reasonable reproductions of the recorded responses (but see the section "weaknesses" below for some reservations).

      Strengths:

      In itself, I find the methodology of high interest, particularly in that it can generate parameter sets to construct models of new recordings at a very low computational cost.

      Weaknesses:

      Nevertheless, I believe there are some weaknesses in the evaluation of the models that should be addressed before the quality of the methodology can be fully assessed. Firstly, at the methodological level, the authors should provide more clarity on the inverse gradient operation they use, as opposed to just simulating the models, as such an inversion depends not only on the parameters but also on the state of the model. How the state is obtained remains unclear here. Secondly, in the evaluation of their models, the authors could provided more information such as IV curves, as whether these would be accurate is difficult to visually infer from their figures. Thirdly, the authors do not address the question of whether all obtained parameter sets are stable when simulated over longer times, while their figures do include hints that this might not be the case for at least some of their models (e.g. voltage traces that do not converge back to the equilibrium after the stimulus, but rather seem to diverge).

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      Generating biophysically detailed computational models that capture the characteristic physiological properties of biological neurons for diverse cell types is an important and difficult problem in computational neuroscience. One major challenge lies in determining the large number of parameters of such models, which are notoriously difficult to fit into experimental data. Thereby, the computational and energy costs can be significant. The study 'ElectroPhysiomeGAN: Generation of Biophysical Neuron Model Parameters from Recorded Electrophysiological Responses' by Kim et al. describes a computationally efficient approach for predicting model parameters of Hodgkin-Huxley neuron models using Generative Adversarial Networks (GANs) trained on simulation data. The method is applied to generate models for 9 non-spiking neurons in C. elegans based on electrophysiological recordings. While the generated models capture the responses of these neurons to some degree, they generally show significant deviations from the empirically observed responses in important features. While interesting, in its current form, the method has not been demonstrated to generate models that faithfully capture empirically observed responses.

      Strengths:

      The authors work on an important and difficult problem. A noteworthy strength of their approach is that once trained, the GANs can generate models from new empirical data with very little computational effort. The generated models reproduce the average voltage during current injections reasonably well.

      Weaknesses:

      Major 1: While the models generated with EP-GAN reproduce the average voltage during current injections reasonably well, the dynamics of the response are not well captured. For example, for the neuron labeled RIM (Figure 2), the most depolarized voltage traces show an initial 'overshoot' of depolarization, i.e. they depolarize strongly within the first few hundred milliseconds but then fall back to a less depolarized membrane potential. In contrast, the empirical recording shows no such overshoot. Similarly, for the neuron labeled AFD, all empirically recorded traces slowly ramp up over time. In contrast, the simulated traces are mostly flat. Furthermore, all empirical traces return to the pre-stimulus membrane potential, but many of the simulated voltage traces remain significantly depolarized, far outside of the ranges of empirically observed membrane potentials. While these deviations may appear small in the Root mean Square Error (RMSE), the only metric used in the study to assess the quality of the models, they likely indicate a large mismatch between the model and the electrophysiological properties of the biological neuron.

      Major 2: Other metrics than the RMSE should be incorporated to validate simulated responses against electrophysiological data. A common approach is to extract multiple biologically meaningful features from the voltage traces before, during and after the stimulus, and compare the simulated responses to the experimentally observed distribution of these features. Typically, a model is only accepted if all features fall within the empirically observed ranges (see e.g. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002107). However, based on the deviations in resting membrane potential and the return to the resting membrane potential alone, most if not all the models shown in this study would not be accepted.

      Major 3: Abstract and introduction imply that the 'ElectroPhysiome' refers to models that incorporate both the connectome and individual neuron physiology. However, the work presented in this study does not make use of any connectomics data. To make the claim that ElectroPhysiomeGAN can jointly capture both 'network interaction and cellular dynamics', the generated models would need to be evaluated for network inputs, for example by exposing them to naturalistic stimuli of synaptic inputs. It seems likely that dynamics that are currently poorly captured, like slow ramps, or the ability of the neuron to return to its resting membrane potential, will critically affect network computations.

    1. eLife assessment

      This valuable study by Wu and Zhou combines neurophysiological recordings and computational modelling to address an interesting question regarding the sequence of events from sensing to action. Neurophysiological evidence remains incomplete: explicit mapping of saccade-related activity in the same neurons and a better understanding of the influence of the spatial configuration of stimulus and targets would be required to pinpoint whether such activity might contribute, even partially, to the observed results and interpretations. These results are of interest for neuroscientists investigating decision-making.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      This valuable study by Wu and Zhou combined neurophysiological recordings and computational modelling to investigate the neural mechanisms that underpin the interaction between sensory evaluation and action selection. The neurophysiological results suggest non-linear modulation of decision-related LIP activity by action selection, but some further analysis would be helpful in order to understand whether these results can be generalised to LIP circuitry or might be dependent on specific spatial task configurations. The authors present solid computational evidence that this might be due to projections from choice target representations. These results are of interest for neuroscientists investigating decision-making.

      Strengths:

      Wu and Zhou combine awake behaving neurophysiology for a sophisticated, flexible visual-motion discrimination task and a recurrent network model to disentangle the contribution of sensory evaluation and action selection to LIP firing patterns. The correct saccade response direction for preferred motion direction choices is randomly interleaved between contralateral and ipsilateral response targets, which allows the dissociation of perceptual choice from saccade direction.<br /> The neurophysiological recordings from area LIP indicate non-linear interaction between motion categorisation decisions and saccade choice direction.

      The careful investigation of a recurrent network model suggests that feedback from choice target representations to an earlier sensory evaluation stage might be the source for this non-linear modulation and that it is an important circuit component for behavioural performance.

      The paper presents a possible solution to a central controversy about the role of LIP in perceptual decision-making, but see below.

      Weaknesses:

      The paper presents a possible solution to a central controversy about the role of LIP in perceptual decision-making. However, the authors could be more clear and upfront about their interpretational framework and potential alternative interpretations.<br /> Centrally, the authors' model and experimental data appears to test only that LIP carries out sensory evaluation in its RFs. The model explicitly parks the representation of choice targets outside the "LIP" module receiving sensory input. The feedback from this separate target representation provides then the non-linear modulation that matches the neurophysiology. However, they ignore the neurophysiological results that LIP neurons can also represent motor planning to a saccade target.<br /> The neurophysiological results with a modulation of the direction tuning by choice direction (contralateral vs ipsilateral) are intriguing. However, the evaluation of the neurophysiological results are difficult, because some of the necessary information is missing to exclude alternative explanations. It would be good to see the actual distributions and sizes of the RF, which were determined based on visual responses not with a delayed saccade task. There might be for example a simple spatial configuration, for example, RF and preferred choice target in the same (contralateral) hemifield, for which there is an increase in firing. It is a shame that we do not see what these neurons would do if only a choice target would be put in the RF, as has been done in so many previous LIP experiments. The authors exclude also some spatial task configurations (vertical direction decisions), which makes it difficult to judge whether these data and models can be generalised. The whole section is difficult to follow, partly also because it appears to mix reporting results with interpretation (e.g. "feedback").

      The model and its investigation is very interesting and thorough, but given the neurophysiological literature on LIP, it is not clear that the target module would need to be in a separate brain area, but could be local circuitry within LIP between different neuron types.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, the authors recorded activity in the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) of monkeys performing a perceptual decision-making task. The monkeys were first shown two choice dots of two different colors. Then, they saw a random dot motion stimulus. They had to learn to categorize the direction of motion as referring to either the right or left dot. However, the rule was based on the color of the dot and not its location. So, the red dot could either be to the right or left, but the rule itself remained the same. It is known from past work that PPC neurons would code the learned categorization. Here, the authors showed that the categorization signal depended on whether the executed saccade was in the same hemifield as the recorded PPC neuron or in the opposite one. That is, if a neuron categorized the two motion directions such that it responded stronger for one than the other, then this differential motion direction coding effect was amplified if the subsequent choice saccade was in the same hemifield. The authors then built a computational RNN to replicate the results and make further tests by simulated "lesions".

      Strengths:

      Linking the results to RNN simulations and simulated lesions.

      Weaknesses:

      Potential interpretational issues due to a lack of evidence on what happens at the time of the saccades.

    1. eLife assessment

      This paper makes a valuable contribution to our understanding of the tradeoffs in eye design - specifically between improvements in optics and in photoreceptor performance. The authors successfully build a formal theory that enables comparisons across a wide range of species and eye types. The conclusion from the modeling is that resources are split relatively evenly between optics and photoreceptors, and hence that both must be considered in eye design. Evidence for this conclusion is solid, and could be strengthened with a more complete comparison with the experiment.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      Two important factors in visual performance are the resolving power of the lens and the signal-to-noise ratio of the photoreceptors. These both compete for space: a larger lens has improved resolving power over a smaller one, and longer photoreceptors capture more photons and hence generate responses with lower noise. The current paper explores the tradeoff of these two factors, asking how space should be allocated to maximize eye performance (measured as encoded information).

      Strengths:

      The topic of the paper is interesting and not well studied. The approach is clearly described and seems appropriate (with a few exceptions - see weaknesses below). In most cases, the parameter space of the models are well explored and tradeoffs are clear.

      Weaknesses:

      - Light level<br /> The calculations in the paper assume high light levels (which reduces the number of parameters that need to be considered). The impact of this assumption is not clear. A concern is that the optimization may be quite different at lower light levels. Such a dependence on light level could explain why the model predictions and experiment are not in particularly good agreement. The paper would benefit from exploring this issue.

      - Discontinuities<br /> The discontinuities and non-monotonicity of the optimal parameters plotted in Figure 4 are concerning. Are these a numerical artifact? Some discussion of their origin would be quite helpful.

      - Discrepancies between predictions and experiment<br /> As the authors clearly describe, experimental measurements of eye parameters differ systematically from those predicted. This makes it difficult to know what to take away from the paper. The qualitative arguments about how resources should be allocated are pretty general, and the full model seems a complex way to arrive at those arguments. Could this reflect a failure of one of the assumptions that the model rests on - e.g. high light levels, or that the cost of space for photoreceptors and optics is similar? Given these discrepancies between model and experiment, it is also hard to evaluate conclusions about the competition between optics and photoreceptors (e.g. at the end of the abstract) and about the importance for evolution (end of introduction).

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      In short, the paper presents a theoretical framework that predicts how resources should be optimally distributed between receptors and optics in eyes.

      Strengths:

      The authors build on the principle of resource allocation within an organism and develop a formal theory for optimal distribution of resources within an eye between the receptor array and the optics. Because the two parts of eyes, receptor arrays and optics, share the same role of providing visual information to the animal it is possible to isolate these from resource allocation in the rest of the animal. This allows for a novel and powerful way of exploring the principles that govern eye design. By clever and thoughtful assumptions/constraints, the authors have built a formal theory of resource allocation between the receptor array and the optics for two major types of compound eye as well as for camera-type eyes. The theory is formalized with variables that are well characterized in a number of different animal eyes, resulting in testable predictions.

      The authors use the theory to explain a number of design features that depend on different optimal distribution of resources between the receptor array and the optics in different types of eyes. As an example, they successfully explain why eye regions with different spatial resolution should be built in different ways. They also explain differences between different types of eyes, such as long photoreceptors in apposition compound eyes and much shorter receptors in camera type eyes. The predictive power in the theory is impressive.

      To keep the number of parameters at a minimum, the theory was developed for two types of compound eye (neural superposition, and apposition) and for camera-type eyes. It is possible to extend the theory to other types of eyes, although it would likely require more variables and assumptions/constraints to the theory. It is thus good to introduce the conceptual ideas without overdoing the applications of the theory.

      The paper extends a previous theory, developed by the senior author, that develops performance surfaces for optimal cost/benefit design of eyes. By combining this with resource allocation between receptors and optics, the theoretical understanding of eye design takes a major leap and provides entirely new sets of predictions and explanations for why eyes are built the way they are.

      The paper is well written and even though the theory development in the Results may be difficult to take in for many biologists, the Discussion very nicely lists all the major predictions under separate headings, and here the text is more tuned for readers that are not entirely comfortable with the formalism of the Results section. I must point out though that the Results section is kept exemplary concise. The figures are excellent and help explain concepts that otherwise may go above the head of many biologists.

    4. Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      This is a proposal for a new theory for the geometry of insect eyes. The novel cost-benefit function combines the cost of the optical portion with the photoreceptor portion of the eye. These quantities are put on the same footing using a specific (normalized) volume measure, plus an energy factor for the photoreceptor compartment. An optimal information transmission rate then specifies each parameter and resource allocation ratio for a variable total cost. The elegant treatment allows for comparison across a wide range of species and eye types. Simple eyes are found to be several times more efficient across a range of eye parameters than neural superposition eyes. Some trends in eye parameters can be explained by optimal allocation of resources between the optics and photoreceptors compartments of the eye.

      Strengths:

      Data from a variety of species roughly align with rough trends in the cost analysis, e.g. as a function of expanding the length of the photoreceptor compartment.

      New data could be added to the framework once collected, and many species can be compared.

      Eyes of different shapes are compared.

      Weaknesses:

      Detailed quantitative conclusions are not possible given the approximations and simplifying assumptions in the models and poor accounting for trends in the data across eye types.

    1. eLife assessment

      This important study provides solid evidence that both psychiatric dimensions (e.g. anhedonia, apathy, or depression) and chronotype (i.e., being a morning or evening person) influence effort-based decision-making. Notably, the current study does not elucidate whether there may be interactive effects of chronotype and psychiatric dimensions on decision-making. This work is of importance to researchers and clinicians alike, who may make inferences about behaviour and cognition without taking into account whether the individual may be tested or observed out-of-sync with their phenotype.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      This study uses an online cognitive task to assess how reward and effort are integrated in a motivated decision-making task. In particular the authors were looking to explore how neuropsychiatric symptoms, in particular apathy and anhedonia, and circadian rhythms affect behavior in this task. Amongst many results, they found that choice bias (the degree to which integrated reward and effort affects decisions) is reduced in individuals with greater neuropsychiatric symptoms, and late chronotypes (being an 'evening person').

      Strengths:

      The authors recruited participants to perform the cognitive task both in and out of sync with their chronotypes, allowing for the important insight that individuals with late chronotypes show a more reduced choice bias when tested in the morning.<br /> Overall, this is a well-designed and controlled online experimental study. The modelling approach is robust, with care being taken to both perform and explain to the readers the various tests used to ensure the models allow the authors to sufficiently test their hypotheses.

      Weaknesses:

      This study was not designed to test the interactions of neuropsychiatric symptoms and chronotypes on decision making, and thus can only make preliminary suggestions regarding how symptoms, chronotypes and time-of-assessment interact.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The study combines computational modeling of choice behavior with an economic, effort-based decision-making task to assess how willingness to exert physical effort for a reward varies as a function of individual differences in apathy and anhedonia, or depression, as well as chronotype. They find an overall reduction in effort selection that scales with apathy and anhedonia and depression. They also find that later chronotypes are less likely to choose effort than earlier chronotypes and, interestingly, an interaction whereby later chronotypes are especially unwilling to exert effort in the morning versus the evening.

      Strengths:

      This study uses state-of-the-art tools for model fitting and validation and regression methods which rule out multicollinearity among symptom measures and Bayesian methods which estimate effects and uncertainty about those estimates. The replication of results across two different kinds of samples is another strength. Finally, the study provides new information about the effects not only of chronotype but also chronotype by timepoint interactions which are previously unknown in the subfield of effort-based decision-making.

      Weaknesses:

      The study has few weaknesses. One potential concern is that the range of models which were tested was narrow, and other models might have been considered. For example, the Authors might have also tried to fit models with an overall inverse temperature parameter to capture decision noise. One reason for doing so is that some variance in the bias parameter might be attributed to noise, which was not modeled here. Another concern is that the manuscripts discuss effort-based choice as a transdiagnostic feature - and there is evidence in other studies that effort deficits are a transdiagnostic feature of multiple disorders. However, because the present study does not investigate multiple diagnostic categories, it doesn't provide evidence for transdiagnosticity, per se.

    4. Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, Mehrhof and Nord study a large dataset of participants collected online (n=958 after exclusions) who performed a simple effort-based choice task. They report that the level of effort and reward influence choices in a way that is expected from prior work. They then relate choice preferences to neuropsychiatric syndromes and, in a smaller sample (n<200), to people's circadian preferences, i.e., whether they are a morning-preferring or evening-preferring chronotype. They find relationships between the choice bias (a model parameter capturing the likelihood to accept effort-reward challenges, like an intercept) and anhedonia and apathy, as well as chronotype. People with higher anhedonia and apathy and an evening chronotype are less likely to accept challenges (more negative choice bias). People with an evening chronotype are also more reward sensitive and more likely to accept challenges in the evening, compared to the morning.

      Strengths:

      This is an interesting and well-written manuscript which replicates some known results and introduces a new consideration related to potential chronotype relationships which have not been explored before. It uses a large sample size and includes analyses related to transdiagnostic as well as diagnostic criteria. I have some suggestions for improvements.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The novel findings in this manuscript are those pertaining to transdiagnostic and circadian phenotypes. The authors report two separate but "overlapping" effects: individuals high on anhedonia/apathy are less willing to accept offers in the task, and similarly, individuals tested off their chronotype are less willing to accept offers in the task. The authors claim that the latter has implications for studying the former. In other words, because individuals high on anhedonia/apathy predominantly have a late chronotype (but might be tested early in the day), they might accept less offers, which could spuriously look like a link between anhedonia/apathy and choices but might in fact be an effect of the interaction between chronotype and time-of-testing. The authors therefore argue that chronotype needs to be accounted for when studying links between depression and effort tasks.<br /> The authors argue that, if X is associated with Y and Z is associated with Y, X and Z might confound each other. That is possible, but not necessarily true. It would need to be tested explicitly by having X (anhedonia/apathy) and Z (chronotype) in the same regression model. Does the effect of anhedonia/apathy on choices disappear when accounting for chronotype (and time-of-testing)? Similarly, when adding the interaction between anhedonia/apathy, chronotype, and time-of-testing, within the subsample of people tested off their chronotype, is there a residual effect of anhedonia/apathy on choices or not?<br /> If the effect of anhedonia/apathy disappeared (or got weaker) while accounting for chronotype, this result would suggest that chronotype mediates the effect of anhedonia/apathy on effort choices. However, I am not sure it renders the direct effect of anhedonia/apathy on choices entirely spurious. Late chronotype might be a feature (induced by other symptoms) of depression (such as fatigue and insomnia), and the association between anhedonia/apathy and effort choices might be a true and meaningful one. For example, if the effect of anhedonia/apathy on effort choices was mediated by altered connectivity of the dorsal ACC, we would not say that ACC connectivity renders the link between depression and effort choices "spurious", but we would speak of a mechanism that explains this effect. The authors should discuss in a more nuanced way what a significant mediation by the chronotype/time-of-testing congruency means for interpreting effects of depression in computational psychiatry.

      (2) It seems that all key results relate to the choice bias in the model (as opposed to reward or effort sensitivity). It would therefore be helpful to understand what fundamental process the choice bias is really capturing in this task. This is not discussed, and the direction of effects is not discussed either, but potentially quite important. It seems that the choice bias captures how many effortful reward challenges are accepted overall which maybe captures general motivation or task engagement. Maybe it is then quite expected that this could be linked with questionnaires measuring general motivation/pleasure/task engagement. Formally, the choice bias is the constant term or intercept in the model for p(accept), but the authors never comment on what its sign means. If I'm not mistaken, people with higher anhedonia but also higher apathy are less likely to accept challenges and thus engage in the task (more negative choice bias). I could not find any discussion or even mention of what these results mean. This similarly pertains to the results on chronotype. In general, "choice bias" may not be the most intuitive term and the authors may want to consider renaming it. Also, given the sign of what the choice bias means could be flipped with a simple sign flip in the model equation (i.e., equating to accepting more vs accepting less offers), it would be helpful to show some basic plots to illustrate the identified differences (e.g., plotting the % accepted for people in the upper and lower tertile for the SHAPS score etc).

      (3) None of the key effects relate to effort or reward sensitivity which is somewhat surprising given the previous literature and also means that it is hard to know if choice bias results would be equally found in tasks without any effort component. (The only analysis related to effort sensitivity is exploratory and in a subsample of N=56 per group looking at people meeting criteria for MDD vs matched controls.) Were stimuli constructed such that effort and reward sensitivity could be separated (i.e., are uncorrelated/orthogonal)? Maybe it would be worth looking at the % accepted in the largest or two largest effort value bins in an exploratory analysis. It seems the lowest and 2nd lowest effort level generally lead to accepting the challenge pretty much all the time, so including those effort levels might not be sensitive to individual difference analyses?

      (4) The abstract and discussion seem overstated (implications for the school system and statements on circadian rhythms which were not measured here). They should be toned down to reflect conclusions supported by the data.

    1. eLife assessment

      This important work advances our understanding of microglial aging trajectory and heterogeneity. The authors provide an in-depth characterization of microglia in aging and aim to identify molecular checkpoints, that while solid are also deemed incomplete to support all the authors' claims. The study should be of interest to neuroimmunologists and biologists interested in aging.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript by Shea and Villeda furnishes the field with a valuable scRNAseq data set detailing microglial aging in the mouse hippocampus. They provide clear evidence that changes in microglial attributes begin in mid-life, well before time points when mice are traditionally considered to be "aging." It also adds to a growing body of data in the field demonstrating that there is substantial heterogeneity in microglial responses to aging. Using in vitro experiments and transgenic manipulations in mice, the authors show that transforming growth factor beta (TGFb1)-based signaling can potently impact microglial state, consistent with previous findings in the field. They also demonstrate that manipulation of microglial TGFb1-based signaling can impact hippocampus-dependent behaviors.

      Limitations of the study lie primarily in reaching too far with interpretations of the data. The authors argue that changes in microglial transcriptome during midlife represent a type of "checkpoint," after which microglial aging can progress along distinct trajectories depending on the status of TGFb1 signaling. They also posit that a specific intermediate "stress response" state in midlife is mechanistically linked to a translational burst that drives the subsequent progression of microglia to an "inflammatory state." Unequivocal data to support these causal links is lacking, however. similarly, key additional experiments would be needed to demonstrate that TGFb1 signaling and microglial progression through these identified intermediate states are causally linked to cognitive decline.

      Guidance for readers along with study strengths and caveats:

      The present manuscript provides valuable strengthening and expansion to a growing body of data showing prominent changes in the microglial state during aging. Microarray(1), bulkRNAseq(2-5), scRNAseq(6,7), snRNAseq(8,9), and spatial transcriptomic(10) approaches have been leveraged to map changes in microglial transcriptome during aging in rodents, non-human primates, and humans. A number of these studies include the hippocampus (1,8,9,11) and have highlighted variation across brain regions in microglial transcriptomic changes during aging (1,11). They have also revealed differences across sex (7) as well as increased cell-to-cell heterogeneity (6-10), consistent with the idea that individual microglia can follow distinct aging trajectories. Several of these studies revealed that changes in microglial attributes begin in middle age (1,7,11), supporting similar observations from studies that did not use omics (12-14). The present manuscript utilizes scRNAseq of hippocampal microglia at adulthood (6mo), middle age (12mo), late middle age (18mo) and aging (24mo) to show that aging-induced changes in microglia begin in middle age and that microglia exhibit ample phenotypic heterogeneity during the progression of aging.

      To gain further insight into the dynamics of microglial aging in the hippocampus, the authors used a bioinformatics method known as "pseudotime" or "trajectory inference" to understand how cells may progress through different functional states, as defined by cellular transcriptome (15,16). These bioinformatics approaches can reveal key patterns in scRNAseq / snRNAseq datasets and, in the present study, the authors conclude that a "stress response" module characterized by expression of TGFb1 represents a key "checkpoint" in microglial aging in midlife, after which the cells can move along distinct transcriptional trajectories as aging progresses. This is an intriguing possibility. However, pseudotime analyses need to be validated via additional bioinformatics as well as follow-up experiments. Indeed, Heumos et al, in their Nature Genetics "Expert Guidelines" Review, emphasize that "inferred trajectories might not necessarily have biological meaning." They recommend that "when the expected topology is unknown, trajectories and downstream hypotheses should be confirmed by multiple trajectory inference methods using different underlying assumptions."(15) Numerous algorithms are available for trajectory inference (e.g. Monocle, PAGA, Sligshot, RaceID/StemID, among many others) and their performance and suitability depends on the individual dataset and nature of the trajectories that are to be inferred. It is recommended to use dynGuidelines(16) for the selection of optimal pseudotime analysis methods. In the present manuscript, the authors do not provide any justification for their use of Monocle 3 over other trajectory inference approaches, nor do they employ a secondary trajectory inference method to confirm observations made with Monocle 3. Finally, follow-up validation experiments that the authors carry out have their own limitations and caveats (see below). Hence, while the microglial aging trajectories identified by this study are intriguing, they remain hypothetical trajectories that need to be proven with additional follow-up experiments.

      To follow up on the idea that TGFb1 signaling in microglia plays a key role in determining microglial aging trajectories, the authors use RNAscope to show that TGFb1 levels in microglia peak in middle age. They also treat primary LPS-activated microglia with TGFb1 and show that this restores expression of microglial homeostatic gene expression and dampens expression of stress response and, potentially, inflammatory genes. Finally, they utilize transgenic approaches to delete TGFb1 from microglia around 8-10mo of age and scRNAseq to show that homeostatic signatures are lost and inflammatory signatures are gained. Hence, findings in this study support the idea that TGFb1 can strongly regulate microglial phenotype. Loss of TGFb1 signaling to microglia in adulthood has already been shown to cause decreased microglial morphological complexity and upregulation of genes typically associated with microglial responses to CNS insults(17-19). TGFb1 signaling to microglia has also been implicated in microglial responses to disease and manipulations to increase this signaling can improve disease progression in some cases(19). In this light, the findings in the present study are largely confirmatory of previous findings in the literature. They also fall short of unequivocally demonstrating that TGFb1 signaling acts as a "checkpoint" for determining subsequent microglial aging trajectory. To show this clearly, one would need to perturb TGFb1 signaling around 12mo of age and carry out sequencing (bulkRNAseq or scRNAseq) of microglia at 18mo and 24mo. Such experiments could directly demonstrate whether the whole microglial population has been diverted to the TGFb1-low aging trajectory (that progresses through a translational burst state to an inflammation state as proposed). Future development of tools to tag TGFb1 high or low microglia could also enable fate tracing type experiments to directly show whether the TGFb1 state in middle age predicts cell state at later phases of aging.

      The present study would also like to draw links between features of microglial aging in the hippocampus and a decline in hippocampal-dependent cognition during aging. To this end, they carry out behavioral testing in 8-10mo old mice that have undergone microglial-specific TGFb1 deletion and find deficits in novel object recognition and contextual fear conditioning. While this provides compelling evidence that TGFb1 signaling in microglia can impact hippocampus-dependent cognition in midlife, it does not demonstrate that this signaling accelerates or modulates cognitive decline (see below). Age-associated cognitive decline refers to cognitive deficits that emerge as a result of the normative brain aging process(20-21). For a cognitive deficit to be considered age-associated cognitive decline, it must be shown that the cognitive operation under study was intact at some point earlier in the adult lifespan. This requires longitudinal study designs that determine whether a manipulation impacts the relationship between brain status and cognition as animals age (22-24). Alternatively, cross-sectional studies with adequate sample sizes can be used to sample the variability in cognitive outcomes at different points of the adult lifespan(22-24) and show that this is altered by a particular manipulation. For this specific study, one would ideally demonstrate that hippocampal-based learning/memory was intact at some point in the lifespan of mice with microglial TGFb1 KO but that this manipulation accelerated or exacerbated the emergence of deficits in hippocampal-dependent learning/memory during aging. In the absence of these types of data, the authors should tone down their claims that they have identified a cellular and molecular mechanism that contributes to cognitive decline.

      A final point of clarification for the reader pertains to the mining of previously generated data sets within this study. The language in the results section, methods, and figure legends causes confusion about which experiments were actually carried out in this study versus previous studies. Some of the language makes it sound as though parabiosis experiments and experiments using mouse models of Alzheimer's Disease were carried out in this study. However, parabiosis and AD mouse model experiments were executed in previous studies (25,26), and in the present study, RNAseq datasets were accessed for targeted data mining. It is fantastic to see further mining of datasets that already exist in the field. However, descriptions in the results and methods sections need to make it crystal clear that this is what was done.

      References:

      (1) Grabert, K. et al. Microglial brain region-dependent diversity and selective regional sensitivities to aging. Nat. Neurosci. (2016). doi:10.1038/nn.4222<br /> (2) Hickman, S. E. et al. The microglial sensome revealed by direct RNA sequencing. Nat. Neurosci. (2013). doi:10.1038/nn.3554<br /> (3) Deczkowska, A. et al. Mef2C restrains microglial inflammatory response and is lost in brain ageing in an IFN-I-dependent manner. Nat. Commun. (2017). doi:10.1038/s41467-017-00769-0<br /> (4) O'Neil, S. M., Witcher, K. G., McKim, D. B. & Godbout, J. P. Forced turnover of aged microglia induces an intermediate phenotype but does not rebalance CNS environmental cues driving priming to immune challenge. Acta Neuropathol. Commun. (2018). doi:10.1186/s40478-018-0636-8<br /> (5) Olah, M. et al. A transcriptomic atlas of aged human microglia. Nat. Commun. (2018). doi:10.1038/s41467-018-02926-5<br /> (6) Hammond, T. R. et al. Single-Cell RNA Sequencing of Microglia throughout the Mouse Lifespan and in the Injured Brain Reveals Complex Cell-State Changes. Immunity 50, 253-271 (2019).<br /> (7) Li, X. et al. Transcriptional and epigenetic decoding of the microglial aging process. Nat. aging 3, 1288-1311 (2023).<br /> (8) Zhang, H. et al. Single-nucleus transcriptomic landscape of primate hippocampal aging. Protein Cell 12, 695-716 (2021).<br /> (9) Su, Y. et al. A single-cell transcriptome atlas of glial diversity in the human hippocampus across the postnatal lifespan. Cell Stem Cell 29, 1594-1610.e8 (2022).<br /> (10) Allen, W. E., Blosser, T. R., Sullivan, Z. A., Dulac, C. & Zhuang, X. Molecular and spatial signatures of mouse brain aging at single-cell resolution. Cell 186, 194-208.e18 (2023).<br /> (11) Soreq, L. et al. Major Shifts in Glial Regional Identity Are a Transcriptional Hallmark of Human Brain Aging. Cell Rep. 18, 557-570 (2017).<br /> (12) Hefendehl, J. K. et al. Homeostatic and injury-induced microglia behavior in the aging brain. Aging Cell (2014). doi:10.1111/acel.12149<br /> (13) Nikodemova, M. et al. Microglial numbers attain adult levels after undergoing a rapid decrease in cell number in the third postnatal week. J. Neuroimmunol. 0, 280-288 (2015).<br /> (14) Moca, E. N. et al. Microglia Drive Pockets of Neuroinflammation in Middle Age. J. Neurosci. 42, 3896-3918 (2022).<br /> (15) Heumos, L. et al. Best practices for single-cell analysis across modalities. Nat. Rev. Genet. 24, 550-572 (2023).<br /> (16) Saelens, W., Cannoodt, R., Todorov, H. & Saeys, Y. A comparison of single-cell trajectory inference methods: towards more accurate and robust tools. (2018). doi:10.1101/276907<br /> (17) Zöller, T. et al. Silencing of TGFβ signalling in microglia results in impaired homeostasis. Nat. Commun. 9, (2018).<br /> (18) Bedolla, A. et al. Microglia-derived TGF-β1 ligand maintains microglia homeostasis via autocrine mechanism and is critical for normal cognitive function in adult mouse brain. bioRxiv Prepr. Serv. Biol. (2023). doi:10.1101/2023.07.05.547814<br /> (19) Spittau, B., Dokalis, N. & Prinz, M. The Role of TGFβ Signaling in Microglia Maturation and Activation. Trends Immunol. 41, 836-848 (2020).<br /> (20) L. Nyberg, M. Lövdén, K. Riklund, U. Lindenberger, L. Bäckman, Memory aging and brain maintenance. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 292-305 (2012).<br /> (21) L. Luo, F. I. M. Craik, Aging and memory: A cognitive approach. Can. J. Psychiatry 53, 346-353 (2008).<br /> (22) Y. Stern, M. Albert, C. Barnes, R. Cabeza, A. Pascual-Leone, P. Rapp.<br /> A framework for concepts of reserve and resilience in aging. Neurobiol. Aging, 124 (2022), pp. 100-103, 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2022.10.015<br /> (23) Y. Stern, C.A. Barnes, C. Grady, R.N. Jones, N. Raz. Brain reserve, cognitive reserve, compensation, and maintenance: operationalization, validity, and mechanisms of cognitive resilience. Neurobiol. Aging, 83 (2019), pp. 124-129, 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2019.03.022<br /> (24) R. Cabeza, M. Albert, S. Belleville, F.I.M. Craik, A. Duarte, C.L. Grady, U. Lindenberger, L. Nyberg, D.C. Park, P.A. Reuter-Lorenz, M.D. Rugg, J. Steffener, M.N. Rajah. Maintenance, reserve and compensation: the cognitive neuroscience of healthy ageing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 19 (11) (2018), Article 11, 10.1038/s41583-018-0068-2<br /> (25) Palovics, R. et al molecular hallmarks of heterochronic parabiosis at single-cell resolution. Nature 603, 309-314 (2022)<br /> (26) Sala Frigerio, C. et al. The major risk factors for Alzheimer's Disease: age, sex, and genes modulate the microglial response to Abeta plaques. Cell Rep, 27, 1293-1306 (2019)

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The goal of the paper was to trace the transitions hippocampal microglia undergo along aging. ScRNA-seq analysis allowed the authors to predict a trajectory and hypothesize about possible molecular checkpoints, which keep the pace of microglial aging. E.g. TGF1b was predicted as a molecule slowing down the microglial aging path and indeed, loss of TGF1 in microglia led to premature microglia aging, which was associated with premature loss of cognitive ability. The authors also used the parabiosis model to show how peripheral, blood-derived signals from the old organism can "push" microglia forward on the aging path.

      Strengths:

      A major strength and uniqueness of this work is the in-depth single-cell dataset, which may be a useful resource for the community, as well as the data showing what happens to young microglia in heterochronic parabiosis setting and upon loss of TGFb in their environment.

      Weaknesses:

      That said, given what we recently learned about microglia isolation for RNA-seq analysis, there is a danger that some of the observations are a result of not age, but cell stress from sample preparation (enzymatic digestion 10min at 37C; e.g. PMID: 35260865). Changes in cell state distribution along aging were made based on scRNA-seq and were not corroborated by any other method, such as imaging of cluster-specific marker expression in microglia at different ages. This analysis would allow confirming the scRNA-seq data and would also give us an idea of where the subsets are present within the hippocampus, and whether there is any interesting distribution of cell states (e.g. some are present closer to stem cells?). Since TGFb is thought to be crucial to microglia biology, it would be valuable to include more analysis of the mice with microglia-specific Tgfb deletion e.g. what was the efficiency of recombination in microglia? Did their numbers change after induction of Tgfb deletion in Cx3cr1-creERT2::Tgfb-flox mice.

      Overall:

      In general, I think the authors did a good job following the initial observations and devised clever ways to test the emerging hypotheses. The resulting data are an important addition to what we know about microglial aging and can be fruitfully used by other researchers, e.g. those working on microglia in a disease context.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      Yang, Hu et al. examined the molecular mechanisms underlying astrocyte activation and its implications for multiple sclerosis. This study shows that the glycolytic enzyme PKM2 relocates to astrocyte nuclei upon activation in EAE mice. Inhibiting PKM2's nuclear import reduces astrocyte activation, as evidenced by decreased proliferation, glycolysis, and inflammatory cytokine release. Crucially, the study identifies TRIM21 as pivotal in regulating PKM2 nuclear import via ubiquitination. TRIM21 interacts with PKM2, promoting its nuclear translocation and enhancing its activity, affecting multiple signaling pathways. Confirmatory analyses using single-cell RNA sequencing and immunofluorescence demonstrate TRIM21 upregulation in EAE astrocytes. Modulating TRIM21 expression in primary astrocytes impacts PKM2-dependent glycolysis and proliferation. In vivo experiments targeting this mechanism effectively mitigate disease severity, CNS inflammation, and demyelination in EAE.

      The authors supported their claims with various experimental approaches, however, some results should be supported with higher-quality images clearly depicting the conclusions and additional quantitative analyses of Western blots.

      Strength:

      This study presents a comprehensive investigation into the function and molecular mechanism of metabolic reprogramming in the activation of astrocytes, a critical aspect of various neurological diseases, especially multiple sclerosis. The study uses the EAE mouse model, which closely resembles MS. This makes the results relevant and potentially translational. The research clarifies how TRIM21 regulates the nuclear import of PKM2 through ubiquitination by integrating advanced techniques. Targeting this axis may have therapeutic benefits since lentiviral vector-mediated knockdown of TRIM21 in vivo significantly reduces disease severity, CNS inflammation, and demyelination in EAE animals.

      Weaknesses:

      The authors reported that PKM2 levels are elevated in the nucleus of astrocytes at different EAE phases compared to cytoplasmic localization. However, Figure 1 also shows elevated cytoplasmic expression of PKM2. The authors should clarify the nuclear localization of PKM2 by providing zoomed-in images. An explanation for the increased cytoplasmic PKM2 expression should provided. Similarly, while PKM2 translocation is inhibited by DASA-58, in addition to its nuclear localization, a decrease in the cytoplasmic localization of PKM2 is also observed. This situation brings to mind the possibility of a degradation mechanism being involved when its nuclear translocation of PKM2 is inhibited.

      In Figure 3D, the authors claim that PKM2 expression causes nuclear retention of STAT3, p65, and p50, and inhibiting PKM2 localization with DASA-58 suppresses this retention. The western blot results for the MOG-stimulated group show high levels of STAT3, p50, and p65 in nuclear localization. However, in the MOG and DASA-58 treated group, one would expect high levels of p50, p65, and STAT3 proteins in the cytoplasm, while their levels decrease in the nucleus. These western blot results could be expanded. Additionally, intensity quantification for these results would be beneficial to see the statistical difference in their expressions, especially to observe the nuclear localization of PKM2.

      The discrepancy between Figure 7A and its explaining text is confusing. The expectation from the knocking down of TRIM21 is the amelioration of activated astrocytes, leading to a decrease in inflammation and the disease state. The presented results support these expectations, while the images showing demyelination in EAE animals are not highly supportive. Clearly labeling demyelinated areas would enhance readers' understanding of the important impact of TRIM21 knockdown on reducing the disease severity.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      This study significantly advances our understanding of the metabolic reprogramming underlying astrocyte activation in neurological diseases such as multiple sclerosis. By employing an experimental autoimmune encephalomyelitis (EAE) mouse model, the authors discovered a notable nuclear translocation of PKM2, a key enzyme in glycolysis, within astrocytes.

      Preventing this nuclear import via DASA 58 substantially attenuated primary astrocyte activation, characterized by reduced proliferation, glycolysis, and inflammatory cytokine secretion.<br /> Moreover, the authors uncovered a novel regulatory mechanism involving the ubiquitin ligase TRIM21, which mediates PKM2 nuclear import. TRIM21 interaction with PKM2 facilitated its nuclear translocation, enhancing its activity in phosphorylating STAT3, NFκB, and c-myc. Single-cell RNA sequencing and immunofluorescence staining further supported the upregulation of TRIM21 expression in astrocytes during EAE.

      Manipulating this pathway, either through TRIM21 overexpression in primary astrocytes or knockdown of TRIM21 in vivo, had profound effects on disease severity, CNS inflammation, and demyelination in EAE mice. This comprehensive study provides invaluable insights into the pathological role of nuclear PKM2 and the ubiquitination-mediated regulatory mechanism driving astrocyte activation.

      The author's use of diverse techniques, including single-cell RNA sequencing, immunofluorescence staining, and lentiviral vector knockdown, underscores the robustness of their findings and interpretations. Ultimately, targeting this PKM2-TRIM21 axis emerges as a promising therapeutic strategy for neurological diseases involving astrocyte dysfunction.

      While the strengths of this piece of work are undeniable, some concerns could be addressed to refine its impact and clarity further; as outlined in the recommendations for the authors.

    3. Reviewer #4 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The authors report the role of the Pyruvate Kinase M2 (PKM2) enzyme nuclear translocation as fundamental in the activation of astrocytes in a model of autoimmune encephalitis (EAE). They show that astrocytes, activated through culturing in EAE splenocytes medium, increase their nuclear PKM2 with consequent activation of NFkB and STAT3 pathways. Prevention of PKM2 nuclear translocation decreases astrocyte counteracts this activation. The authors found that the E3 ubiquitin ligase TRIM21 interacts with PKM2 and promotes its nuclear translocation. In vivo, either silencing of TRIM21 or inhibition of PKM2 nuclear translocation ameliorates the severity of the disease in the EAE model.

      Strengths:

      This work contributes to the knowledge of the complex action of the PKM2 enzyme in the context of an autoimmune-neurological disease, highlighting its nuclear role and a novel partner, TRIM21, and thus adding a novel rationale for therapeutic targeting.

      Weaknesses:

      Despite the relevance of the work and its goals, some of the conclusions drawn would require more thorough proof:

      I believe that the major weakness is the fact that TRIM21 is known to have per se many roles in autoimmune and immune pathways and some of the effects observed might be due to a PKM2-independent action. Some of the experiments to link the two proteins, besides their interaction, do not completely clarify the issue. On top of that, the in vivo experiments address the role of TRIM21 and the nuclear localisation of PKM2 independently, thus leaving the matter unsolved.

      Some experimental settings are not described to a level that is necessary to fully understand the data, especially for a non-expert audience: e.g. the EAE model and MOG treatment; action and reference of the different nuclear import inhibitors; use of splenocyte culture medium and the possible effect of non-EAE splenocytes.

      The statement that PKM2 is a substrate of TRIM21 ubiquitin ligase activity is an overinterpretation. There is no evidence that this interaction results in ubiquitin modification of PKM2; the ubiquitination experiment is minimal and is not performed in conditions that would allow us to see ubiquitination of PKM2 (e.g. denaturing conditions, reciprocal pull-down, catalytically inactive TRIM21, etc.).

    1. eLife assessment

      This valuable study showing that sleep deprivation increases functional synapses while depleting silent synapses supports previous findings that excitatory signaling, in particular via AMPA receptors, increases during wakefulness. The consistency with the literature increases confidence in the conclusions, which otherwise are supported by incomplete evidence. An interesting aspect of this manuscript is the inclusion of a model for the accumulation of sleep need that is based upon the MEF2C transcription factor but also links to the sleep-regulating SIK3-HDAC4/5 pathway. As such, the manuscript is as much of a perspective as a primary research paper.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript by Vogt et al examines how the synaptic composition of AMPA and NMDA receptors changes over sleep and wake states. The authors perform whole-cell patch clamp recordings to quantify changes in silent synapse numbers across conditions of spontaneous sleep, sleep deprivation, and recovery sleep after deprivation. They also perform single nucleus RNAseq to identify transcriptional changes related to AMPA/NMDA receptor composition following spontaneous sleep and sleep deprivation. The findings of this study are consistent with a decrease in silent synapse number during wakefulness and an increase during sleep. However, these changes cannot be conclusively linked to sleep/wake states. Measurements were performed in the motor cortex, and sleep deprivation was achieved by forced locomotion, raising the possibility that recent levels of neuronal activity/induction of plasticity, rather than sleep/wake states, are responsible for the observed results.

      Strengths:

      This study examines an important question. Glutamatergic synaptic transmission has been a focus of studies in the sleep field, but AMPA receptor function has been the primary target of these studies. Silent synapses, which contain NMDA receptors but lack AMPA receptors, have important functional consequences for the brain. Exploring the role of sleep in regulating silent synapse numbers is important to understanding the role of sleep in brain function. The electrophysiological approach of measuring the failure rate ratio, supported by AMPA/NMDA ratio measurements, is a rigorous tool to evaluate silent synapse numbers.

      The authors also perform snRNAseq to identify genes differentially expressed in the spontaneous sleep and sleep deprivation groups. This analysis reveals an intriguing pattern of upregulated genes controlled by HDAC4 and Mef2c, along with synaptic shaping component genes and genes associated with autism spectrum disorder, across cell types in the sleep deprivation group. This unbiased approach identifies candidate genes for follow-up studies.

      Weaknesses:

      A major weakness of this study is the experimental design. Measurements are made from the motor cortex, and sleep deprivation was achieved using forced locomotion on a treadmill. Therefore, the effects observed could be due to recent high levels of activity or plasticity induction in the motor cortex from locomotion, rather than lack of sleep per se. In support of this interpretation, other groups have failed to find a difference in AMPA/NMDA ratio in mice with different spontaneous sleep/wake histories, although sleep deprivation was not performed (Bridi et al., Neuron 2020).

      The electrophysiological measurements are problematic in several ways. First, the methods lack crucial details such as inclusion/exclusion criteria for each cell based on input and series resistance, stability of input/series resistance, polysynaptic responses, etc. that make it difficult to interpret the data. The holding potential (-90mV) used for AMPA receptor current recordings is much more hyperpolarized than typically used for these measurements. The statistical analysis of these experiments is also problematic. The number of mice used is low (3/group) and more should be added to account for inter-animal variability. Comparing the raw data with the statistical tests in supplementary table 1 (FR ratio), it appears that a data point has been dropped from the analysis, but it is unclear why. In addition, a false discovery rate (FDR) correction for multiple comparisons is used to evaluate group differences following the ANOVAs. Correcting for the FDR is less stringent and is typically used when a large number of hypotheses are tested and false positives are more acceptable. In this analysis, few comparisons are made, and the standard approach of correcting for the family-wise error rate is more appropriate.

      The snRNAseq data are intriguing, but a more thorough discussion of the candidate genes and pathways that are upregulated during sleep deprivation is warranted. Several genes relevant to the AMPA/NMDA ratio are mentioned, but upregulation of most of these genes would not be expected to increase the AMPA/NMDA ratio based on the literature cited. The model presented in Figure 4C is not consistent with the data (e.g. many candidate genes could alter NMDAR function without receptor insertion/removal), and it is unclear how the current study fits into the model presented in 4D.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      Here Vogt et al., provide new insights into the need for sleep and the molecular and physiological response to sleep loss. The authors expand on their previously published work (Bjorness et al., 2020) and draw from recent advances in the field to propose a neuron-centric molecular model for the accumulation and resolution of sleep need and the basis of restorative sleep function. While speculative, the proposed model successfully links important observations in the field and provides a framework to stimulate further research and advances on the molecular basis of sleep function. In my review, I highlight the important advances of this current work, and the clear merits of the proposed model, and indicate areas of the model that can serve to stimulate further investigation.

      Strengths:

      Reviewer comment on new data in Vogt et al., 2024<br /> Using classic slice electrophysiology, the authors conclude that wakefulness (sleep deprivation (SD)) drives a potentiation of excitatory glutamate synapses, mediated in large part by "un-silencing" of NMDAR-active synapses to AMPAR-active synapses. Using a modern single nuclear RNAseq approach the authors conclude that SD drives changes in gene expression primarily occurring in glutamatergic neurons. The two experiments combined highlight the accumulation and resolution of sleep need centered on the strength of excitatory synapses onto excitatory neurons. This view is entirely consistent with a large body of extant and emerging literature and provides important direction for future research.

      Consistent with prior work, wakefulness/SD drives an LTP-type potentiation of excitatory synaptic strength on principle cortical neurons. It has been proposed that LTP associated with wake, leads to the accumulation of sleep need by increasing neuronal excitability, and by the "saturation" of LTP capacity. This saturation subsequently impairs the capacity for further ongoing learning. This new data provides a satisfying mechanism of this saturation phenomenon by introducing the concept of silent synapses. The new data show that in mice well rested, a substantial number of synapses are "silent", containing an NMDAR component but not AMPARs. Silent synapses provide a type of reservoir for learning in that activity can drive the un-silencing, increasing the number of functional synapses. SD depletes this reservoir of silent synapses to essentially zero, explaining how SD can exhaust learning capacity. Recovery sleep led to restoration of silent synapses, explaining how recovery sleep can renew learning capacity. In their prior work (Bjorness et al., 2020) this group showed that SD drives an increase in mEPSC frequency onto these same cortical neurons, but without a clear change in pre-synaptic release probability, implying a change in the number of functional synapses. This prediction is now born out in this new dataset.

      The new snRNAseq dataset indicates the sleep need is primarily seen (at the transcriptional level) in excitatory neurons, consistent with a number of other studies. First, this conclusion is corroborated by an independent, contemporary snRNAseq analysis recently available as a pre-print (Ford et al., 2023 BioRxiv https://doi.org/10.1101/2023.11.28.569011). A recently published analysis on the effects of SD in drosophila imaged synapses in every brain region in a cell-type dependent manner (Weiss et al., PNAS 2024), concluding that SD drives brain wide increases in synaptic strength almost exclusively in excitatory neurons. Further, Kim et al., Nature 2022, heavily cited in this work, show that the newly described SIK3-HDAC4/5 pathway promotes sleep depth via excitatory neurons and not inhibitory neurons.

      The new experiments provided in Fig1-3 are expertly conducted and presented. This reviewer has no comments of concern regarding the execution and conclusions of these experiments.

      Reviewer comment on the model in Vogt et al., 2024

      In the view of this reviewer the new model proposed by Vogt et al., is an important contribution. The model is not definitively supported by new data, and in this regard should be viewed as a perspective, providing mechanistic links between recent molecular advances, while still leaving areas that need to be addressed in future work. New snRNAseq analysis indicates that SD drives the expression of synaptic shaping components (SSCs) consistent with the excitatory synapse as a major target for the restorative basis of sleep function. SD-induced gene expression is also enriched for autism spectrum disorder (ASD) risk genes. As pointed out by the authors, sleep problems are commonly reported in ASD, but the emphasis has been on sleep amount. This new analysis highlights the need to understand the impact on sleep's functional output (synapses) to fully understand the role of sleep problems in ASD.

      Importantly, SD-induced gene expression in excitatory neurons overlaps with genes regulated by the transcription factor MEF2C and HDAC4/5 (Figure 4). In their prior work, the authors show loss of MEF2C in excitatory neurons abolished the SD transcriptional response and the functional recovery of synapses from SD by recovery sleep. Recent advances identified HDAC4/5 as major regulators of sleep depth and duration (in excitatory neurons) downstream of the recently identified sleep-promoting kinase SIK3. In Zhou et al., and Kim et al., Nature 2022, both groups propose a model whereby "sleep-need" signals from the synapse activate SIK3, which phosphorylates HDAC4/5, driving cytoplasmic targeting, allowing for the de-repression and transcriptional activation of "sleep genes". Prior work shows that HDAC4/5 are repressors of MEF2C. Therefore, the "sleep genes" derepressed by HDAC4/5 may be the same genes activated in response to SD by MEF2C. The new model thereby extends the signaling of sleep need at synapses (through SIK3-HDAC4/5) to the functional output of synaptic recovery by expression of synaptic/sleep genes by MEF2C. The model thereby links aspects of the expression of sleep need with the resolution of sleep need by mediating sleep function: synapse renormalization.

      Weaknesses:

      Areas for further investigation

      In the discussion section Vogt et al., explore the links between excitatory synapse strength, arguably the major target of "sleep function", and NREM slow-wave activity (SWA), the most established marker of sleep need. SIK3-HDAC4/5 have major effects on the "depth" of sleep by regulating NREM-SWA. The effects of MEF2C loss of function on NREM SWA activity are less obvious, but clearly impact the recovery of glutamatergic synapses from SD. The authors point out how adenosine signaling is well established as a mediator of SWA, but the links between adenosine and glutamatergic strength are far from clear. The mechanistic links between SIK3/HDAC4/5, adenosine signaling, and MEF2C, are far from understood. Therefore, the molecular/mechanistic links between a synaptic basis of sleep need and resolution with NREM-SWA activity require further investigation.

      Additional work is also needed to understand the mechanistic links between SIK3-HDAC4/5 signaling and MEF2C activity. The authors point out that constitutively nuclear (cn) HDAC4/5 (acting as a repressor) will mimic MEF2C loss of function. This is reasonable, however, there are notable differences in the reported phenotypes of each. Notably, cnHDAC4/5 suppresses NREM amount and NREM SWA but had no effect on the NREM-SWA increase following SD (Zhou et al., Nature 2022). Loss of MEF2C in CaMKII neurons had no effect on NREM amount and suppressed the increase in NREM-SWA following SD (Bjorness et al., 2020). These instances indicate that cnHDAC4/5 and loss of MEF2C do not exactly match suggesting additional factors are relevant in these phenotypes. Likely HDAC4/5 have functionally important interactions with other transcription factors, and likewise for MEF2C, suggesting areas for future analysis.

      One emerging theme may be that the SIK3-HDAC4/5 axis is a major regulator of the sleep state, perhaps stabilizing the NREM state once the transition from wakefulness occurs. MEF2C is less involved in regulating sleep per se, and more involved in executing sleep function, by promoting restorative synaptic modifications to resolve sleep need.

      Finally, advances in the roles of the respective SIK3-HDAC4/5 and MEF2C pathways point towards transcription of "sleep genes", as clearly indicated in the model of Figure 4. Clearly, more work is needed to understand how the expression of such genes ultimately leads to the resolution of sleep need by functional changes at synapses. What are these sleep genes and how do they mechanistically resolve sleep need? Thus, the current work provides a mechanistic framework to stimulate further advances in understanding the molecular basis for sleep need and the restorative basis of sleep function.

    1. eLife assessment

      The important study by Ding and colleagues identifies subpopulations of neurons recorded in the monkey subthalamic nucleus (STN) with distinct activity profiles and causal contributions during perceptual decision-making. The combination of neuronal recording, microstimulation, and computational methods provides convincing evidence for a heterogenous neural population that could support multifaceted roles in decision formation. This study should be of wide interest to computational and experimental neuroscientists interested in cognitive function.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      The study reports that STN neurons recorded while monkeys performed a random-dot motion task show diverse activation timecourses relative to task events and dependencies on coherence, reaction time, and saccade-choice direction. Different neuron types could be grouped into functional subpopulations, e.g., coherence sensitivity emerging early only in choice-coding neurons. Clustering techniques identified three functionally defined neuron clusters whose dynamic activity profiles related to computational predictions of different decision models in the literature. Microstimulation at different STN recording sites affected behavioral performance in varying but well-conceptualized ways that were captured by the parameters of drift-diffusion models and related to the presence of STN functional clusters at recording sites. The authors conclude that their results validate key aspects of decision models and identify novel aspects of decision-related STN activity.

      This is an interesting and high-quality paper that will be of interest across computational and decision neuroscience fields. The recordings and data analyses seem carefully conducted. The study has an attractive theoretical starting point of three specific computational signals that are then mapped onto identified neuron clusters. The combination of single-cell recordings, microstimulation, and computational modelling is a distinct strength of the paper. I only have a few questions and suggestions for clarification.

      (1) It would be helpful to explain the criteria for choosing a given number of clusters and for accepting the final clustering solution more clearly. The quantitative results (silhouette plots, Rand index) in Supplementary Figure 2 should perhaps be included in the main figure to justify the parameter choices and acceptance of specific clustering solutions.

      (2) It would be helpful to show how the activity profiles in Figure 3 would look like for 3 or 5 (or 6) clusters, to give the reader an impression of how activity profiles recovered using different numbers of clusters would differ.

      (3) The authors attempt to link the microstimulation effects to the presence of functional neuron clusters at the stimulation site. How can you rule out that there were other, session-specific factors (e.g., related to the animal's motivation) that affected both neuronal activity and behavior? For example, could you incorporate aspects of the monkey's baseline performance (mean reaction time, fixation breaks, error trials) into the analysis?

      (4) Line 84: What was the rationale for not including both coherence and reaction time in one multiple regression model?

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      This study uses single-unit recordings in the monkey STN to examine the evidence for three theoretical models that propose distinct roles for the STN in perceptual decision-making. Importantly, the proposed functional roles are predictive of unique patterns of neural activity. Using k-means clustering with seeds informed by each model's predictions, the current study identified three neural clusters with activity dynamics that resembled those predicted by the described theoretical models. The authors are thorough and transparent in reporting the analyses used to validate the clustering procedure and the stability of the clustering results. To further establish a causal role for the STN in decision-making, the researchers applied microstimulation to the STN and found effects on response times, choice preferences, and latent decision parameters estimated with a drift diffusion model. Overall, the study provides strong evidence for a functionally diverse population of STN neurons that could indeed support multiple roles involved in perceptual decision-making. The manuscript would benefit from stronger evidence linking each neural cluster to specific decision roles in order to strengthen the overall conclusions.

      The interpretation of the results, and specifically, the degree to which the identified clusters support each model, is largely dependent on whether the artificial vectors used as model-based clustering seeds adequately capture the expected behavior under each theoretical model. The manuscript would benefit from providing further justification for the specific model predictions summarized in Figure 1B. Further, although each cluster's activity can be described in the context of the discussed models, these same neural dynamics could also reflect other processes not specific to the models. That is, while a model attributing the STN's role to assessing evidence accumulation may predict a ramping up of neural activity, activity ramping is not a selective correlate of evidence accumulation and could be indicative of a number of processes, e.g., uncertainty, the passage of time, etc. This lack of specificity makes it challenging to infer the functional relevance of cluster activity and should be acknowledged in the discussion.

      Additionally, although the effects of STN microstimulation on behavior provide important causal evidence linking the STN to decision processes, the stimulation results are highly variable and difficult to interpret. The authors provide a reasonable explanation for the variability, showing that neurons from unique clusters are anatomically intermingled such that stimulation likely affects neurons across several clusters. It is worth noting, however, that a substantial body of literature suggests that neural populations in the STN are topographically organized in a manner that is crucial for its role in action selection, providing "channels" that guide action execution. The authors should comment on how the current results, indicative of little anatomical clustering amongst the functional clusters, relate to other reports showing topographical organization.

      Overall, the association between the identified clusters and the function ascribed to the STN by each of the models is largely descriptive and should be interpreted accordingly. For example, Figure 3 is referenced when describing which cluster activity is choice/coherence dependent, yet it is unclear what specific criteria and measures are being used to determine whether activity is choice/coherence "dependent." Visually, coherence activity seems to largely overlap in panel B (top row). Is there a statistically significant distinction between low and high coherence in this plot? The interpretation of these plots and the methods used to determine choice/coherence "dependence" needs further explanation.

      In general, the association between cluster activity and each model could be more directly tested. At least two of the models assume coordination with other brain regions. Does the current dataset include recordings from any of these regions (e.g., mPFC or GPe) that could be used to bolster claims about the functional relevance of specific subpopulations? For example, one would expect coordinated activity between neural activity in mPFC and Cluster 2 according to the Ratcliff and Frank model. Additionally, the reported drift-diffusion model (DDM) results are difficult to interpret as microstimulation appears to have broad and varied effects across almost all the DDM model parameters. The DDM framework could, however, be used to more specifically test the relationships between each neural cluster and specific decision functions described in each model. Several studies have successfully shown that neural activity tracks specific latent decision parameters estimated by the DDM by including neural activity as a predictor in the model. Using this approach, the current study could examine whether each cluster's activity is predictive of specific decision parameters (e.g., evidence accumulation, decision thresholds, etc.). For example, according to the Ratcliff and Frank model, activity in cluster 2 might track decision thresholds.

    4. Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The authors provide compelling evidence for the causal role of the subthalamic nucleus (STN) in perceptual decision-making. By recording from a large number of STN neurons and using microstimulation, they demonstrate the STN's involvement in setting decision bounds, scaling evidence accumulation, and modulating non-decision time.

      Strengths:

      The study tested three hypotheses about the STN's function and identified distinct STN subpopulations whose activity patterns support predictions from previous computational models. The experiments are well-designed, the analyses are rigorous, and the results significantly advance our understanding of the STN's multi-faceted role in decision formation.

      Weaknesses:

      While the study provides valuable insights into the STN's role in decision-making, there are a few areas that could be improved. First, the interpretation of the neural subpopulations' activity patterns in relation to the computational models should be clarified, as the observed patterns may not directly correspond to the specific signals predicted by the models. Second, the authors could consider using a supervised learning method to more explicitly model the pattern correlations between the three profiles. Third, a neural population model could be employed to better understand how the STN population jointly contributes to decision-making dynamics. Finally, the added value of the microstimulation experiments should be more directly addressed in the Results section, as the changes in firing patterns compared to the original patterns are not clearly evident.

    1. eLife assessment

      This important study uses calcium imaging to show an increase in the selectivity of the sensory-evoked response in the apical dendritic tuft of layer 5 barrel cortex neurons as mice learn a whisker-dependent discrimination task. The evidence supporting the conclusions is compelling, and this work will be of great interest to neuroscientists working on reward-based learning and sensory processing.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      What neurophysiological changes support the learning of new sensorimotor transformations is a key question in neuroscience. Many studies have attempted to answer this question at the neuronal population level - with varying degrees of success - but few, if any, have studied the change in activity of the apical dendrites of layer 5 cortical neurons. Neurons in layer 5 of the sensory cortex appear to play a key role in sensorimotor transformations, showing important decision and reward-related signals, and being the main source of cortical and subcortical projections from the cortex. In particular, pyramidal track (PT) neurons project directly to subcortical regions related to motor activity, such as the striatum and brainstem, and could initiate rapid motor action in response to given sensory inputs. Additionally, layer 5 cortical neurons have large apical dendrites that extend to layer 1 where different neuromodulatory and long-range inputs converge, providing motor and contextual information that could be used to modulate layer 5 neurons output and/or to establish the synaptic plasticity required for learning a new association.

      In this study, the authors aimed to test whether the learning of a new sensorimotor transformation could be supported by a change in the evoked response of the apical dendrites of layer 5 neurons in the mouse whisker primary somatosensory cortex. To do this, they performed longitudinal functional calcium imaging of the apical dendrites of layer 5 neurons while mice learned to discriminate between two multi-whisker stimuli. The authors used a simple conditioning task in which one whisker stimulus (upward or backward air puff, CS+) is associated with a reward after a short delay, while the other whisker stimulus (CS-) is not. They found that task learning (measured by the probability of anticipatory licking just after the CS+) was not associated with a significant change in the average population response evoked by the CS+ or the CS-, nor a change in the average population selectivity. However, when considering individual dendritic tufts, they found interesting changes in selectivity, with approximately equal numbers of dendrites becoming more selective for CS+ and dendrites becoming more selective for CS-.

      One of the major challenges when assessing changes in neural representation during the learning of such Go/NoGo tasks is that the movements and rewards themselves may elicit strong neural responses that may be a confounding factor, that is, inexperienced mice do not lick in response to the CS+, while trained mice do. In this study, the authors addressed this issue in three ways: first, they carefully monitored the orofacial movements of mice and showed that task learning is not associated with changes in evoked whisker movements. Second, they show that whisking or licking evokes very little activity in the dendritic tufts compared to whisker stimuli (CS+ and CS-). Finally, the authors introduced into the design of their task a post-conditioning session after the last conditioning session during which the CS+ and the CS- are presented but no reward is delivered. During this post-session, the mice gradually stopped licking in response to the CS+. A better design might have been to perform the pre-conditioning and post-conditioning sessions in non-water-restricted, unmotivated mice to completely exclude any lick response, but the fact that the change in selectivity persists after the mice stopped licking in the last blocks of the post-conditioning session (in mice relying only on their whiskers to perform the task) is convincing.

      The clever task design and careful data analysis provide compelling evidence that learning this whisker discrimination task does not result in a massive change in sensory representation in the apical dendritic tufts of layer 5 neurons in the primary somatosensory cortex on average. Nevertheless, individual dendritic tufts do increase their selectivity for one or the other sensory stimulus, likely enhancing the ability of S1 neurons to accurately discriminate the two stimuli and trigger the appropriate motor response (to lick or not to lick).

      One limitation of the present study is the lack of evidence for the necessity of the primary somatosensory cortex in the learning and execution of the task. As the authors have strongly emphasized in their previous publications, the primary somatosensory cortex may not be necessary for the learning and execution of simple whisker detection tasks, especially when the stimulus is very salient. Although this new task requires the discrimination between two whisker stimuli, the simplicity and salience of the whisker stimuli used could make this task cortex-independent. Especially when considering that some mice seem to not rely entirely on their whiskers to execute the task.

      Nevertheless, this is an important result that shows for the first time changes in the selectivity to sensory stimuli at the level of individual apical dendritic tufts in correlation with the learning of a discrimination task. This study sheds new light on the cortical cellular substrates of reward-based learning and opens interesting perspectives for future research in this area. In future studies, it will be important to determine whether the change in selectivity of dendritic calcium spikes is causally involved in the learning of the task or whether it simply correlates with learning, as a consequence of changes in synaptic inputs caused by reward. The dendritic calcium spikes may be involved in the establishment of synaptic plasticity required for learning and impact the output of layer 5 pyramidal neurons to trigger the appropriate motor response. It would be important also to study the changes in selectivity in the apical dendrite of the identified projection neurons.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The authors did not find an increased representation of CS+ throughout reinforcement learning in the tuft dendrites of Rbp4-positive neurons from layer 5B of the barrel cortex, as previously reported for soma from layer 2/3 of the visual cortex.

      Alternatively, the authors observed an increased selectivity to both stimuli (CS+ and CS-) during reinforcement learning. This feature:

      (1) was not present in repeated exposures (without reinforcement),<br /> (2) was not explained by the animal's behaviour (choice, licking, and whisking), and<br /> (3) was long-lasting, being present even when the mice disengaged from the task.

      Importantly, increased selectivity was correlated with learning (% correct choices), and neural discriminability between stimuli increased with learning.

      In conclusion, the authors show that tuft dendrites from layer 5B of the barrel cortex increase the representation of conditioned (CS+) and unconditioned stimuli (CS-) applied to the whiskers, during reinforcement learning.

      Strengths:

      The results presented are very consistent throughout the entire study, and therefore very convincing:

      (1) The results observed are very similar using two different imaging techniques (2-photon -planar imaging- and SCAPE-volumetric imaging). Figure 3 and Figure 4 respectively.

      (2) The results are similar using "different groups" of tuft dendrites for the analysis (e.g. initially unresponsive and responsive pre- and post-learning). Figure 5.

      (3) The results are similar from a specific set of trials (with the same sensory input, but different choices). Figure 7.

      (4) Additionally, the selectivity of tuft dendrites from layer 5B of the barrel cortex was higher in the mice that exclusively used the whisker to respond to the stimuli (CS+ and CS-).<br /> The results presented are controlled against a group of mice that received the same stimuli presentation, except for the reinforcement (reward).

      Additionally, the behaviour outputs, such as choice, whisking, and licking could not account for the results observed.

      Although there are no causal experiments, the correlation between selectivity and learning (percentage of correct choices), as well as the increased neural discriminability with learning, but not in repeated exposure, are very convincing.

      Weaknesses:

      The biggest weakness is the absence of causality experiments. Although inhibiting specifically tuft dendritic activity in layer 1 from layer 5 pyramidal neurons is very challenging, tuft dendritic activity in layer 1 could be silenced through optogenetic experiments as in Abs et al. 2018. By manipulating NDNF-positive neurons the authors could specifically modify tuft dendritic activity in the barrel cortex during CS presentations, and test if silencing tuft dendritic activity in layer 1 would lead to the lack of selectivity and an impairment of reinforcement learning. Additionally, this experiment will test if the selectivity observed during reinforcement learning is due to changes in the local network, namely changes in local synaptic connectivity, or solely due to changes in the long-range inputs.

    1. eLife assessment:

      This study reported that cold exposure induced mRNA expression of genes related to lipid metabolism in the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus (PVH). The authors provide useful data highlighting the potential role of lipid metabolism in the brain during cold exposure. However, the study is incomplete and would require specific experiments to solidify the claims being made.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      This study focuses on metabolic changes in the paraventricular hypothalamic (PVH) region of the brain during acute periods of cold exposure. The authors point out that in comparison to the extensive literature on the effects of cold exposure in peripheral tissues, we know relatively little about its effects on the brain. They specifically focus on the hypothalamus, and identify the PVH as having changes in Atgl and Hsl gene expression changes during cold exposure. They then go on to show accumulation of lipid droplets, increased Fos expression, and increased lipid peroxidation during cold exposure. Further, they show that neuronal activation is required for the formation of lipid droplets and lipid peroxidation.

      Strengths:

      A strength of the study is trying to better understand how metabolism in the brain is a dynamic process, much like how it has been viewed in other organs. The authors also use a creative approach to measuring in vivo lipid peroxidation via delivery of a BD-C11 sensor through a cannula to the region in conjunction with fiber photometry to measure fluorescence changes deep in the brain.

      Weaknesses:

      Although the topic and findings are of interest, there are a few key weaknesses in the study that would improve the work if addressed. One weakness was many of the experiments were done in a manner that could not distinguish between the contributions of neurons and glial cells, limiting the extent of conclusions that could be made. While this is not easily doable for all experiments, it can be done for some. For example, the Fos experiments in Figure 3 would be more conclusive if done with the labeling of neuronal nuclei with NeuN, as glial cells can also express Fos. To similarly show more conclusively that neurons are being activated during cold exposure, the calcium imaging experiments in Figure S3 can be done with cold exposure. Additionally, many experiments are only done with the minimal three animals required for statistics and could be more robust with additional animals included. Another weakness is that the authors do not address whether manipulating lipid droplet accumulation or lipid peroxidation has any effect on PVH function (e.g. does it change neuronal activity in the region?).

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      Cold-induced lipid metabolism is well-established in adipose tissues. The authors set out to determine whether cold could alter brain lipid metabolism. By QPCR analysis of brain punches after acute cold, they found that mRNA expressions of several lipolysis-related genes were upregulated compared to RT controls. By combining fluorescent sensors and in vivo fiberphotometry, they observed cold-induced lipid peroxidation/lipolysis, which could be blocked by pharmacological inhibitors of neuronal activity (muscimol and kynurenic acid). The brain is not traditionally considered an organ with high lipid metabolism (vs carbohydrate); therefore, the observation and hypothesis proposed by the authors are unexpected and can be interesting. However, the experiments and data were rather preliminary and superficial and did not support the authors' conclusions. In addition, the main hypothesis, in relationship to the role of cold/temperature, remains incoherent and needs a major update.

      Strengths:

      A set of relatively novel and interesting observations.

      Creative use of several in vivo sensors and techniques.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The physiological relevance of lipolysis and thermogenesis genes in the PVH. The authors need to provide quantitative and substantial characterizations of lipid metabolism in the brain beyond a panel of qPCRs, especially considering these genes are likely expressed at very low levels. mRNA and protein level quantification of genes in Fig 1, in direct comparison to BAT/iWAT, should be provided. Besides bulk mRNA/protein, IHC/ISH-based characterization should be added to confirm to cellular expression of these genes.

      (2) The fiberphotometry work they cited (Chen 2022, Andersen 2023, Sun 2018) used well-established, genetically encoded neuropeptide sensors (e.g., GRABs). The authors need to first quantitatively demonstrate that adapting BD-C11 and EnzCheck for in vivo brain FP could effectively and accurately report peroxidation and lipolysis. For example, the sensitivity, dynamic range, and off-time should all be calibrated with mass spectrometry measurements before any conclusions can be made based on plots in Figures 4, 5, and 6. This is particularly important because the main hypothesis heavily relies on this unvalidated technique.

      (3) Generally, the histology data need significant improvement. It was not convincing, for example, in Figure 3, how the Fos+ neurons can be quantified based on the poor IF images where most red signals were not in the neurons.

      (4) The hypothesis regarding the direct role of brain temperature in cold-induced lipid metabolism is puzzling. From the introduction and discussion, the authors seem to suggest that there are direct brain temperature changes in responses to cold, which could be quite striking. However, this was not supported by any data or experiments. The authors should consolidate their ideas and update a coherent hypothesis based on the actual data presented in the manuscript.

    1. eLife assessment

      This is an important study to reveal local circuit mechanisms in the POA that control body temperature and also highlight how neurotransmitter GABA and neuropeptide NTS from the same neurons differentially modulate temperature. This study was carefully executed, providing convincing evidence for the conclusions in this paper. The findings have emphasized the importance of considering multiple diverse functions of the same neuron populations and will be of interest to neuroscientists working on central regulations of energy metabolism and temperature homeostasis.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Little is known about the local circuit mechanisms in the preoptic area (POA) that regulate body temperature. This carefully executed study investigates the role of GABAergic interneurons in the POA that express neurotensin (NTS). The principal finding is that GABA-release from these cells inhibits neighboring neurons, including warm-activated PACAP neurons, thereby promoting hyperthermia, whereas NTS released from these cells has the opposite effect, causing a delayed activation and hypothermia. This is shown through an elegant series of experiments that include slice recordings alongside matched in vivo functional manipulations. The roles of the two neurotransmitters are distinguished using a cell-type-specific knockout of Vgat as well as pharmacology to block GABA and NTS receptors. Overall, this is an excellent study that is noteworthy for revealing local circuit mechanisms in the POA that control body temperature and also for highlighting how amino acid neurotransmitters and neuropeptides released from the same cell can have opposing physiologic effects. I have only minor suggestions for revision.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The study has demonstrated how two neurotransmitters and neuromodulators from the same neurons can be regulated and utilized in thermoregulation.

      The study utilized electrophysiological methods to examine the characteristics and thermoregulation of Neurotensin (Nts)-expressing neurons in the medial preoptic area (MPO). It was discovered that GABA and Nts may be co-released by neurons in MPO when communicating with their target neurons.

      Strengths:

      The study has leveraged optogenetic, chemogenetic, knockout, and pharmacological inhibitors to investigate the release process of Nts and GABA in controlling body temperature.

      The findings are relevant to those interested in the various functions of specific neuron populations and their distinct regulatory mechanisms on neurotransmitter/neuromodulator activities

      Weaknesses:

      Key points for consideration include:

      (1) The co-release of GABA and Nts is primarily inferred rather than directly proven. Providing more direct evidence for the release of GABA and the co-release of GABA and Nts would strengthen the argument. Further in vitro analysis could strengthen the conclusion regarding this co-releasing process.

      (2) The differences between optogenetic and chemogenetic methods were not thoroughly investigated. A comparison of in vitro results and direct observation of release patterns could clarify the mechanisms of GABA release alone or in conjunction with Nts under different stimulation techniques.

      (3) Neuronal transcripts were mainly identified through PCR, and alternative methods like single-cell sequencing could be explored.

      (4) In Figure 6, the impact of GABA released from Nts neurons in MPO on CBT regulation appears to vary with ambient temperatures, requiring a more detailed explanation for better comprehension.

      (5) The model should emphasize the key findings of the study.

    4. Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      Understanding the central neural circuits regulating body temperature is critical for improving health outcomes in many disease conditions and in combating heat stress in an ever-warming environment. The authors present important and detailed new data that characterizes a specific population of POA neurons with a relationship to thermoregulation. The new insights provided in this manuscript are exactly what is needed to assemble a neural network model of the central thermoregulatory circuitry that will contribute significantly to our understanding of regulating the critical homeostatic variable of body temperature. These experiments were conducted with the expertise of an investigator with career-long experience in intracellular recordings from POA neurons. They were interpreted conservatively in the appropriate context of current literature.

      The Introduction begins with "Homeotherms, including mammals, maintain core body temperature (CBT) within a narrow range", but this ignores the frequent hypothermic episodes of torpor that mice undergo triggered by cold exposure. Although the author does mention torpor briefly in the Discussion, since these experiments were carried out exclusively in mice, greater consideration (albeit speculative) of the potential for a role of MPO Nts neurons in torpor initiation or recovery is warranted. This is especially the case since some 'torpor neurons' have been characterized as PACAP-expressing and a population of PACAP neurons represent the target of MPO Nts neurons.

    1. eLife assessment

      This technical study presents a novel sampling strategy for detecting synaptic coupling between neurons from dual pipette patch-clamp recordings in acute slices of mammalian brain tissue in vitro. The authors present solid evidence that this strategy, which incorporates automated patch clamp electrode positioning and cleaning for reuse with strategic neuron targeting, has the potential to substantially improve the efficiency of neuronal sampling with paired recordings. This technique and the extensions discussed will be useful for neuroscientists wanting to apply or already conducting automated multi-pipette patch clamp recording electrophysiology experiments in vitro for neuron connectivity analyses.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      In this technical paper, the authors introduce a useful variation on the fully automated multi-electrode patch-clamp recording technique for probing synaptic connections that they term "patch-walking". The patch-walking approach involves coordinated pipette route-planning and automated pipette cleaning procedures for pipette reuse to improve recording throughput efficiency, which the authors argue can theoretically yield almost twice the number of connections to be probed by paired recordings on a multi-patch electrophysiology setup for a given number of cells compared to conventional manual patch-clamping approaches used in brain slices in vitro. The authors show solid results from recordings in mouse in vitro cortical slices, demonstrating the efficient recording of dozens of paired neurons with a two-patch pipette configuration for paired recordings and detection of synaptic connections. This approach will be of interest and valuable to neuroscientists conducting automated multi-patch in vitro electrophysiology experiments and seeking to increase the efficiency of neuron connectivity detection while avoiding the more complex recording configurations (e.g., 8-10 pipette multi-patch recording configurations) used by several laboratories that are not readily implementable by most of the neuroscience community.

      Strengths:

      (1) The authors introduce the theory and methods and show experimental results for a fully automated electrophysiology dual patch-clamp recording approach, which uses coordinated patch-clamp pipette route-planning and automated pipette cleaning procedures to "patch-walk" across an in vitro brain slice.

      (2) The patch-walking approach improves throughput efficiency over manual patch clamp recording approaches, especially for investigators looking to utilize paired patch electrode recordings in electrophysiology experiments in vitro.

      (3) Experimental results are presented from in vitro mouse cortical slices demonstrating the efficiency of recording dozens of paired neurons with a two-patch pipette configuration for paired recordings and detecting synaptic connections, demonstrating the feasibility and efficiency of the patch-walking approach.

      (4) The authors suggest extensions of their technique while keeping the number of recording pipettes employed and recording rig complexity low, which are important practical technical considerations for investigators wanting to avoid the more complex recording configurations (e.g., 8-10 pipette multi-patch recording configurations) used by several laboratories that are not readily implementable by most of the neuroscience community.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      In this study, the authors aim to combine automated whole-cell patch clamp recording simultaneously from multiple cells. Using a 2-electrode approach, they are able to sample as many cells (and connections) from one slice, as would be achieved with a more technically demanding and materially expensive 8-electrode patch clamp system. They provide data to show that this approach is able to successfully record from 52% of attempted cells, which was able to detect 3 pairs in 71 screened neurons. The authors state that this is a step forward in our ability to record from randomly connected ensembles of neurons.

      Strengths:

      The conceptual approach of recording multiple partner cells from another in a stepwise manner indeed increases the number of tested connections. An approach that is widely applicable to both automated and manual approaches. Such a method could be adopted for many connectivity studies using dual recording electrodes.

      The implementation of automated robotic whole-cell patch-clamp techniques from multiple cells simultaneously is a useful addition to the multiple techniques available to ex vivo slice electrophysiologists.

      The approach using 2 electrodes, which are washed between cells is economically favourable, as this reduces equipment costs for recording multiple cells, and limits the wastage of capillary glass that would otherwise be used once.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The premise of this article is based upon the fact that even a "skilled" whole-cell electrophysiologist is only capable of recording ~10 cells per day are flawed. Many studies have shown that capable electrophysiologists can record upwards of 50 cells a day, given adequate slice quality and reliable recording conditions with multiple electrodes (e.g. Pastoll et al., 2020 eLife, Booker et al., 2014, JoVE, Peng et al., 2017); often with over 80% success rates for recording. It is not convincing that this approach is a dramatic improvement on such approaches - except when a less skilled researcher is beginning recordings.

      Importantly, could the patch walk protocol not be alternatively implemented using manual recording approaches? Yes, the use of a semi-automated robotic system aids recording from many cells by a less experienced colleague, but the inferences about the number of connections tested are common to the approach, not the technique used. This seems like a crucial conceptual point to include.

      (2) A key omission of this study is the absence of brain area, cell type, and layer recorded from. It is mentioned in Figure 2 that this is the somatosensory and visual cortices. Which were these, and how were they confirmed?

      (3) A comparison of measurements shown in Figure 2 to other methods - e.g. conventional dual patch, 8-electrode patch, single electrode. How do the values obtained for cell quality measurements compare to those expected for the cell population recorded (which is unclear - see point 2)?

      (4) What is the reliability of performing outside-out patch configuration to obtain sealed and biocytin-filled cells under these conditions? A key tenet of performing high-throughput paired recordings is the ability to identify the cell types involved in the local microcircuit, and if their axon has been preserved in the slice configuration (which varies between cell types). Not having confirmation of morphological identity and integrity likely leads to a dramatic underestimation of connection probability, given that main axon collaterals could be severed during acute brain slice preparation.

      (5) The quality control criteria used in this manuscript require further clarification. An upper limit of 50 MΩ access resistance is extremely high (i.e. 20-30 MΩ is a more typical and stringent cut-off), which is worsened as no real information is given to the degree of resistance change that could be accepted. This is simply listed as "If the seal quality decreased during recording, the cell is excluded from analysis". Indeed, the range of access resistances plotted in Figure 2 is from 10-100 MΩ, which implies that some neurons included in this data did not meet recording criteria. Also, it is widely accepted in the field that a 10-20% change in access during recording is acceptable - within a more defined range. I would consider re-assessing the recorded cells to only include cells with access resistances <30MΩ and those that did not fluctuate by more than 20%.

      Appraisal of aims:

      The authors certainly established a system that is useful for interrogating synaptic connectivity in an automated manner. However, it remains unclear how widely used this would be in the field, and whether this truly represents an advancement from manual recordings or >4 electrode recordings.

      Discussion of impact:

      This approach, particularly the conceptual approach to paired testing, is of use to the field. However, in practice, many researchers using conventional dual-electrode paired recording likely implement similar approaches - especially when targeting specific cell types (see Booker et al., 2014 JoVE, Qi et al., 2020 Front Synaptic Neurosci.). This may pave the way for greater implementation of dual and multi-electrode recordings using robotic patch-clamp techniques.

    4. Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, Yip and colleagues incorporated the pipette cleaning technique into their existing dual-patch robotic system, "the PatcherBot", to allow sequential patching of more cells for synaptic connection detection in living brain slices. During dual-patching, instead of retracting all two electrodes after each recording attempt, the system cleaned just one of the electrodes and reused it to obtain another recording while maintaining the other. With one new patch clamp recording attempt, new connections can be probed. By placing one pipette in front of the other in this way, one can "walk" across the tissue, termed "patch-walking." This application could allow for probing additional neurons to test the connectivity using the same pipette in the same preparation.

      Strengths:

      Compared to regular dual-patch recordings, this new approach could allow for probing more possible connections in brain slices with dual-patch recordings, thus having the potential to improve the efficiency of identifying synaptic connections

      Weaknesses:

      While this new approach offers the potential to increase efficiency, it has several limitations that could curtail its widespread use.

      Loss of Morphological Information: Unlike traditional multi-patch recording, this approach likely loses all detailed morphology of each recorded neuron. This loss is significant because morphology can be crucial for cell type verification and understanding connectivity patterns by morphological cell type.

      Spatial Restrictions: The robotic system appears primarily suited to probing connections between neurons with greater spatial separation (~100µm ISD). This means it may not reliably detect connections between neurons in close proximity, a potential drawback given that the connectivity is much higher between spatially close neurons. This limitation could help explain the low connectivity rate (5%) reported in the study.

      Limited Applicability: While the approach might be valuable in specific research contexts, its overall applicability seems limited. It's important to consider scenarios where the trade-off between efficiency and specific questions that are asked.

      Scalability Challenges: Scaling this method beyond a two-pipette setup may be difficult. Additional pipettes would introduce significant technical and logistical complexities.

    1. eLife assessment

      In this work, the authors put forward a valuable methodological advancement for imaging deeper in the intact spinal cord of anaesthetized mice. The authors measured blood flow across different vessel types within the spinal cord and observed the cellular responses following venule occlusion. The demonstration is solid, although, a more quantitative comparison with state-of-the-art two-photon excited fluorescence microscopy and a discussion about applicability to functional imaging (e.g., calcium imaging) would have strengthened the study.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Cheng, Yu-Ting, et al. demonstrate the capabilities of three-photon excited fluorescence (3PEF) microscopy for in vivo imaging of the mouse spinal cord. It enables imaging up to ~550 µm in depth, overcoming the limitations of two-photon excited fluorescence (2PEF) microscopy. The authors used 3PEF to visualize and quantify blood flow across different vessel types within the spinal cord and observed the cellular responses following venule occlusion. They showed depth-dependent structural changes in neurites and the behavior of microglia with a high contrast. The findings show that 3PEF can provide high-resolution, multicolor imaging of dynamic cellular interactions and vascular architecture, helping studies of spinal cord physiology and pathology.

      The experiments are well done and supported by data but some points need to be clarified:

      (1) For the two vs three-photon comparison, the authors should provide more information about how they performed the 2PEF: power and pulse duration. This comparison is primarily focused on imaging depth and signal-to-background ratio (SBR), but imaging speed should also be discussed.

      (2) A comparison with state-of-the-art 2PEF would have been more convincing. For instance, the use of adaptive optics, or red-shifted fluorophores allowing better 2PEF SBR, or deeper imaging.

      (3) The study focuses on structural imaging and does not provide extensive data on real-time dynamic processes, which could be crucial for understanding rapid cellular responses in the spinal cord.<br /> By addressing these weaknesses, future studies could enhance the applicability and reliability of 3PEF microscopy for spinal cord research.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      In this work, the authors attempt to advance our capacity to image the intact spinal cord in living mice, with the ultimate goal of allowing optical access to all spinal layers, from the dorsal (sensory-related) to the ventral (motor-related) laminae. They demonstrate the potency of 3-photon excited fluorescence imaging (3PEF) to collect fluorescent signals in anesthetized adult mice to depths of up to 450 µm from the dorsal surface.

      Strengths:

      • 3PEF is convincingly demonstrated as a significant improvement over previously used 2-photon imaging.

      • The images show very good spatial resolution and stable signal-to-noise ratio up to 450 µm from the dorsal surface, providing unprecedented access to intermediate ventral laminae.

      Weaknesses:

      • The paper in its current form lacks a detailed description of the experimental apparatus used, including its invasiveness (removal of vertebrae and muscles) and its impact on animal behavior. One can hope that, in the future, a similar implantation chamber may be used for awake, freely-moving animals.

      • In general, non-optic specialists may find it difficult to appreciate some of the findings due to technical writing at times, and minimally described metrics.

      • The possibility that the 3-photon illumination may cause tissue damage, notably by heat induction, is not evaluated or discussed.

      • At this stage, no attempt has been made to image cellular activity. The reader should keep in mind that motor neurons, as well as most of their upstream circuits, are located between 500 and 900 µm from the dorsal surface. Hence, although the method is a significant advancement, it still does not allow for the evaluation of morphological (or possibly, activity) changes in the whole spinal cord, particularly excluding motor-related laminae."

    1. eLife assessment

      This valuable study describes an apparatus, workflow, and proof-of-concept data for a system to study social cooperation in marmosets, an increasingly popular primate model for neuroscience. The apparatus and methodology have clear and convincing advantages over conventional methods based on manual approaches. However, the claims of faster social learning or of finer-grained behavioural analysis in their setup require further corroboration.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript by Meissner and colleagues described a novel take on a classic social cognition paradigm developed for marmosets. The classic pull task is a powerful paradigm that has been used for many years across numerous species, but its analog approach has several key limitations. As such, it has not been feasible to adopt the task for neuroscience experiments. Here the authors capture the spirit of the classic task but provide several fundamental innovations that modernize the paradigm - technically and conceptually. By developing the paradigm for marmosets, the authors leverage the many advantages of this primate model for studies of social brain functions and their particular amenability to freely-moving naturalistic approaches.

      Strengths:

      The current manuscript describes one of the most exciting paradigms in primate social cognition to be developed in many years. By allowing for freely-moving marmosets to engage in high numbers of trials, while precisely quantifying their visual behavior (e.g. gaze) and recording neural activity this paradigm has the potential to usher in a new wave of research on the cognitive and neural mechanisms underlying primate social cognition and decision-making. This paradigm is an elegant illustration of how naturalistic questions can be adapted to more rigorous experimental paradigms. Overall, I thought the manuscript was well written and provided sufficient details for others to adopt this paradigm. I did have a handful of questions and requests about topics and information that could help to further accelerate its adoption across the field.

      Weaknesses:

      LN 107 - Otters have also been successful at the classic pull task (https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s10071-017-1126-2)

      LN 151 - Can you provide a more precise quantification of timing accuracy than the 'sub-second level'. This helps determine synchronization with other devices.

      Using this paradigm, the marmosets achieved more trials than in the conventional task (146 vs 10). While this is impressive, given that only ~50 are successful Mutual Cooperation trials it does present some challenges for potential neurophysiology experiments and particular cognitive questions. The marmosets are only performing the task for 20 minutes, presumably because they become sated and are no longer motivated. This seems a limitation of the task and is something worth discussing in the manuscript. Did the authors try other food rewards, reduce the amount of reward, food/water restrict the animals for more than the stated 1-3 hours? How might this paradigm be incorporated into in-cage approaches that have been successful in marmosets? Any details on this would help guide others seeking to extend the number of trials performed each day.

      Can you provide more details on the DLC/Anipose procedure? How were the cameras synchronized? What percentage of trials needed to be annotated before the model could be generalized? Did each monkey require its own model, or was a single one applied to all animals?

      Will the schematics and more instructions on building this system be made publicly available? A number of the components listed in Table 1 are custom-designed. Although it is stated that CAD files will be made available upon request, sharing a link to these files in an accessible folder would significantly add to the potential impact of this paradigm by making it easier for others to adopt.

      In the Discussion, it would be helpful to have some discussion of how this paradigm might be used more broadly. The classic pulling paradigm typically allows one to ask a specific question about social cognition, but this task has the potential to be more widely applied to other social decision-making questions. For example, how might this task be adopted to ask some of the game-theory-type approaches common in this literature? Given the authors' expertise in this area, this discussion could serve to provide a roadmap for the broader field to adopt.

      Although this paradigm was developed specifically for marmosets, it seems to me that it could readily be adopted in other species with some modifications. Could the authors speak to this and their thoughts on what may need to be changed to be used in other species? This is particularly important because one of the advantages of the classic paradigm is that it has been used in so many species, providing the opportunity to compare how different species approach the same challenge. For example, though both chimps and bonobos are successful, their differences are notably illuminating about the nuances of their respective social cognitive faculties.

    3. Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      This important work by Meisner et al., developed an automated apparatus (MarmoAPP) to collect a wide array of behavioral data (lever pulling, gaze direction, vocalizations) in marmoset monkeys, with the goal of modernizing collection of behavioral data to coincide with the investigation of neurological mechanisms governing behavioral decision making in an important primate neuroscience model. The authors show a variety of "proof-of-principle" concepts that this apparatus can collect a wide range of behavioral data, with higher behavioral resolution than traditional methods. For example, the authors highlight that typical behavioral experiments on primate cooperation provide around 10 trials per session, while using their approach the authors were able to collect over 100 trials per 20-minute session with the MarmoAAP.

      Overall the authors argue that this approach has a few notable advantages:<br /> (1) it enhances behavioral output which is important for measuring small or nuanced effects/changes in behavior;<br /> (2) allows for more advanced analyses given the higher number of trials per session;<br /> (3) significantly reduces the human labor of manually coding behavioral outcomes and experimenter interventions such as reloading apparatuses for food or position;<br /> (4) allows for more flexibility and experimental rigor in measuring behavior and neural activity simultaneously.

      Strengths:

      The paper is well-written and the MarmoAPP appears to be highly successful at integrating behavioral data across many important contexts (cooperation, gaze, vocalizations), with the ability to measure significantly many more behavioral contexts (many of which the authors make suggestions for).

      The authors provide substantive information about the design of the apparatus, how the apparatus can be obtained via a long list of information Apparatus parts and information, and provide data outcomes from a wide number of behavioral and neurological outcomes. The significance of the findings is important for the field of social neuroscience and the strength of evidence is solid in terms of the ability of the apparatus to perform as described, at least in marmoset monkeys. The advantage of collecting neural and freely-behaving behavioral data concurrently is a significant advantage.

      Weaknesses:

      While this paper has many significant strengths, there are a few notable weaknesses in that many of the advantages are not explicitly demonstrated within the evidence presented in the paper. There are data reported (as shown in Figures 2 and 3), but in many cases, it is unclear if the data is referenced in other published work, as the data analysis is not described and/or self-contained within the manuscript, which it should be for readers to understand the nature of the data shown in Figures 2 and 3.

      (1) There is no data in the paper or reference demonstrating training performance in the marmosets. For example, how many sessions are required to reach a pre-determined criterion of acceptable demonstration of task competence? The authors reference reliably performing the self-reward task, but this was not objectively stated in terms of what level of reliability was used. Moreover, in the Mutual Cooperation paradigm, while there is data reported on performance between self-reward vs mutual cooperation tasks, it is unclear how the authors measured individual understanding of mutual cooperation in this paradigm (cooperation performance in the mutual cooperation paradigm in the presence or absence of a partner; and how, if at all, this performance varied across social context). What positive or negative control is used to discern gained advantages between deliberate cooperation vs two individuals succeeding at self-reward simultaneously?

      (2) One of the notable strengths of this approach argued by the authors is the improved ability to utilize trials for data analysis, but this is not presented or supported in the manuscript. For example, the paper would be improved by explicitly showing a significant improvement in the analytical outcome associated with a comparison of cooperation performance in the context of ~150 trials using MarmoAAP vs 10-12 trials using conventional behavioral approaches beyond the general principle of sample size. The authors highlight the dissection of intricacies of behavioral dynamics, but more could be demonstrated to specifically show these intricacies compared to conventional approaches. Given the cost and expertise required to build and operate the MarmoAAP, it is critical to provide an important advantage gained on this front. The addition of data analysis and explicit description(s) of other analytical advantages would likely strengthen this paper and the advantages of MarmoAAP over other behavioral techniques.

    4. Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The authors set out to devise a system for the neural and behavioral study of socially cooperative behaviors in nonhuman primates (common marmosets). They describe instrumentation to allow for a "cooperative pulling" paradigm, the training process, and how both behavioral and neural data can be collected and analyzed. This is a valuable approach to an important topic, as the marmoset stands as a great platform to study primate social cognition. Given that the goals of such a methods paper are to (a) describe the approach and instrumentation, (b) show the feasibility of use, and (c) quantitatively compare to related approaches, the work is easily able to meet those criteria. My specific feedback on both strengths and weaknesses is therefore relatively limited in scope and depth.

      Strengths:

      The device is well-described, and the authors should be commended for their efforts in both designing this system but also in "writing it up" so that others can benefit from their R&D.

      The device appears to generate more repetitions of key behavior than other approaches used in prior work (with other species).

      The device allows for quantitative control and adjustment to control behavior.

      The approach also supports the integration of markerless behavioral analysis as well as neurophysiological data.

      Weaknesses:

      A few ambiguities in the descriptions are flagged below in the "Recommendations for authors".

      The system is well-suited to marmosets, but it is less clear whether it could be generalized for use in other species (in which similar behaviors have been studied with far less elegant approaches). If the system could impact work in other species, the scope of impact would be significantly increased, and would also allow for more direct cross-species comparisons. Regardless, the future work that this system will allow in the marmoset will itself be novel, unique, and likely to support major insights into primate social cognition.

    1. Arbitrary or unjustified disconnection or exclusion from water services or facilities. This is a violation of the obligation to _________ the right to water.

      Why is this considered an obligation to "respect"? Shouldn't it be fulfil?

    1. The Scholarship of Electronic Literature

      This article highlights how this literary form, which must be experienced digitally, is gaining academic recognition through publications, organizations, and conferences.

    1. The Role of the Electronic Literature Organization

      As a student, learning about electronic literature is eye-opening. It's fascinating to see how literary works can evolve in digital environments, using multimedia and interactivity to create new reading experiences. Understanding this innovative form of literature broadens our appreciation of how technology can shape storytelling and creativity.

    1. 第一次正经尝试做学习笔记是用在计算物理这门课的期末考

      车还是

    1. She had A heart—how shall I say?— too soon made glad, Too easily impressed; she liked whate’er She looked on, and her looks went everywhere. Sir, ’twas all one! My favour at her breast, The dropping of the daylight in the West, The bough of cherries some officious fool Broke in the orchard for her, the white mule She rode with round the terrace—all and each Would draw from her alike the approving speech, Or blush, at least. She thanked men—good! but thanked Somehow—I know not how—as if she ranked My gift of a nine-hundred-years-old name With anybody’s gift.

      I feel like this whole passage is an example of Situational Irony. It seems to me like the narrator of the poem is trying to point out all of the things that made his "Last Duchess" unfit to carry his "gift of a nine-hundred-years-old name", but by doing so, he actually reveals that he is a very unpleasant and harsh man who doesn't live up to the archetype of a refined aristocrat himself. I think this foreshadows Ferrara's true character for the rest of the poem.

    2. As if alive

      I really enjoy this callback from the beginning of the poem. In the context of the rest of this passage, especially the line about all the smiles stopping, I think the statement "As if alive" serves as an inference that he may have had her killed. I feel like this inference is supported by the rest of his woeful acts throughout the poem.

    3. Notice Neptune, though, Taming a sea-horse, thought a rarity, Which Claus of Innsbruck cast in bronze for me!

      I think Ferrara pointing out the statue as an object to be marveled is a metaphor for how he also sees women. It seems to me like he values looking at the painting of his last wife more than he enjoyed her as a person. Now he uses the painting to impress guests, in the same way that he uses the statue of Neptune Taming a Sea Horse to impress guests. I think this final passage wraps up the true theme of the poem, and cements Ferrara's character as a bonafide asshole.

    4. my last Duchess

      The word last here has a double meaning that could be "final" or "most recent. This trend continues throughout the poem as the speaker describes the woman in an endearing yet critical way.

    1. Der größte Fehler der Linken war, dass sie die Kapitalismuskritik aufgegeben und den Faschisten überlassen haben.
    1. RRID:AB_162543

      DOI: 10.1016/j.celrep.2021.108899

      Resource: (Molecular Probes Cat# A-31572, RRID:AB_162543)

      Curator: @Naa003

      SciCrunch record: RRID:AB_162543


      What is this?

    2. RRID:AB_162543

      DOI: 10.1016/j.celrep.2021.108899

      Resource: (Molecular Probes Cat# A-31572, RRID:AB_162543)

      Curator: @Naa003

      SciCrunch record: RRID:AB_162543


      What is this?

    1. The picture that this chart presents to the viewer is indeed revealing, but it remains only a "single view."

      I'm not sure that I totally understood the goal of this section / how it relates to Playfair actually, but I think the idea we're landing on here is that we needed multiple views into the data to really understand what's happening?

      If so, I wonder if it might make more sense to show multiple versions of the chart simultaneously as a small multiples instead of having the interactive buttons, which still only show you a "single view" at a time?

    2. affirms the value of visualization when a particular dataset is aligned with an appropriate visual form.

      Having a hard time tracking the argument here. What am I supposed to be seeing that is demonstrating the value of visualization?

    3. Here we see the introduction of the web development team in 2017, and with it, the rise in contributions to the project's codebase.

      Is iCal supposed to disappear here?

    4. Perhaps this is due to the design of the charts themselves: how the bold data lines, enhanced by the hand-tinting that shades the areas between them, and set against the stark black gridlines, emblematize the graphical authority that visualization can command.

      Wait, is this really why we think contemporary people don't understand Playfair's chart as making a colonial argument? It feels unexpected to make a really nuanced point about the social situatedness of these artifacts and then attribute it all to design decisions. Doesn't a contemporary inability to read Playfair's charts a certain way do something to make us critically examine our standpoint, by revealing something about the present's social and ideological landscape and how we construct histories? Maybe the design is part of that, but perhaps there is more to say?

    5. What is surprising is that we, in the present, have not yet come to see these design choices as theargument of Playfair's charts.

      This is super interesting and feels important. I'm reading this and wishing for help here on how to unpack the implications of this. I kind of wish that this main point was signposted in a thesis statement rather than at the end of a paragraph, and expanded on further.

      When we talk about Clarkson in Ch 1, we maybe say that abstraction was central to his argument because it helps immediately convey the violence of slavery. Here, we talk about Playfair as intentionally using (even more) abstraction to obscure that violence.

      To what extent is the use of abstraction central to making his colonial argument? Also, what's the difference between intentionally obscuring violence through the design of a chart, and not being able to capture the full nuance of a situation as a limitation of a chart? One feels like a malicious act of design and the other feels like an unintended consequence of using data as a medium. These questions feel kind of confusing and I wish there was a bit more clarity here on how we're supposed to interpret what Playfair is doing.

    6. From these lines, it would seem that Playfair believes that his "simple and complete" images can not only capture a clear "picture of the past," but also retain their utility in a range of possible future scenarios

      I would prefer this to come before the quote so that when I read it, I understand how to interpret it in the context of your argument. Right now, I read the quote first but it doesn't feel self evident why it's been selected

    7. --

      replace with real em dash?

    1. Weiter unten wird von Usability gesprochen. Ich denke, die Aussage kann ein Fehlschluss sein. Viele Produkte sind sehr nützlich.

      Was kommerzielle Angebote anders machen, ist, dass sie den kognitiven Aufwand sie zu benutzen künstlich verringern. Vereinfachenden Oberflächen, verkindlichte Verfahrensweisen und wahrnehmungspsychologisch optimierte Anwendungen reduzieren oft die Möglichkeiten einer Anwendung auf sehr spezielle, teils triviale Fälle.

      Gleichzeitig sind diese Anwendungen und die in ihnen aufgehobenen Daten schlecht miteinander verbuunden und ermöglichen oft keinen Einblick darin, welche Daten verarbeitet werden. Das Verständnis einer "hapernden Usability" kann auch als ein Ausdruck einer konditionierten Erwartungshaltung betrachtet werden, die gezielt gesteuert und gefüttert wurde, um Menschen in Abhängigkeit zu den vermeintlich "einfacheren" System der Oligopole zu bringen.

    1. Outsourcing of the various components of the infrastructure of an organizationis only part of supply chain management. Successful management of the supplychain will rely on strategic partnerships and may also extend to joint-venturearrangements.

      what supply chain management actually is

    2. Supply chainmanagement and risk management are interrelated.

      when you outsource you now have to worry about both your risks and whoever else is involved in outsourcing

    3. thepotential downside of outsourcing needs to be identified and mitigated with thesame level of diligence as the upside or assumed benefit of outsourcing is

      essence of supply chain management

      risks of outsourcing should be considered just as much as the benefits of it

    4. There may be quality and availability issues that could causedissatisfaction amongst fans and result in reduced sales. There are also questionsof corporate social responsibility that need to be addressed.

      examples of risks related to the supply chain

    5. adequate attention to risk management matters can reduce thelikelihood and magnitude of any disruption to supply

      supply chain management reduces risk to supply chain

    6. definition of supply chain

      the process of that starts by sourcing raw materials and ends with the delivery of product to end users. The supply chain includes all aspects of this process such as vendors, manufactures, factories, warehouses, etc.

    7. Typically, internal controls have the following purposes:safeguard and protect the assets of the organization;ensure the keeping of accurate records;promote operational effectiveness and efficiency;adhere to policies and procedures, including control procedures;enhance reliability of internal and external reporting;ensure compliance with laws and regulations;safeguard the interests of shareholders/stakeholders.

      purposes of internal controls

      all of these help the organization achieve their objectives in some way or another

    8. how seriously aparticular department takes risk management and internal contro

      relation of internal controls to risk assurance and the audit committee

      the internal auditor should have the answer to this question

    9. The audit committee will seek assurance that all of the significant risksare being adequately managed and that all of the critical controls are effectiveand that they have been efficiently implemented

      risk assurance is the assurance that risks are managed and that controls and implementation thereof are effective

    10. The audit committee is concerned with internal control

      monitors the use and effectiveness of internal controls

    11. Although the audit committee may be considered to be the guardian ofcompliance within the organization, the terms of reference are usually muchbroader than just compliance

      audit committee functions: ensure compliance evaluate governance standards advocate for risk management

    12. udit committee consists of non-executive directors, with senior executive directors in attendance at auditcommittee meetings

      audit committee is meant to be objective facilitates the evaluation of activities in the organization and it's board

    13. Control environment

      the measure of an organization's risk culture a good control environment is indicative of future success with implementation of risk management and internal control activities

    14. enable directors to drive the organization forward

      internal controls overlap with governance

    15. When designing effective internal controls, the organization should look at thearrangements in place to achieve the following:maintenance of reliable systems;timely preparation of reliable information;safeguarding of assets;optimum use of resources;preventing and detecting fraud and error.

      Outcomes of effective (use of) internal control(s)

    16. internal control

      The system(s) in place that ensure a business meets its objectives. These include managerial actives that facilitate the direction of actions that increase the chance of objectives being achieved.

    17. The principles require the equitable treatment of all stakeholders and aninfluential role for stakeholders in corporate governance. Finally, the principlesrequire disclosure and transparency

      stakeholder treatment and influence, and transparency are essential parts of governance

    Annotators

    1. We must consider the perspectives of the people for whom these charts have been designed, the people who will benefit from looking, and the people who are merely looked upon

      We're now returning to audience a few paragraphs after the question of audience was introduced and then quickly pivoted away from. There have also been implicit connections to audience throughout the chapter -- both in discussions of Clarkson's audience, and even the idea that there is an ethics to how we as viewers choose to read depictions of violence (are we witnesses, spectators, voyeurs, etc). I would love to see these connections made more explicit.

      I agree with others that have said that this feels very important but I would love to see this more explicitly foregrounded in the structure and signposting of the chapter. A stylistic pattern I've noticed a lot here is introducing an important idea, then switching to talking about something totally different, before arriving back at that idea. As a reader, I think it's both making it hard to follow the argument and harder to explicitly draw those conceptual connections across all of the cases and examples.

    2. Indeed, there is a version of the history of data visualization that need not be significantly reconfigured in order to provide Clarkson with a more prominent place.

      If the goal here in these few paragraphs is ultimately to argue that Clarkson's use of water is continuous with mainstream histories of visualization, and that this is a reason he should not be overlooked, then it might make sense to open with that argument rather than pivoting to water first and burying the lede (thesis statement)

    3. Why would Clarkson use water—the very site of the dehumanization that enslavement brought about—as the anchoring metaphor of his account?

      I found it confusing that the paragraph opens with the question of audience, but instead of going on to fully address that question, concludes by introducing a different question seemingly unrelated to audience (why water) and then discussing that

    4. to see more

      Probably because I didn't understand the preceding sentence, but I'm confused by the move here. Clarkson would agree with the preceding paragraph but also because we know about Haraway, Browne, & co., we would see more that Clarkson would miss?

    5. Given Clarkson's own writing on the subject, he would likely not disagree. Clarkson celebrated how the chart "brought forth tears of sympathy in behalf of the sufferers, and it fixed their sufferings in [the viewer's] heart."

      Wait, I actually don't understand the point being made here. Clarkson would not disagree that "Description" is a view from nowhere, and that's supported by the fact that it made an emotional appeal? I think I need the connection drawn for me much more explicitly.

    6. Setting "Description of a Slave Ship" at the center of the story we tell

      The rest of this chapter starts getting very narratively complex very quickly. We introduce Clarkson as the main figure, but the immediately pivot to Elford, compare Elford's diagram to the French image, reintroduce Clarkson, and so on. It took me a few reads to keep track of who was who. I think it would help here to set up either 1) who the figures are, or 2) what the the artifacts are that are being compared/critically examined, and then talk about what we're going to be doing (compare, discuss, evaluate, etc). I think giving a very explicit road map at the level of an "in this essay I will" will be helpful here.

    7. Here is where we will return to the idea of the "god trick," mentioned earlier in this chapter

      I don't know if the back reference is necessary because the first reference was so tangential that I feel like a reader will see this and wonder if they missed something

    8. The diagram that Clarkson showed to Equiano depicted the configuration of captive bodies in the hold of a slave ship—a "scene of horror almost inconceivable," as Equiano, in his autobiography, described his own first view into the hold, and that his fellow Black British antislavery activist Quobna Ottobah Cugoano, in his own treatise, Thoughts and Sentiments on the Evil and Wicked Traffic of the Slavery and Commerce of the Human Species (1787), recalled as such a trauma that he chose not to describe it at all

      This sentence is long and complex -- consider breaking it up

    1. La historia de la escritura es un tema apasionante que abarca miles de años de evolución y desarrollo humano. Desde los primeros sistemas de escritura como el cuneiforme en Mesopotamia y los jeroglíficos egipcios, hasta la invención del alfabeto y la proliferación de diferentes sistemas de escritura en todo el mundo, cada etapa en esta historia nos revela cómo la humanidad ha buscado formas de registrar y transmitir información a lo largo del tiempo. La escritura ha sido fundamental para el progreso de la sociedad y el intercambio de conocimientos, y su evolución continúa hasta nuestros días con las nuevas tecnologías digitales.

    2. La historia de la escritura me parece fascinante. Importante la protoescritura ya que fue la primera etapa de la escritura. Comenzó en la Edad del Bronce con símbolos sencillos y se transformó en sistemas complejos como el cuneiforme en Sumeria y los jeroglíficos en Egipto. Es asombroso cómo distintas culturas, como los chinos y los mayas, desarrollaron la escritura de manera independiente. En la Edad del Hierro, surgieron los alfabetos semíticos, que influyeron en los sistemas griego y fenicio, y que son la base de muchos alfabetos modernos. Cada región creó sus propios sistemas, como el rongorongo en la Isla de Pascua y el silabario cheroqui. La escritura ha sido esencial para preservar la historia, las leyes y el conocimiento, permitiendo el desarrollo de sociedades complejas. Sin la escritura, gran parte de nuestro pasado se habría perdido.

    1. urgency of rebuilding “islands of trust” in communities before building out from there
    2. open world
    3. “It’s harder to protect your reputation for reliability than to damage it. It turns out that, as usual, offence is cheaper than defence,” he says. “Everybody who cares about the preservation of any institution has to stop everything, ring the alarm bell, and start thinking about how to preserve that ‘membrane’ in a way that is morally permissible.”
    4. People haven’t really come to grips with the fact that it’s not just personal privacy that matters, it’s also institutional privacy
    5. Delere Auctorem Rerum Ut Universum Infinitum Noscas. (Destroy the author of things to understand the infinite universe.)
    1. practical truth
    2. all control in human minds is via emotion
    3. You don’t need miracles. You just need to understand the world the way it really is, and it’s unbelievably wonderful. We’re so lucky to be alive!
    1. When connected via an applicable platform such as Thunderbolt 3 or later Windows 10 machines, you can expect maximum frequency rates up to 40 Gigabits per second (Gbps) when transferring data through aUSB4cable

      Good for nothing?

    1. Another pandemic is coming, this time attacking the fragile control systems in our brains—namely, our capacity to reason with one another—that we have used so effectively to keep ourselves relatively safe in recent centuries.
    2. strict liability laws, removing the need to prove either negligence or evil intent
    1. Además, en este trabajo se incluyeron 598 genotipos de un conjunto de datos disponible públicamente ( Colli et al., 2018 ).

      Genotipos incluidos. Base de datos publicos.

    1. Levin defines “intelligence” as the capacity to achieve the same goal via different means
    1. The appified society is wrong when apps become necessary infrastructure, since infrastructure should be controlled by the people democratically, not privately owned by corporations.
    1. Résumé de la vidéo [00:00:00][^1^][1] - [00:30:54][^2^][2] : Cette vidéo est un replay d'un webinaire sur la santé mentale des enfants, présentant le rapport du Haut Conseil de la Famille, de l'Enfance et de l'Âge (HCFEA). Elle aborde l'importance de la santé mentale chez les jeunes, les défis rencontrés par les professionnels et les familles, ainsi que les recommandations pour améliorer les soins et le soutien.

      Points forts : + [00:00:00][^3^][3] Introduction au webinaire * Accueil des participants et présentation du 40e webinaire * Importance de maintenir les liens dans la communauté professionnelle + [00:05:16][^4^][4] Présentation des intervenants * Introduction d'Agnès Florin et Sylviane Giampino * Discussion sur le rôle du HCFEA et ses publications + [00:08:51][^5^][5] Discussion sur la santé mentale des enfants * Examen des défis actuels dans le domaine de la santé mentale des jeunes * Importance d'une approche multidisciplinaire pour aborder ces questions + [00:14:01][^6^][6] Méthodologie et constats du rapport * Processus d'élaboration du rapport et les constats clés * Augmentation des troubles mentaux chez les enfants et manque de ressources + [00:20:02][^7^][7] Effet ciseaux et manque de soins * Disparité entre l'augmentation des besoins et le déficit de l'offre de soins * Impact sur les politiques publiques et recommandations pour l'avenir + [00:26:04][^8^][8] Prescriptions de psychotropes chez les enfants * Hausse de la consommation de psychotropes et réduction d'accès à d'autres aides * Nécessité d'équilibrer les types d'offres de soins pour les enfants

      Résumé de la vidéo [00:30:58][^1^][1] - [01:04:28][^2^][2]:

      Cette vidéo présente un webinaire sur le rapport du Haut Conseil de la famille, de l'enfance et de l'âge (HCFEA) concernant la santé mentale des enfants. Le rapport met en lumière les problèmes liés à la prescription de médicaments psychotropes chez les enfants et les adolescents, ainsi que l'importance d'une approche plus globale et interdisciplinaire de la santé mentale.

      Points forts: + [00:31:07][^3^][3] Prescription de médicaments psychotropes * Utilisation non étudiée chez l'enfant * Augmentation des prescriptions en urgence * 40% des prescriptions en médecine de ville + [00:32:53][^4^][4] Troubles du neurodéveloppement * Focus sur le TDAH et les états dépressifs et anxieux * Influence de l'âge et des facteurs sociaux sur le diagnostic * Plus grande prévalence chez les enfants défavorisés + [00:39:01][^5^][5] Approches psychothérapeutiques * Recommandation de diverses méthodes thérapeutiques * Importance de la pluralité des approches * Intégration de l'environnement éducatif et familial + [00:42:20][^6^][6] Pratiques éducatives et prévention * Pédagogies comme outils de prévention et de soins * Nécessité d'une école inclusive et d'environnements adaptés * Impact des facteurs sociaux et environnementaux sur la santé mentale

      Résumé de la vidéo [00:30:58][^1^][1] - [01:04:28][^2^][2]:

      Cette vidéo présente un webinaire sur le rapport du Haut Conseil de la famille, de l'enfance et de l'âge (HCFEA) concernant la santé mentale des enfants. Le rapport met en lumière les problèmes liés à la prescription de médicaments psychotropes chez les enfants et les adolescents, ainsi que les facteurs sociaux et environnementaux influençant leur santé mentale.

      Points forts: + [00:31:10][^3^][3] Prescription de psychotropes * 50 à 98% des psychotropes utilisés en pédiatrie non étudiés chez l'enfant * Forte augmentation des prescriptions en urgence * 40% des prescriptions faites en médecine de ville + [00:32:53][^4^][4] Troubles déficit de l'attention/hyperactivité (TDAH) * TDAH diagnostiqué plus fréquemment chez les enfants les plus jeunes de leur classe * Les enfants défavorisés sont plus diagnostiqués et médicamentés + [00:39:01][^5^][5] Approches psychothérapeutiques * Le conseil recommande une pluralité d'approches reconnues * Focus sur les thérapies dynamiques, cognitives et comportementales, et groupales/familiales + [00:42:36][^6^][6] Pratiques éducatives * Les pédagogies comme outils de prévention et de soins * Importance de l'inclusion et des environnements éducatifs adaptés + [00:45:54][^7^][7] Facteurs sociaux et environnementaux * Les politiques publiques sont efficaces sur les déterminants sociaux * Nécessité de renforcer les aides pour les enfants + [00:47:51][^8^][8] Recommandations de l'OMS * Changement des mentalités et élimination des pratiques coercitives * Approche globale de la santé mentale au-delà du modèle biomédical

    1. Résumé de la vidéo [00:00:00][^1^][1] - [00:27:12][^2^][2]:

      Cette vidéo est un replay d'un webinaire gratuit sur les troubles des apprentissages, présentant les actualités et l'évaluation avec la BMT-i. Les intervenants discutent des méthodes d'évaluation et de l'importance de l'identification précoce des troubles pour une intervention efficace.

      Points forts: + [00:00:31][^3^][3] Introduction au webinaire * Présentation du 41e webinaire de la saison 2023-2024 * Accueil des participants, notamment des orthophonistes + [00:02:00][^4^][4] Présentation des intervenants * Introduction de Catherine Billard et Monique Touzin, spécialistes des troubles des apprentissages * Discussion sur leur travail et contributions dans le domaine + [00:07:46][^5^][5] Étalonnage et ressources de la BMT-i * Explication de l'étalonnage de la BMT-i avec 1074 enfants * Présentation des modules d'apprentissage, de langage oral et de fonctions non verbales + [00:10:16][^6^][6] Évaluation de la lecture et de la transcription * Détails sur l'évaluation de la lecture et de la transcription chez les enfants * Importance de la compréhension et de la vitesse de lecture + [00:15:55][^7^][7] Cognition mathématique * Discussion sur l'évaluation des compétences mathématiques * Présentation des épreuves de calcul mental et de résolution de problèmes + [00:22:06][^8^][8] Langage oral * Importance du langage oral dans l'apprentissage * Présentation des épreuves de phonologie, d'expression syntaxique et de compréhension lexicale

      Résumé de la vidéo [00:27:15][^1^][1] - [00:54:02][^2^][2]:

      Cette partie de la vidéo aborde l'évaluation des troubles des apprentissages à l'aide de la BMT-i, un outil d'évaluation multidimensionnel. Il est discuté comment la BMT-i peut aider à identifier les difficultés en graphisme, écriture, visioconstruction, raisonnement, attention et fonctions exécutives chez les enfants. L'outil est conçu pour être utilisé par des professionnels de santé et pourrait potentiellement être intégré dans les écoles avec une formation appropriée.

      Points forts: + [00:27:15][^3^][3] Graphisme et écriture * Évaluation du graphisme par des copies de figures * Cotation subjective expliquée dans un livret spécial * Importance de l'indice calligraphique pour la disgraphie + [00:30:26][^4^][4] Épreuve spatiale * Deux versions d'une construction spatiale pour différents âges * Critère de précision pour l'ajustement entre les cubes * Discussion sur l'interprétation des résultats + [00:33:17][^5^][5] Raisonnement et attention * Épreuve de matrice pour le raisonnement * Épreuves informatisées pour l'attention et les fonctions exécutives * Importance de tester les troubles de l'attention associés à la cognition mathématique + [00:42:00][^6^][6] Intégration de la BMT-i dans les écoles * Formation nécessaire pour les enseignants spécialisés * Discussion sur l'organisation et l'accès à la BMT-i * L'importance de la coordination multidisciplinaire autour de l'enfant

      Résumé de la vidéo [00:54:04][^1^][1] - [01:08:48][^2^][2]:

      Cette partie du webinaire aborde les formations sur les troubles des apprentissages et l'évaluation avec la BMT-i. Les intervenants discutent de la nécessité d'un pool de formateurs, des avantages de la BMT-i par rapport à d'autres épreuves, et de l'importance de l'évaluation personnalisée pour chaque enfant.

      Points forts: + [00:54:04][^3^][3] Formation et évaluation * Nécessité de formations pour les professionnels sur les troubles des apprentissages * Discussion sur la création d'un pool de formateurs spécialisés * Présentation de la BMT-i comme outil d'évaluation + [00:57:17][^4^][4] Avantages de la BMT-i * Comparaison avec d'autres épreuves comme l'ANEPSIE * Pertinence des épreuves attentionnelles de la BMT-i * Coût et étalonnage en population française + [00:59:31][^5^][5] Préconisations et remédiations * Utilisation de la BMT-i pour le dépistage et l'évaluation * Importance de cibler les adaptations et remédiations appropriées * Discussion sur l'état actuel des interventions pédagogiques en France

    1. Résumé de la vidéo [00:00:00][^1^][1] - [00:25:11][^2^][2]:

      Cette vidéo explore l'art de l'éloquence, en se concentrant sur l'importance de développer des compétences psychosociales et émotionnelles chez les enfants et les adolescents. Elle souligne l'impact positif de ces compétences sur le bien-être et la réussite scolaire, et encourage l'intégration de l'éducation émotionnelle dans les pratiques pédagogiques.

      Points forts: + [00:00:00][^3^][3] Introduction à l'éloquence * Présentation du webinaire sur l'éloquence et l'art de persuader * Célébration du 50e webinaire avec une audience diverse + [00:03:00][^4^][4] Les compétences psychosociales * Importance des compétences émotionnelles pour les enseignants et les élèves * Impact sur le bien-être et la réussite scolaire + [00:13:43][^5^][5] Émotions et apprentissage * Les émotions sont indissociables des contextes d'apprentissage * Influence des émotions sur la motivation et la participation des élèves + [00:18:07][^6^][6] Développement des compétences émotionnelles * Identification, compréhension, utilisation et gestion des émotions * Traduction des compétences émotionnelles en objectifs pédagogiques

      Résumé de la vidéo [00:25:14][^1^][1] - [00:51:36][^2^][2] : La vidéo traite de l'éloquence et de l'art de persuader et de convaincre. Elle explore les compétences émotionnelles chez les enfants et les adolescents, l'importance de l'expression émotionnelle positive, et les stratégies pour développer la conscience émotionnelle.

      Points forts : + [00:25:14][^3^][3] L'expression émotionnelle positive * Importance de communiquer sans nuire aux autres * Utilisation de la parole, de la danse, du chant et de la peinture + [00:27:10][^4^][4] Développement des compétences émotionnelles * Utilisation de supports pour identifier et différencier les émotions * Importance de l'expérience subjective dans l'apprentissage émotionnel + [00:33:00][^5^][5] Formation des enseignants et professionnels * Discussion sur les outils disponibles pour les enseignants * Importance de la formation continue et de l'intégration des compétences émotionnelles + [00:38:01][^6^][6] Adaptation des supports éducatifs * Utilisation de la littérature jeunesse et des supports artistiques adaptés aux adolescents * L'importance de l'expérience culturelle et sensible pour l'expression personnelle

      Résumé de la vidéo [00:51:40][^1^][1] - [01:04:31][^2^][2] : La vidéo aborde l'art de l'éloquence, en se concentrant sur l'importance de la créativité et de l'individualité dans l'expression personnelle. Elle souligne l'utilisation de supports artistiques pour développer la sensibilité chez les enfants et les adolescents, et discute des défis liés à l'enseignement des compétences émotionnelles dans divers contextes éducatifs.

      Points forts : + [00:51:40][^3^][3] L'importance de la créativité * La créativité se manifeste dans les détails * Elle permet d'exprimer l'individualité * Les supports artistiques aident à développer la sensibilité + [00:54:16][^4^][4] Les compétences émotionnelles dans l'éducation * Les enseignants ne peuvent pas gérer les émotions de chaque élève à tout moment * Intégrer l'apprentissage émotionnel dans les objectifs pédagogiques * Accompagner les élèves dans leur développement émotionnel + [00:57:03][^5^][5] Les dimensions des émotions * Les émotions ont des dimensions physiologiques, subjectives et comportementales * Il est important de comprendre et d'exprimer les émotions * Pas de normes ou de règles strictes sur les émotions + [01:02:01][^6^][6] L'accompagnement émotionnel * Rejeter l'idée de rééducation basée sur des normes émotionnelles * Se concentrer sur l'identification et la compréhension des émotions * Utiliser des outils pour progresser dans la gestion des émotions

    1. Then we will need to teach students how to work with AI

      This I think is the endpoint for academic tech/ instructional designers, but how does this happen? Does "train the trainer" work better or integrating IDs/AT staff into the curriculum design process?

    2. Talking directly and candidly with her undergraduate students

      Love how talking to the students is included in this!

    1. "I believe the ELO's definition effectively captures the essence of Electronic Literature, or E-Lit. This concept is poised to expand and become the standard for future generations."

  2. drive.google.com drive.google.com
    1. Uma característica importante é que, em mercados onde nªo há nªo-consumidores, umasoluçªo híbrida é a única opçªo viável para uma nova tecnologia cujo desempenho é inferiorà tecnologia anterior, de acordo com a definiçªo original de desempenho. Isto significaque as inovações híbridas tendem a dominar em mercados de consumo pleno, em vez dasdisrupções puras

      A predominância de soluções híbridas nos mercados onde não há consumidores torna-se uma abordagem prática e estratégica. Em mercados de consumo absoluto, em que todos os potenciais consumidores já estão utilizam uma tecnologia existente, introduzir uma nova tecnologia que apresenta um desempenho inferior àquele estabelecido é, sem dúvida, um desafio.

      Nesse contexto, as soluções híbridas representam uma ponte crucial para a adoção de novas tecnologias.

      A integração de tecnologias distintas pode ser complexa e requer um grande investimento inicial em pesquisa e desenvolvimento. Além disso, é possível que a solução híbrida não seja tão eficiente quanto a tecnologia existente, o que pode causar uma percepção de valor limitada.

    1. We are therefore we interact. This is random.

      Canard et al. neutral food webs

    2. ou as a prey item fit in my gob
    3. Here it may be meaningful to contextualise the different ‘types’ of food webs within the larger research programmes (or even practical needs) that have been driving the construction of them.

      is this calling for more metadata

    4. generates a unique representation of the mechanisms

      I'd be a whole lot carefuler about the phrasing here -- a lot of models do not rely on mechanisms at all

    5. Putting the parts together; what does it mean?

      u good?

    6. the magnitude of the edges

      that would be quantitative v. binary, not binary v. probabilistic

    7. As highlighted in Poisot, Stouffer, and Kéfi

      all due respect to Daniel and Sonia, but I'm sure other people made this point before

    8. discussion

      not just a discussion - don't sell this paper short, this is a more systematic assessment of model families / questions

    9. predict

      see? predict! a perfectly cromulent word

    10. ‘code’/define

      I really don't like this == why not "predict"?

    11. their goal

      part of the confusion around networks, beside [REDACTED], is that we have different goals - I think splitting the big paragraph before into many paragraphlets will help you articulate this, and this last paragraph will flow better as a result

    12. This will allow us to ensure the right models are being used to answer the right questions, particularly within the context of trying to accelerate cross-cutting research in the face of global change.

      NO

      Or rather yes, but before writing this, you need to tell us (1) what the right questions are, (2) why we care about them re. climate change, and (3) how we know that there are wrong models.

      I think you can make strong cases for all of them by looking at gaps in the litterature. Why is there nothing about interactions in the KM global biodiversity framework? It's because the field has not been able to go beyond "some data, loosely connected to models" in the way e.g. SDM did.

    13. [do we need an e.g., ref??]

      Dansereau, Barros, et Poisot 2024 is ok I guess (but I haven't read it)

    14. Navigating food web prediction

      Connecting food webs to their models: what are we making the data say?