6,780 Matching Annotations
  1. Nov 2020
    1. Reviewer #2:

      This manuscript represents a very considerable amount of work, both wet lab and analytic, constituting excellent science. This may be the best paper yet produced on Bdelloids. Despite this glowing recommendation I have some very significant concerns about certain parts, their conclusions section, and the evidence for "enhanced cellular defence mechanisms" in the abstract. Some parts are very rigorous, but others give in to excess speculation. This paper does not really need additional work, it needs some re-writing. Afterwards this important manuscript would be a welcome addition to the field, even without the supposedly unique defence mechanisms.

      Substantive concerns:

      1) Line 273 onwards: There is a comparison in the manuscript between Bdelloids and Monogonants. It wasn't clear to me however that these groups had been sampled sufficiently. The Monogonants are represented by 5 species (8 genomes) within a single genus in no way representing the diversity of Monogonants and the sampling of Bdelloids is also small. The authors should take a more cautious tone to any conclusions.

      2) Line 276-278: The rationale for focussing on this specific group of TEs did not appear robust. The authors say "this class of TEs is thought to be least likely to undergo horizontal transfer and thus the most dependent on sex for transmission". But other groups are not evolving predominantly by horizontal transfer, transposons can change without meiotic sex and this section needs writing a little more clearly. The following lines make a case that some transposon groups increase, and some decrease in frequency. The obvious hypothesis is drift, but the writing was unclear, I always felt that some other mechanism was being proposed but never really stated clearly.

      3) Lines 288-300, comparison of TE abundances across animals; this section was very poorly done. I thought the authors could delete this comparison and have a better manuscript. How were these other species chosen? Is C. elegans a good representative of the entire phylum Nematoda? Are the tardigrades representatives of their phylum? Assembly and annotation methods vary enormously across datasets so what can the authors conclude without standardising assembly and annotation for these other animal groups? The authors say "as expected, both the abundance and diversity of TEs varied widely across taxa" This was indeed expected, Figure 2b seems to show noise, and suggests to me that the inclusion of this data was not a good idea. I suggest it is removed, or a very substantive analysis and discussion of the way in which it is an accurate and representative sample of animal transposon loads is written.

      4) Line 350-353: This section is weak and needs to be improved. The authors need to make it very clear that this is not a test, it is a single observation. The phrase "as predicted by theory for elements dependent on vertical transmission" seems rather unsupported. Does this relate to the argument put forward in lines 276-278? It was unconvincing at this point also. The current description that some families increase and some decrease is couched in what sounds like too meaningful sounding language, which could be improved to be more consistent with the results. Lines 353-355 here seem to make an argument that the variation of TEs in bdelloids is purely a phylogenetic effect variably present in some bdelloid lineages and related groups. If this is their view (and it seems very reasonable indeed) then the manuscript would be improved considerably if they stated it more clearly.

      5) Lines 533-535 "consistent with a high fit of the data to the phylogeny under a Brownian motion model as would be expected if TE load evolves neutrally along branches of the phylogeny." I felt that this was a truly excellent result that needed to be put forward more strongly in other areas of the manuscript. In this area, and some others in this manuscript the authors have truly unique data dramatically improving our understanding of bdelloids. The manuscript would be improved if authors concentrated much less throughout on ideas this data is exceptional and different from other animals, and instead followed their own analysis that this fits with current biological thought.

      6) Lines 621-632: "no significant difference between monogononts and bdelloids, or between desiccating and non-desiccating bdelloids" It is not clear to me here what statistical test is being carried out. All tests require phylogenetic control of course. I do agree that they are quite similar, perhaps this should be rewritten to reflect only that?

      7) 705-706 The authors look at 3 gene families concerned with transposon control to examine copy number. In one of them they say "the RdRP domain in particular is significantly expanded". I am unclear of what test of significance was carried out and where to find this analysis. Unlike the query concerning desiccating and non-desiccating above I think this analysis is essential. The authors make a really big thing about the expansion of this gene family, including it in the abstract. If they wish to keep its prominence then they need to clearly show whether there is evidence that the size of this domain family is significantly expanded along the branch leading to bdelloids. I understand that this is illustrated in Figure 7 but this is not a test. This needs to be made much clearer in a quantitative rather than descriptive way. There is a need for broad taxonomic sampling, standardisation of assembly and annotation, and a phylogenetic design for this analysis. Else it should be removed or at the least described more conservatively.

      8) Line 725: "Why do bdelloids possess such a marked expansion of gene silencing machinery?" There is no evidence presented that they do. There may be a hypothesis that they do it differently, rather than more, but that also needs testing. There is a lot of speculation in this paragraph, and I think removing this whole paragraph would improve the manuscript.

      9) If there is an expansion of this family what can we then conclude? The authors say in the abstract "bdelloids share a large and unusual expansion of genes involved in RNAi-mediated TE suppression. This suggests that enhanced cellular defence mechanisms might mitigate the deleterious effects of active TEs and compensate for the consequences of long-term asexuality" yet they also review that animal groups can utilize different gene families for transposon control. Is there evidence that clade 5 nematodes with PIWI have a quantitatively different transposon defence mechanism? No, they just use a different pathway to some other groups, and the default position surely has to be the same for bdelloids, there is no evidence presented that their defence is enhanced. I would strongly recommend that the authors reduce the strength of their claims about the significance of bdelloid transposon control gene families in this manuscript.

      10) I felt that the Conclusions (and Abstract) were too speculative and not fully supported by the existing data, though this can easily be addressed by a substantial re-write.

    1. Note: This rebuttal was posted by the corresponding author to Review Commons. Content has not been altered except for formatting.

      Learn more at Review Commons


      Reply to the reviewers

      Reviewer__ #1 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity (Required)):__ Septins are highly conserved small GTPase cytoskeletal proteins that function as molecular scaffolds for dynamic cell wall and plasma membrane-remodeling, as well as diffusion barriers restricting movement of membrane and cell wall-associated molecules. Recent work has started to unravel the functional connections between the septins, cell wall integrity MAPK pathway signaling, and lipid metabolism, however most studies have focused on a small sub-set of septin monomers and/or were conducted in primarily yeast-type fungi. Here the authors show in the filamentous fungus A. nidulans that the core hexamer septins are required for proper coordination of the cell wall integrity pathway, that all septins are involved in lipid metabolism. Especially sphingolipid, but not sterols and phosphoinositides, contributes to the localization and stability of core septins at the plasma membrane. The experiments are simple and clear, therefore the conclusion is convincing. Fig.8 model, I would like to see the situation of septin mutant.

      We thank the reviewer for the positive comments. In response to the request from this reviewer and a similar one from reviewer 2 for more on the effect of the loss of individual septins, we added text clarifying the roles of core hexamer, core octamer and noncore septins throughout the manuscript including in the legend to Fig 8 (li 439-444) and the discussion (li 388-402). Please see responses to reviewer 2 comments for more detail.

      Reviewer #1 (Significance (Required)):

      Since localization of cell wall synthesis proteins, lipid domains and septins are likely to depend on each other, sometimes difficult to evaluate the effect is direct or indirect. The comprehensive analyses like performed here are helpful to catch the overview in the field.

      Reviewer__ #2 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity (Required)):__ **Summary** The study by Mela and Momany describes the function of core septins of A. nidulans and links with the requirement of the cell wall integrity pathway and the sphingolipids which, are required for membrane and cell wall stability. The study is of interest for the fungal genetics community, and the authors have conducted a substantial amount of work in a field they have substantial experience. However, one of the main weaknesses of the manuscript is the assumption whether the CWI pathway controls de septin function of if the core septins control it.

      We agree that while our data clearly indicate interactions between the septins and the CWI pathway, which component controls the other is not clear. We have modified the text to address this concern in several places as detailed in responses to the reviewer’s specific comments below.

      **Major comments** In the abstract, the authors claim that double mutant analysis suggested core septins function downstream of the final kinase of the cell wall integrity pathway. However, from the experiments showed, it is difficult to be convinced about that. The authors should make efforts do make it clear in the manuscript and the discussion. For example: -Line 25-26 (abstract): "Double mutant analysis suggested core septins function downstream of the final kinase of the cell wall integrity pathway."

      We agree that while the double mutant analysis shows interaction of septins with the CWI pathway, the evidence for them being downstream is not strong. We have revised the abstract as follows:

      Li29-30: Double mutant analysis with Δ**mpkA suggested core septins interact with the cell wall integrity pathway.”

      -Line 181-182; 219-220 (results) "Double mutant analyses suggest core septins modulate the cell wall integrity pathway downstream of the kinase cascade." This conclusion is one of the most important of the manuscript. However, this reviewer argues that it cannot be convincingly addressed if at least the phosphorylation ok the MAP kinase MpkA in the septins background is not evaluated under conditions of cell stress and sphingolipid biosynthesis inhibition. The genetic analysis alone maybe not enough to infer if septins control the CWI or the other way around. There may have compensatory effects when the CWI pathway is impaired. For example, most of the septins and mpkA double mutants seems to suppress the defect of the delta mpkA under cell wall stress. The authors should consider this idea.

      Although we discuss the epistasis experiments as one possible interpretation, we agree the genetic analysis is not enough to definitively show that the septins are upstream of the CWI pathway or the other way around. The suppression of cell wall defects by deletion of septins in a mpkA null mutant background under cell wall stress suggests a bypass of the CWI pathway for remediation of the cell wall or some other alternate regulatory node. One possible interpretation of these data could be that by inactivation of normal CWI integrity function through deletion of the final kinase, in addition to deletion of septins (possibly acting as negative regulators of CWI components), there may be a parallel node by which cell wall remediation could still occur.

      Wording throughout the abstract, results, and discussion has been modified accordingly.

      Li 29-30: Double mutant analysis with Δ**mpkA suggested core septins interact with the cell wall integrity pathway.

      Li 208-209: Double mutant analyses suggest the core septin aspB cdc3 modulates the cell wall integrity pathway in the ∆mpkA background under cell wall stress.

      Li 221-225: When challenged with low concentrations of CASP and CFW, the ∆aspBcdc3**∆mpkAslt2 and ∆aspE ∆mpkA slt2 mutants were more sensitive than ∆aspBcdc3 and ∆aspE single mutants, but suppressed the colony growth defects of ∆mpkA slt2. The novel phenotype of the double mutants shows that septins are involved in cell wall integrity and raises the possibility that they act in a bypass or parallel node for remediation of cell wall defects (Fig 4).

      Li 227-228: Fig 4. Double mutant analyses suggest core septins modulate the cell wall integrity pathway.

      Li 464-468: Double mutant analyses between septins and CWI pathway kinases also support a role for core septins in maintaining cell wall integrity under stress (Fig 4). Suppression of cell wall defects under cell wall stress by deletion of septins in an ∆mpkA slt2 background suggests a parallel node by which septins negatively regulate cell wall integrity pathway sensors or kinases could exist.

      There is no clear evidences on the manuscript that the core septins AspA, AspB, AspC, and ApsD are epithastic in A. nidulans. Therefore, the authors choice of using different Asp deletion mutants as a proxy for all the septins mutants is questionable. For example, there is no mention of why AspB was chosen for Figure 2 (chitin and β-1,3-glucan deposition), and AspA was chosen for Figure 3 (chitin synthase localization) since these experiments are correlated. The same is true for Figure S1 where AspB and AspE were used. One can wonder if some of the core septins would have a major impact in the chitin content.

      We agree with the reviewer that not all four core septins are equivalent. Previously published work from our lab shows that AspACdc11, AspBCdc3, AspCCdc12, and AspDCdc10 form octamers and that AspACdc11, AspBCdc3, and AspCCdc12 form hexamers, that both of these heteropolymers co-exist, and that the noncore septin AspE is not part of either core heteropolymer, though it appears to influence them possibly through brief interactions (Lindsay et al., 2010; Hernandez-Rodriguez et al., 2012; Hernandez-Rodriguez et al., 2014). This previous work also clearly shows that strains in which the hexameric septins have been deleted (ΔaspA, ΔaspB, and ΔaspC) have very similar phenotypes while strains in which the octamer-exclusive septin has been deleted (ΔaspD) have different phenotypes.

      In our attempt to simplify the current manuscript we discussed the four core septins as a group. In retrospect this caused us to miss important distinctions on the roles of hexamer vs octamer septins and we are grateful to the reviewer for pointing this out. We have modified language throughout the revised manuscript to specify whether results and interpretations apply to core hexamer septins, core octamer septins, the noncore septin, or individual septins. This more detailed analysis has given us several new ideas to test in future work.

      While we cannot exclude the possibility that interesting results might be produced by analyzing null alleles of each individual septin gene for all experiments, we agree with the cross-reference by Reviewer #3 that there is a very low likelihood that we would see different results by analyzing all individual septins within each subgroup (hexamer, octamer or noncore).

      To the reviewer’s questions on choice of septins for Fig 2, Fig 3, and Fig S1:

      ΔaspA, ΔaspB, and ΔaspC showed similar sensitivity to cell wall-disturbing agents in the plate-based assays in Fig 1 and are all part of the core hexamer. We have modified text including the figure legends to make it clear which septins were used in the experiments and which group they belong to.

      In a related comment about Figure 3, the reallocation of chitin synthases in the absence of septins is very interesting, but consider that all the core septin genes should be tested. Without a fully functioning cell wall, the formation of septa will be impaired. It makes their results less surprising.

      In the case of Fig 3, we were unable to recover ChsB-GFP in the ΔaspB or ΔaspC backgrounds but were able to recover it in the ΔaspA background. We have clarified as follows:

      Li184-187: To determine the localization of synthases, a chitin synthase B-GFP (chsB-GFP) strain was crossed with strains in which core hexamer septins were deleted. After repeated attempts, the only successful cross was with core hexamer deletion strain ∆aspA cdc11.

      Figure 3, Panels A and B, chitin was also labeled by Calcofluor White which clearly shows that the formation of septa was not impaired even in the septin null mutant background (this is in agreement with previous work form our lab which shows that septa still forms in individual septin null mutants). The results showed that unlike WT cells, chitin synthase is not only absent in most branch tips in the septin null mutant background, but seems to be limited primarily to longer (presumably actively growing/non-aborted) branches; these findings were surprising to us, considering other major cell wall synthesis events, such as targeting of cell wall synthases to septa during septation appeared to be unimpaired (based on the presence of fully-developed, chitin-labeled septa).

      The labeling of septa by calcofluor is now noted in the legend to Figure 3 as follows:

      Li 201: Calcofluor White labeling shows the presence of the polymer chitin at septa, main hyphal tips, branches, and …

      Why was chitin synthase B chosen to be analyzed in terms of reallocation? How many chitin synthases are in the A. nidulans genome. This rationale should be explained in the manuscript.

      We have added the following:

      Lines 173-182: A. nidulans contains six genes for chitin synthases: chsA, chsB, chsC, chsD, csmA, and csmB. Chitin synthase B localizes to sites of polarized growth in hyphal tips, as well as developing septa in vegetative hyphae and conidiophores, a pattern very similar to septin localization. Deletion of chitin synthase B shows severe defects in most filamentous fungi analyzed thus far, and repression of the chitin synthase b gene expression in chsA, chsC, and chsD double mutants exacerbated growth defects from a number of developmental states observed in each single mutant, suggesting it plays a major role in chitin synthesis at most growth stages (Fukuda et al., 2009). For these reasons, we chose chitin synthase B as a candidate to observe in septin mutant background for possible defects in localization.

      Figure 3 and Figure 4. The authors should make efforts to quantify the phonotypes they claim. They are overall very subtle, especially for Figure 3. Also, a decrease of fluorescence is a tricky observation that should be better reported by quantification.

      Line scans of aniline blue and CFW label were conducted and added as Fig S1. Quantitation was performed and added as Fig S3. See author’s response to Reviewer #3 below for details.

      Again, in Figures 5, 6, and 7, it is clear that the different septins respond differently when ergosterol or sphingolipids synthesis is impaired. It also raises the question again if there are differences in the role of septin genes. Can the authors use previous information about differences in septin function to improve the model (Figure 8)

      As described above, we have modified the manuscript throughout to clarify which phenotypes are seen for core hexamer, core octamer, and noncore septin deletions. As the reviewer notes, these are especially relevant for the sphingolipid-disrupting agents. Our model includes interaction of septins with sterol rich domains that contain both sphingolipids and ergosterol. Because it is not yet clear how subgroups of septins interact with each other and are organized at SRDs, we show all core septins in our model without distinguishing hexamers and octamers in the drawing, but we have now added text to clarify roles and outstanding questions.

      The changes are summarized in the abstract as follows:

      Li 37-40: Our data suggest that the core hexamer and octamer septins are involved in cell wall integrity signaling with the noncore septin playing a minor role; that all five septins are involved in monitoring ergosterol metabolism; that the hexamer septins are required for sphingolipid metabolism; and that septins require sphingolipids to coordinate the cell wall integrity response.

      The clarifications are reflected in the Figure 8 legend (and associated sections of the discussion) as follows:

      Li 436-441: As described in the text, our data suggest that all five septins are involved in cell wall and membrane integrity coordination. The core septins that participate in hexamers appear to be most important for sphingolipid metabolism while all septins appear to be involved in ergosterol metabolism and cell wall integrity. Because SRDs contain both sphingolipids and ergosterol and because it is not yet clear how subgroups of septins interact with each other at SRDs, we show all core septins in our model without distinguishing hexamers and octamers.

      For the above-discussed reasons, the conclusion on lines 384-388 (discussion) is not completely supported by the experiments shown in the manuscript. The authors need to make a better structured and more straightforward story emphasizing the stronger points and reducing descriptions of more speculative points.

      As discussed above, we have made changes throughout the manuscript to clarify which subgroups of septins are involved in which process and to refine our conclusions accordingly. The beginning of the discussion section has been changed as follows:

      Li 384-399: Our data show that A. nidulans septins play roles in both plasma membrane and cell wall integrity and that distinct subgroups of septins carry out these roles. Previous work has shown that the five septins of A. nidulans septins form hexamers (AspACdc11, AspBCdc3, and AspCCdc12) and octamers (AspACdc11, AspBCdc3, AspCCdc12, and AspDCdc10) and that the noncore septin AspE does not appear to be a stable member of a heteropolymer (20). The current work suggests that though all septins are involved in coordinating cell wall and membrane integrity, the roles of hexamers, octamers, and the noncore septin are somewhat different. Core hexamer septins appear to be most important for sphingolipid metabolism, all five septins appear to be involved in ergosterol metabolism, and core septins are most important for cell wall integrity pathway with the noncore septin possibly playing a minor role. As summarized in Figure 8 and discussed in more detail below, our previous and current data are consistent with a model in which: (A) All five septins assemble at sites of membrane and cell wall remodeling in a sphingolipid-dependent process; (B) All five septins recruit and/or scaffold ergosterol and the core hexamer septins recruit and/or scaffold sphingolipids and associated sensors at these sites, triggering changes in lipid metabolism; and (C) The core septins recruit and/or scaffold cell wall integrity machinery to the proper locations and trigger changes in cell wall synthesis. The noncore septin might play a minor role in this process.

      Minor comments Overall the figure caption could be shortened. They are too descriptive and contain details that are easily inferred for the images and from the materials and methods.

      Legends to the following figures have been streamlined by removing portions that belong in the methods: Figure 2, Fig 3, and Fig 6

      The authors made every effort to cove the precedent literature, but the manuscript has 115 references. The authors should evaluate if all the cited literature is extremely relevant. The manuscript would benefit for that conciseness.

      Because this manuscript addresses septins, ergosterol, sphingolipids, cell wall integrity, and multiple different pathways, there is a lot of literature underlying our approaches. Our strong preference is to cite primary literature, however we can shorten our reference list by relying on reviews if requested by the journal.

      Line 124, 493: Replace 10ˆ7, 10ˆ4 to 107, 104, etc

      “10^7” and all other scientific notation was altered to replace carrots “^7” with superscripts “7” throughout.

      The use of fludioxonil as a probe to detect cell wall impairment is perhaps out of context. This drug responds primarily to the HOG pathway and also respond to oxidative damage. So, these results could be suppressed.

      Previous work by Kojima et al., 2006 showed that in addition to the HOG pathway, cell wall integrity is required for resistance to fludioxonil treatment. C. neoformans cell wall integrity mutants bck1, mkk1, and mpk1 (Aspergillus nidulans bckA, mkkA, and mpkA homologues) all exhibit hypersensitivity to fludioxonil, and this was shown to be remediated by the addition of osmotic stabilizers, suggesting cell wall impairment was involved in the growth defect produced by this treatment. Although this drug seems to act primarily through the HOG pathway, the CWI and HOG pathways have been shown to antagonize/negatively regulate one another through a parallel pathway (SVG pathway in yeast) (Lee and Elion, 1999). It has been hypothesized that internal accumulation of glycerol by constitutive activation of the HOG pathway causes decreased cell wall integrity. Due to the apparent cross-pathway control between the HOG and CWI pathways, as well as the high level of conservation of these pathway components in filamentous fungi, we thought this treatment was rightfully dual-purposed to investigate both cell wall impairment in the septin mutants and any possible involvement of the HOG pathway. This seems to be would a reasonable drug treatment to look at cell wall impairment that is not likely to be redundant with the modes of action observed in the other Figure 1 treatments (e.g. CFW, Congo Red, and Caspofungin).

      The text clarifies this point as follows: li 110-112: Fludioxonil (FLU), a phenylpyrrol fungicide that antagonizes the group III histidine kinase in the osmosensing pathway and consequently affects cell wall integrity pathway signaling (Fig 1)(58-67).

      Line 140: "exposure" would be more appropriate than architecture. Please also consider that the difference in the cell wall reported in Figure S1 are very subtle. Are they relevant?

      The differences in the cell wall content reported in Figure S1 (Figure S2 in the revised manuscript) showed that the peak for 4-Glc was almost identical in WT and aspB null mutant, however the overall ratio of peaks switched, where 4-GlcNac content exceeded the 4-Glc content in the mutant compared to WT. By comparison, this was not the case with the septin aspE null mutant. Although this could be considered a ‘subtle’ change in chitin content, we believe this was an important unbiased analysis of the cell wall polysaccharide content and addressed some of the cell wall sensitivity phenotypes we observed, not only between WT and the septin mutants, but also between the septin null mutants which showed sensitivity to cell wall disturbing agents (i.e. aspA, aspB, and aspC) vs. those that did not show significant sensitivity (e.g. aspE). For these reasons we believe this warranted at the very least a supplemental figure for these data.

      Though our idea of cell wall architecture includes changes in polymer exposure, as pointed out by the reviewer, others might use the phrase to mean only content changes. To avoid this misunderstanding, we have replaced the word “architecture” with “organization” in Li 147-148: These data show that cell wall organization is altered in ∆aspB cdc3 and raise the possibility that it might be altered in other core hexamer septin null mutants as well.

      Line 144: explain briefly what it is about and why it was chosen instead of the total detection of chitin sugar monomers. Line 538: Cell wall extraction section. Is this a new method? There is no supporting literature.

      We chose this method because it provides an analysis of all cell wall polysaccharide components and associated linkages. Detection of chitin sugar monomers would have also been a reasonable analysis if this were the only component of the cell wall we were investigating initially. The results showed differences in cell wall chitin content, so these were the data we presented.

      This was addressed on lines 574-576: “Cell walls were isolated from a protocol based on (Bull, 1970); cell wall extraction and lyophilization were conducted as previously described in (Guest and Momany, 2000) with slight modifications listed in full procedure below.”

      The results described on lines 232-257 are marginal to the study and are not exploited by the authors to address the central question of the manuscript, which is the role of the CWI pathway, septins, and sphingolipids. This section could be suppressed or very briefly mentioned in the preceding section.

      We agree that these data did not show any additional involvement of septins in the Calcineurin and cAMP-PKA pathways, and the relevance of the TOR signaling pathway connection is still quite unclear. For this reason, these data were added as a supplemental figure. On the other hand, there are a number of important signaling pathways which have been shown to affect the Cell Wall Integrity pathway directly and indirectly (these three pathways in particular), which is part of the central question of the manuscript. Considering such extensive ‘cross-talk’ between pathways (references produced on Line 65) in filamentous fungi, we felt it necessary to inspect possible involvement of these pathways in septin function via plate-based assays and feel that this s most clearly communicated as its own brief section in the text.

      Reviewer #2 (Significance (Required)): The topic of the manuscript is highly relevant to the fungal biology field and employs a very important genetic model. The cooperation of signaling pathways in mains aspects of fungal physiology is the main significant contribution of this manuscript. Reviewer__ #3 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity (Required)):__ **Summary:** In this work the authors use genetic analysis in Aspergillus nidulans to identify phenotypes of septin mutants that point to roles for septins in coordinating the cell wall integrity pathway with lipid metabolism in a manner involving sphingolipids. Most of the major conclusions derive from monitoring the effects of combined genetic or chemical manipulations that target specific components of the pathways of interest. Additionally, the authors monitor the subcellular localization of septins, cell-wall modifying enzymes, and components of the cell wall itself. **Major comments:** The key conclusions are convincing, with the unavoidable caveat that null mutations of this sort and chemical inhibitors of these kinds could have unanticipated effects, such as upregulation of unexpected pathways or other compensatory alterations. The authors qualify their conclusions appropriately in this regard. The methods are explained very clearly and the data are presented appropriately. In some cases results are shown as representative images illustrating altered localization of a protein or a cell wall component. The changes observed in the experimental conditions are fairly obvious, but some quantification would not be difficult and would likely make the results even more obvious. For example, the Calcofluor White staining patterns might be nicely quantified by linescans along the hyphal length, and the same is true for AspB-GFP localization upon addition of drugs.

      We thank the reviewer for the positive comments and have made the suggested changes as follows:

      Line scans of aniline blue and CFW label were conducted and added as Fig S1. Text has been modified accordingly (Li 140-147).

      Quantification of Chitin synthase-GFP localization and CFW staining and statistical analysis have now been added as Figure S3 and main text (Li 187-191) has been modified accordingly.

      I could imagine one simple experiment that might generate interesting and relevant results, but by no means would this be a critical experiment for this study. In yeast, exposure to Calcofluor triggers increased chitin deposition in the wall. It would be interesting to know how Calcofluor staining looks in WT or septin-mutant cells that have been growing the presence of Calcofluor for some time, particularly with regard to the localization of chitin deposition in these cells. Such experiments could help connect the idea of septins as sensors of membrane lipid status and also effectors of CWI signaling.

      This is a cool idea that we will pursue in future work. Thanks!

      **Minor comments:** • Body text refers to Figure 1A and 1B but the figure itself does not have panels labeled A or B.

      Figure 1 was revised to show panels A and B labeled clearly.

      • Line 885: "S3" is missing from the beginning of the title of the figure.

      “S” was added to the figure title.

      Reviewer Identity: This is Michael McMurray, PhD, Associate Professor of Cell and Developmental Biology, University of Colorado Anschutz Medical Campus

      Reviewer #3 (Significance (Required)): This is an important conceptual advance in our understanding of septin function because previous work in fungal septins mostly points toward them being important in directing or restricting the localization of other proteins that modify the cell wall or plasma membrane. This new work suggests that septins can play a sensing role, as well. As a fungal (budding yeast) septin researcher myself, I think that other fungal septin researchers would be very interested in these results, and I also think the broader septin community would appreciate it. Additionally, those studying fungal cell wall and plasma membrane biogenesis and coordination, including the Cell Wall Integrity Pathway, will be interested. REFEREES CROSS COMMENTING After reading Reviewer #1's comments, I agree that it would be appropriate to modify the wording of the authors' conclusions about where the septins lie in the CWI pathway (upstream or downstream). While they do mention that there may be other ways to interpret their results, a reader would have to search for the mention of these caveats and if the reader did not, then the strong conclusion statements might be taken as fact.

      The abstract, main text, and discussion have been modified to show that while there is evidence that the septins interact with the CWI pathway, it is not clear which component is upstream vs downstream. See response to reviewer 2 above for details.

      On the other hand, I don't think additional experiments looking at deletions of the other core septins will be worthwhile. I think that there is sufficient evidence to suspect that any single core septin deletion mutant will behave similar to another, and therefore that any one can be taken as representative. While it's possible that the authors might find something informative by looking at other mutants, I personally find the likelihood too low to justify additional experimentation along those lines.

      Based on results from previous work from our lab, there are two subgroups of core septins in A. nidulans (hexamer and octamer) and septins within subgroups appear to behave similarly. The results from the current work support this idea with the same groups of mutants behaving in very similar ways. So, the core hexamer septins, AspACdc11, AspBCdc3, and AspCCdc12 can be used to make predictions about each other, but not about the octamer-exclusive septin AspDCdc10 or the noncore septin AspE. We agree with reviewer 3 that repeating analysis on multiple septins within a subgroup is not likely to give new insight. However, we were not careful in the original version of the manuscript to distinguish between core hexamer and octamer septins. As detailed in the response to reviewer 2 above, we have modified the manuscript throughout to make clear which subgroup of septins were being examined and to put conclusions into this context.

    1. We put high requirements on the companies we acquire. They are successful and well-managed, with a defined market and distinct customer offering. They are run by entrepreneurs who are passionate about their business concept, who want to further develop their company and who share our values on sustaina-ble entrepreneurship. We acquire companies where the owners want to continue leading and developing the company without being its owner. Reasons for wanting to sell can vary. For example, a company may need a new owner because it is in an expansion or investment phase. Or, perhaps new ownership is needed to ensure the company’s longevity. Indutrade has no exit strategy, and by selling to Indutrade, the company’s future is secured

      Think of the business model as a pure holdings company with many different businesses under it's portfolio.

    1. Note: This rebuttal was posted by the corresponding author to Review Commons. Content has not been altered except for formatting.

      Learn more at Review Commons


      Reply to the reviewers

      Reviewer #1 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity (Required)):

      In this study, the authors use focused-ion beam (FIB) milling coupled with cryo-electron tomography and subtomogram averaging to uncover the structure of the elusive proximal and distal centrioles, as well as different regions of the axoneme in the sperm of 3 mammalian species: pig, horse, and mouse. The in-situ tomograms of the sperm neck region beautifully illustrate the morphology of both the proximal centriole, confirming the partial degeneration of mouse sperm, and intriguingly, asymmetry in the microtubule wall of pig sperm. In distal centrioles, the authors show that in all mammalian species, microtubule doublets of the centriole wall are organized around a pair of singlet microtubules. The presented segmentation of the connecting piece is beautiful and nicely shows the connecting piece forming a nine-fold, asymmetric, chamber the centrioles. The authors further use subtomogram averaging to provide the first maps of the mammalian central pair and identify sperm-specific radial spoke-bridging barrel structures. Lastly, the authors perform further subtomogram averaging to show to the connecting site of the outer dense fibers to the microtubule doublet of the proximal principal piece and confirm the presence of the TAILS microtubule inner protein complex (Zabeo et al, 2018) in the singlet microtubules occupying the tip of sperm tails.

      The manuscript provides the clearest insight into flagellar base morphology to date, giving insight into the morphological difference between different mammalian cilia and centriole types. The manuscript is suitable for publication, once the following questions are addressed.

      We are ecstatic that the reviewer shares our enthusiasm for this work. We are particularly grateful that the reviewer appreciates the significance of the unique, and hitherto under-explored biology of the sperm centrioles and the flagellar base.

      **Major Points:**

      How many centrioles and axonemes were used in generating the averages presented in the paper? If too few samples were used, especially in centrioles undergoing dramatic remodeling or degeneration, the reality of MIPs and MAPs being present might be completely affected. For instance, In figure 1d, the authors present a cryoET map of the centriole microtubule triplet. However, centrioles are divided into several regions with different accessory elements. Here, the authors could show the presence of only part of the A-C linker. The A-C linker covers only 40% of the centriole, so does it mean that this centriole is made only of the accessories that characterize the proximal side of the centriole? In the same line, what were the boundaries governing subtomogram extraction? For example, in the distal centriole, were microtubules extracted from just before the start of the transition zone, to the end of the microtubule vaulting, more pronounced at the end of the proximal region? There are known heterogeneities in centriole, as well as flagella, ultrastructure along the proximal distal axis. If no pre-classification was performed for subtomogram longitudinal position along with the centriole and axoneme, structural features may be averaged out, and or present and not reflecting their real longitudinal localization. The classification should be applied here if it was not the case.

      These are all valid points. Because there is no easy way to target the PC/DC when cryo-FIB milling, and because there is only one of each structure in every cell, the chances of catching them in ~150-nm-thin lamellae are slim (not to mention the number of things that can and do go wrong when doing cryo-ET on lamellae). As such, the averages of the PC were generated from 3 tomograms (3 cells) and those of the DC from 2 tomograms (2 cells). We do have more tomograms with the PC/DC, but these were used for segmentation/visual inspection since we only used the best tomograms for averaging. These numbers are not entirely atypical for cryo-FIB datasets; the only other in situ centriole structures are from 5-6 centrioles (from Chlamydomonas, from Le Guennec et al 2020 doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aaz4137 and Klena et al 2020 doi: 10.15252/embj.2020106246).

      To allow readers to adjust their interpretations according to the small number of cells analysed, we explicitly stated the number of animals/cells/tomograms used to generate averages in Table S1. Furthermore, we amended the text to clarify which regions of the centrioles our averages represent. These changes are detailed below:

      (1) proximal centriole

      The lamellae used for averaging PC triplets caught mostly the proximal end of the centriole, and essentially all of the particles come from the most proximal ~ 400 nm. In a sense, this was a form of pre-classification. We now state explicitly that our structure represents only the proximal region and that proximal/distal differences may be identified in the future (see section on distal centriole below). Despite the limited particle number, we are confident in the presence of the MIPs as these are also visible in the raw data (the striations in Fig. 1a, now Fig. 1d, for instance). Page 7, Line 165 was edited accordingly as well as the legend to Fig. 1.

      (2) distal centriole

      The subtomograms used for the DC average were extracted from the region of the distal centriole closest to the base of the axoneme (i.e; the region marked “distal centriole” in Fig. 2h-i). Because the DC doublet average in Fig. 2j was generated from very few particles, we tried to be very conservative when interpreting it. Page 9, Line 216 was edited accordingly likewise the legend to Fig. 2.

      (3) axoneme

      We did attempt to average the axoneme from different regions of flagella (midpiece, proximal principal piece, distal principal piece). This is shown in Fig. 6d-l. The major difference we found was at the doublet-ODF connection. We did not find any striking differences in MIP densities, or in radial spoke densities along the proximodistal axis. As such, the averages in Fig. 5 are from the entire principal piece (but not the midpiece), which we state in the figure legend.

      Because mammalian sperm flagella are very long, it is possible that we missed more subtle differences. We now state this in the Discussion (page 20, line 491):

      **Minor Points:**

      • In line 3, motile cilia are not only used to swim, they can move liquid or mucus for instance.

      Done. Page 3, line 64

      • In line 175, the authors stated " a prominent MIP associated with protofilament A9, was also reported in centrioles isolated from CHO cells (Greenan et al. 2018) and in basal bodies from bovine respiratory epithelia (Greenan et al 2020). Actually, this MIP has been seen in many other centrioles from other species, such as Trichonympha (https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.06.061 ), Chlamydomonas, and Paramecium ( DOI: 10.1126/sciadv.aaz4137 ). Citing these studies will reinforce the evolutionary conservation of this MIP and therefore its potential crucial role in the A microtubule.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out these very important papers, we added them to the manuscript (page 7, lines 175-176).

      • In Line178, the authors stated: "Protofilaments A9 and A10 are proposed to be the location of the seam (Ichikawa et 2017)". High-resolution cryoEM maps confirmed it: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2019.09.030 . This publication should be cited. Moreover, authors should also refer to this paper when discussing MIPs in the microtubule doublet.

      Done (page 7, lines 178-179 and page 13, line 329).

      We also now cite Ma et al (along with Ichikawa et al 2019 doi: 10.1073/pnas.1911119116 and Khalifa et al 2020 doi: 10.7554/eLife.52760) in the Discussion when alluding to high-resolution structures as a possible means of identifying MIPs (page 19, lines 479).

      • In Line 187-189 the authors stated, "We resolved density of the A-C linker (gold) which is associated with protofilaments C9 and C10." The A-C linker interconnects the triplets of the proximal centriole (Guichard et. al. 2013, Li et. al. 2019, Klena et. al. 2020) with distinct regions binding the C-tubule, as shown by the authors in gold, as well as an A-link, making contact with the A-tubule through various protofilaments in a species-specific manner, but always on protofilament A9. The authors may have identified the A-link, labeled in green, on the outside of protofilament A8/A9 in Figure 1d.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing this out. The position of the olive green density associated with A8/A9 is indeed consistent with the A-link, and this is also now illustrated more clearly in the new version of Fig. 1e (now Fig. 1h, see below). We accordingly edited page 8, lines 187-188.

      • In figure 1e, the authors provide a 9-fold representation of the centriole based on their map. How relevant is this model ? the distance between triplet is inconsistent here, which has not been observed before. Do they use true 3D coordinates to generate this model? The A-C linker, which is only partially reconstructed, does not contact the A microtubule. Is it really the case? did the authors see that the A-link density of the A-C linker has disappeared? If these points are not clearly specified, this representation might be misleading.

      In order to avoid misleading readers, we replaced this panel with a model generated directly by plotting back the averages into their original positions and orientations in the tomogram (new Fig. 1h). This model now shows that the olive green density on A8/A9 is in the right position to form part of the A-C linker (as Reviewer 1 correctly pointed out in their previous point). We have amended the figure legend accordingly. We also described how the plotback was generated in the Materials and Methods section (page 26, line 648).

      As the reviewer points out, the distance between triplets does indeed seem inconsistent in the plotback. This is an interesting observation, but we feel it is a bit too preliminary to discuss in detail here. This can be explored in a follow-up study more focused on sperm centriole geometry.

      • The nomenclature regarding MIPs is sometimes confusing in this manuscript. For example, in lines 228-229 "We then determined the structure of DC doublets, revealing the presence of MIPs distinct from those in the PC." Does this include the gold and turquoise labeled structures in Figure 2j? These densities appear to correspond to the inner scaffold stem in the gold density presented in Figure 2j, and armA, presented in the turquoise density (Li et. al. 2011, Le Guennec et. al. 2020). The presence of this Stem here is important as it correlates with the presence of the molecular player making the inner scaffold (POC5, POC1B, CENTRIN): https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-018-04678-8

      While we were initially very conservative with interpreting the DC doublet average (as stated above it comes from very few particles), we agree with the reviewer’s assessment that the gold and turquoise densities in Fig. 2j are consistent with the Stem and armA respectively of the inner scaffold. Because the inner scaffold contributes to centriole rigidity, it will be interesting to determine if and how it changes during remodelling of the atypical DC in mammalian sperm. Intriguingly, at least some inner scaffold components (including POC5, POC1B) reorganise into two rods in the mammalian sperm DC (Fishman et al 2018 doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-04678-8). We expanded the section on the DC average (page 9, lines 218-220):

      • The connecting piece is composed of column vaults emanating from the striated columns is compelling and beautiful segmentation data. However, it is important to note how many pig sperm proximal centrioles had immediate-short triplet side contact with the Y-shaped segmented column 9, as well as in how many mouse centrioles have the two electron-dense structures flanking the striated columns.

      Done. Material and Methods Page 25, lines 615-619.

      The resolution of the mammalian central pair is an important development brought by this work. The structural similarity between the central pair of pig and horse is convincing. However, with only 281 subtomograms being averaged for the murine central pair, corresponding to an estimated resolution of 49Å, the absence of the helical MIP of C1 with 8 nm periodicity suggests that there is simply not enough signal to capture it in the average. The same could be said for the smaller MIP displayed in Figure 4 c, panel ii. This point should be clearly stated.

      We agree with the reviewer that the quality of the mouse CPA structure is not on par with the pig and horse CPA structures. We now explicitly state this caveat in the text (pages 11, lines 276-277):

      Another piece of compelling data presented in this study is the attachment of the outer dense fibers to the axoneme of the midpiece and proximal and distal principal pieces. From the classification data presented along the flagellar length, it is clear that the only ODF contact made with the axoneme is at the proximal principle plate. However, this is far from obvious in the native top view images presented. Is it possible to include a zoomed inset of the connection between the A-tubule and ODF connection?

      We are very happy that the reviewer finds this data exciting. As Fig. 6 is quite cluttered as is, we instead tried to better annotate the cross-section views of the axoneme by tracing one doublet-ODF pair in each image (or only a doublet in the case of the distal principal piece). This shows that there is a gap between the doublet and the ODF in the midpiece, and that there is no such gap in the principal piece. We also hope that annotating one doublet-ODF pair helps the reader see that the same pattern holds true for the other doublets/ODFs. The legend to Fig. 6 was changed accordingly.

      Reviewer #1 (Significance (Required)):

      This work is of good quality and provides crucial information on the structure of centriole and axoneme in 3 different species. This work complements well the previous works.

      The audience for this type of study is large as it is of interest to researchers working on centrioles, cilium, and sperm cell architecture.

      We are pleased the reviewer appreciate the quality of our work and see the interest for broad audience.

      My expertise is cryo-tomography and centriole biology

      Reviewer #2 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity (Required)):

      In this study, Leung et al. used state-of-the-art EM imaging techniques, including FIB cryo-milling, Volta Phase plate, cryo-electron tomography and subtomogram averaging, to study the structure of sperm flagella from three mammalian species, pig, horse and mouse. First, they described two unique centrioles in the sperm, the PC and the DC. They found the PCs are composed of a mixture of triplet and doublet MTs. In contrast, the DCs are composed mainly of doublet and singlet MTs. By using subtomogram averaging, they identified a number of accessory proteins, including many MIPs bound to the MT wall. Many are unique to the mammalian sperm. They further described the connecting piece region of the sperm enclosing the centrioles and found an asymmetric arrangement. Furthermore, the authors presented the structure of sperm axonemes from all three species. These include the DMT and the CPA. Finally, they described the tail region of the sperm and described how the DMTs transitioned to the singlet MTs.

      This is a beautiful piece of work! It is by far the most comprehensive structural study of mammalian sperm cells. These findings will serve as a valuable resource for structure and function analysis of the mammalian flagella in the future. Now the stage is set for identifying the molecular nature of the structures and densities described in this study.

      We thank the reviewer for their positive evaluation! We are very happy that they share our excitement for the work, and that they also see it as “setting the stage” for future studies at the molecular level.

      The manuscript is clearly written. The data analysis is thorough. The conclusions are solid and not overstated. I don't have any major issues for its publication. A number of minor suggestions are listed below. Most are related to the figures and figure legends.

      Figure 1d, the figure legend should mention this is the subtomogram average of PC triplet MTs from pig sperm, though this is mentioned in the text. Also, for convenience, the color codes for the MIPs should be mentioned in the figure legend.

      Done.

      Figure 2J, similarly, the figure legend should mention this is the subtomogram average of DC doublets. It also needs a description of the color codes of the identified MIPs. For the DMT, please indicate the A- and B-tubule, which are colored in light or dark blue.

      Done, except we would prefer not to enumerate the MIPs as we did not name them nor discuss them extensively in the main text as we do not want to over-interpret the MIPs at this point as the average is from relatively small number of particles. However, we did specify that the gold and turquoise densities on the luminal surface are consistent with the inner scaffold. The figure legend was edited accordingly.

      Line 228, "We then determined the structure of DC doublet by subtomogram averaging"

      Done.

      For both Fig 2 and Fig 3. the DC doublets are colored in dark and light blue, please specify which is the A- or B-tubule in the figure legends.

      Done.

      Line 273, need space between "goldenrod"

      We would prefer to keep “goldenrod” spelled as is since this is how the color is referred to in Chimera and ChimeraX.

      Figure 4. need to expand the figure legend. Panels I, ii, iii, iv, are cut-through view of the lumen of CPA microtubules C1 and C2.

      Done.

      Line 338, Interestingly, the RS1 barrel is radially distributed asymmetrically around the axoneme

      Done.

      Figure 5, need color codes for the arrowheads (light pink, pink, magenta) in panels i~n,

      Done.

      Figure 7, (a-c) please use arrowheads to indicate the location of caps in the singlet MT.

      Done.

      Reviewer #2 (Significance (Required)):

      This is a beautiful and significant work - by far the most comprehensive analysis of mammalian sperm structure

      We are thrilled the reviewer appreciate the novelty of our work.

      Reviewer #3 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity (Required)):

      This is a very interesting study that explores the structural diversity of mammalian sperm flagella, in pig, mouse and horse, at high resolution using cryo-FIB milling and cryo-tomography. The study provides the first in situ cryo-EM structure of a mammalian centriole and describes a number of microtubule associated structures, such as MIPs and plugs at the plus-end of microtubules, that were not been reported so far. Additionally, the authors identify several asymmetries in the overall structure of the flagellum in the three species, which have implications for the understanding of the flagellar beat and waveform geometry in sperm, which are discussed by the authors. Although this study does not provide mechanistic novel information on the function of the described structures, it will undoubtedly serve as a reference for future theoretical and empirical work on the role of these structures in shaping the flagellar beat.

      With the exception of a couple of "eclectic word choices" in the Introduction (see detailed feedback in Minor Comments), the manuscript is also well written. Image acquisition and analysis are sound.

      We thank the reviewer for positively evaluating our work. We are glad that they feel our study will “serve as a reference” to inform future studies.

      However, I have some suggestions that should help the authors to strengthen their claims and present their results. The study is in principle suitable to be published, after the following points will be addressed:

      **Major comments:**

      • A major concern is that it is not clear how many animals, sperms and lamellae the authors used to acquire the data presented in the manuscript. This information needs to be provided, because it not uncommon to encounter aberrant flagella, even in a wildtype animal. The authors should state how many animals, and how many flagella per each animal were analyzed, in order to allow the reader to have an opinion on the reliability of their observations.

      • The figures are esthetically pleasing; however, the figures legends should be carefully revised to include necessary information about color codes, image annotations.

      We thank the reviewer for raising these points. We completely agree that the numbers of animals and cells are important pieces of information. As such, we now explicitly state the number of animals/cells/tomograms used for each average in Table S1. For more qualitative observations (such as the relationship between the asymmetry of the pig sperm PC and the Y-shaped segmented columns), we now state in the number of cells and animals in which we see each feature (see detailed response to Reviewer 1).

      **Minor comments:**

      • Line 26. I do not think that the word "menagerie" is properly used in this context.

      • Line 29. The same is true for the word "Bewildering" in this sentence.

      We apologise for our somewhat eclectic word choice. We see the reviewer’s point that unconventional word choice may distract readers, so we replaced these two words with ‘diverse’ and ‘an extensive’, respectively.

      • Line 286 "Our structures of the CPA are the first from any mammalian system, and our structures of the doublets are the first from any mammalian sperm, thus filling crucial gaps in the gallery of axoneme structures." Sentences like this one would fit much better in the Conclusions or at least in the Discussion.

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion, but we would prefer to keep this sentence where it is, if possible. We think it is useful to tell the audience upfront why these structures are significant, especially since readers who aren’t deep in the field may be bogged down by all the details.

      • Line 377 "Large B-tubule MIPs have so far only been seen in human respiratory cilia (Fig. 5j) and in Trypanosoma (the ponticulus, Fig. 5n), but the morphometry of these MIPs differs from the helical MIPs in mammalian sperm." Please insert the citations for the studies about respiratory cilia and Trypanosoma flagella.

      Done.

      • In Figure 1. What do the stars shown in panel a and a' indicate?

      We indeed failed to specify what the asterisks/stars indicate. They are meant to emphasise that the electron-dense material in the lumen of the PC is continuous with the CP. We have now specified this in the text (page 10, lines 245).

      Given the complexity of the structures that compose the flagellar system of sperms, it would be helpful to add an illustration of the sperm with careful annotation of the centriole structures and the various segments of the flagellum.

      This is an excellent suggestion. To help orient readers, we added three panels to Fig. 1 (Fig. 1a-c) showing low-magnification images of whole sperm cells. We annotated different parts of the flagellum (neck, midpiece, principal piece, endpiece) so that readers can refer back to these panels in case they want to know which part of the cell the averages are from.

      • Figure 2. Explanation of the used color codes is missing. Additionally, the authors should include an explanation for the black and white arrows and for the 2 insets in i.

      Done. For the color code, please see response to Reviewer 2. For the black and white arrows, we edited the figure legend.

      • In "(j) In situ structure of the pig sperm DC with the tubulin backbone in grey and microtubule inner protein densities colored individually" ...it should be written "...sperm DC microtubule doublet..."

      Done.

      • In this figure, but also in every other figure that shows centriole, axoneme, or even microtubule averages it is important to indicate the microtubule polarity. Please add the symbol + and - to indicate microtubule polarity in the figures.

      Done. In order to avoid overcrowding, we only labelled the pig structures as the horse and the mouse structures are always shown in the same orientations as the pig.

      • Figure 3. Additional to the images in a,b, and c, the original tomographic slices (without segmentation) should be shown here, to allow the reader to visualize the structure.

      We now include three additional supplementary movies slicing through the respective tomograms.

      • Figure 7. Scale bars are missing in d-f.

      Done.

      • Scale bars are missing in most Supplementary figures.

      Done.

      • Table S1. The Information about horse and mouse centriole data is missing.

      The reviewer is correct, but this information is missing because we did not average from the horse and the mouse. For the mouse, the triplets were in various stages of degeneration, resulting in heterogeneity that precluded us from averaging. For the horse, we simply did not catch enough centrioles to generate a meaningful structure.

      Reviewer #3 (Significance (Required)):

      This study provides several novel structural insights in to the sperm flagellum structure that have implications for the understanding of the flagellar beat and waveform geometry in sperm. Although this study does not provide mechanistic novel information on the function of the described structures, it will undoubtedly serve as a reference for future theoretical and empirical work on the role of these structures in shaping the flagellar beat.

      Great to see the reviewer appreciate the novelty of our work.

    1. Author Response

      1) Please comment on why many of the June samples failed to provide sufficient sequence information, especially since not all of them had low yields (supp table 2 and supp figure 5).

      An extended paragraph about experimental intricacies of our study has been added to the Discussion. It has also been also slightly restructured to give a better and wider overview of how future freshwater monitoring studies using nanopore sequencing can be improved (page 18, lines 343-359).

      We wish to highlight that all three MinION sequencing runs here analysed feature substantially higher data throughput than that of any other recent environmental 16S rRNA sequencing study with nanopore technology, as recently reviewed by Latorre-Pérez et al. (Biology Methods and Protocols 2020, doi:10.1093/biomethods/bpaa016). One of this work's sequencing runs has resulted in lower read numbers for water samples collected in June 2018 (~0.7 Million), in comparison to the ones collected in April and August 2018 (~2.1 and ~5.5 Million, respectively). While log-scale variabilities between MinION flow cell throughput have been widely reported for both 16S and shotgun metagenomics approaches (e.g. see Latorre-Pérez et al.), the count of barcode-specific 16S reads is nevertheless expected to be correlated with the barcode-specific amount of input DNA within a given sequencing run. As displayed in Supplementary Figure 7b, we see a positive, possibly logarithmic trend between the DNA concentration after 16S rDNA amplification and number of reads obtained. With few exceptions (April-6, April-9.1 and Apri-9.2), we find that sample pooling with original 16S rDNA concentrations of ≳4 ng/µl also results in the surpassing of the here-set (conservative) minimum read threshold of 37,000 for further analyses. Conversely, all June samples that failed to reach 37,000 reads did not pass the input concentration of 4 ng/µl, despite our attempt to balance their quantity during multiplexing.

      We reason that such skews in the final barcode-specific read distribution would mainly arise from small concentration measurement errors, which undergo subsequent amplification during the upscaling with comparably large sample volume pipetting. While this can be compensated for by high overall flow cell throughput (e.g. see August-2, August-9.1, August-9.2), we think that future studies with much higher barcode numbers can circumvent this challenge by leveraging an exciting software solution: real-time selective sequencing via “Read Until”, as developed by Loose et al. (Nature Methods 2016, doi:10.1038/nmeth.3930). In the envisaged framework, incoming 16S read signals would be in situ screened for the sample-barcode which in our workflow is PCR-added to both the 5' and 3' end of each amplicon. Overrepresented barcodes would then be counterbalanced by targeted voltage inversion and pore "rejection" of such reads, until an even balance is reached. Lately, such methods have been computationally optimised, both through the usage of GPUs (Payne et al., bioRxiv 2020, https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.02.03.926956) and raw electrical signals (Kovaka et al., bioRxiv 2020, https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.02.03.931923).

      2) It would be helpful if the authors could mention the amount (or proportion) of their sequenced 16S amplicons that provided species-level identification, since this is one of the advantages of nanopore sequencing.

      We wish to emphasize that we intentionally refrained from reporting the proportion of 16S rRNA reads that could be classified at species level, since we are wary of any automated species level assignments even if the full-length 16S rRNA gene is being sequenced. While we list the reasons for this below, we appreciate the interest in the theoretical proportion of reads at species level assignment. We therefore re-analyzed our dataset, and now also provide the ratio of reads that could be classified at species level using Minimap2 (pages 16-17, lines 308-314).

      To this end, we classified reads at species level if the species entry of the respective SILVA v.132 taxonomic ID was either not empty, or neither uncultured bacterium nor metagenome. Therefore, many unspecified classifications such as uncultured species of some bacterial genus are counted as species-level classifications, rendering our approach lenient towards a higher ratio of species level classifications. Still, the species level classification ratios remain low, on average at 16.2 % across all included river samples (genus-level: 65.6 %, family level: 76.6 %). The mock community, on the other hand, had a much higher species classification rate (>80 % in all three replicates), which is expected for a well-defined, well-referenced and divergent composition of only eight bacterial taxa, and thus re-validates our overall classification workflow.

      On a theoretical level, we mainly refrain from automated across-the-board species level assignments because: (1) many species might differ by very few nucleotide differences within the 16S amplicon; distinguishing these from nanopore sequencing errors (here ~8 %) remains challenging (2) reference databases are incomplete and biased with respect to species level resolution, especially regarding certain environmental contexts; it is likely that species assignments would be guided by references available from more thoroughly studied niches than freshwater

      Other recent studies have also shown that across-the-board species-level classification is not yet feasible with 16S nanopore sequencing, for example in comparison with Illumina data (Acharya et al., Scientific Reports 2019, doi:10.25405/data.ncl.9693533) which showed that “more reliable information can be obtained at genus and family level”, or in comparison with longer 16S-ITS-23S amplicons (Cusco et al., F1000Research 2019, doi: 10.12688/f1000research.16817.2), which “remarkably improved the taxonomy assignment at the species level”.

      3) It is not entirely clear how the authors define their core microbiome. Are they reporting mainly the most abundant taxa (dominant core microbiome), and would this change if you look at a taxonomic rank below the family level? How does the core compare, for example, with other studies of this same river?

      The here-presented core microbiome indeed represents the most abundant taxa, with relatively consistent profiles between samples. We used hierarchical clustering (Figure 4a, C2 and C4) on the bacterial family level, together with relative abundance to identify candidate taxa. Filtering these for median abundance > 0.1% across all samples resulted in 27 core microbiome families. To clarify this for the reader, we have added a new paragraph to the Material and Methods (section 2.7; page 29, lines 653-658).

      We have also performed the same analysis on the bacterial genus level and now display the top 27 most abundant genera (median abundance > 0.2%), together with their corresponding families and hierarchical clustering analysis in a new Supplementary Figure 4. Overall, high robustness is observed with respect to the families of the core microbiome: out of the top 16 core families (Figure 4b), only the NS11-12 marine group family is not represented by the top 27 most abundant genera (Supplementary Figure 4b). We reason that this is likely because its corresponding genera are composed of relatively poorly resolved references of uncultured bacteria, which could thus not be further classified.

      To the best of our knowledge, there are only two other reports that feature metagenomic data of the River Cam and its wastewater influx sources (Rowe et al., Water Science & Technology 2016, doi:10.2166/wst.2015.634; Rowe et al., Journal of Antimicrobial Chemotherapy 2017, doi:10.1093/jac/dkx017). While both of these primarily focus on the diversity and abundance of antimicrobial resistance genes using Illumina shotgun sequencing, they only provide limited taxonomic resolution on the river's core microbiome. Nonetheless, Rowe et al. (2016) specifically highlighted Sphingobium as the most abundant genus in a source location of the river (Ashwell, Hertfordshire). This genus belongs to the family of Sphingomonadaceae, which is also among the five most dominant families identified in our dataset. It thus forms part of what we define as the core microbiome of the River Cam (Figure 4b), and we have therefore highlighted this consistency in our manuscript's Discussion (page 17, lines 316-319).

      4) Please consider revising the amount of information in some of the figures (such as figure 2 and figure 3). The resulting images are tiny, the legends become lengthy and the overall impact is reduced. Consider splitting these or moving some information to the supplements.

      To follow this advice, we have split Figure 2 into two less compact figures. We have moved more detailed analyses of our classification tool benchmark to the supplement (now Supplementary Figure 1). Supplementary Figure 1 notably also contains a new summary of the systematic computational performance measurements of each classification tool (see minor suggestions).

      Moreover, we here suggest that the original Figure 3 may be divided into two figures: one to visualise the sequencing output, data downsampling and distribution of the most abundant families (now Figure 3), and the other featuring the clustering of bacterial families and associated core microbiome (now Figure 4). We think that both the data summary and clustering/core microbiome analyses are of particular interest to the reader, and that they should be kept as part of the main analyses rather than the supplement – however, we are certainly happy to discuss alternative ideas with the reviewers and editors.

      5) Given that the authors claim to provide a simple, fast and optimized workflow it would be good to mention how this workflow differs or provides faster and better analysis than previous work using amplicon sequencing with a MinION sequencer.

      Data throughput, sequencing error rates and flow cell stability have seen rapid improvements since the commercial release of MinION in 2015. In consequence, bioinformatics community standards regarding raw data processing and integration steps are still lacking, as illustrated by a thorough recent benchmark of fast5 to fastq format "basecalling" methods (Wick et al., Genome Biology 2019, doi: 10.1186/s13059-019-1727-y).

      Early on during our analyses, we noticed that a plethora of bespoke pipelines have been reported in recent 16S environmental surveys using MinION (e.g. Kerkhof et al., Microbiome 2017, 10.1186/s40168-017-0336-9; Cusco et al., F1000 Research 2018, 10.12688/f1000research.16817.2; Acharya et al., Scientific Reports 2019, 10.1038/s41598-019-51997-x; Nygaard et al., Scientific Reports 2020, doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-59771-0). This underlines a need for more unified bioinformatics standards of (full-length) 16S amplicon data treatment, while similar benchmarks exist for short-read 16S metagenomics approaches, as well as for nanopore shotgun sequencing (e.g. Ye et al., Cell 2019, doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2019.07.010; Latorre-Pérez et al., Scientific Reports 2020, doi:10.1038/s41598-020-70491-3).

      By adding a thorough speed and memory usage summary (new Supplementary Figure 1b), in addition to our (mis)classification performance tests based on both mock and complex microbial community analyses, we provide the reader with a broad overview of existing options. While the widely used Kraken 2 and Centrifuge methods provide exceptional speed, we find that this comes with a noticeable tradeoff in taxonomic assignment accuracy. We reason that Minimap2 alignments provide a solid compromise between speed and classification performance, with the MAPseq software offering a viable alternative should memory usage limitation apply to users.

      We intend to extend this benchmarking process to future tools, and to update it on our GitHub page (https://github.com/d-j-k/puntseq). This page notably also hosts a range of easy-to-use scripts for employing downstream 16S analysis and visualization approaches, including ordination, clustering and alignment tests.

      The revised Discussion now emphasises the specific advancements of our study with respect to freshwater analysis and more general standardisation of nanopore 16S sequencing, also in contrast to previous amplicon nanopore sequencing approaches in which only one or two bioinformatics workflows were tested (page 16, lines 297-306).

      They also mention that nanopore sequencing is an "inexpensive, easily adaptable and scalable framework" The term "inexpensive" doesn't seem appropriate since it is relative. In addition, they should also discuss that although it is technically convenient in some aspects compared to other sequencers, there are still protocol steps that need certain reagents and equipment that is similar or the same to those needed for other sequencing platforms. Common bottlenecks such as DNA extraction methods, sample preservation and the presence of inhibitory compounds should be mentioned.

      We agree with the reviewers that “inexpensive” is indeed a relative term, which needs further clarification. We therefore now state that this approach is “cost-effective” and discuss future developments such as the 96-sample barcoding kits and Flongle flow cells for small-scale water diagnostics applications, which will arguably render lower per-sample analysis costs in the future (page 18, lines 361-365).

      Other investigators (e.g. Boykin et al., Genes 2019, doi:10.3390/genes10090632; Acharya et al., Water Technology 2020, doi:10.1016/j.watres.2020.116112) have recently shown that the full application of DNA extraction and in-field nanopore sequencing can be achieved at comparably low expense: Boykin et al. studied cassava plant pathogens using barcoded nanopore shotgun sequencing, and estimated costs of ~45 USD per sample, while we calculate ~100 USD per sample in this study. Acharya et al. undertook in situ water monitoring between Birtley, UK and Addis Ababa, Ethiopia, estimated ~75-150 USD per sample and purchased all necessary equipment for ~10,000 GBP – again, we think that this lies roughly within a similar range as our (local) study's total cost of ~3,670 GBP (Supplementary Table 6).

      The revised manuscript now mentions the possibility of increasing sequencing yield by improving DNA extraction methods, by taking sample storage and potential inhibitory compounds into account in the planning phase (page 18, lines 348-352).

      Minor points:

      -Please include a reference to the statement saying that the river Cam is notorious for the "infections such as leptospirosis".

      There are indeed several media reports that link leptospirosis risk to the local River Cam (e.g. https://www.cambridge-news.co.uk/news/cambridge-news/weils-disease-river-cam-leptosirosis-14919008 or https://www.bbc.com/news/uk-england-cambridgeshire-29060018). As we, however, did not find a scientific source for this information, we have slightly adjusted the statement in our manuscript from referring to Cambridge to instead referring to the entire United Kingdom. Accordingly, we now cite two reports from Public Health England (PHE) about serial leptospirosis prevalence in the United Kingdom (page 13, lines 226-227).

      -Please check figure 7 for consistency across panels, such as shading in violet and labels on the figures that do not seem to correspond with what is stated in the legend. Please mention what the numbers correspond to in outer ring. Check legend, where it says genes is probably genus.

      Thank you for pointing this out. We have revised (now labelled) Figure 8 and removed all inconsistencies between the panels. The legend has also been updated, which now includes a description of the number labelling of the tree, and a clearer differentiation between the colour coding of the tree nodes and the background highlighting of individual nanopore reads.

      -Page 6. There is a "data not shown" comment in the text: "Benchmarking of the classification tools on one aquatic sample further confirmed Minimap2's reliable performance in a complex bacterial community, although other tools such as SPINGO (Allard, Ryan, Jeffery, & Claesson, 2015), MAPseq (Matias Rodrigues, Schmidt, Tackmann, & von Mering, 2017), or IDTAXA (Murali et al., 2018) also produced highly concordant results despite variations in speed and memory usage (data not shown)." There appears to be no good reason that this data is not shown. In case the speed and memory usage was not recorded, is advisable to rerun the analysis and quantify these variables, rather than mentioning them and not reporting them. Otherwise, provide an explanation for not showing the data please.

      This is a valid point, and we agree with the reviewers that it is worth properly following up on this initial observation. To this end, our revised manuscript now entails a systematic characterisation of the twelve tools' runtime and memory usage performance. This has been added as Supplementary Figure 1b and under the new Materials and Methods section 2.2.4 (page 26, lines 556-562), while the corresponding results and their implications are discussed on page 16, lines 301-306. Particularly with respect to the runtime measurements, it is worth noting that these can differ by several orders of magnitude between the classifiers, thus providing an additional clarification on our choice of the - relatively fast - Minimap2 alignments.

      -In Figure 4, it would be important to calculate if the family PCA component contribution differences in time are differentially significant. In Panel B, depicted is the most evident variance difference but what about other taxa which might not be very abundant but differ in time? One can use the fitFeatureModel function from the metagenomeSeq R library and a P-adjusted threshold value of 0.05, to validate abundance differences in addition to your analysis.

      To assess if the PC component contribution of Figure 5 (previously Figure 4) significantly differed between the three time points, we have applied non-parametric tests to all season-grouped samples except for the mock community controls. We first applied Kruskal-Wallis H-test for independent samples, followed by post-hoc comparisons using two-sided Mann-Whitney U rank tests.

      The Kruskal-Wallis test established a significant difference in PC component contributions between the three time points (p = 0.0049), with most of the difference stemming from divergence between April and August samples according to the post-hoc tests (p = 0.0022). The June sampled seemed to be more similar to the August ones (p = 0.66) than to the ones from April (p = 0.11), recapitulating the results of our hierarchical clustering analysis (Figure 4a).

      We have followed the reviewers' advice and applied a complementary approach, using the fitFeatureModel of metagenomeSeq to fit a zero-inflated log-normal mixture model of each bacterial taxon against the time points. As only three independent variables can be accounted for by the model (including the intercept), we have chosen to investigate the difference between the spring (April) and summer (June, August) months to capture the previously identified difference between these months. At a nominal P-value threshold of 0.05, this analysis identifies seven families to significantly differ in their relative composition between spring and summer, namely Cyanobiaceae, Armatimonadaceae, Listeriaceae, Carnobacteriaceae, Azospirillaceae, Cryomorphaceae, and Microbacteriaceae. Three out of these seven families were also detected by the PCA component analysis (Carnobacteriacaea, Azospirillaceae, Microbacteriaceae) and two more (Listeriacaea, Armatimonadaceae) occured in the top 15 % of that analysis (out of 357 families).

      This approach represents a useful validation of our principal component analysis' capture of likely seasonal divergence, but moreover allows for a direct assessment of differential bacterial composition across time points. We have therefore integrated the analysis into our manuscript (page 10, lines 184-186; Materials and Methods section 2.6, page 29, lines 641-647) – thank you again for this suggestion.

      -Page 12-13. In the paragraph: "Using multiple sequence alignments between nanopore reads and pathogenic species references, we further resolved the phylogenies of three common potentially pathogenic genera occurring in our river samples, Legionella, Salmonella and Pseudomonas (Figure 7a-c; Material and Methods). While Legionella and Salmonella diversities presented negligible levels of known harmful species, a cluster of reads in downstream sections indicated a low abundance of the opportunistic, environmental pathogen Pseudomonas aeruginosa (Figure 7c). We also found significant variations in relative abundances of the Leptospira genus, which was recently described to be enriched in wastewater effluents in Germany (Numberger et al., 2019) (Figure 7d)."

      Here it is important to mention the relative abundance in the sample. While no further experiments are needed, the authors should mention and discuss that the presence of DNA from pathogens in the sample has to be confirmed by other microbiology methodologies, to validate if there are viable organisms. Definitively, it is a big warning finding pathogen's DNA but also, since it is characterized only at genus level, further investigation using whole metagenome shotgun sequencing or isolation, would be important.

      We agree that further microbiological assays, particularly target-specific species isolation and culturing, would be essential to validate the presence of living pathogenic bacteria. Accordingly, our revised Discussion now contains a paragraph that encourages such experiments as part of the design of future studies (with a fully-equipped laboratory infrastructure); page 17, 338-341.

      -Page 15: "This might help to establish this family as an indicator for bacterial community shifts along with water temperature fluctuations."

      Temperature might not be the main factor for the shift. There could be other factors that were not measured that could contribute to this shift. There are several parameters that are not measured and are related to water quality (COD, organic matter, PO4, etc).

      We agree that this was a simplified statement, given our currently limited number of samples, and have therefore slightly expanded on this point (page 17, lines 323-325). It is indeed possible that differential Carnobacteriaceae abundances between the time point measurements may have arisen not as a consequence of temperature fluctuations (alone), but instead as a consequence of the observed hydrochemical changes like e.g. Ca2+, Mg2+, HCO3- (Figure 6b-c) or possible even water flow speed reductions (Supplementary Figure 6d).

      -"A number of experimental intricacies should be addressed towards future nanopore freshwater sequencing studies with our approach, mostly by scrutinising water DNA extraction yields, PCR biases and molar imbalances in barcode multiplexing (Figure 3a; Supplementary Figure 5)."

      Here you could elaborate more on the challenges, as mentioned previously.

      We realise that we had not discussed the challenges in enough detail, and the Discussion now contains a substantially more detailed description of these intricacies (page 18, lines 343-359).

    1. It affords an immediate step, however, to associative indexing, the basic idea of which is a provision whereby any item may be caused at will to select immediately and automatically another. This is the essential feature of the memex. The process of tying two items together is the important thing.

      What Bush called "associative indexing" is the key idea behind the memex. Any item can immediately select others to which it has been previously linked.

    2. Thereafter, at any time, when one of these items is in view, the other can be instantly recalled merely by tapping a button below the corresponding code space.

      Once two items are linked, tapping a button would take you from one to the other.

    3. It is exactly as though the physical items had been gathered together from widely separated sources and bound together to form a new book. It is more than this, for any item can be joined into numerous trails.

      Although Bush envisioned associative trails to be navigable sequences of original content and notes interspersed, what seems to make more sense when viewed through today's technology, is a rich document of notes where the relevant pieces from external documents are transcluded.

    4. And his trails do not fade. Several years later, his talk with a friend turns to the queer ways in which a people resist innovations, even of vital interest. He has an example, in the fact that the outraged Europeans still failed to adopt the Turkish bow. In fact he has a trail on it. A touch brings up the code book. Tapping a few keys projects the head of the trail. A lever runs through it at will, stopping at interesting items, going off on side excursions. It is an interesting trail, pertinent to the discussion. So he sets a reproducer in action, photographs the whole trail out, and passes it to his friend for insertion in his own memex, there to be linked into the more general trail.

      I find this idea of saved associative trails very interesting. In Roam the equivalent would be that you can save a sequence of opened Pages.

    5. Selection by association, rather than indexing, may yet be mechanized. One cannot hope thus to equal the speed and flexibility with which the mind follows an associative trail, but it should be possible to beat the mind decisively in regard to the permanence and clarity of the items resurrected from storage.

      It should be easy to surpass the mind's performance in terms of storage capacity as well as lossiness. It might be more difficult to surpass it in terms of the speed and flexibility with which it "follows an associative trail"

    6. The human mind does not work that way. It operates by association. With one item in its grasp, it snaps instantly to the next that is suggested by the association of thoughts, in accordance with some intricate web of trails carried by the cells of the brain. It has other characteristics, of course; trails that are not frequently followed are prone to fade, items are not fully permanent, memory is transitory. Yet the speed of action, the intricacy of trails, the detail of mental pictures, is awe-inspiring beyond all else in nature.

      The human mind doesn't work according to the file-cabinet metaphor — it operates by association.

      "With one items in its gras, it snaps instantly to the next that is suggested by the association of thoughts, in accordance with some intricate web of trails carried by the cells of the brain."

    7. The real heart of the matter of selection, however, goes deeper than a lag in the adoption of mechanisms by libraries, or a lack of development of devices for their use. Our ineptitude in getting at the record is largely caused by the artificiality of systems of indexing. When data of any sort are placed in storage, they are filed alphabetically or numerically, and information is found (when it is) by tracing it down from subclass to subclass. It can be in only one place, unless duplicates are used; one has to have rules as to which path will locate it, and the rules are cumbersome. Having found one item, moreover, one has to emerge from the system and re-enter on a new path.

      Bush emphasises the importance of retrieval in the storage of information. He talks about technical limitations, but in this paragraph he stresses that retrieval is made more difficult by the "artificiality of systems of indexing", in other words, our default file-cabinet metaphor for storing information.

      Information in such a hierarchical architecture is found by descending down into the hierarchy, and back up again. Moreover, the information we're looking for can only be in one place at a time (unless we introduce duplicates).

      Having found our item of interest, we need to ascend back up the hierarchy to make our next descent.

    8. So much for the manipulation of ideas and their insertion into the record. Thus far we seem to be worse off than before—for we can enormously extend the record; yet even in its present bulk we can hardly consult it. This is a much larger matter than merely the extraction of data for the purposes of scientific research; it involves the entire process by which man profits by his inheritance of acquired knowledge. The prime action of use is selection, and here we are halting indeed. There may be millions of fine thoughts, and the account of the experience on which they are based, all encased within stone walls of acceptable architectural form; but if the scholar can get at only one a week by diligent search, his syntheses are not likely to keep up with the current scene.

      Retrieval is the key activity we're interested in. Storage only matters in as much as we can retrieve effectively. At the time of writing (1945) large amounts of information could be stored (extend the record), but consulting that record was still difficult.

    9. There is a growing mountain of research. But there is increased evidence that we are being bogged down today as specialization extends. The investigator is staggered by the findings and conclusions of thousands of other workers—conclusions which he cannot find time to grasp, much less to remember, as they appear. Yet specialization becomes increasingly necessary for progress, and the effort to bridge between disciplines is correspondingly superficial.

      As scientific progress extends into increased specializations, efforts at integrating across disciplines are increasingly superficial.

    10. A record if it is to be useful to science, must be continuously extended, it must be stored, and above all it must be consulted.

      Bush emphasises the need for notes to not only be stored, but also to be queried (consulted).

    11. The summation of human experience is being expanded at a prodigious rate, and the means we use for threading through the consequent maze to the momentarily important item is the same as was used in the days of square-rigged ships.

      The rate at which we're generating new knowledge is increasing like never before (and this was written in 1945), but our ability to deal with that information has remained largely unimproved.

    12. Professionally our methods of transmitting and reviewing the results of research are generations old and by now are totally inadequate for their purpose. If the aggregate time spent in writing scholarly works and in reading them could be evaluated, the ratio between these amounts of time might well be startling. Those who conscientiously attempt to keep abreast of current thought, even in restricted fields, by close and continuous reading might well shy away from an examination calculated to show how much of the previous month's efforts could be produced on call. Mendel's concept of the laws of genetics was lost to the world for a generation because his publication did not reach the few who were capable of grasping and extending it; and this sort of catastrophe is undoubtedly being repeated all about us, as truly significant attainments become lost in the mass of the inconsequential.

      Specialization, although necessary, has rendered it impossible to stay up to date with the advances of a field.

    1. Note: This rebuttal was posted by the corresponding author to Review Commons. Content has not been altered except for formatting.

      Learn more at Review Commons


      Reply to the reviewers

      Reviewer #1 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity):

      This manuscript follows on from previous work from the Rhind lab to investigate whether the load of MCMs at origins is a factor in when the origin activate (as a population average) during S phase. The authors use budding yeast and a auxin degron system to modulate the levels of an MCM subunit. This allows them to titrate down the concentration of the MCM hexamer and observe the effect. Crucially, they assay both the reduction in MCM load at origins and the subsequent replication dynamics in the same experiment. This is the power of their approach and allows them to rigorously test their hypothesis.

      **Major comments**

      1.I found the introductory paragraph discussing the Rhind lab hypothesis about the possibility of multiple MCM being loaded at origins somewhat misleading. The first paragraph of the discussion was much clear. However, I feel that the introductory paragraph should deal with the difference between the two proposals: 0-1 MCM-DH per origin (de Moura et al), vs 0-50+ MCM-DH (Yang et al). It s also important to note that Foss et al find that "In budding yeast, [MCM] complexes were present in sharp peaks comprised largely of single double-hexamers" - i.e. consistent with 0-1 MCM-DH per origin.

      To improve the balance of the introduction, I think the authors should briefly introduce the concepts behind the 0-1 MCM-DH per origin; this was defined as origin competence by Stillman and clearly described by McCune et al (2008; see figure 8) prior to the work from de Moura et al.

      Furthermore, in the discussion the authors should be more even-handed. To date there is no data to conclusively rule one way or the other in distinguishing between single vs multiple MCMs. The authors cite Lynch et al and state "overexpression of origin-activating factors in S phase causes most all origins to fire early in S phase, consistent with most origins having at least one MCM loaded". However, Lynch et al report equivalent (roughly equal) origin efficiencies, but the assay doesn't distinguish between all going up to high efficiency or all going to a lower intermediary efficiency. Given that fork factors (polymerases, etc) are likely to become limiting at some point (or checkpoints could be activated due to limited dNTP supplies) it would seem plausible that uniform origin efficiency could be a consequence of less than maximal origin firing. As part of this discussion it would be useful for the authors to include what conclusions have been reached on MCM load from in vitro systems (with chromatin substrates).

      Because the main focus of the paper is not dependent on whether MCM stoichiometry varies from 0 to 1 or 0 to many, we had relegated our discussion of absolute stoichiometry to the Discussion. However, it is clear from multiple reviewer's comments that it is something very much on readers minds. Therefore, we have now included a brief introduction to the 0-to-1 and 0-to-many scenarios in the Introduction and moved the bulk of the discussion of the data supporting the two scenarios to the Discussion.

      2.The authors are not the first to look at the consequence of reduced MCM concentrations on origin function. This was essentially the basis for the MCM screen undertaken by Bik Tye's lab that first identified the MCM genes. In addition to temperature sensitive mutants, the Tye group also examined heterozygotes (Lei et al., 1996) to show differential effect on the ability of two origins to support plasmid replication. The authors finds are entirely consistent with these early studies, particularly since ARS416 (formerly ARS1) was found to highly sensitive to reduced MCM levels and ARS1021 (formerly ARS121) was found to be insensitive to MCM levels. The authors find a signifiant reduction in MCM load at ARS416, but the MCM load at ARS1021 is unaltered by reduced MCM concentration. It would be worth the authors noting this consistency. The authors do cite the Lei study, but not in this context. The original MCM screen was published here:

      Maine, G., Sinha, P., Tye, B. (1984). Mutants of S. cerevisiae defective in the maintenance of minichromosomes Genetics 106(3), 365 - 385.

      Furthermore, at the end of the discussion the authors state that "it will be interesting to dissect the specific cis- and trans-acting factors that make origins sensitive or resistant to changes in MCM levels". The equivalent effect reported by the Tye lab has already been dissected by the Donaldson lab (Nieduszynski et al., 2006) and perhaps it would be worth briefly mentioning their findings.

      We have included both of these literature precedents in the Discussion.

      3.The authors should show the flow cytometry data for each of their cell cycle experiments, if only in supplementary figures. This is important to allow a reader (and reviewer) to judge the level of synchrony achieved when interpreting the results.

      This data is now included as Figure S1

      4.I think the authors should show the ChIP signal at some example origins, including ones sensitive and insensitive to the reduction in MCM concentration. Currently all the high resolution ChIP data (i.e. over 1400 bp, e.g. Fig 3a) is presented as meta-analyses of many origins.

      We will include this analysis in a subsequent revision.

      5.When describing the results in Fig 4a the authors focus on changes (highlighted in black boxes) that fit their expectation. However, there are other sites that should at least be mentioned that don't seem to fit the authors model, e.g. ARS517, ARS518. It would be worth discussing what fraction of the timing data can be explained by the reduced MCM load.

      We now explicitly point out that Figures 4c and 4d address this issue of the robustness of the correlation. Although there is significant variation, as the reviewer points out, the trend is seen genome wide. As it happens, both ARS517 and ARS518 do fit the model reasonably well. They have intermediate loss of MCM signal and intermediate delay in timing.

      **Minor comments**

      -These data, rather than this data (throughout).

      I suspect that the journal style and/or copy editors will make the final call. However, I will point out that although 'data' is most certainly plural in Latin, its predominate modern English usage is as a mass noun, such as water or sand or information. In general, users do not think of, or use, 'data' as a collection of discrete elements, each on being a 'datum', a contention supported by the very infrequent use of the word datum. For instance, in ChIP-seq experiment, what is a datum? Each individual read? Each individual nucleotide in each read? The quality score for each individual nucleotide in each read? Each pixel in each image from the sequencer? When one wants to refer to an individual piece of data, common usage is to refer to a data point, just as one would refer to a grain of sand. Moreover, if 'data' were plural, it would be incorrect to use it in phrases such as "there is very little data available". Would the review really suggest using "there are very few data available"?

      -the authors should clearly state in figure legends what window size has been used in analysing genomic data.

      All analyses were done using 1kb windows, as now stated in the figure legends.

      -in figure 2a the authors show pairwise comparisons between conditions, it would be nice to see the 3rd pairwise comparisons perhaps as a supplementary figure

      We have included the third comparison in Figure 2a.

      -in figure 2c it would be clearer to use the same colour for the lines and the points

      The regression lines are in the same colors as the data points they fit. x=y is shown in blue for comparison, as now noted in the figure legend.

      -the authors should avoid the use of red/green colour combinations in their figures (see: https://thenode.biologists.com/data-visualization-with-flying-colors/research/)

      All figures will be redrawn in colorblind-accessible colors in a subsequent revision.

      -in the text the authors state "ORC binding to the ACS and subsequent MCM loading is a directional process dependent on a ACS- site and a similar but inverted nearby sequence (Xu et al., 2006)". I think it would be more appropriate to cite the following study here:

      Coster, G., Diffley, J. (2017). Bidirectional eukaryotic DNA replication is established by quasi-symmetrical helicase loading Science (New York, NY) 357(6348), 314 - 318. https://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.aan0063

      The Coster reference has been included.

      -the list of factors that influence replication timing should include Rif1, whereas it is less clear that Rpd3 acts within the unique genome (as opposed to indirectly via repetitive DNA, e.g. rDNA)

      Rif1 has been added to the list.

      -figure 4 - it might help to mark the centromere on panel a. Also, why do the ChIP peaks and annotated origins appear to line up so poorly?

      The shift between the peaks and the ACS positions was introduced during the construction of the figure. Thanks for catching it. The alignment has been corrected and the centromere annotation has been added.

      -figure 4d - would it not be better to use fraction of lost MCM signal on the x-axis as in previous figures?

      If T_rep was a linear function of MCM stoichiometry, fraction lost would work as well as amount lost. However, we find that there is a lower correlation between fraction of MCM signal lost and T_rep delay than between absolute MCM signal lost and T_rep delay, suggesting a more complicated relationship.

      -"with galactose or raffinose, to induce or repress Mcm2-7 overexpression, respectively." This is incorrect, raffinose does not repress this promoter (that requires glucose).

      Fixed.

      -the S. pombe spike in is a great addition to the over expression experiments. It's a shame that it wasn't included in the auxin experiments.

      Yes, we agree.

      -why does the data in fig 5d appear to be at much lower resolution that the previous ChIP data?

      The resolution was inadvertently reduced during the rendering of the figure. The resolution has restored.

      -in the sequencing analysis pipeline for MCM ChIP the authors use a 650 bp upper size limit; why have such a large threshold compared to the size of a nucleosome? Are the analyses and findings sensitive to this size threshold?

      Although the MNase digestion was optimized to produce mostly mononucleosomal-sized digestion, some di- and very little tri- nucleosomal fragments still remain. In order to capture as many of the MCM-protected immunoprecipitated fragments as possible, the upper limit was set at 650 bp (up to 4 nucleosomes-worth of DNA). However, there is a very minimal contribution from fragments larger than mononucleosomes, qualitatively as well as quantitatively in 1kb windows around origins. Figure 3a provides a qualitative depiction of the contribution of dinucleosomes (input, ~300bp).

      -the repliscope package was published here:

      Batrakou, D., Müller, C., Wilson, R., Nieduszynski, C. (2020). DNA copy-number measurement of genome replication dynamics by high-throughput sequencing: the sort-seq, sync-seq and MFA-seq family. Nature Protocols 15(3), 1255 - 1284. https://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41596-019-0287-7

      The reference has been corrected.

      Reviewer #1 (Significance):

      This work builds upon a body of work from the Rhind group (and others) to determine the contribution of MCM load to replication origin activation dynamics. To my mind this is the most convincing dataset and analysis to date and goes a long way to supporting the model that the efficiency of MCM loading is a major factor in determining the mean replication time of an origin. As the authors state, they are still not able to distinguish between two different models of MCM load (single vs multiple). It would be interesting for the authors to discuss how these two models could be distinguished in the future (perhaps with single cell/molecule experiments).

      This study will be of interest to those in the fields of DNA replication and genome stability.

      My field of expertise is DNA replication and replication origin function.

      Reviewer #2 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity):

      **Summary:**

      This is a nice study that characterizes the consequences of limiting or increasing Mcm expression on the replication program. Prior ChIP experiments in yeast have observed that not all origins exhibit the same level of Mcm enrichment and that increased mcm enrichment was correlated with origin activity. These observations led to two different models -- a) that multiple Mcm2-7 double hexamer complexes are loaded at some origins and b) a probabilistic model where the differential enrichment of Mcm2-7 reflected the fraction of cells in a population that had loaded the Mcm2-7 complex at a specific origin. While the titration experiments presented here don't provide any conclusive support for either model, they do provide some novel and relevant insights for the replication field, in part, due to the increased resolution and quantification afforded by the MNase ChIP-seq approach (and S. pombe spike in). The authors very nicely demonstrate that origins are differentially sensitive to Mcm2-7 depletion and that loss of Mcm2-7 loading results in an altered replication timing profile. The origins most impacted by loss of Mcm2-7 are 'weak' origins as described by the Fox group. Intriguingly, the authors find that the 5X overexpression of Mcm2-7 does not perturb the relative Mcm2-7 loading at individual origins, but rather instead globally represses Mcm2-7 association at all origins. They also find that overexpression of both Cdt1 and Mcm2-7 is detrimental to the cell (although no obvious replication phenotype was observed). Finally, the authors present a reasonable interpretation of their data in the context of models for replication timing which was very well articulated.

      **Major Comments:**

      From the methods it appears that different analyses were performed with different replicates?

      "Replicate #1 was used for all analyses except for V plots, for which the higher resolution Replicate #2 was used."

      Ideally all of the conclusions should be supported by all the replicates independently, or if the replicates are concordant -- they should be merged (at a similar sequencing depth) prior to doing the analyses.. Even the v-plots with merged replicates will be informative due to the greater sequencing depth.

      Though we agree that greater sequencing depth would be informative for aggregation analysis, we think that one of the main strengths of our study is the analysis of MCM quantitation and replication timing in the same population of cells. Although the experiments were performed in exactly the same way, there is always slight biological or temporal differences between the replicates, due to the complicated nature of the experimental design. This variation increases the noise between the MCM ChIP and the replication timing analyses. Therefore, were analyzed the replicates separately. However, we did do all of the analyses on both replicates and got similar results. We have now explicitly stated as much.

      The authors should provide a separate analysis for the larger nucleosomal sized fragments and smaller putative MCM double hexamer fragments with regards to the Mcm loading and relationship to ACS and orientation. They may represent an interesting intermediate with mechanistic consequences for the interpretation.

      We will include the suggested analysis in a subsequent revision.

      The authors should present the v-plots and an analysis of which side the Mcm's load for the overexpression studies. I was surprised that there was no further in-depth analysis for these two extremes. Perhaps similar conclusions will be reached, but it should at least be mentioned/presented as a supplementary figure.

      We will include the suggested analysis in a subsequent revision.

      **Minor Comments:**

      This is largely semantic, but the majority of MNase ChIP-seq signal recovered is associated with the nucleosomes and not in the NDR and as the signal in the NDR is differentially sensitive to digestion, I would suggest rephrasing the following sentence:

      "In contrast to previous genome-wide reports (Belsky et al., 2015), but in agreement with recent in-vitro cryo-EM structures (Miller et al., 2019), we also observe MCM signal in the nucleosome-depleted region (NDR) of origins. "

      to :

      "In agreement with a previous genome-wide report (belsky 2015), we found that the bulk of the MCM signal was associated with nucleosomal sized fragments; however the increased resolution afforded by our approach allowed us to also detect protected fragments in the NDR as predicted by recent in vitro cryo em structures..."

      We have modified the sentence as suggested.

      As a sanity check, please double check V-plots and presence of small fragments with the digestion conditions. In the Henikoff manuscript the bulk of sub-nucleosomal fragments were lost with the longer digestion time. Specifically, the TF footprints were more pronounced with minimal digestion. While it might be argued that the longer digestion more tightly resolved the binding site, in many cases they were completely lost with the 20 minute digestion. This is just a simple check -- I don't doubt the results as reported given the experimental conditions are very different. For example, the henikoff manuscript did not use cross linking or an antibody enrichment step.

      We double checked and confirmed that more small fragments are found in the more digested library. The reason that we see more small fragments when we digest more, in contrast to the contrary observation in the Henikoff paper is presumably because MCM has a larger footprint than a transcription factor and protects that footprint more effectively.

      Last paragraph of the "MCM associates with nucleosomes section" which reports that the Mcm2-7 complex is loaded up or downstream from the ACS independent of orientation should cite Belsky 2015 (Figure 5 and discussion) for the initial observation.

      Done.

      The authors argue that the global reduction in MCM loading associated with overexpression may be a technical artifact given that all origins exhibit a proportional reduction in mcm2-7 loading. However, this is exactly what the S. pombe spike in control is intended for. The relative difference between individual origins resulting from Mcm2-7 depletion would still be evident without the spike in. The authors do discuss different possibilities, but I would not be so keen to discard this as technical artifact.

      We, too, are reluctant to dismiss this result as a technical artifact. However, we are at a loss to offer any other explanation. We raise a handful of biological possibilities in the Discussion, but dismiss each one as failing to account for our results. We would be happy to entertain other suggestions.

      Reviewer #2 (Significance):

      This work has several advances that will be appreciated by the replication field -- including a high resolution view of Mcm2-7 loading in the context of chromatin; the impact of titrating (low and high) MCM expression on MCM loading and replication timing program; and a well reasoned discussion of how different models of MCM loading would impact origin activation and replication timing program. The work builds on prior studies in the field (eg. Belsky 2015), while some of the conclusions regarding the localization of the Mcm2-7 complex relative to the ACS and surrounding nucleosomes are confirmatory, the increased resolution provides new insight (like the enrichment of small fragments in the NDR) that could be further strengthened by additional analysis (see above).

      My expertise is DNA replication and chromatin.

      Reviewer #3 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity (Required)):

      In this study, the authors use Auxin-mediated degradation of Mcm4 to reduce the concentration of the MCM helicase complex in yeast, and determine the effects of this reduction on both MCM-origin association (interpreted as MCM loading) by MNase-MCM-ChIPSeq and on replication origin function by Sync-Seq replication timing experiments (deep sequencing of a yeast population as it progresses through a synchronized S-phase). Complementary experiments testing the effect of induced MCM complex over-expression on MCM-origin association are also performed.

      The authors find that reducing Mcm4 levels (and thus loading-competent MCM complexes) causes yeast cells to be more sensitive to DNA replication stress. In addition, not all origins are equally susceptible to reductions in MCM levels; the origins that do lose MCM binding at reduced MCM levels show a reduction in activity and an associated delay in their replication time under those conditions. Finally, over-expression of the MCM complex has no effect on MCM-origin association or origin function, suggesting that MCM levels are not limiting for origin licensing in yeast under normal lab conditions. The strengths of the study are the well-executed experiments and very nice data that are presented. However, there are several weaknesses. The authors make conclusions that are not supported by their data; and several of the outcomes are not at all unexpected based on extensive published studies in yeast and mammalian cells, raising issues about whether this study advances and/or clarifies the current gaps in the field. While some of the relevant past studies were referenced, the authors did not place their own study in the context to published work and current models in the field, which reduced the scholarly value of their study. Because the work was not placed in context of the field, some of the rationale and conclusions were misleading.

      **Some specific major comments:**

      1,The title is misleading. The authors have clearly shown that when MCM levels are be made limiting in an engineered system, some origins are substantially less active, which means that these origin loci are replicated "passively" (i.e. by a Replication Fork (RF) emanating from a distal origin) rather than actively (i.e. by "firing" and initiating replication). Their own replication data show that. But this competition is only revealed when MCM levels are artificially/experimentally lowered. What is the evidence that competition for MCM complexes among individual origins establishes replication timing patterns in yeast? If anything, the over-expression experiment suggests the opposite--that MCM levels are not limiting and therefore do not play a substantial role in establishing the replication timing patterns that are observed in yeast. Instead those patterns appear to result primarily from the fact that MCM complex activation factors are present in limiting concentrations relative to origins.

      We agree with the reviewer's analysis and have revised the title to "The Capacity of Origins to Load MCM Establishes Replication Timing Patterns".

      2,The abstract states that "the number of MCMs loaded onto origins has been proposed to be a key determinant of when those origins initiate DNA replication during S-phase". While it is true that this lab has proposed this model in budding yeast, the current study performs no experiments that directly address this model--i.e. that i. individual origins possess a different number of MCM complexes and or ii that these differences underlie timing differences. They acknowledge this point in their Discussion--a ChIPSeq experiment is an ensemble experiment--there is no way to know that differences in MCM signals correspond to a different number of MCM complexes per origin versus a differences in the fraction of cells that contain and MCM complex at all at a given origin . But this statement in the abstract, combined with their conclusion in the same section of the paper: "Our results support a model in which the loading activity of origins, controlled by their ability to recruit ORC and compete for MCM, determines the number of helicases loaded, which in turn affects replication timing" implies that they have tested a model that they have not tested. Given how quickly readers "skim" the literature these days, a misleading abstract can do a lot of damage to a field. The results presented in this study neither support nor refute the model for the number of helicases loaded per origin, and the fact that reducing origin licensing efficiency by making the major substrate limiting reduces the number of licensed origins in a cell population is fully expected based on the current state of the field .

      Four questions are addressed in this comment. The first is whether there is variable MCM stoichiometry at origins. The second is whether that variation ranges from 0 to 1 and 0 to many. The third is if the variation is stoichiometry affects replication timing. The fourth is how this variation in stoichiometry comes about.

      Our work is based on the conclusion, supported by a substantial body of literature, that MCM loading stoichiometry varies among origins. Our data in this paper further supports this conclusion.

      As the reviewer notes, and as we had tried to make clear, the data is this paper does not address the range of the variation. Moreover, as we also tried to make clear, our hypotheses, results and conclusions are not affected by whether the range is 0 to 1 or 0 to many.

      This paper focuses on Questions 3 and 4. We have reworked the introduction to make these distinctions more clear.

      We have also corrected the abstract to refer to "the stoichiometry", instead of "the number", of MCMs.

      3,The rationale for the study as stated in the Introduction: "Although the molecular biochemistry of initiation at individual origins continues to be elucidated in great detail (Bleichert, 2019), the mechanism governing the time at which different regions of the genome replicate has remained largely elusive (Boos and Ferreira, 2019)." Is also misleading. In fact, in budding yeast (and other organisms) there have been several advances in this area particularly with respect to DNA replication origin activation. The S-phase origin activation factors are limiting for origin function, and factors such as Ctf19 at centromeres and Fkh1/2 at non-centromeric early-acting origins help to directly recruit the limiting S-phase factor, Dbf4, to origins. It is misleading to ignore this substantial progress and not make an effort to place this current study, which is important and one of the first to look directly at MCM loading control in yeast, into a relevant context with respect to what is known. What's interesting is that this S-phase model assumes/requires that most origins are, in fact, licensed and thus that differences in licensing efficiency are not a major driving of replication timing patterns in yeast. But we do not know why there are only subtle differences in MCM loading---this study may help explain that.

      We have broadened the scope of our Introduction and Discussion to address these points. However, it is not the case that "there are only subtle differences in MCM loading". MCM ChIP-seq (, and this paper) and MCM ChEC-seq both show well over ten-fold variation in MCM stoichiometry at origins. We have now explicitly made this point in the Introduction.

      4,The authors link the differential ability of MCM loading deficiencies when MCM is made limiting to differences in ORC binding categories. The "weak" origins, that presumably bind ORC weakly, were most affected by reductions in MCM. Are these origins less efficient than the other categories, DNA and chromatin-dependent (using the origin efficiency metric data from the Whitehouse lab) where MCM binding is not reduced as much? In normal cells are these early or late origins? Is the idea that the role of excess MCM is to achieve a sufficient number or "back up" origins per cell to deal with potential stress, as proposed by the Blow and Schwob labs in tissue culture cells many years ago? It seems likely that the data reported here are in fact confirmations of those early studies in mammalian cells---which is useful to know even if not unexpected.

      We will include the suggested analyses in a subsequent revision.

      Excess MCM do, as has been long appreciated and as we discuss, contribute to replication-stress tolerance. However, that is not a major point of our paper.

      5,Aren't the results that losing MCM signal corresponds to loss of origin activity peaks entirely expected? The same result would be obtained if you made a point mutation in that origin's ACS. Of course preventing an origin from being licensed will delay that region's replication time in S-phase because it now must be replicated passively. Licensing affects replication timing patterns because the MCM complex is the substrate for limiting S-phase factors, but that is far different from concluding that the number of MCMs at an origin is what controls the time in S-phase when an origin is activated.

      Yes, "the results that losing MCM signal corresponds to loss of origin activity peaks [are] entirely expected". However, this is not the important result. The key result is that the distribution of MCM at origins is not uniformly affected, which leads to our conclusions that, in wild-type cells, origin capacity dominates MCM stoichiometry and that, when MCM become limiting, origin activity (probably determined by ORC affinity) becomes critical—neither of which were expected results. In any case, the expected correlation between MCM loading and origin activity was observed as a consequence of measuring MCM stoichiometry and replication timing and is an obvious analysis to include, so we did so.

      6,The authors stated that the measured MCM abundance for the 43% of origins that are not known to be controlled by the multiple mechanisms that have been shown to control origin replication time. Is this because they think that MCM loading contributes to the timing control of only these origins? Was MCM loading not affected at any of these other origins when MCM levels were reduced? Are those 43% of origins in the "weak" binding category in terms of ORC? The rationale for eliminating so many origins from these analyses were not clear.

      We propose that the probability of origin activation is the product of the stoichiometry of MCM at the origin and the rate of MCM activation, which may be affected by trans-acting factors. For the 43% of origins for which there is no known trans-acting regulation, the correlation with stoichiometry is stronger. However, the correlation holds when looking at all origin, too. The suggestion to look at only the 57% of origins with known trans-action regulation is a good one. We will include this analysis and the other suggested analyses in a subsequent revision.

      7,Doesn't the data in Figure 4c at 0 mM auxin support the conclusion that differences in MCM ChIP signals have negligible effects on origin activation time, in contrast to the publication by Das, 2015 from this lab? Or is the point that these origins are sensitive to reductions in MCM levels and the more sensitive they are the more delayed their replication time (but again, doesn't that have to be true? If they are losing MCM signals they cannot function as origins, so they are replicated passively and, by definition, will show delayed replication timing. An origin is defined as such by a loaded MCM complex.)

      No. The reason the correlation in 4c is not a good as in our previous work is that in Das 2015 we compared origin-activation efficiency (calculated from our stochastic model in Yang 2010), instead of T_rep, which we used here. T_rep is a convolution of origin-activation time and passive-replication time, reducing to correlation. The important observation is that the correlation gets better as MCM levels are reduced.

      The correlation between MCM stoichiometry and activation efficiency may seem trivial, but just because a model is simple does not mean it is not correct. If stoichiometry was the only factor regulating origin activation, we would expect a stronger correlation. So, we conclude that there are other factors at play, quite possible the trans-acting factors that the reviewer mentions in their second point. However, if stoichiometry played no role, we would expect no correlation. So, we propose that MCM stoichiometry is "an important determinant of replication timing".

      8,I do not understand the conclusions from Figure 4d. There is an extremely small positive correlation between how much of an MCM signal is lost and delay in replication time of an origin, but this correlation is not surprising as an unlicensed origin cannot be an origin and will be replicated passively. What seems most surprising about these data is that the effect is so weak, not that it exists. There is quite a lot of scatter in this plot at 500 uM auxin, with some origins losing a given amount of signal (x) and being only slightly delayed in replication time, and others losing the same amount of signal (x) and being substantially delayed. What underlies this outcome?--Are the ones that are not substantially delayed closer to origins that have not been affected at all by MCM reductions? Why is the correlation so weak? The other regulators of origin activation time have stronger and more precise effects--for example the centromere-control can be precisely eliminated so that only the replication time of the centromere-proximal origins are delayed.

      We believe that much of the noise in Figure 4d is due, as the reviewer suggests, to passive replication of origins which lose most of their MCM signal and become inactive but happen to reside next to origins which don’t lost any MCM signal and fire early. And excellent example is ARS 510 (see Figure 4a). ARS510 loses most of its MCM signal and clearly loses its initiation peak in the T_rep plot. However, because it is next to ARS511, which does not lose much MCM signal and which remains a efficient origin, ARS510 is still replicated early. We will include this example in a subsequent revision.

      9,Multiple studies in yeast and mammalian cells indicate that MCM subunits are in excess relative to other licensing and S-phase initiation factors, so it is not unexpected that over-expressing MCM did not lead to enhanced levels of licensing. It seems much more plausible that Cdc6 or Cdt1 or both factors are present in limiting amounts for MCM loading, so I did not understand the point of over-producing MCM subunits. If the "weak" origins are the ones that are most dramatically affected by reducing MCM to "limiting" levels, isn't the question whether you can increase licensing at these origins when you over-produce a factor that is likely limiting for licensing, such as Cdt1 or Cdc6 (or both) while leaving MCM at its normal levels. The fact that MCM levels are not limiting for licensing is not surprising and, if anything, argues against these levels having a regulatory role in origin activation timing---which seems to be the opposite of what the authors want to conclude.

      Orc1-6, Cdc6 and Cdt1 are all substoichiometric to MCM. However, they all act catalytically to load MCM. So, although they may be kinetically limiting, they do not prevent most or all MCMs being loaded in wild-type cells. The fact that overexpressing MCMs (with or without Cdt1) does not allow for more MCM loading suggests that under normal conditions origins are saturated with MCMs and have little or no capacity to load more MCM, even when given plenty of time to do so. From this result, we conclude that origin capacity is a major determinant of MCM loading in wild-type cells. From our MCM-reduction experiments, we also conclude that, when MCM is limiting, origin competition affects which origins load MCMs faster. However, we agree with the reviewer's first point, that our title gave the incorrect impression that we concluded that origin competition is the primary determinant of MCM loading in wild-type cells. Thus, as suggested, we have changed the title. We have also reworked the Introduction and Discussion to more clearly explain that competition is only a determining factor when MCMs are limited.

      In summary, I think the technical aspects of the experiments were quite strong, but I do not think that the experiments answered the question that was posed by the authors.

      **Minor points:**

      Many places where "This data" should be changed to "These data". Data are plural.

      See comments on this point in the response to Reviewer #2.

    2. Note: This preprint has been reviewed by subject experts for Review Commons. Content has not been altered except for formatting.

      Learn more at Review Commons


      Referee #3

      Evidence, reproducibility and clarity

      In this study, the authors use Auxin-mediated degradation of Mcm4 to reduce the concentration of the MCM helicase complex in yeast, and determine the effects of this reduction on both MCM-origin association (interpreted as MCM loading) by MNase-MCM-ChIPSeq and on replication origin function by Sync-Seq replication timing experiments (deep sequencing of a yeast population as it progresses through a synchronized S-phase). Complementary experiments testing the effect of induced MCM complex over-expression on MCM-origin association are also performed.

      The authors find that reducing Mcm4 levels (and thus loading-competent MCM complexes) causes yeast cells to be more sensitive to DNA replication stress. In addition, not all origins are equally susceptible to reductions in MCM levels; the origins that do lose MCM binding at reduced MCM levels show a reduction in activity and an associated delay in their replication time under those conditions. Finally, over-expression of the MCM complex has no effect on MCM-origin association or origin function, suggesting that MCM levels are not limiting for origin licensing in yeast under normal lab conditions. The strengths of the study are the well-executed experiments and very nice data that are presented. However, there are several weaknesses. The authors make conclusions that are not supported by their data; and several of the outcomes are not at all unexpected based on extensive published studies in yeast and mammalian cells, raising issues about whether this study advances and/or clarifies the current gaps in the field. While some of the relevant past studies were referenced, the authors did not place their own study in the context to published work and current models in the field, which reduced the scholarly value of their study. Because the work was not placed in context of the field, some of the rationale and conclusions were misleading.

      Some specific major comments:

      1,The title is misleading. The authors have clearly shown that when MCM levels are be made limiting in an engineered system, some origins are substantially less active, which means that these origin loci are replicated "passively" (i.e. by a Replication Fork (RF) emanating from a distal origin) rather than actively (i.e. by "firing" and initiating replication). Their own replication data show that. But this competition is only revealed when MCM levels are artificially/experimentally lowered. What is the evidence that competition for MCM complexes among individual origins establishes replication timing patterns in yeast? If anything, the over-expression experiment suggests the opposite--that MCM levels are not limiting and therefore do not play a substantial role in establishing the replication timing patterns that are observed in yeast. Instead those patterns appear to result primarily from the fact that MCM complex activation factors are present in limiting concentrations relative to origins.

      2,The abstract states that "the number of MCMs loaded onto origins has been proposed to be a key determinant of when those origins initiate DNA replication during S-phase". While it is true that this lab has proposed this model in budding yeast, the current study performs no experiments that directly address this model--i.e. that i. individual origins possess a different number of MCM complexes and or ii that these differences underlie timing differences. They acknowledge this point in their Discussion--a ChIPSeq experiment is an ensemble experiment--there is no way to know that differences in MCM signals correspond to a different number of MCM complexes per origin versus a differences in the fraction of cells that contain and MCM complex at all at a given origin . But this statement in the abstract, combined with their conclusion in the same section of the paper: "Our results support a model in which the loading activity of origins, controlled by their ability to recruit ORC and compete for MCM, determines the number of helicases loaded, which in turn affects replication timing" implies that they have tested a model that they have not tested. Given how quickly readers "skim" the literature these days, a misleading abstract can do a lot of damage to a field. The results presented in this study neither support nor refute the model for the number of helicases loaded per origin, and the fact that reducing origin licensing efficiency by making the major substrate limiting reduces the number of licensed origins in a cell population is fully expected based on the current state of the field .

      3,The rationale for the study as stated in the Introduction: "Although the molecular biochemistry of initiation at individual origins continues to be elucidated in great detail (Bleichert, 2019), the mechanism governing the time at which different regions of the genome replicate has remained largely elusive (Boos and Ferreira, 2019)." Is also misleading. In fact, in budding yeast (and other organisms) there have been several advances in this area particularly with respect to DNA replication origin activation. The S-phase origin activation factors are limiting for origin function, and factors such as Ctf19 at centromeres and Fkh1/2 at non-centromeric early-acting origins help to directly recruit the limiting S-phase factor, Dbf4, to origins. It is misleading to ignore this substantial progress and not make an effort to place this current study, which is important and one of the first to look directly at MCM loading control in yeast, into a relevant context with respect to what is known. What's interesting is that this S-phase model assumes/requires that most origins are, in fact, licensed and thus that differences in licensing efficiency are not a major driving of replication timing patterns in yeast. But we do not know why there are only subtle differences in MCM loading---this study may help explain that.

      4,The authors link the differential ability of MCM loading deficiencies when MCM is made limiting to differences in ORC binding categories. The "weak" origins, that presumably bind ORC weakly, were most affected by reductions in MCM. Are these origins less efficient than the other categories, DNA and chromatin-dependent (using the origin efficiency metric data from the Whitehouse lab) where MCM binding is not reduced as much? In normal cells are these early or late origins? Is the idea that the role of excess MCM is to achieve a sufficient number or "back up" origins per cell to deal with potential stress, as proposed by the Blow and Schwob labs in tissue culture cells many years ago? It seems likely that the data reported here are in fact confirmations of those early studies in mammalian cells---which is useful to know even if not unexpected.

      5,Aren't the results that losing MCM signal corresponds to loss of origin activity peaks entirely expected? The same result would be obtained if you made a point mutation in that origin's ACS. Of course preventing an origin from being licensed will delay that region's replication time in S-phase because it now must be replicated passively. Licensing affects replication timing patterns because the MCM complex is the substrate for limiting S-phase factors, but that is far different from concluding that the number of MCMs at an origin is what controls the time in S-phase when an origin is activated.

      6,The authors stated that the measured MCM abundance for the 43% of origins that are not known to be controlled by the multiple mechanisms that have been shown to control origin replication time. Is this because they think that MCM loading contributes to the timing control of only these origins? Was MCM loading not affected at any of these other origins when MCM levels were reduced? Are those 43% of origins in the "weak"binding category in terms of ORC? The rationale for eliminating so many origins from these analyses were not clear.

      7,Doesn't the data in Figure 4c at 0 mM auxin support the conclusion that differences in MCM ChIPsignals have negligible effects on origin activation time, in contrast to the publication by Das, 2015 from this lab? Or is the point that these origins are sensitive to reductions in MCM levels and the more sensitive they are the more delayed their replication time (but again, doesn't that have to be true? If they are losing MCM signals they cannot function as origins, so they are replicated passively and, by definition, will show delayed replication timing. An origin is defined as such by a loaded MCM complex.)

      8,I do not understand the conclusions from Figure 4d. There is an extremely small positive correlation between how much of an MCM signal is lost and delay in replication time of an origin, but this correlation is not surprising as an unlicensed origin cannot be an origin and will be replicated passively. What seems most surprising about these data is that the effect is so weak, not that it exists. There is quite a lot of scatter in this plot at 500 uM auxin, with some origins losing a given amount of signal (x) and being only slightly delayed in replication time, and others losing the same amount of signal (x) and being substantially delayed. What underlies this outcome?--Are the ones that are not substantially delayed closer to origins that have not been affected at all by MCM reductions? Why is the correlation so weak? The other regulators of origin activation time have stronger and more precise effects--for example the centromere-control can be precisely eliminated so that only the replication time of the centromere-proximal origins are delayed.

      9,Multiple studies in yeast and mammalian cells indicate that MCM subunits are in excess relative to other licensing and S-phase initiation factors, so it is not unexpected that over-expressing MCM did not lead to enhanced levels of licensing. It seems much more plausible that Cdc6 or Cdt1 or both factors are present in limiting amounts for MCM loading, so I did not understand the point of over-producing MCM subunits. If the "weak" origins are the ones that are most dramatically affected by reducing MCM to "limiting" levels, isn't the question whether you can increase licensing at these origins when you over-produce a factor that is likely limiting for licensing, such as Cdt1 or Cdc6 (or both) while leaving MCM at its normal levels. The fact that MCM levels are not limiting for licensing is not surprising and, if anything, argues against these levels having a regulatory role in origin activation timing---which seems to be the opposite of what the authors want to conclude.

      In summary, I think the technical aspects of the experiments were quite strong, but I do not think that the experiments answered the question that was posed by the authors.

      Minor points:

      Many places where "This data" should be changed to "These data". Data are plural.

      Significance

      Significance: see above

      Referees Cross Commenting

      Reviewer 3. My overall conclusions about this study are that the data are extremely nice and useful to the field, but that their potential to advance the field or clarify it for 'outsiders' are limited by 1, a biased. model-centric presentation that fails to put the work in context of a lot of strong previous work. Some of the conclusions cannot event be tested by the experimental design 2, some of the data analyses, for example the parsing of origins for analyses of MCM effects versus effects on replication time seem arbitrary and were not clearly justified. 3, The correlation between reductions in MCM loading and Trep delay seemed weak, even after parsing for origins expected to experience the largest effects, which is actually kind of interesting, but was ignored in favor of the pre-determined interpretation.

    1. Note: This rebuttal was posted by the corresponding author to Review Commons. Content has not been altered except for formatting.

      Learn more at Review Commons


      Reply to the reviewers

      Note: This preprint has been reviewed by subject experts for Review Commons. Content has not been altered except for formatting.

      *Reviewer #1 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity (Required)):

      The manuscript by Abrams and Nance describes how the polarity proteins PAR-6 and PKC-3/aPKC promote lumen extension of the unicellular excretory canal in C. elegans. Using tissue-specific depletion methods they find that CDC-42 and the RhoGEF EXC-5/FGD are required for luminal localization of PAR-6, which recruits the exocyst complex required for lumen extension. Interestingly, they show that the ortholog of the mammalian exocyst receptor, PAR-3, is dispensable for luminal membrane extension. Overall, this is a well-written and interesting manuscript.*

      1.Because depletion of PAR-3 in the canal causes milder defects than PAR-6 or CDC-42 the authors suggest that they cannot rule out the possibility that an alternative isoform of PAR-3 is expressed and buffering the defect. They should perform canal-specific RNAi-mediated depletion of the entire PAR-3 gene to determine if this is true.

      We agree with Reviewer 1 that it would be useful to provide additional evidence that an alternative isoform of PAR-3 lacking the ZF1 degron is not expressed. While tissue-specific RNAi could be used, we have not been successful obtaining complete knockdown in previous tissue-specific RNAi experiments. Moreover, this approach does not target any maternal PAR-3 protein that may be inherited by the excretory cell. As an alternative approach to address this point, we will analyze par-3::zf1::yfp/par-3(null) worms following excretory-cell-specific expression of zif-1, and compare to par-3::zf1::yfp/par-3::zf1::yfp siblings. We would expect the excretory cell phenotype to become more severe if additional, ‘phenotype-buffering’ forms of PAR-3 were present, or if there was incomplete degradation of PAR-3::ZF1::YFP in our previous experiments.

      2.The authors suggest that GTP-loaded (activated) CDC-42 recruits PAR-6 to the luminal membrane. It would be nice if they could use a biosensor, such as the GBD-WSP-1 reagent from Buechner's lab to confirm that EXC-5 depletion also reduces activated CDC-42, as would be expected. This should be achievable since there is strong CDC-42 signal, even in the cytoplasm.

      This is an excellent suggestion. We will utilize a CDC-42 biosensor – an integrated cdc42p::gfp::wsp-1(gbd) strain created in our lab and previously validated and characterized (Zilberman et al. 2017). We have confirmed that the biosensor is detected in the excretory canal and appears enriched at or near the lumenal membrane. We will cross the biosensor into the exc-5::zf1::mScarlet background. This will allow us to assess lumenal enrichment, and using heat shock inducible ZIF-1, determine if there is a reduction in biosensor lumenal enrichment when EXC-5::ZF1::mScarlet is acutely degraded.

      If the biosensor is difficult to measure at the canal lumen, an alternative approach would be to use an available exc-5 null allele to examine genetically if cdc-42 and exc-5 are acting in the same pathway. We could cross CDC-42exc(-) larvae into exc-5(rh232) and quantify excretory canal phenotypes. If CDC-42 and EXC-5 are indeed functioning in the same pathway we would expect no enhancement of the CDC-42exc(-) phenotype.

      3.Related to point 2, (i) does mutation of the CRIB domain of PAR-6 impair its recruitment to the luminal membrane, and (ii) does this mutant exacerbate canal defects when PAR-3 is depleted?

      (i) Our lab has previously generated and characterized a transgenic par6P::par-6(**CRIB)::gfp strain (Zilberman et al., 2017). We will examine this strain to determine if expression is detectable in the excretory canal, and if so, we will compare lumenal enrichment of PAR-6(CRIB)::GFP to control worms expressing wild-type PAR-6::GFP.

      (ii) This is a very interesting experiment, as it would help address if the mild phenotype observed in PAR-3 depleted animals is due to the remaining PAR-6 that is recruited by CDC-42. Our lab has previously shown that par6P::par-6(**CRIB)::gfp cannot rescue the embryonic lethality of a par-6 mutant, in contrast to par-6::gfp (Zilberman et al. 2017). This indicates that the CRIB domain is needed for PAR-6 function during embryogenesis and suggests that CRIB domain mutations introduced by CRISPR would almost certainly be lethal, precluding analysis of the excretory cell.

      As an alternative experiment, we would determine if PAR-3 localizes to the lumenal membrane independently of CDC-42; such a finding would imply that PAR-3 and CDC-42 likely have independent contributions to PAR-6 localization (rather than CDC-42 promoting PAR-6 localization by localizing PAR-3). To do this, we will degrade ZF1::YFP::CDC-42 in the excretory cell and examine the localization of PAR-3::mCherry compared to controls. We have all of the strains needed for this experiment.

      4.The authors hypothesize that partial recruitment of PAR-6 by CDC-42 is sufficient for luminal membrane extension to explain the mild defects caused by PAR-3 depletion. Since depletion of PAR-6 and CDC-42 alone causes milder canal truncations the authors should co-deplete these proteins (as well as PAR-3 and CDC-42) to determine if there is an additive effect.

      This is an excellent suggestion in principal. However, it is not possible to know in any given degradation experiment whether the targeted protein is completely degraded; we can only say it is no longer detectable by fluorescence. Thus, any degron allele (in the presence of ZIF-1) could behave like a strong hypomorph rather than a null. It would not be possible to interpret double degradation experiments in such a case, as a more severe phenotype in the double could simply be a result of combining two hypomorphic alleles, further reducing pathway activity even if the genes function together in the same pathway. To interpret this experiment properly, a null allele of at least one of the genes would have to be used. This is not possible since par and cdc-42 null mutants are lethal and there is also maternal contribution. As an alternative to these double depletion experiments, we will deplete PAR-6::ZF1::YFP or PAR-3::ZF1::YFP in exc-5 null mutant larvae, as unlike cdc-42, exc-5 is not an essential gene.

      5.In figure 2, the authors show that depletion of PKC-3 causes more severe canal truncations than PAR-6. Since these proteins function in the same complex what do they think is the reason for this difference? This point could be discussed more in the manuscript.

      As described in the previous point, incomplete degradation could produce modestly different phenotypes even for genes that act in the same pathway. Therefore, it is not possible to determine whether PAR-6 and PKC-3 have different roles using this approach. We will add text to the discussion bringing up this point.

      6.Related to point 5, more experiments with PKC-3 should be done to determine if, for example, localization of SEC-10 is similarly affected as ablation of PAR-3, PAR-6 and CDC-42.

      We agree, and will address this point by acutely degrading ZF1::GFP::PKC-3 and examining transgenic SEC-10::mCherry, as we have done for other par genes.

      Reviewer #2 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity (Required)): The manuscript by Abrams & Nance describes a precise investigation of the role of PAR proteins in the recruitment of the exocyst during and after the extension of the C. elegans excretory canal. State-of-the-art genetic techniques are used to acutely deplete proteins only in the targeted cell, and examine the localization of endogenously expressed markers. Experiments are well described and carefully quantified, with systematic statistical analysis. The manuscript is easy to follow and the bibliography is very good. Most conclusions are well supported.

      1) I am not entirely convinced by the presence of CDC-42 at the lumenal membrane (Fig3G); it seems to be more sub-lumenal that really lumenal. It peaks well before PAR-6 (Fig3H) which itself seem slightly less apical that PAR-3 (Fig3F). Could you use super-resolution microscopy (compatible with endogenous expression levels) to more precisely localize CDC-42? Similar point for PAR-3 and PAR-6 which do not seem to colocalize completely - a longitudinal line scan along the lumenal membrane might provide the answer even without super-resolution; this could help explain why these two proteins do not have the same function. These suggestions are easy to do provided the authors can have access to super-resolution (Airyscan to name it; although other methods will be perfectly acceptable I believe it is the most simple one).

      We agree that the CDC-42 localization peak does not precisely match the PAR-6 peak. As the reviewer notes, resolving the subcellular localization of these two proteins will not be feasible using standard confocal microscopy. We will image the ZF1::YFP::CDC-42; PAR-6:mKate strain using a Zeiss LSM 880 with Airyscan to determine if their subcellular localization patterns are distinct.

      To examine PAR-3 and PAR-6 colocalization at the lumen, we will acquire additional confocal images of the PAR-6-ZF1-YFP; PAR-3-mCherry strain and examine colocalization of the clusters along the lumenal membrane. As a positive control for two proteins that should co-localize, we will image ZF1::GFP::PKC-3; PAR-6-mKate; these two proteins bind directly and co-localize in nearly all cells in which they have been examined.

      2) The same group has described a CDC-42 biosensor to detect its active form. It could be used here to precisely pinpoint where active CDC-42 is required: in the cytoplasm? At the lumenal membrane? colocalizing with what other protein? This will require the expression of a transgene under an excretory cell specific promotor and a simple injection strategy while helping to strengthen the description of the CDC-42 role.

      See Reviewer 1 point #2.

      3) As the authors certainly know, there is a PAR-6 mutation which prevents its binding to CDC-42. They could express this construct in the excretory canal a simple extrachromosomal array should be sufficient) to validate the direct interaction between these proteins in this cell.

      See Reviewer 1 point #3.

      4) What is the lethality of ZIF-1-mediated depletion of the various factors under the exc promoter? Can homozygous strains be maintained? Authors just have to add a sentence in the Mat&Met section.

      All of the strains with excretory cell-specific degradation we have examined are viable when grown on NGM plates. We will add this point to the materials and methods.

      Provided that the authors have access to an Airyscan, all the questions asked here can be answered in two months (one month for constructs, one month for injection and data analysis) at a very minor cost.

      Reviewer #3 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity (Required)):

      Strengths of this manuscript include the use of endogenously tagged proteins (rather than over-expressed transgenes) for high resolution imaging and a cell-type specific acute depletion strategy that avoids complicating pleiotropies and allows tests of molecular epistasis. While some results were fairly expected based on prior studies of Cdc42, PAR proteins, and the exocyst in other tissues or systems, differences in the requirements for par-6 and pkc-3 vs. par-3 strongly suggest that the former genes play more important roles in exocyst recruitment. I was also excited to see a connection made between EXC-5 and PKC-3 localization.

      1.Lumen formation vs. lumen extension. The abstract and introduction use these two terms almost interchangeably, but they are not the same and more care should be taken to avoid the former term. The data here do not demonstrate any roles for par or other genes in lumen formation, but do demonstrate roles in lumen extension and organization/shaping.

      We agree and will correct wording to indicate that lumen extension is affected.

      2.Related to the above, mutant phenotypes here are surprisingly mild and variable. The authors discuss possible reasons for the particularly mild phenotype of par-3 mutants, but don't specifically address the mild phenotypes of the others. Clearly quite a bit of polarization and apical membrane addition occurs in ALL of the mutants. Is this because those early steps use other/redundant molecular players, or is depletion too late or incomplete to reveal an early role?

      We agree with Reviewer 3 and will bring up these points in the discussion. Degradation of proteins strongly predicted to function together (RAL-1 and SEC-5; PAR-6 and PKC-3) produce similar although not identical phenotypes; as discussed above we consider it likely that these differences reflect minor differences in degradation efficiency below our ability to detect by fluorescence. As Reviewer 3 points out, the excretory-specific driver we use to express ZIF-1 may not be active at the very earliest stages of lumen formation, and degradation could take 45 minutes or more after the promoter becomes active (Armenti et al, 2014). Thus, we agree that phenotypes could be more severe if it were possible to completely deplete each tagged protein prior to the onset of lumen formation. However, this caveat does not change the interpretations of our experiments since all proteins are degraded with the same driver. We have avoided mentioning that the phenotypes we observe reflect the ‘null’ phenotype for these reasons. We will emphasize these points in the discussion.

      The authors introduce a new reagent, "excP" (the promoter for T28H11.8), which they use to drive canal cell expression of ZIF-1 for their degron experiments. Please provide more information about when in embryogenesis this promoter becomes active, how that compares to when the par genes, sec-5, ral-1 and cdc-42 are first expressed, and what canal length is at that time. It would also be helpful to show the timeframe for degron-based depletion using this reagent (Figure 1C shows only depletion at L4, days later).

      Publicly available single cell RNA seq data (https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/31488706/ and https://cello.shinyapps.io/celegans_explorer/) suggest that canal expression of the endogenous T28H11.8 gene doesn't really ramp up until the 580-650 minute timepoint, which is several hours after par gene canal expression (270-390 minutes) and the initiation of canal lumen formation (bean stage, 400-450 minutes). These data suggest that excP might come on too late to test requirements in lumen formation and early stages of extension. This caveat should be at least mentioned.

      See point #2 above. We agree that providing more information on expression from the T28H11.8 promoter would be important for interpreting the severity of phenotypes. We will raise this point in the discussion, and include existing published expression data and a more detailed analysis of the excP::mCherry transgene.

      3.There are two major aspects to the mutant phenotypes observed here: short lumens and cystic lumens. A short lumen makes sense intuitively, but the cysts could use a little more explanation. (What are cysts? What is thought to be the basis of their formation?). It is intriguing that cysts in sec-5 vs. ral-1 mutants (Figure 1) and par-6 vs. pkc-3 mutants (Figure 4) seem to have a very different size and overall appearance. Are these consistent differences, and if so, what could be the explanation for them?

      This is an interesting point. Since it is not practical to perform time-lapse imaging to watch canal cysts form, we analyzed only L1 and L4 larvae. We believe from our imaging that these are discontinuous regions of the lumen. One explanation for the expansion and dilation of the cystic lumens by L4 stage could be that the canal lumen has been expanded by fluid buildup resulting from a defect in canal function in osmoregulation, but we have not tested this directly. The reviewer also raises an interesting point regarding different appearances of cysts in SEC-5 and RAL-1 depleted larvae compared to PAR-6 and PKC-3. It is possible that these differences arise because SEC-5 and RAL-1 might direct whether vesicles will fuse at all, whereas PAR proteins direct where they will fuse in the cell (i.e. there could be fusion at basal surfaces, or just reduced apical fusion). We will bring up these points in the discussion.

      4.The authors did not test if PKC-3, like PAR-6, is required to recruit exocyst to the canal cell apical membrane, but their prior studies in the embryo suggested that it is (Armenti et al 2014). They also did not test if EXC-5 is required to recruit PAR-6 and the exocyst (along with PKC-3), or if CDC-42 is required to recruit PKC-3 (along with PAR-6). There seems to be an assumption that PAR-6 and PKC-3 are regulated and function in a common manner (as is often the case), but that has not been demonstrated here specifically. The basis for this assumption and alternatives to the linear model should be acknowledged.

      As discussed above (Reviewer 1 point #6), we will directly test whether PKC-3 is required to recruit SEC-10::mCherry to the lumenal membrane. We agree with Reviewer 3 that we have not shown that PAR-6 and PKC-3 always function similarly, although this is expected based on their similar phenotypes and co-dependent functions in other cells. We will mention this caveat in the discussion.

      5.EXC-5 is presumed to act upstream of CDC-42 based on shared phenotypes and the known Rho GEF activity of its mammalian homologs. However, direct evidence for this is currently lacking. In future, the authors might test if depleting EXC-5 affects CDC-42 activation/GTP-loading by using CDC-42 biosensors that have been reported in the literature (e.g. Lazetic et al 2018).

      See Reviewer 1 point #2.

      \*Minor comments:** Figure 1, Figure 4, Figure S3, Figure S4 Blue color/CFP indicates the apical/luminal membrane or the apical region of the canal cytoplasm, not the actual lumen as the labels suggest. The lumen is a hollow cavity on the opposite side of the plasma membrane from these markers, and it is shown as white in the Figure 1A upper right cartoon.*

      Thank you for pointing this out. We will correct the figure labelling.

      Figure 2, Figure S2 I'm not confident in the statistical analysis used here (Fisher's Exact test on two bins, 50% canal length), given that four length bins (not two) were defined. I recommend consulting a statistician.

      Our rationale for using two bins for the statistical analysis was because control larvae nearly all have a similar canal length (L1 stage: 99% of larvae have canal length that is 51-75% of body length; L4 stage: 98% of larvae have canal length that is 76-100% of body length), making it straightforward to ask if mutants are shorter. We chose not to make more granular phenotypic comparisons, as we cannot rule out that subtle differences in degradation efficiency, rather than differences in biological function, underlie any differences in canal length of the degron mutants. We will consult with a statistician to determine if this is an acceptable way to statistically compare controls and mutants.

      p.3 "Born during late embryogenesis..." Actually, the canal cell is born at ~270 minutes after first cleavage, which is in the first half of embryogenesis, not what I would call "late".

      We agree and will correct the wording.

    1. Note: This rebuttal was posted by the corresponding author to Review Commons. Content has not been altered except for formatting.

      Learn more at Review Commons


      Reply to the reviewers

      We are grateful to Review Commons for the opportunity to get valuable comments on our manuscript “Trim39 regulates neuronal apoptosis by acting as a SUMO-targeted E3 ubiquitin-ligase for the transcription factor NFATc3”. We would like to acknowledge the very nice and constructive reviews that our manuscript received. We found all of the reviewer comments well founded and we are taking them into careful consideration in preparing the revised version. We are currently performing additional experiments to address the questions raised by the reviewers. We are not yet able to provide a revised version of the manuscript, but you will find below our response to the reviewers and our plan of revision. It is difficult to anticipate exactly how much time we will need to get the requested results and to prepare a complete revised version, as it will depend on whether we can work normally and whether we encounter technical problems. However, it should be possible within a few months.

      Reviewer #1

      **Summary:**

      Desagher and co-workers investigate the regulation of the NFAT family member NFATc3, a transcription factor in neurons with a pro-apoptotic role. They identify TRIM39 as a ubiquitin E3 ligase regulating NFATc3. They demonstrate that TRIM39 can bind and ubiquitinate NFATc3 in vitro and in cells. They identify a critical SUMO interaction motif in TRIM39, that is required for its interaction with NFATc3 and for its ability to ubiquitinate NFATc3. Moreover, mutating sumoylation sites in NFATc3 reduces the interaction with TRIM39 and reduces its ubiquitination. Silencing TRIM39 increases the protein levels of NFATc3 and its transcriptional activity, leading to apoptosis of neurons. TRIM17 modulates the TRIM39-NFATc3 axis. Combined, TRIM39 appears to be a SUMO-targeted ubiquitin ligase (STUbL) for NFATc3 in neurons.

      **Major points:**

      1.This manuscript containing two stories: the rather exciting story that TRIM39 is a STUbL for NFATc3 (as mentioned in the title) and the second less exciting story: TRIM17 modulates the regulation of NFATc3 by TRIM39. These stories are now mixed in a confusing manner, disrupting the flow of the first story. It would be better to focus the current manuscript on the first story and strengthen it further and develop the second story in a second manuscript.

      We understand that the reviewer is more interested in the part of our manuscript related to the characterization of Trim39 as a STUbL due to his/her field of expertise. However, the two other reviewers are also interested in the other parts of our work. Notably the third reviewer would like us to highlight the physiological importance of our findings. Indeed, the main goal of this article is to describe the mechanisms regulating the stability of the transcription factor NFATc3. Trim17 plays a role in this regulation by inhibiting Trim39. It is particularly important for understanding the impact of these mechanisms on neuronal apoptosis as Trim17 is induced in these conditions. As we want to reach a wide audience, we prefer not to focus our manuscript on the identification of a new STUbL. However, we agree with the reviewer that it would be very interesting to strengthen this part of our work and we are grateful for his/her suggestions.

      2.Whereas the cellular experiments to indicate that TRIM39 acts as a STUbL are properly carried out, the observed effects are not necessarily direct. Direct evidence that TRIM39 is indeed a STUbL for sumoylated NFATc3 needs to be obtained in vitro, using purified recombinant proteins. Does TRIM39 indeed preferentially ubiquitinate sumoylated NFATc3? Is ubiquitination reduced for non-sumoylated NFATc3? Is ubiquitination of sumoylated NFATc3 dependent on SIM3 of TRIM39? Do other SIMs in TRIM39 contribute?

      We agree with the reviewer that additional in vitro experiments using purified recombinant proteins would strengthen the characterization of Trim39 as a STUbL. In order to answer the specific questions of the reviewer, we propose to perform in vitro ubiquitination using different forms of GST-Trim39 (WT/mSIM3/mSIM1&2) following in vitro SUMOylation (or not) of NFATc3 produced by TnT (wheat germ) and purified by immunoprecipitation. Preliminary results using WT Trim39 show that indeed the in vitro ubiquitination of NFATc3 is improved by prior in vitro SUMOylation. We have to confirm these results and to test the SIM mutants of Trim39 in the same conditions.

      3.Rule out potential roles for other STUbLs by including control knockdowns of RNF4 and RNF111 and verify the sumoylation of NFATc3 and ubiquitination of wildtype and sumoylation-mutant NFATc3.

      Our data show that silencing of Trim39 deeply decreases the ubiquitination level of NFATc3 in Neuro2A cells, indicating that Trim39 plays a major role in this process. We agree that this does not exclude the possible involvement of other STUbLs in NFATc3 ubiquitination in this model but their potential contribution would be limited. This point will be better addressed in the discussion.

      4.Figure 6B: use SUMO inhibitor ML-792 to demonstrate that ubiquitination of wildtype NFATc3 by TRIM39 is dependent on sumoylation.

      We thank the reviewer for suggesting this experiments that can easily improve the strength of our demonstration. Our preliminary results indeed indicate that in vivo ubiquitination of NFATc3 by Trim39 is strongly decreased following treatment with the SUMO inhibitor ML-792. We have to confirm these results.

      **Minor points:**

      5.Figure 1A and B: demonstrate by immunoprecipitation and Western that the endogenous counterparts indeed interact.

      We are currently setting the conditions to immunoprecipitate endogenous NFATc3 and Trim39 in order to demonstrate that they indeed interact.

      6.Figure 1C and 1E: Quantify the PLA results properly and perform statistics.

      We will perform these quantification and statistical analysis as requested.

      7.Figure 2B: Correct unequal loading of samples.

      We agree with the reviewer (as with reviewer #2) that the blots showing the total lysates of this experiment are confusing. As mentioned in the legend, some material has been lost during the TCA precipitation resulting in unequal loading. However, these experiments have been performed a very long time ago and we do not have the protein extracts anymore. We are currently trying to produce efficient shRNA-expressing lentiviruses to reproduce this experiment and provide a better figure.

      8.Figure 6B: proper statistics are needed here from at least three independent experiments.

      The reviewer is right. Statistics are needed to reinforce the significance of these results. We have quantified three independent experiments and made graphs and statistics that will be presented in the revised version of the manuscript. They better support our conclusion.

      Reviewer #1 (Significance (Required)):

      Humans have over 600 different ubiquitin E3s. Currently, RNF4 and RNF111 are the only known human SUMO-Targeted Ubiquitin Ligases (STUbLs). Here, the authors present evidence that the ubiquitin E3 ligase TRIM39 is a STUbL for sumoylated NFATc3. Identification of a new STUbL is an exciting finding for the ubiquitin and SUMO field and for the field of ubiquitin-like signal transduction in general, but needs to be strengthened as outlined above. My field of expertise is SUMO and ubiquitin signal transduction.

      Reviewer #2

      In this manuscript, the authors analyze the effect of TRIM39, a ubiquitin E3 ligase, on NFATc3, a transcription factor that regulates apoptosis in the nervous system. The authors show that TRIM39 can promote the ubiquitination of NFATc3 and regulate its half-life. Furthermore, ubiquitination depends on the SUMOylation state of NFATc3, which suggests that TRIM39 could be a new example of SUMOylation-dependent ubiquitin ligase or STUbL. **In addition, the authors show that TRIM17 interferes with TRIM39 ubiquitination, representing a new regulatory level for NFATc3 degradation. This has consequences on the regulation of apoptosis in cells derived from the nervous system.

      The authors show well-controlled, sound results for the most part. The manuscript is well written, and argumentation is convincing. Given the fact that only 2 STUbLs were previously characterized in mammals, the results are relevant and represent an advance in the field. Overall, this is a nice piece of work. Here are some comments.

      **Major comments**

      -In Fig. 2B, the levels of material loaded are uneven, which difficult the interpretation.

      We agree with the reviewer (as with reviewer #1) that the blots showing the total lysates of this experiment are confusing. As mentioned in the legend, some material has been lost during the TCA precipitation, resulting in unequal loading. In the other experiments, we have the same problem or the background is too high. We are currently trying to produce efficient shRNA-expressing lentiviruses to reproduce this experiment and provide a better figure.

      However, it seems that the control shRNA also has an effect on NFATc3 ubiquitination, which should not be the case.

      It is true that, in the present figure, the ubiquitination signal is decreased in cells transduced with the control shRNA. However, this is likely due to reduced expression of transfected NFATc3 following lentiviral infection, as it can be seen on the western blot of total lysates.

      Also, by reducing ubiquitination by TRIM39, shouldn't you expect an increase in the levels of NFATc3, if this ubiquitination was driving degradation? The authors do not specify whether those cells were treated or not with proteasomal inhibitor.

      We agree that an increase in the protein level of NFATc3 is expected following silencing of Trim39. However, in the assay presented in Figure 2B, NFATc3 is transfected and the part of overexpressed NFATc3 that is ubiquitinated by endogenous Trim39 is certainly low. Therefore, silencing of Trim39 cannot have a visible impact on the total protein level of NFATc3.

      Indeed, cells were treated with proteasome inhibitor. It is mentioned in the legend of Figure 2A. To avoid repeating it in the legend of Figure 2B, we just wrote that, after 24h transfection, cells were treated as in A, with includes MG-132 treatment for 6h.

      Same applies in Figure 4B, where no reduction in NFATc3 are seen after including TRIM39 in the reaction (beyond the fact that it looks reduced because the presence of ubiquitinated forms).

      In Figure 4B, the reaction of ubiquitination is performed in an acellular medium with purified recombinant proteins. Although NFATc3 is produced by in vitro transcription/translation in wheat germ extract, it is purified by immunoprecipitation before in vitro ubiquitination. Therefore, the reaction does not contain any proteasome and NFATc3 should not be degraded following its ubiquitination by TRIM39.

      -After the experiments in vitro shown in Fig. 2C, the authors conclude that the NFATc3 is a direct substrate of TRIM39. I think the authors used the right approach by using bacterially produced GST-TRIM39 for the ubiquitination reaction. However NFATc3 is produced by an in vitro transcription-translation system, which could in principle provide other contaminant proteins to the reaction. Did the authors try to use bacterially produced NFATc3? This might be difficult in the case of big proteins, in which case the authors could add some caution note in the text. Same applies in Figure 4B.

      The reviewer is right. It would have been preferable to use NFATc3 produced in bacteria. Indeed, we started with this approach. However, it was very difficult to get NFATc3 expressed in bacteria, and when we succeeded, most of the protein was degraded. We tried different protease inhibitor cocktails and we used a strain of bacteria (BL21-CodonPlus(DE3)-RP) that is mutated on the genes coding for the proteases Lon and OmpT and is further engineered to express tRNAs that are often limiting when expressing mammalian proteins. Unfortunately, this did not improve our production enough.

      We agree that, in principle, in vitro transcription-translation (TnT) systems can include contaminant proteins. However, we used wheat germ extract to produce NFATc3 by TnT. Moreover, we immunopurified NFATc3 from the TnT reaction prior to the ubiquitination reaction. The probability that proteins modifying NFATc3 are expressed in plants and are co-purified with NFATc3 is low. Nevertheless, we will discuss this point in the result section of the revised version of the manuscript, when describing results of Figure 2B and 4B.

      -In Fig. 6B, higher levels of ubiquitination in the different SUMOylation mutants are shown. Is this effect consistent? How this can be explained?

      We are grateful to the reviewer for pointing out this inconsistency in our manuscript. It will be corrected. Indeed, the values indicated in red in Figure 6B are confusing and are certainly not consistent. We calculated them by normalizing the intensity of the ubiquitination signal by the intensity of NFATc3 in total lysates, which seems to have introduced a bias. Variations in NFATc3 levels are probably responsible for the artificially higher levels of ubiquitination for different SUMOylation mutants after normalization. When quantifying three independent experiments, as requested by reviewer #1, we realized that results are much more consistent without normalization. Therefore, in the revised version of manuscript, we will add a graph showing the average and standard deviation of three independent experiments quantified without normalization. We will also replace the experiment currently presented in Figure 6B by another one in which the levels of NFATc3 show lower variations in the total lysates.

      In addition, variations in the levels of NFATc3 are shown in the total lysate, despite the use of proteasomal inhibitors. How the author explain this effect?

      These variations in NFATc3 levels in the total lysates may be due to differential protein precipitation by TCA. That is why, in more recent experiments, we collected a portion of the homogenous cell suspension before lysis in the guanidinium buffer, to assess the expression level of transfected proteins (as presented in Figures 4A and 7E).

      It is true that treatment with proteasome inhibitor should attenuate differences in protein level due to different ubiquitination levels. However, cells are transfected for 24h and then treated with MG-132 for 6h before lysis. Proteasome inhibition cannot compensate for what occurred in the cells during the 24h transfection. It is added essentially to accumulate poly-ubiquitinated forms of NFATc3.

      Somehow, this is contradictory with the general message of SUMOylation-dependent ubiquitination.

      The reduced levels of SUMOylation mutants in total lysates may appear to be contradictory with SUMOylation-dependent ubiquitination. However, as mentioned above, this could be due to differential protein precipitation by TCA or to different transfection efficiencies. In contrast, the half-life measurement of WT and EallA mutant, that does not rely on initial expression levels, clearly shows a stabilization of the SUMOylation mutant. Moreover, the average of the three ubiquitination experiments is really convincing. Therefore, we believe that the data that will be presented in the revised manuscript will strongly support our hypothesis.

      -In Fig. 7E, not clear to me what the big bands above 130 KDa is after the nickel beads. Do they correspond to monoUb NFATc3 or to the unmodified protein that is sticky to the beads? Do the authors have side-by-side gels of the initial lysate next to the nickel beads eluates to show the increase in molecular weight?

      The big bands above 130 kD among nickel bead-purified proteins in Figure 7A are unlikely to be unmodified NFATc3 sticking to the beads. Indeed, in the control condition, in which NFATc3 is overexpressed in the absence of His-ubiquitin, these bands are not visible. Therefore, they might be mono-ubiquitinated forms of NFATc3, or degradation products of poly-ubiquitinated NFATc3. We will correct the figure to clarify this point. Unfortunately, we do not have a gel with nickel bead eluates and total lysates side by side for this experiment.

      -Quantifications in some pictures (i.e. Figures 5A, 5B, 6B, 7) is shown in red above or below the bands. Not clear whether the quantifications shown correspond to that single experiment or is the average of several experiments. In the first case, the number would not be very valuable. Authors could add quantification graphs with standard deviations or error bars to the experiments if they want to make the point of changes (significant or not) in the levels. Alternatively, indicate in the Figure legends whether the numbers correspond to the average of several experiments.

      These quantifications correspond to the representative experiments shown in the different figures. We will clarify this point in the Figure legends of the revised manuscript. We added these quantifications to normalize the amount of co-precipitated proteins by the amount of the precipitated partner (Fig 5A, 5B, 7B, 7C, 7D) which is not always precipitated with the same efficiency in the different conditions. We think that it should help the reader to assess the degree of interaction. We also added quantifications to Figure 7E to normalize the ubiquitination signal by the amount of NFATc3 expressed in the total lysate. However, we did not want to overload the figures by adding too many graphs.

      For Figure 6B, where TCA precipitation of total lysates created an inconsistency, we will provide a graph with the average and standard deviation of three independent experiments, as requested by reviewer #1.

      -In Fig. 8, the quantification of apoptotic nuclei has been done just based on the morphology after DAPI staining. Could you use an apoptosis marker (i.e. cleaved caspase Abs) to label the apoptotic cells?

      We have been using primary cultures of cerebellar granule neurons (CGN) as an in vitro model of neuronal apoptosis for many years. Nuclear condensation, visualized after DAPI staining, is very characteristic in these neurons and allows a reliable assessment of neuronal apoptosis. In a previous study (Desagher et al. JBC 2005), we have shown that the kinetics of apoptosis in CGN is the same whether we measure cytochrome c release, active caspase 3 or nuclear condensation (Fig 1b). We therefore believe that the counting of apoptotic nuclei is sufficient to support our conclusions, notably for transfection experiments in Figure 8A which would require a lot of work to be repeated with active caspase 3 staining. However, if we can produce efficient shRNA-expressing lentiviruses, we will reproduce the experiment presented in Figure 8B and we will perform a western blot using anti-active caspase 3 to confirm our conclusion.

      **Minor comments**

      -In Figs. 1 and 5, the red channel should be put in black and white, as it is much easier to see the signal. Not relevant to have DAPI alone in B&W (it does not hurt either), as it is well visible in the merge picture. Also, quantification of the PLA positive dots should be shown in Fig. 1.

      We thank the reviewer for these suggestions. We will modify the figures and we will quantify the PLA dots in Figure 1 as requested.

      -In Fig. 3C, is the difference in TRIM17 expression between empty plasmid and NFATc3 plasmid significant? If so, indicate it in the graph. The same in panels D, E, indicate all significant differences. Same in other Figures.

      No, the difference in Trim17 expression is not statistically significant between NFATc3 and empty plasmid although it clearly increases. However, we agree with the reviewer that more significant differences could be shown in the figures, particularly in Figure 3. Nonetheless, we will try not to overload the figures and will restrict ourselves to comparisons that make sense.

      -It would be nice to show a scheme on the location of SIMs in TRIM39 in relation to the other feature of the protein.

      We are grateful to the reviewer for this suggestion. We will be happy to add a scheme of Trim39 showing its different domains and the location of its SIMs in the revised Figure 7.

      -In Fig. 2 legend, "Note that in the presence of ubiquitin the unmodified form of WT GST-Trim39 is lower due to high Trim39 ubiquitination." Please change to "...in the presence of ubiquitin the levels of the unmodified form..."

      -In Fig. 7 legend, the phrases "The intensity of the bands ... " are not clear. Please rephrase.

      -In Fig. 8 legend, "\** * PWe thank the reviewer for pointing out typographical errors and awkward sentences in our manuscript. Changes will be made as requested.

      Reviewer #2 (Significance (Required)):

      In this manuscript, the authors analyze the effect of TRIM39, a ubiquitin E3 ligase, on NFATc3, a transcription factor that regulates apoptosis in the nervous system. The authors show that TRIM39 can promote the ubiquitination of NFATc3 and regulate its half-life. Furthermore, ubiquitination depends on the SUMOylation state of NFATc3, which suggests that TRIM39 could be a new example of SUMOylation-dependent ubiquitin ligase or STUbL. In addition, the authors show that TRIM17 interferes with TRIM39 ubiquitination, representing a new regulatory level for NFATc3 degradation. This has consequences on the regulation of apoptosis in cells derived from the nervous system.

      The authors show well-controlled, sound results for the most part. The manuscript is well written, and argumentation is convincing. Given the fact that only 2 STUbLs were previously characterized in mammals, the results are relevant and represent an advance in the field. Overall, this is a nice piece of work.

      Audience: researchers interested on proteostasis in general and on nervous system regulation

      My expertise: postranslational modifications

      Reviewer #3

      **Summary:**

      In this study, Shrivastava et al. elucidated the previously unknown function of TRIM39 in regulating protein stability of NFATc3, the predominant member of the NFAT family of transcription factor in neurons, where it plays a pro-apoptotic role. NFATs have been shown to be regulated by multiple mechanisms, including at the level of protein stability. In this study, the authors identify TRIM39 as the E3 ligase for NFATc3. Interestingly, TRIM39 recognizes the SUMOylated form of NFATc3 and the interaction facilitates its ubiquitylation and subsequent proteasomal degradation. They further showed that binding of TRIM39 to NFATc3 can also be regulated by TRIM17. Like TRIM39, TRIM17 is a ring-finger containing protein previously shown by this group that it binds NFATc3 but the interaction resulted in an up- rather than down-regulation of NFATc3. In this study, they offer insight to the paradox that overexpression of TRIM17 binding to TRIM39 is to inhibit TRIM39-mediated ubiquitylation of NFATc3. Furthermore, they showed activation of NFATc3 transcriptionally activates TRIM17 expression, thus forming a feedback loop between NFATc3 and TRIM17. Hence, an TRIM17-TRIM39-NFATc3 signaling axis for modulating the protein stability for promoting the activity of NFATc3 in regulating apoptosis in the cerebellar granule neurons induced by KCl deprivation is proposed

      The key conclusions are convincing. The data in general are of good quality and with many of the key interactions vigorously documented **by conducting reciprocal interaction analysis. For knockdown expeRIMents, two shRNA independent sequences were used. However, some issues remain to be addressed:

      **Major comments:**

      1.Figure 1D - the authors should demonstrate that the depletion of TRIM39 expression by shRNA in Neuro2A by Western blotting

      We agree with the reviewer that it would be better to provide this control. Unfortunately, we have never been able to observe a convincing decrease in the protein level of Trim39, following knockdown, by Western blotting in Neuro2A cells. This is surprising because the decrease is clearly visible by immunofluorescence in Neuro2A cells, and by western blotting in neurons (see Figure 8C). It is possible that Neuro2A cells, but not neurons, express a protein that is non-specifically recognized by our best anti-Trim39 antibody in western blots and that migrates at the same size as Trim39, thus preventing the investigator to detect the depletion of Trim39. We will test additional anti-Trim39 antibodies to address this question.

      2.Figure 3 - the author should show overexpression of TRIM39 resulted in reduction of basal level of endogenous NFATc3 due to its effect on protein stability by using CHX or other pulse chase method.

      This is an important point and we have performed many experiments using cycloheximide to measure the half-life of NFATc3 in the presence or the absence of overexpressed Trim39. The results were neither consistent nor reproducible. This is certainly due to the fact that the half-life of endogenous NFATc3 is longer than that of overexpressed Trim39 and that cycloheximide inhibits the expression of both proteins. Therefore, we will perform pulse-chase experiments after metabolic labelling of cells with [35S]-Met. We are currently setting up the conditions to immunoprecipitate endogenous NFATc3 to be able to perform these experiments.

      3.Figure 3 - Does overexpression or knockdown of TRIM39 has an effect on affecting levels of NFATc3 mRNAs?

      The reviewer is right. It is important to control that overexpression and knockdown of Trim39 do not modify the mRNA level of NFATc3. Therefore, we are currently measuring NFATc3 mRNA levels in all the experiments used to make the graphs of Figure 3. These results will be added to the revised version of the manuscript as supplemental data. First results show no significant change of NFATc3 mRNA levels in these experiments.

      4.Figure 6A - the authors should confirm the multiple bands that are slower migrating are SUMO form of NFATc9 by demonstrating the presence of SUMO in these forms of NFATc3, or alternatively, perform His-SUMO pull-down and probe for NFATc3.

      The reactions shown in Figure 6B have been performed in vitro, with purified recombinant proteins and with NFATc3 produced by in vitro transcription/translation. The wheat germ extract used to produce NFATc3 is unlikely to provide the material needed for post-translation modification of a mammalian protein. However, we agree that it would be better to confirm that slower migrating bands are indeed SUMOylated forms of NFATc3. We may hybridize the membranes with an anti-SUMO antibody but it would give a smear as the enzymes added to the reaction mix are themselves SUMOylated. Therefore, we will show an experiment in which the reaction mix has been incubated with and without SUMO. The results show no slower migrating bands in the absence of SUMO although all conditions were otherwise identical. This will be added to the revised Figure 6.

      5.Figure 7C - the quantification for mSIM1 does not seem to agree with the band intensity.

      Yes, we agree with the reviewer that the quantification (122%) does not seem to reflect the amount of SUMO-chains bound to GST-Trim39 mSIM1. This is due to the normalization of the SUMO signals by the intensity of GST-Trim39 bands. Indeed, it is difficult to control exactly how much recombinant protein is used. GST-Trim39 mSIM1 was slightly less abundant than the other GST-Trim39 proteins in this experiment, explaining why less SUMO-chains were eluted in this condition. The normalization is mentioned in the legend of Figure 7C.

      6.TRIM17 reduces TRIM39/NFATc3 interaction and inhibits TRIM39 E3 activity, which results in stabilization of NFATc3. NFATc3 in turn transcriptionally induces TRIM17 expression, thus forming a feedback loop between NFATc3 and TRIM17. It will be good if the authors can discuss the possibility of the existence of this feedback mechanism in physiological context? Is the protein level of NFATc3 level, which should be low abundance at the resting state, elevated by KCI deprivation? If so, can the authors discuss the possible signalling event(s) that that lead to activation of NFATc3 upon KCI deprivation? For instance, does KCL deprivation cause de-SUMOylation of NFATc3?

      We thank the reviewer for these suggestions. Our preliminary results suggest that the protein level of NFATc3 is increased in neurons following KCl deprivation. We are currently performing additional experiments to confirm this result. If proved, this increase may be due to the transcriptional induction of Trim17 that should result in the stabilization of NFATc3 through the inhibition of Trim39. It may also be due to a possible deSUMOylation of NFATc3 following apoptosis induction, as suggested by the reviewer. To address the latter point, we are currently setting up PLA using anti-NFATc3 and anti-SUMO antibodies to assess the SUMOylation level of endogenous NFATc3 in neurons. If they are of good quality, we will add these data to Figure 8 and we will discuss the possible existence of feedback loops in neuronal apoptosis, as suggested by the reviewer.

      **Minor comments:**

      1.Line 294 - it should be "SUMOylation" instead of "SUMO".

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out this typographical error that will be corrected.

      2.Figure 8 - to include TRIM39/NFATc3 double knockdown to show the effect on increased neuronal apoptosis in the cells with TRIM39 knocked down was due to elevation of NFATc3 rather than other target(s) of TRIM39.

      We agree that it would be interesting to test whether the increase on neuronal apoptosis following Trim39 silencing is mainly due to its effect on NFATc3. We will therefore perform double silencing of Trim39 and NFATc3 in neurons in order to address this point.

      3.The discussion may be shortened and revised to highlight the physiological importance of the findings linked to cerebellar granule neurons survival.

      As suggested by the reviewer, we will modify the discussion to better highlight the physiological implications of our data, particularly by discussing the results of the additional experiments we will conduct in neurons.

      Reviewer #3 (Significance (Required)):

      Prior to this study, the mechanism by which protein stability of NFATc3, the pre-dominant member of the NFAT family of transcription factor in neurons, is regulated remains poorly understood. Shrivastava et al. have unravelled the interplay between ubiquitylation and SUMOylation involving TRIM39 and TRIM17 to have an important role in regulating protein stability of NFATc3. The work is interesting and bears significance towards understanding how apoptosis could be finely controlled in cerebellar granule neurons. Furthermore, the study has also expanded the understanding of the role and regulation of the TRIM family of proteins. The senior author is an expert in this field and over the years, her group has contributed many key discoveries on the function of TRIM family of E3 ubiquitin ligases and their critical ubiquitylation substrates in neuronal survival and its relevance to neuronal biology and diseases.

      The referee's field of expertise in in the field of mitochondrial apoptosis signalling. The referee extensively involved in studying how protein stability of regulators in apoptosis signalling are regulated by the ubiquitin-proteasome system (UPS) and how does the regulation play a role in physiology and diseases.

      Key words: apoptosis, ubiquitylation, cell signaling, liver diseases

    1. Note: This rebuttal was posted by the corresponding author to Review Commons. Content has not been altered except for formatting.

      Learn more at Review Commons


      Reply to the reviewers

      Reviewer #1 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity):

      **A. Summary:**

      In this modeling study, the authors devised a multicellular model to investigate how circadian clocks in different parts (organs) of plants coordinate their timing. The model uses a plausible mechanism to explain how having a different sensitivity to light leads to different phase and period of circadian clock, which is observed in different plant organs. The model allows for entrainment in Light-Dark (LD) cycles and then a release in always-light (LL) environments. The model disentangles numerous factors that have confounded previous experiments. In one instance, the authors assigned different light sensitivities to the different organs (e.g., root tip, hypocotyl, etc.) which unambiguously show that this one element alone - spatially differing sensitivity to light - is sufficient for recapitulating experimentally observed differences in periods and phases between plant organs. The model also recapitulates the spatial waves of gene expression within and between organs that experimentalists reported. At the sub-tissue level, the model-produced waves have similar patterns as the experimentally observed waves. This confirmation further validates the model. By having the cells share clock mRNA, from any clock component genes, showed the same, experimentally observed spatial dynamics. The main conclusion of the study is that regional differences (e.g., between different organs) in light senilities, when combined with cell-to-cell sharing of clock-gene mRNAs, enables a robust, yet flexible, circadian timing under noisy environmental cycles.

      Thank you for your assessment of our work. We plan to make the following revisions based on your feedback.

      **B. Specific points:**

      1.Lines 125-127: "To simulate the variability observed in single cell clock rhythms, we multiplied the level of each mRNA and protein by a time scaling parameter that was randomly selected from a normal distribution." - Why not add a white (Gaussian) noise term to these equations? How does multiplying by a random variable (for rescaling time) different from my proposal? Some explanation should be given in the text here.

      We opted for a time scaling approach as this generates between-cell period differences but avoids within-cell period differences. This is consistent with single cell experiments (S1 Fig; Gould et al., 2018, eLife). We will provide an explanation of this in the text.

      2.Does the spatial network model simplify calculations by assuming separations of timescales (e.g., for equilibration in concentrations of mRNAs that diffuse between cells)? If so, it would be good to spell these out in the beginning of the Results section (where the model is described).

      We agree that a more detailed discussion of the model assumptions would be beneficial and we will provide this in the text.

      3.Lines 161-162: "....in a phase only model by local...." should be "....in a phase model only by local...."

      Thank you for your correction.

      4.Lines 188-190: The authors observed that qualitatively similar/indistinguishable behaviors arose regardless of which elements are varied (e.g., global versus local cell-cell coupling, setting light input to be equal in all regions of the seedling, etc.). Then they claim here that "...these results show that the assumptions of local cell-to-cell coupling and differential light sensitivity between regions are the key aspects of our model that allow a match to experimental data." - I don't see how this follows from the observation almost any of the variations lead to the same behaviors in this section (spatial waves). Show the reasoning in the text here.

      We observed spatial waves with different local coupling regimes (4 v. 8 nearest neighbours). However, we did not observe spatial waves with global coupling (S10 Fig). This led us to conclude that local coupling is a key aspect. In addition, we do not observe waves when setting the light input to be equal in all regions of the seedling (S11 Fig). This confirms that local differences in light sensitivity are also required in our simulations to generate spatial waves. We will clarify these points with revisions to the text.

      5.Pgs. 9-10: Section on "Cell-to-cell coupling maintains global coordination under noisy light-dark cycles": The simulation results rigorously support the authors' main conclusion here, which is that local cell-to-cell coupling allows for global coordination under noisy LD cycles. But I'm missing an intuitive explanation (or just any explanation) for why this is. At the end of this section, the authors should provide some intuition or qualitative explanation for the observations that they produced using their model in this section.

      We will revise the text to provide an intuitive explanation of these results. The coupling decreases the within-region phase differences. Despite the between-regions phase differences persisting, this effect is sufficient to improve the overall global synchrony.

      6.Lines 261-262: Replace the present tenses with past tenses.

      Thank you for your correction.

      7.Is the main idea that cell-to-cell coupling allows for averaging of fluctuations, between organs or cells within the same organ, while allowing for coordination of the average quantities? Is this responsible for both the flexibility and robustness observed under noisy environmental cycles?

      The cell-to-cell-coupling allows for the averaging of fluctuations between cells and the regional flexibility arises from the different light sensitivities in each region. What was interesting to us was that under light-dark cycles the regional flexibility was not lost due to either the noise in the light or the averaging effect of the cell-to-cell coupling. We will revise the text to emphasize these points. Thank you for your prompts.

      8.Line 304: Is it really true that the mammalian circadian rhythm is centralized? Don't some parts of our bodies have different circadian clock (e.g., slight differences in phase) than some other parts of our bodies?

      There are indeed some small phase differences between parts of our bodies because the mammalian system, like the plant system, is imperfectly coupled. However, the mammalian system is considered more centralized because the suprachiasmatic nucleus in the brain receives the key entraining signal of light and then coordinates rhythms across the body (Bell-Pedersen et al., 2005, Nat Rev Gen; Brown & Azzi, 2013, Circadian Clocks). We will expand on these interesting points by adding a paragraph to the discussion.

      Reviewer #1 (Significance):

      **Overall assessment:**

      I enthusiastically recommend this work for publication after the authors address my comments below (please see "Specific points").

      The model's main strength is that the authors could vary each ingredient separately - light sensitivity of each cell/organ, which gene's mRNA diffuses between cells, cellular noise, local versus global cell-cell coupling, etc. Afterwards, the authors could determine which of these variations produces which experimentally observed behaviors. Another strength of the model is that it can reproduce not just one, but numerous, experimentally observed behaviors that are important for understanding circadian clocks in plants. Thus, the model is grounded in experimental truth and produces experimentally observed results. Crucially, since the authors could vary every single element in the model independently of the other elements, the authors are able to provide plausible explanations for why the experiments produced the results that they did (experimentally, a number of confounding factors prevented one from pinpointing to which element produced which observation).

      Another strength of the model is also extendable, by other researchers to investigate other plant physiologies in the future (e.g., circadian clock's influence on cell division). The authors highlight these future uses in the discussion section. Therefore, I believe that this work will be valuable to plant biologists, non-plant biologists who are interested in circadian clocks, and systems biologists in general.

      The manuscript is also well written and relatively easy to follow, even for non-plant biologists like myself.

      Thank you for the positive feedback - we are pleased that you find the manuscript of broad interest to a range of readers.

      Comment on Reviewer #2:

      I agree with his/her major criticism #3 (ELF4 long-distance movement). I find this to be a reasonable request. Fulfilling it would increase the paper's impact.

      Please see our response to reviewer #2.

      Comment on Reviewer #3:

      The reviewer's point (1) asks for a reasonable request.

      Regarding his/her point (2): This is also reasonable. I'd recommend his/her suggestion (a). In the end, I'd be interested to see how the authors respond to this (what function they choose to let adjacent cells be subjected to some correlated light-input intensity. I'd be happy with something simple such as + noise, where is a deterministic term that, for example, decreases exponentially as one moves away from some central cell. Basically, I'd let the authors decide how to implement this and accept their current implementation - no correlation in light-intensity between adjacent cells - as an extreme scenario, as this reviewer points out.

      Please see our response to reviewer #3.

      Reviewer #2 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity):

      **Summary:**

      The manuscript presents an improved model of the circadian clock network that accounts for tissue-specific clock behavior, spatial differences in light sensitivity, and local coupling achieved through intercellular sharing of mRNA. In contrast to whole-plant or "phase-only" models, the authors' approach enables them to address the mechanism behind coupling and how the clock maintains regional synchrony in a noisy environment. Using 34 parameters to describe clock activity and applying the properties mentioned above, the authors demonstrate that their model can recapitulate the spatial waves in circadian gene expression observed and can simulate how the plant maintains local synchrony with regional differences in rhythms under noisy LD cycles. Spatial models that incorporate cell-type-specific sensitivities to environmental inputs and local coupling mechanisms will be most accurate for simulating clock activity under natural environments.

      Thank you for your assessment of our work. We plan to make the following revisions based on your feedback.

      *We have the following **major criticisms** as follows*

      1) When assigning light sensitivities in different regions of the plant, the authors assign a higher sensitivity value to the root tip (L=1.03) than they do to the other part of the root (L=0.90). We are curious why the root tip would have higher light sensitivity than the rest of the root. Is this based on experimental data (if so, please cite in this section or methods)? It seems that these L values were assigned simply to make sure they recapitulated the period differences observed in Fig. 2A. Are these values based on PhyB expression in those organs? Or perhaps based on cell density in those locations?

      We assign the light sensitivity to match observed experimental period differences across the plant (Fig 2A,B). This is based on previous experiments demonstrating that experimental period differences are dependent on light input through the light sensing gene PHYB (Greenwood et al., 2019, PLoS Bio; Nimmo et al., 2020, Physiologia Plantarum). For example, in WT seedlings, the root tip oscillates faster than the root, but this difference is lost in the phyb-9 mutant (Greenwood et al., 2019). Thus, we assume the root tip to be more sensitive to light than the roots.

      Further supporting this assumption, there is evidence that expression of phytochromes and cryptochromes are increased in the root tip relative to the root (e.g., Somers & Quail, 1995, Plant J; Bognar et al., 1999, PNAS; Toth et al., 2001, Plant Physiol), as the reviewer proposes. However, further experiments would be needed to verify that these differences in expression are what lead to the differences in clock timing. We will add a discussion of these experiments to the text.

      2) In the discussion of the test where they set the "light inputs to be equal" in all regions to simulate the phyb-9 mutant, could the authors please clarify whether that means they set the L light sensitivity value equal in all regions?

      This is indeed what we mean, we will rephrase the text for clarity.

      a. If they are referring to setting the L value equal to all regions, we suggest that this discussion be moved to the section about different light sensitivities instead of the local sharing of mRNA section.

      Thank you for your suggestion, we agree and will move this discussion.

      b. Additionally, is it possible to set the light sensitivity to zero for all parts of the plant? We think this would be more suitable to simulate the phyb-9 mutant phenotype.

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. We will include a simulation with light sensitivity set to zero in the revised manuscript, in addition to the existing simulations with light sensitivity set to 1.

      3) Based on the recent Chen et al. (2020) paper showing ELF4 long-distance movement, we think it would be of great interest for the authors to model ELF4 protein synthesis/translation as the coupling factor, in addition to the modeling using CCA1/LHY mRNA sharing. We understand you may be saving this analysis for a future modeling paper, but this addition to the paper could increase the impact of this paper.

      Thank you for the suggestion to improve our manuscript. We agree it will be of interest to model ELF4 protein as the local coupling factor. In the revision, we will simulate each clock protein (including ELF4) as the local coupling factor and compare.

      In addition, we will also modify the coupling mechanism to simulate the long-distance transport of ELF4 proposed by Chen et al., 2020. Our preliminary simulations show that we can couple shoot rhythms to those in the root tip, but that this long range coupling can not on its own generate the spatial structure observed in experiments. We agree with the reviewers that this analysis and an associated discussion will further increase the impact of the paper.

      4) This model is able to simulate circadian rhythms under 12:12 LD cycles, which represents two days of the year-the equinoxes. We are curious if the model can simulate rhythms under short days and long days as well. We understand this analysis may be outside the scope of this paper and may require changing the values of the 34 parameters used but think it could be a useful addition here or in future work.

      We agree it would be interesting to observe the behavior of the model under different day lengths. We will include simulations under short and long days in the revision.

      *And **minor criticisms** as follows*

      1) In the first paragraph of the results section, it would be helpful for the authors to reference Table S1 when they mention the 34 parameters used to model oscillator function

      We agree and we will implement this helpful suggestion.

      2) In the first paragraph of the section titled "Local flexibility persists under idealized and noisy LD cycles", it would be helpful for the authors to reference S12 Fig after the last sentence that starts "However, ELF4/LUX appeared more synchronized..."

      We agree and we will implement this helpful suggestion.

      3) In the first paragraph of the section titled "Cell-to-cell coupling maintains global communication under noisy light-dark cycles", the authors refer to a "Table 1" but I think they mean to refer to Table S1"

      Thank you, we will implement this helpful suggestion.

      4) In Fig. 1, panel C is described as demonstrating the cell-to-cell coupling through the "level of CCA1/LHY". This phrasing is vague and we think could be improved to the "mRNA level of CCA1/LHY".

      We agree and will implement this helpful suggestion.

      Reviewer #2 (Significance (Required)):

      This work would be broadly interesting to other researchers studying cell-to-cell signaling and coupling of circadian rhythms in plants and other species where spatial waves of gene expression have been observed (i.e., mice and humans). Additionally, the computational modeling aspect of this work was easily interpretable for someone outside this expertise. Our expertise lies in plant circadian biology.

      We thank the reviewer for recognising the broad appeal of our work.

      Reviewer #3 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity):

      **Summary:**

      The authors start by taking a previously published model of the plant circadian clock and implement five changes: 1) updating the network topology to reflect some recent experimental findings, 2) make a spatial model loosely based on a seedling template 3) introduce coupling between cells based on shared levels of CCA1/LHY 4) randomly rescale time in each cell to induce inter-cell differences in period, 5) include a light sensitivity that depends on the region considered.

      For a certain configuration of light sensitivities/intensities, the different periods of oscillations in each seedling region roughly match that of experiments. With a sufficiently high coupling between cells, the system can also generate spatial waves, which are also observed in the experimental system.

      With pulsed light inputs the spatial pattern is still produced. The authors then investigate the robustness to environmental noise by generating stochastic light signals and show that the global synchrony, as measured with a synchronisation index, increases with cell-to-cell coupling strength. The paper is overall well-written, and the background and details of the analysis are well presented.

      Thank you for your assessment of our work. We plan to make the following revisions based on your feedback.

      **Major comments:**

      For the first part of paper, the output of the model is certainly the focus. There is virtually no discussion of the inferred parameters and how much confidence the authors have in their values.

      Thank you for this point. We will add discussion of the inferred parameters to the initial part of the results.

      My main issue with the paper is about the section with noisy light signals, which is included in the title and is ultimately one of the main themes of the article.

      Specifically, on line 224:

      "This decrease in cell-to-cell variation revealed an underlying spatial structure (Fig 4D, middle and right, and S13 Fig), comparable to that observed under idealized LD cycles (Fig 4B, middle and right, and S12 Fig)."

      Firstly, I don't feel these conclusions match with the data presented. Comparing figure 4D middle and right with figure 4B middle and right shows a clear and pronounced loss in spatial structure. In its current form, this statement has to change, but I believe there are at least two other major issues with this figure:

      We agree there are some differences in the spatial structure between idealized (Fig 4B) and noisy (Fig 4D) LD cycles. Preliminary simulations suggest that this is due to the way the noisy LD cycles are programmed.

      In the current implementation of noisy LD cycles, the maximum intensity of L, L**max, differs between each region, such that relative differences in light sensitivity between regions are maintained. This means that some phase differences between regions are maintained. However, as the reviewer correctly points out in point 1 below, due to the noise fluctuations, the average level of light is lower than under idealized LD cycles, and with considerable day-to-day variation. We believe this is why the spatial structure differs.

      Preliminary simulations suggest that if we normalize the mean light intensity such that the mean is equal between the two conditions (as the reviewer suggests in point 1 below), the spatial structure appears similar. We will present this analysis in the revision.

      1) The figure is clearly designed to invite a comparison between the noise-free light cycles on the left with the noisy cycles on the right. However, due to how the noisy light is simulated, the variance of light signal increases AND the average intensity of light decreases by 50%. When comparing the left and the right, we therefore don't know whether the changes are due to differences in the average signal or differences from the stochasticity. I think the authors should simulate a noisy light signal with the same mean intensity level as the deterministic signal.

      As discussed above, we agree that the average intensity of the light decreases due to the noise, and this complicates interpretation. We will simulate idealized and noisy light cycles with the same mean light level upon revision.

      2) The noise model for the light doesn't seem realistic. On line 484 is says:

      "We made the simplifying assumption that each cell is exposed to an independent noisy LD cycle due to their unique positions in the environment. LD cycles were input to the molecular model through the parameter L".

      In fact, this could be considered as an incredibly complex signal, because for 800 cells it means drawing 800 random light signals. The implication is that two adjacent cells receive statistically independent light signals. Depending on chance, one cell might receive tropical levels of light while its neighbour experiences a cloudy day. This affects the interpretation and conclusions from figures 4 and 5. I propose two different ways of improving the simulation of the noisy light signal:

      a) In one extreme case, all cells receive the same noisy light signal, and the other extreme, they all receive independent signals. You could consider a mixture model of light signals, where each cell receives \lambda L_global(t) + (1-\lambda) L_individual(t), where L_global(t) is a global light signal that is shared by all cells and L_individual(t) is a light signal unique to an individual cell. The mixing parameter \lambda controls how similar the light signal is between cells

      b) Clearly the light signal will differ depending on the region, but there will be some spatial correlation. You could also consider methods of simulating light such that neighbouring cells receive correlated signals, although this might be difficult.

      Thank you for your proposals. We agree that our current implementation of noisy LD cycles represents an extreme scenario. Given that there is no environmental data at sufficient resolution to reliably evaluate which implementation is most realistic, we will explore different approaches based on your suggestions and present them in our revision.

      Assuming that the problem with the mean signal is corrected, do you expect the average spatial pattern to be the same between figure 4 B and D with no coupling (J=0) (although an increase in the variance between cells)? Perhaps not (owing to nonlinearities in the system), but it would be interesting to comment.

      We agree that the decreased light intensity complicates interpretation of the spatial structure. Although in the current implementation relative light differences between regions are maintained, the spatial structure is altered because the mean intensities are lower. Preliminary simulations with the mean intensity fixed do result in spatial patterns more similar to that seen in Fig 4B, but with increased variance. Comprehensive simulations will be included in the revised manuscript.

      The different periods in the different regions of the seedling are caused by differences in light sensitivity, which the authors claim is justified from refs 12-15. An alternative hypothesis is the that biochemical parameters such as degradation rates are different between regions. This is briefly alluded to in the introduction, but I think it would be interesting to discuss further. What would be the pros and cons of the two different mechanisms?

      We agree that an alternative hypothesis is that biochemical parameters such as degradation rates may differ between regions. Experimental evidence, however, more supports the light sensitivity hypothesis. This is because, for example, mutations in light signalling remove the spatial differences between regions. We agree though that this is an important point, and will add a paragraph to the discussion discussing the pros and cons of the two different mechanisms.

      I understand that the authors used a pre-existing model, but I must say that I find the way that light is incorporated into the model a bit confusing.

      On line 345 it says:

      "L(t) represents the input light signal (L = 0, lights off; L > 0, lights on) and D(t) denotes a corresponding darkness input signal (D = 1, lights off; D = 0, lights on)."

      Surely the only thing that matters biophysically is the number of photons hitting the plant? Could you explain why the model needs to have a separate "darkness signal" compared to just a single light signal?

      A darkness signal has been introduced in many circadian clock models because degradation rates of the clock genes can depend upon the light or dark condition. We agree with the reviewer that we should explain this clearer in the text.

      In the model, the light intensity changes depending on the region. It might make more sense for interpretability if instead there is an additional light-sensitivity coefficient that depends on the region, because at the moment I'm not sure what units L(t) is supposed to take.

      Thank you for your suggestion. We will try to implement this approach.

      **Minor comments**

      Could you more explicitly describe a possible molecular mechanism through which the coupling acts?

      Thank you for your suggestion. We will more explicitly discuss likely transport mechanisms in the text.

      In Figure 1C it looks like different genes are coupling to different genes, so you may need to rearrange it.

      In our model, the level of CCA1/LHY is shared. Thus, CCA1/LHY from one cell can be considered to repress the expression of other interacting genes in the neighbour cell.

      Line 103: "We found that regional differences persist even under LD cycles, but cell to-cell minimized differences between neighbor cells." Missing word.

      Thank you for your correction.

      Line 124: "The coupling strength was set to 2 (Methods)." This is meaningless in isolation, so it would be better to briefly explain what the coupling parameter is before mentioning its value.

      Thank you for your suggestion, we will describe the coupling function in more detail.

      Through the text, I think De Caluwe should be corrected to De Caluwé

      Thank you for your correction.

      Typo line 493

      Thank you for your correction.

      Code and data are not made available.

      Model code will be made available from our project GitLab page: https://gitlab.com/slcu/teamJL/greenwood_tokuda_etal_2020

      Output of analysis of experimental data and simulations will also be made available on the GitLab page.

      Reviewer #3 (Significance (Required)):

      The authors motivate the paper by highlighting that their proposed model improves on phase-based models in that it describes underlying molecular mechanisms.

      From an experimental side, it's interesting that a model is developed and directly compared with measured spatio-temporal waves of gene expression. From a theoretical side, the authors address questions relating to oscillations, multi-scale modelling and noise robustness that also generalise to other systems. I therefore expect that both experimental and theoretical audiences will be interested in the results.

      There are many possible additions and modifications that could be made to the model, and so the model and analysis could provide a platform for future research. However, I can't comment on whether there are similar pre-existing models of the plant circadian clock that contain both a molecular description of the circadian clock as well as a spatial scale.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s view that the work is interesting to both experimental and theoretical audiences.

      Comments on Review #1:

      The time is rescaled in each cell, meaning that each cell has a unique period, but the dynamics remain deterministic and hence the peak-to-peak times will be exactly the same for each cell. I imagine this isn't completely consistent with single-cell data (if available), where peak-to-peak times are very likely to be variable due to noisy gene expression. In a future paper it would be interesting to analyse the system using stochastic differential equations.

      Please see our response to reviewer #1.

      Comments on Review #2:

      I agree on the following two points:

      1) It would add value to discuss whether the different ranking of light sensitivities by organ matches any available experimental data.

      Please see our response to reviewer #2.

      2) As the Reviewers point out, there are many possibilities for testing the robustness of the system to light clues, including varying the length of the day. Although outside of the scope of this paper, I wonder if it's possible to find data from a light sensor measuring light intensity across an entire year? Plugging such data into the model and measuring how the amplitude and period changes would be really interesting, in my opinion.

      Thank you for your suggestion. We also see this as an interesting future direction.

    2. Note: This preprint has been reviewed by subject experts for Review Commons. Content has not been altered except for formatting.

      Learn more at Review Commons


      Referee #3

      Evidence, reproducibility and clarity

      Summary:

      The authors start by taking a previously published model of the plant circadian clock and implement five changes: 1) updating the network topology to reflect some recent experimental findings, 2) make a spatial model loosely based on a seedling template 3) introduce coupling between cells based on shared levels of CCA1/LHY 4) randomly rescale time in each cell to induce inter-cell differences in period, 5) include a light sensitivity that depends on the region considered.

      For a certain configuration of light sensitivities/intensities, the different periods of oscillations in each seedling region roughly match that of experiments. With a sufficiently high coupling between cells, the system can also generate spatial waves, which are also observed in the experimental system.

      With pulsed light inputs the spatial pattern is still produced. The authors then investigate the robustness to environmental noise by generating stochastic light signals and show that the global synchrony, as measured with a synchronisation index, increases with cell-to-cell coupling strength. The paper is overall well-written, and the background and details of the analysis are well presented.

      Major comments:

      For the first part of paper, the output of the model is certainly the focus. There is virtually no discussion of the inferred parameters and how much confidence the authors have in their values.

      My main issue with the paper is about the section with noisy light signals, which is included in the title and is ultimately one of the main themes of the article.

      Specifically, on line 224:

      "This decrease in cell-to-cell variation revealed an underlying spatial structure (Fig 4D, middle and right, and S13 Fig), comparable to that observed under idealized LD cycles (Fig 4B, middle and right, and S12 Fig)."

      Firstly, I don't feel these conclusions match with the data presented. Comparing figure 4D middle and right with figure 4B middle and right shows a clear and pronounced loss in spatial structure. In its current form, this statement has to change, but I believe there are at least two other major issues with this figure:

      1) The figure is clearly designed to invite a comparison between the noise-free light cycles on the left with the noisy cycles on the right. However, due to how the noisy light is simulated, the variance of light signal increases AND the average intensity of light decreases by 50%. When comparing the left and the right, we therefore don't know whether the changes are due to differences in the average signal or differences from the stochasticity. I think the authors should simulate a noisy light signal with the same mean intensity level as the deterministic signal. . 2) The noise model for the light doesn't seem realistic. On line 484 is says:

      "We made the simplifying assumption that each cell is exposed to an independent noisy LD cycle due to their unique positions in the environment. LD cycles were input to the molecular model through the parameter L".

      In fact, this could be considered as an incredibly complex signal, because for 800 cells it means drawing 800 random light signals. The implication is that two adjacent cells receive statistically independent light signals. Depending on chance, one cell might receive tropical levels of light while its neighbour experiences a cloudy day. This affects the interpretation and conclusions from figures 4 and 5. I propose two different ways of improving the simulation of the noisy light signal:

      a) In one extreme case, all cells receive the same noisy light signal, and the other extreme, they all receive independent signals. You could consider a mixture model of light signals, where each cell receives \lambda L_global(t) + (1-\lambda) L_individual(t), where L_global(t) is a global light signal that is shared by all cells and L_individual(t) is a light signal unique to an individual cell. The mixing parameter \lambda controls how similar the light signal is between cells

      b) Clearly the light signal will differ depending on the region, but there will be some spatial correlation. You could also consider methods of simulating light such that neighbouring cells receive correlated signals, although this might be difficult.

      Assuming that the problem with the mean signal is corrected, do you expect the average spatial pattern to be the same between figure 4 B and D with no coupling (J=0) (although an increase in the variance between cells)? Perhaps not (owing to nonlinearities in the system), but it would be interesting to comment.

      The different periods in the different regions of the seedling are caused by differences in light sensitivity, which the authors claim is justified from refs 12-15. An alternative hypothesis is the that biochemical parameters such as degradation rates are different between regions. This is briefly alluded to in the introduction, but I think it would be interesting to discuss further. What would be the pros and cons of the two different mechanisms?

      I understand that the authors used a pre-existing model, but I must say that I find the way that light is incorporated into the model a bit confusing.

      On line 345 it says: "L(t) represents the input light signal (L = 0, lights off; L > 0, lights on) and D(t) denotes a corresponding darkness input signal (D = 1, lights off; D = 0, lights on)."

      Surely the only thing that matters biophysically is the number of photons hitting the plant? Could you explain why the model needs to have a separate "darkness signal" compared to just a single light signal?

      In the model, the light intensity changes depending on the region. It might make more sense for interpretability if instead there is an additional light-sensitivity coefficient that depends on the region, because at the moment I'm not sure what units L(t) is supposed to take.

      Minor comments

      Could you more explicitly describe a possible molecular mechanism through which the coupling acts?

      In Figure 1C it looks like different genes are coupling to different genes, so you may need to rearrange it.

      Line 103: "We found that regional differences persist even under LD cycles, but cell to-cell minimized differences between neighbor cells." Missing word.

      Line 124: "The coupling strength was set to 2 (Methods)." This is meaningless in isolation, so it would be better to briefly explain what the coupling parameter is before mentioning its value.

      Through the text, I think De Caluwe should be corrected to De Caluwé

      Typo line 493

      Code and data are not made available.

      Significance

      The authors motivate the paper by highlighting that their proposed model improves on phase-based models in that it describes underlying molecular mechanisms.

      From an experimental side, it's interesting that a model is developed and directly compared with measured spatio-temporal waves of gene expression. From a theoretical side, the authors address questions relating to oscillations, multi-scale modelling and noise robustness that also generalise to other systems. I therefore expect that both experimental and theoretical audiences will be interested in the results.

      There are many possible additions and modifications that could be made to the model, and so the model and analysis could provide a platform for future research. However, I can't comment on whether there are similar pre-existing models of the plant circadian clock that contain both a molecular description of the circadian clock as well as a spatial scale.

      REFEREE'S CROSS-COMMENTING

      Comments on Review #1:

      The time is rescaled in each cell, meaning that each cell has a unique period, but the dynamics remain deterministic and hence the peak-to-peak times will be exactly the same for each cell. I imagine this isn't completely consistent with single-cell data (if available), where peak-to-peak times are very likely to be variable due to noisy gene expression. In a future paper it would be interesting to analyse the system using stochastic differential equations.

      Comments on Review #2:

      I agree on the following two points:

      1) It would add value to discuss whether the different ranking of light sensitivities by organ matches any available experimental data.

      2) As the Reviewers point out, there are many possibilities for testing the robustness of the system to light clues, including varying the length of the day. Although outside of the scope of this paper, I wonder if it's possible to find data from a light sensor measuring light intensity across an entire year? Plugging such data into the model and measuring how the amplitude and period changes would be really interesting, in my opinion.

    3. Note: This preprint has been reviewed by subject experts for Review Commons. Content has not been altered except for formatting.

      Learn more at Review Commons


      Referee #2

      Evidence, reproducibility and clarity

      Summary:

      The manuscript presents an improved model of the circadian clock network that accounts for tissue-specific clock behavior, spatial differences in light sensitivity, and local coupling achieved through intercellular sharing of mRNA. In contrast to whole-plant or "phase-only" models, the authors' approach enables them to address the mechanism behind coupling and how the clock maintains regional synchrony in a noisy environment. Using 34 parameters to describe clock activity and applying the properties mentioned above, the authors demonstrate that their model can recapitulate the spatial waves in circadian gene expression observed and can simulate how the plant maintains local synchrony with regional differences in rhythms under noisy LD cycles. Spatial models that incorporate cell-type-specific sensitivities to environmental inputs and local coupling mechanisms will be most accurate for simulating clock activity under natural environments.

      We have the following major criticisms as follows

      1) When assigning light sensitivities in different regions of the plant, the authors assign a higher sensitivity value to the root tip (L=1.03) than they do to the other part of the root (L=0.90). We are curious why the root tip would have higher light sensitivity than the rest of the root. Is this based on experimental data (if so, please cite in this section or methods)? It seems that these L values were assigned simply to make sure they recapitulated the period differences observed in Fig. 2A. Are these values based on PhyB expression in those organs? Or perhaps based on cell density in those locations?

      2) In the discussion of the test where they set the "light inputs to be equal" in all regions to simulate the phyb-9 mutant, could the authors please clarify whether that means they set the L light sensitivity value equal in all regions? a. If they are referring to setting the L value equal to all regions, we suggest that this discussion be moved to the section about different light sensitivities instead of the local sharing of mRNA section. b. Additionally, is it possible to set the light sensitivity to zero for all parts of the plant? We think this would be more suitable to simulate the phyb-9 mutant phenotype.

      3) Based on the recent Chen et al. (2020) paper showing ELF4 long-distance movement, we think it would be of great interest for the authors to model ELF4 protein synthesis/translation as the coupling factor, in addition to the modeling using CCA1/LHY mRNA sharing. We understand you may be saving this analysis for a future modeling paper, but this addition to the paper could increase the impact of this paper.

      4) This model is able to simulate circadian rhythms under 12:12 LD cycles, which represents two days of the year-the equinoxes. We are curious if the model can simulate rhythms under short days and long days as well. We understand this analysis may be outside the scope of this paper and may require changing the values of the 34 parameters used but think it could be a useful addition here or in future work.

      And minor criticisms as follows

      1) In the first paragraph of the results section, it would be helpful for the authors to reference Table S1 when they mention the 34 parameters used to model oscillator function

      2) In the first paragraph of the section titled "Local flexibility persists under idealized and noisy LD cycles", it would be helpful for the authors to reference S12 Fig after the last sentence that starts "However, ELF4/LUX appeared more synchronized..."

      3) In the first paragraph of the section titled "Cell-to-cell coupling maintains global communication under noisy light-dark cycles", the authors refer to a "Table 1" but I think they mean to refer to Table S1"

      4) In Fig. 1, panel C is described as demonstrating the cell-to-cell coupling through the "level of CCA1/LHY". This phrasing is vague and we think could be improved to the "mRNA level of CCA1/LHY".

      Significance

      This work would be broadly interesting to other researchers studying cell-to-cell signaling and coupling of circadian rhythms in plants and other species where spatial waves of gene expression have been observed (i.e., mice and humans). Additionally, the computational modeling aspect of this work was easily interpretable for someone outside this expertise. Our expertise lies in plant circadian biology.

    1. Reviewer #2:

      Extracting ion channel kinetic models from experimental data is an important and perennial problem. Much work has been done over the years by different groups, with theoretical frameworks and computational algorithms developed for specific combinations of data and experimental paradigms, from single channels to real-time approaches in live neurons. At one extreme of the data spectrum, single channel currents are traditionally analyzed by maximum likelihood fitting of dwell time probability distributions; at the other extreme, macroscopic currents are typically analyzed by fitting the average current and other extracted features, such as activation curves. Robust analysis packages exist (e.g., HJCFIT, QuB), and they have been put to good use in the literature.

      Münch et al focus here on several areas that need improvement: dealing with macroscopic recordings containing relatively low numbers of channels (i.e., hundreds to tens of thousands), combining multiple types of data (e.g., electrical and optical signals), incorporating prior information, and selecting models. The main idea is to approach the data with a predictor-corrector type of algorithm, implemented via a Kalman filter that approximates the discrete-state process (a meta-Markov model of the ensemble of active channels in the preparation) with a continuous-state process that can be handled efficiently within a Bayesian estimation framework, which is also used for parameter estimation and model selection.

      With this approach, one doesn't fit the macroscopic current against a predicted deterministic curve, but rather infers - point by point - the ensemble state trajectory given the data and a set of parameters, themselves treated as random variables. This approach, which originated in the signal processing literature as the Forward-Backward procedure (and the related Baum-Welch algorithm), has been applied since the early 90s to single channel recordings (e.g., Chung et al, 1990), and later has been extended to macroscopic data, in a breakthrough study by Moffatt (2007). In this respect, the study by Münch et al is not necessarily a conceptual leap forward. However, their work strengthens the existing mathematical formalism of state inference for macroscopic ion channel data, and embeds it very nicely in a rigorous Bayesian estimation framework.

      The main results are very convincing: basically, model parameters can be estimated with greater precision - as much as an order of magnitude better - relative to the traditional approach where the macroscopic data are treated as noisy but deterministic (but see my comments below). Estimate uncertainty can be further improved by incorporating prior information on parameters (e.g., diffusion limits), and by including other types of data, such as fluorescence. The manuscript is well written and overall clear, and the mathematical treatment is a rigorous tour-de-force.

      There are several issues that should be addressed by the authors, as listed below.

      1) I think packaging this study as a single manuscript for a broad-audience is not optimal. First, the subject is very technical and complex, and the target audience is probably small. Second, the study is very nice and ambitious, but I think clarity is a bit impaired by dealing with perhaps too many issues. The state inference and the bayesian model selection are very important but completely different issues that may be better treated separately, perhaps for a more specialized readership where they can be developed in more detail. Tutorial-style computational examples must be provided, along with well commented/documented code. The interested readers should be able to implement the method described here in their own code/program.

      2) The authors should clearly discuss the types of data and experimental paradigms that can be optimally handled by this approach, and they must explain when and where it fails or cannot be applied, or becomes inefficient in comparison with other methods. One must be aware that ion channel data are very often subject to noise and artifacts that alter the structure of microscopic fluctuations. Thus, I would guess that the state inference algorithm would work optimally with low noise, stable, patch-clamp recordings (and matching fluorescence recordings) in heterologous expression systems (e.g., HEK293 cells), where the currents are relatively small, and only the channel of interest is expressed (macropatches?). I imagine it would not be effective with large currents that are recorded with low gain, are subject to finite series resistance, limited rise time, restricted bandwidth, colored noise, contaminated by other currents that are (partially) eliminated with the P/n protocol with the side effect of altering the noise structure, power line 50/60 Hz noise, baseline fluctuations, etc. This basically excludes some types of experimental data and experimental paradigms, such as recordings from neurons in brain slices or in vivo, oocytes, etc. Of course, artifacts can affect all estimation algorithms, but approaches based on fitting the predicted average current have the obvious benefit of averaging out some of these artifacts.

      The discussion in the manuscript is insufficient in this regard and must be expanded. Furthermore, I would like to see the method tested under non-ideal but commonly occurring conditions, such as limited bandwidth and in the presence of contaminating noise. For example, compare estimates obtained without filtering with estimates obtained with 2, 3 times over-filtering, with and without large measurement noise added (whole cell recordings with low-gain feedback resistors and series resistance compensation are quite noisy), with and without 50/60 Hz interference. How does the algorithm deal with limited bandwidth that distorts the noise spectrum? How are the estimated parameters affected? The reader will have to get a sense of how sensitive this method is to artifacts.

      3) A better comparison with alternative parameter estimation approaches is necessary. First of all, explain more clearly what is different from the predictor-corrector formalism originally proposed by Moffatt (2007). The manuscript mentions that it expands on that, but exactly how? If it is only an incremental improvement, a more specialized audience is more appropriate.

      Second, the method proposed by Celentano and Hawkes, 2004, is not a predictor-corrector type but it utilizes the full covariance matrix between data values at different time points. It seems to me that the covariance matrix approach uses all the information contained in the macroscopic data and should be on par with the state inference approach. However, this method is only briefly mentioned here and then it's quickly dismissed as "impractical". I am not at all convinced that it's impractical. We all agree that it's a slower computation than, say, fitting exponentials, but so is the Kalman filter. Where do we draw the line of impracticability? Computational speed should be balanced with computational simplicity, estimation accuracy, and parameter and model identifiability. Moreover, that method was published in 2004, and the computational costs reported there should be projected to present day computational power. I am not saying that the authors should code the C&H procedure and run it here, but should at least give it credit and discuss its potential against the KF method.

      The only comparison provided in the manuscript is with the "rate equation" approach, by which the authors understand the family of methods that fit the data against a predicted average trajectory. In principle, this comparison is sufficient, but there are some issues with the way it's done.

      Table 3 compares different features of their state inference algorithm and the "rate equation fitting", referencing Milescu et al, 2005. However, there seems to be a misunderstanding: the algorithm presented in that paper does in fact predict and use not only the average but also - optionally - the variance of the current, as contributed by stochastic state fluctuations and measurement noise. These quantities are predicted at any point in time as a function of the initial state, which is calculated from the experimental conditions. In contrast, the KF calculates the average and variance at one point in time as a projection of the average and variance at the previous point. However, both methods (can) compare the data value against a predicted probability distribution. The Kalman filter can produce more precise estimates but presumably with the cost of more complex and slower computation, and increased sensitivity to data artifacts.

      Fig. 3 is very informative in this sense, showing that estimates obtained with the state inference (KF) algorithm are about 10 times more precise that those obtained with the "rate equation" approach. However, for this test, the "rate equation" method was allowed to use only the average, not the variance.

      Considering this, the comparison made in Fig 3 should be redone against a "rate equation" method that utilizes not only the expected average but also the expected variance to fit the data, as in Milescu et al, 2005. Calculating this variance is trivial and the authors should be able to implement it easily (and I'll be happy to provide feedback). The comparison should include calculation times, as well as convergence.

      4) As shown in Milescu et al, 2005, fitting macroscopic currents is asymptotically unbiased. In other words, the estimates are accurate, unless the number of channels is small (tens or hundreds), in which case the multinomial distribution is not very well approximated by a Gaussian. What about the predictor-corrector method? How accurate are the estimates, particularly at low channel counts (10 or 100)? Since the Kalman filter also uses a Gaussian to approximate the multinomial distribution of state fluctuations, I would also expect asymptotic accuracy. Parameter accuracy should be tested, not just precision.

      5) The manuscript nicely points out that a "rate equation" approach would need 10 times more channels (N) to attain the same parameter precision as with the Kalman filter, when the number of channels is in the approximate range of 10^2 ... 10^4. With larger N, the two methods become comparable in this respect.

      This is very important, because it means that estimate precision increases with N, regardless of the method, which also means that one should try to optimize the experimental approach to maximize the number of channels in the preparation. However, I would like to point out that one could simply repeat the recording protocol 10 times (in the same cell or across cells) to accumulate 10 times more channels, and then use a "rate equation" algorithm to obtain estimates that are just as good. Presumably, the "rate equation" calculation is significantly faster than the Kalman filter (particularly when one fits "features", such as activation curves), and repeating a recording may only add seconds or minutes of experiment time, compared to a comprehensive data analysis that likely involves hours and perhaps days. Although obvious, this point can be easily missed by the casual reader and so it would be useful to be mentioned in the manuscript.

      6) Another misunderstanding is that a current normalization is mandatory with "rate equation" algorithms. This is really not the case, as shown in Milescu et al, 2005, where it is demonstrated clearly that one can explicitly use channel count and unitary current to predict the observed macroscopic data. Consequently, these quantities can also be estimated, but state variance must be included in the calculation. Without variance, one can only estimate the product i x N, where i is unitary current and N is channel count. This should be clarified in the manuscript: any method that uses variance can be used to estimate i and N, not just the Kalman filter. In fact, the non-stationary noise analysis does exactly that: a model-blind estimation of N and i from non-equilibrium data. Also, one should be realistic here: in some circumstances it is far more efficient to fit data "features", such as the activation curve, in which case the current needs to be normalized.

      7) I think it's great that the authors develop a rigorous Bayesian formalism here, but I think it would be a good idea to explain - even briefly - how to implement a (presumably simpler) maximum likelihood version that uses the Kalman filter. This should satisfy those readers who are less interested in the Bayesian approach, and will also be suitable for situations when no prior information is available.

      8) The Bayesian formalism is not the only way of incorporating prior knowledge into an estimation algorithm. In fact, it seems to me that the reader would have more practical and pressing problems than guessing what the parameter prior distribution should be, whether uniform or Gaussian or other. More likely one would want to enforce a certain KD, microscopic (i)reversibility, an (in)equality relationship between parameters, a minimum or maximum rate constant value, or complex model properties and behaviors, such as maximum Popen or half-activation voltage. A comprehensive framework for handling these situations via parameter constraints (linear or non-linear) and cost function penalty has been recently published (Salari et al/Navarro et al, 2018). Obviously, the Bayesian approach has merit, but the authors should discuss how it can better handle the types of practical problems presented in those papers, if it is to be considered an advance in the field, or at least a usable alternative.

      9) Discuss the practical aspects of optimization. For example, how is convergence established? How many iterations does it take to reach convergence? How long does it take to run? How does it scale with the data length, channel count, and model state count? How long does it take to optimize a large model (e.g., 10 or 20 states)? Provide some comparison with the "rate equation method".

      10) Here and there, the manuscript somehow gives the impression that existing algorithms that extract kinetic parameters by fitting the average macroscopic current ("fitting rate equations") are less "correct", or ignorant of the true mathematical description of the data. This is not the case. Published algorithms that I know of clearly state what data they apply to, what their limitations are, and what approximations were made, and thus they are correct within that defined context and are meant to be more effective than alternatives. Some quick editing throughout the manuscript should eliminate this impression.

      11) The manuscript refers to the method where the data are fitted against a predicted current as "rate equations". I don't actually understand what that means. The rate equation is something intrinsic to the model, not a feature of any algorithm. An alternative terminology must be found. Perhaps different algorithms could be classified based on what statistical properties are used and how. E.g., average (+variance) predicted from the starting probabilities (Milescu et al, 2005), full covariance (Celentano and Hawkes, 2004), point-by-point predictor-corrector (Moffatt, 2007).

    1. “These are (1) a providential vision, in which the natural world has a purpose, to serve the human needs richly, but only if people do their part by filling it up with labor and development; (2) a Romantic vision, in which a key part of the world’s value is aesthetic and spiritual, found in the inspiration of mountain peaks, sheer canyon walls, and deep forests; (3) a utilitarian picture, in which nature is a storehouse of resources requiring expert management, especially by scientists and public officials; and (4) an ecological view of the world as being formed of complex and interpenetrating systems, in which both sustenance and poison may travel through air, water, and soil, and in and out of flesh, as each thing becomes something else.”

      CONTEXTUALIZE:

      In an interview with Landscape Architecture Magazine, Jedidiah Purdy discusses two statements in the preface of his book, This Land is Our Land. The first: "Land is perennially the thing we share that holds us apart." Second: "We have made a world that overmasters us." He explains in the interview, "Who people are on the land and how they can use it, what claims they have to it- is, in our history- the original way that people get sorted into different social fates. Do you own? Do you take the profit? Do you labor? Are you tied to the land?...That division is a way of sorting out and ranking interdependence. Together and apart are inseparable there"(LA MAG, July 2020). "It is the great achievement of human beings to build a world in which we have all these powers that we don't naturally have- we're so helpless- and yet that built world tells us how to live in it in a way that actually radically constrains and gives a very damaging form to our inhabiting the kind of larger living world" (LA MAG, July 2020). In this interview Purdy refers to the land as "...what determines circumstance of individuals' socioeconomic conditions; forming and modifying class, labor, economics, and value of the human race. In After Nature, Purdy describes human beings as what determines the formation of landscape according to what is valued and what is ignored. The two relationships described embody the definition of the Anthropocene era, which, according to Purdy in After Nature, is to emphasize what we think is most important in that relationship (between humans and nature)... Because we shape everything, from the upper atmosphere to the deep seas, there is no more nature that stands apart from human beings" (Purdy, 2015).

      RELATE: "A brief sampling might note the ill-fated hydrological reengineering of Tenochtitlan, the replacement of community forests by scientifically managed imperial woodlots, the substitution of Cartesian-grid, monocrop planting for native polycultures adapted to local soils and rains, the violent suppression of women's practical healing knowledge by an all-male elite, the new enclosures of landscapes and forests by today's agro-efficiency engineers and would-be "global" conservation organizations acting in the name of nature and the best interests of "humanity" (McAfee, 2016). In the era of the Anthropocene, both scarcity and abundance are caricatured into megaprivelege and megapoverty more than ever before. The impact of land and the reciprocated impact on the land is more obvious and more likely to be either addressed or ignored by the multiplicity of divisions within the human race, globally. Even if the changing landscape is ignored, the dependence human beings have on the land for natural resources, for space, and for inspiration is only increasing. The land, in response, relies on the honorable actions of human beings. The Anthropocene era defines the transition from reciprocity in relationship to toxic codependence.

      McAfee, Kathleen. “The Politics of Nature in the Anthropocene” In: “Whose Anthropocene? Revisiting Dipesh Chakrabarty’s ‘Four Theses,’” edited by Robert Emmett and Thomas Lekan, RCC Perspectives: Transformations in Environment and Society 2016, no. 2, 65–72.

    1. other agents, very much like ourselves

      Maybe robot rights is the wrong question, or even in the short term dangerous, but it may be heading in the right direction. If robots can do these things, then why don't they get rights...

      What about the biosphere, or communities?

      Why are we even having this debate about robot rights? If that's the wrong question, then maybe it's still trying to address a serious issue that needs to be addressed sooner rather than later. Cog Sci, computer science, and so on, affect the foundations of what humans can do. Literally speaking we don't have it but as a thought experiment it's valid b/c we have things that are close enough.

      As a philosophical issue, if we respond to this as trolling, then the whole thing becomes a flame war rather than thinking about how Cog Sci and so on has helped us think about what it means to be human.

      What then does it mean to have human dignity in a situation where we have current technology? Where do things like rights go w/ embeddedness?

      Maybe there's some continuous thread from rights of kings to rights of man and beyond — what if we take the fundamental unit to be the community, in which case, then we get different conclusions. Maybe even Burke has something like this — the thing about the isolated individual was the community. Communities can have have special communities this is why you get such things as rights of kings: this is why the king gets special rights. It wasn't just that it was exploiting kings rampaging around with other responsibilities.

    1. Note: This rebuttal was posted by the corresponding author to Review Commons. Content has not been altered except for formatting.

      Learn more at Review Commons


      Reply to the reviewers

      Reviewer #1 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity (Required)):

      The authors generated and analyzed a great amount of single-cell RNA FISH data over time on circadian genes (Nr1d1, Cry1, Bmal1), and performed model selection/fitting to explain the observed mRNA distributions. They decomposed the mRNA variability into distinct sources, and showed that intrinsic noise (transcription burst) dominates the variance. Therefore, looking at transcript counts may not be feasible to estimate single-cell circadian phase. However, the study is quite descriptive and ends up being a bit dissatisfying, so if the authors could improve this aspect by perhaps analyzing a mechanism on cell-specific burst size (F5), gene-specific dependence on cell size (beta), or the positive/negative gene-pair correlations (rho), it would help quite a bit in this regard. The model selection/fitting itself was not really sufficient to compensate for this, as it stands .

      We thank the reviewer for appreciating the new smFISH data, the analyses performed, and the consequences regarding phase inference from single cell snapshots.

      The reviewer suggests “perhaps analyzing a mechanism on cell-specific burst size (F5), gene-specific dependence on cell size (beta), or the positive/negative gene-pair correlations (rho)”, and we have thus added a new Results paragraph (lines 281-316) and two new Supp Figures 13 and 14 to directly address this point.

      Specifically, we have added a dynamic, stochastic model of the circadian clock in order to add mechanistic insight into the parameters of the preferred model M4. Concerning \rho, in the initial manuscript we suggested that the correlations of cell-specific burst sizes (described by the parameter \rho) in the preferred model M4 could result from the underlying network topology. To substantiate this claim, we have now added an analysis of a stochastic model of the clock that includes gene-gene interaction amongst the core-clock genes. The core-clock network involves variables (such as protein levels), parameters (such as mRNA/ protein half-lives) and additional genes (such as Clock) that are not directly measurable in our experiments; and thus offering a detailed mechanistic mathematical model for our data is therefore not realistic. We therefore developed a simplified mathematical model for the three measured genes to explore the underlying mechanisms that could control the parameter \rho, as the referee suggests. As a starting point, we used the circadian clock gene network topology for Nr1d1, Cry1 and Bmal1 as modelled in Relógio et al. (Relógio et al., 2011) (see new Supplementary Material). To keep the model close to the inference framework, we used oscillatory functions for the burst frequency while the transcription rate (and hence the burst size) for each gene is affected by the protein levels of the other genes in the network. Using stochastic simulations we show that, for particular configurations of feedback where the negative repression of Nr1d1 by CRY1 is high, the network can generate positive mRNA correlation between Bmal1/Cry1 mRNA and negative correlation between Nr1d1/Cry1mRNA, as observed in our data (Figure 2C). Furthermore, using the same inference framework as for our data on the simulated mRNA distributions, the obtained \rho is positive for Bmal1/Cry1 and negative for Nr1d1/Cry1, which was also found for our data (Figure 3C). Even though the model is clearly a simplified representation of the clock, these simulations give credence to the scenario that the \rho parameter obtained from the data is a signature of the underlying network topology.

      While the emphasis of the paper is certainly on parameter inference of the single-cell RNA FISH data, we believe the addition of this dynamic model provides more mechanistic insight into the results of the model fitting and hence significantly more depth to the article.

      \*Specific comments:** *

      1.It is hard to distinguish the RNA FISH signals (Figure 1A, 2B). It is probably technically challenging as the mRNAs are of low abundance. I think it may help if they adjust the contrast for the cytoplasm stain or just delineate the cell boundaries.

      Thank you for pointing this out, and we agree that our rendering of the FISH images was not optimal and have now significantly improved it (see new Figure 1A and 2B). Considering the other reviewers’ comments related to the images, we have now 1) added the cell contours as requested; 2) use red/green for the smFISH signal in the pairs of genes; 3) we have improved the contrast to make it easier to distinguish the RNA FISH signals.

      2.In Figure 2C, the authors showed gene-pair correlations with cells of all sizes. Could the authors do a size-dependent extrinsic-noise filtering (Padovan-Merhar, Dev. Cell, 2015; Hansen et al., 2018, Cell Systems) to better dissect the correlations?

      We used negative binomial distributions to directly model the number of mRNA in the cells, which is a natural choice given that the raw smFISH are integer counts. The model incorporates cell size dependencies in a unified framework, which predicts the joint distribution of raw counts, which is why we showed raw counts in the main figure. That being said, as the referee suggests, it can be useful for exploratory purposes to see the relationship between the measured genes while regressing out the contribution of cell area, and we have now added this analysis as Supp Figure 9. On line 156-161 we write:

      “To also estimate the correlation between genes while accounting for cell area, we regressed out the area for each gene and recalculated the correlation coefficients [37,38]. Since all genes are positively correlated with area (Fig. 2A), this processing shifted the correlations for both pairs of genes. Specifically, the correlation coefficients for the area-filtered mRNA counts decreased but remained positive for Bmal1/Cry1 and became more negative for Nr1d1/Cry1(Supp Figure 9).”

      3.For fitting model M3, as the authors pointed out, there are many local minima. Is the fitting score truly sufficient to eliminate the possibility for partial synchrony especially considering that the authors didn't show how effective the Dex treatment was to synchronize the circadian phase?

      Thank you for this comment. In fact, we didn't mean to fully eliminate the possibility of imperfect synchronization, but have tried our best to address it both experimentally and with modeling.

      Experimentally, in addition to the Dex treatment, we also compared with a condition in which we entrained the cells using temperature cycles, which is a standard in the field to achieve the best synchronization. We obtained a fold change of 2.1, which was in the range of previous studies (Saini, et al, 2012) and was slightly higher than with Dex synchronisation (1.6). Given that the improvement was not high and that it was important for us to study the system under free-running conditions and not in an entrained state (i.e. phase locking, which distorts the free dynamics and noise characteristics of the oscillator), we used the Dex protocol.

      Model 3 was used as a computational approach to correct for the individual phases. In addition to the difficult optimisation landscape, the challenge with model M3 also resides in the difficulty of estimating an individual phase for each cell, as the two mRNA counts measured in each cell do not contain sufficient phase information. This could potentially be resolved by either measuring more genes simultaneously, but is, however, beyond the scope of the present manuscript. We have added discussion on this to the text on lines 244-248:

      “Thus, it was apparently difficult to use model M3 to correct the individual phase for each cell, likely due to the fact that the two mRNA counts measured in each cell do not contain sufficient phase information, and that the global optimisation problem contains many local minima. This could potentially be improved by measuring more genes simultaneously.”

      We have also added a new Results section (lines 305-316) and Supp Figure 14 to show that imperfect synchrony alone cannot explain the correlation structure observed in our data. Indeed, if two genes have a similarly phased oscillation, the expression of the two genes will be positively correlated (as shown in the new Supp Figure 14). Similarly, when the oscillations are in anti-phase, negative correlations will be found. Given that Nr1d1 and Cry1 are closer in phase than Bmal1 and Cry1, one would expect that the correlation between Nr1d1 and Cry1 (once accounting for area) would be more positive than for Bmal1 and Cry1, which was not found in the data (area-corrected correlations shown in Supp Figure 9). It therefore seems unlikely that the observed correlations could be caused by imperfect synchrony alone. Together with our simulations of the gene network (described above), we therefore argue that gene-gene interactions are a more plausible mechanistic explanation of the correlations observed in our measured bivariate mRNA distributions.

      4.Regarding model M4, the authors added a cell-specific noise term without specifying the contributing factors. Typically adding degrees of freedom should improve fitting and make it easier for a model to fit, why not in this case? Can the authors provide some explanations/mechanisms.

      We believe there has been a misunderstanding regarding model M4. By adding parameters, model M4 is indeed easier to fit. There is even a problem of overfitting whereby the burst frequency becomes unrealistically high and the model effectively fits a Poisson distribution to each individual cell. To avoid this, we lock the burst frequency values to the posterior mean values from model M2. After describing model M4, we write (lines 260-265):

      “When all parameters are free, we noticed that the burst frequency can become unrealistically high due to a tendency to overfit to individual cells, and we therefore locked the burst frequency to the posterior mean values from model M2. The PSIS-LOO scores overall favoured model M4 (Fig. 3B), and the predicted joint probability density shows good similarity to the observed data (Fig. 3D) (all time points shown in Supp figure 11).”

      Regarding the above comment in the reviewer’s summary on contributing factors of model M4 we added a simple dynamical model that attempts to explain at least one possible mechanism of generating correlations in cell-specific bursting parameters (see above).

      5.The authors should include the number (range) of cells analyzed in the figure legends.

      We have now added the number of cells used at each time point to the legend of Figure 1D. To respond to Reviewer #2 we have also added details on the number of smFISH replicates used at each time point. The number of cells for each replicate is shown in Supp Figures 2-5.

      Reviewer #1 (Significance (Required)):

      Overall, we felt conflicted about the manuscript. On one hand, the authors generated and analyzed a great amount of single-cell RNA FISH data over time on circadian genes. On the other hand, the manuscript was a bit dissatisfying/descriptive. If the authors could provide and analyze some sort of mechanisms on cell-specific burst size (F5), gene-specific dependence on cell size (beta), or the positive/negative gene-pair correlations (rho) it should help improve the manuscript.

      We thank the review for the suggestion to expand on the mechanistic interpretation, which we have followed. In addition, we would like to emphasise that a similar smFISH analysis of the core circadian oscillator has never been done, and we believe our data represents a significant contribution to the field. Moreover, our quite generic probabilistic inference framework for smFISH using mixture models to describe intrinsic (transcriptional bursting) and extrinsic fluctuations is also novel and the code provided (written using the Stan probabilistic programming language) might find a wide applicability.

      Concerning the mechanistic description, as described above, we added a stochastic, dynamic model of gene expression and propose that gene-gene interactions within the core-clock network topology represent a plausible mechanism for generating correlated burst parameters between genes, which are a feature of the preferred model M4 found during inference. We additionally added an explanatory figure to argue that, given the phase relationship between genes, imperfect synchronisation alone cannot explain the observed correlations that we observe between the pairs of genes. Together, this analysis provides more mechanistic insight into the underlying factors controlling the gene-gene relationships in our measured bivariate mRNA distributions.

      \*Referees cross-commenting** *

      I agree with Reviewer #3 regarding expanding the discussion to include the Shah & Tyagi and Raj et al citations on buffering. However caution should be exercised regarding ref 26 as it is quite controversial and subsequent analyses came to different conclusions (PMID: 30359620 and 30243562). The general consensus is that nuclear buffering of transcript noise (proposed in ref 26) is not a general phenomenon (ref 27 is specific to the calcium response pathway). In fact, the presence and evolution of specific pathways to buffer transcriptional noise, such as protein-protein mechanisms (Shah & Tyagi) or extended half-life proteins (Raj et al. and others), argues that transcript fluctuations are not probably buffered in general.

      Following the suggestion of Reviewer #3, we have expanded the Discussion to include the references cited (Shah & Tyagi, Raj and others).

      Previous work from our lab is also nuancing the conclusions from references 26 and 27. Specifically, buffering effects are expected to be highly gene-specific (3’UTR), and in fact we have not seen those with our unstable construct during live-cell imaging (Suter et al., 2011; Zoller et al., 2015). We have also added text in order to explicitly state that subsequent papers have nuanced the general claims in references 26 and 27. In the text we write (lines 335-342):

      “One explanation for the low intrinsic fluctuation in these studies is that transcriptional fluctuations are filtered by nuclear retention, though other reports suggest that Fano factors (variance/mean, a measure of overdispersion compared to the Poisson distribution) can be even larger in the cytoplasm than in the nucleus [38]. In the cells used here, the strong signature of transcriptional bursting and high intrinsic noise is consistent with live imaging of a Bmal1transcriptional reporter in the same cell line under similar growth conditions, where intrinsic noise was estimated to be 4-times larger than extrinsic noise [23].”.

      Reviewer #2 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity (Required)):

      \*Summary:** *

      The authors study experimentally and computationally the dynamic transcription of circadian clock genes over time in individual cells with single molecule RNA-FISH with the aim to understand how different noise sources contribute to single cell transcription variability and basic functions of circadian clocks. The authors integrate experiments with computational modeling to understand biology.

      \*Major comments:** *

      This study has some major limitations that need to be addressed to test the model usefulness, to understand noise sources and to gain biological insights into circadian clocks.

      We thank this reviewer for the constructive feedback which enabled us to significantly strengthen the revised manuscript.

      The limitations are on the experiments, the computational implementation of the modeling and the integration of experiments with models.

      Although the experimental datasets contain several hundred cells per time point for multiple time points, only a single replica experiment is presented. From the presented data it is not clear how reproducible these temporal patterns are and if indeed differences between timepoints can be resolved if multiple biological replica experiments have been analyzed. To address this point at least three biological experiments needs to be presented and analyzed for each of the genes. Plotting the SEM on the means in figure 1B is misleading because several hundred cells have been measured which automatically makes the error small. The SEM just describes how well we can determine the mean from a distribution. Instead a mean and std from the biological replicas need to be plotted to show how experimental variability in experiments is resulting in the described expression pattern. This is similar to RNA-seq data or RT-PCR from multiple replica.

      We certainly agree that demonstrating reproducibility is important. Note that our smFISH data is from three independent cell culture dishes and microscopy slides, which included independent cell synchronization. This was described in the Methods but we agree that the data presentation was not showing the individual replicas, which we have now added. In Figure 1B, we now show the mean of each replicate for each time point. While the reviewer suggested displaying the mean and standard deviation across replicates, we show all data points at each time point to make it even more transparent. The mRNA distribution of each replicate is also shown in Supp Figures 2-5, together with individual quantification of mean, coefficient of variation and number of cells.

      In addition, to further demonstrate the reproducibility of the temporal patterns we have performed an additional independent experiment on four time points. This experiment shows that the oscillatory patterns for Nr1d1 and Cry1are clearly significant and reproducible (new Supp Figure 7). The combination of the replicates shown for the main experiment (Supp Figures 2-5) and the new replicate experiment (Supp Figure 7) shows that the oscillatory temporal patterns for the mean mRNA levels are robust and reproducible, and in fact similar as those found in bulk analyses (Ukai-Tadenuma et al., 2011; Hughes et al., 2009), which is expected.

      It is also not clear how good the cell segmentation works and how does cell segmentation influence the analysis. In figure 1A show the segmentation of the cell boundary together with the membrane stain.

      Thanks to this and other reviewers’ comments, we have now significantly improved the presentation of the FISH images. We have now 1) added the cell contours as requested; 2) used red/green for the smFISH signal in the pairs of genes; 3) we have improved the contrast to make it easier to distinguish the RNA FISH signals.

      We have also added Supp Figure 1 to show that the cell segmentation we used is reliable. In fact, as we had described, we used the sum Z-stack projections of the red channel (Wu et al., 2018), which we found provides the most accurate cell segmentation. We now show in Supp Figure 1 that the obtained segmentation shows convincing agreement with the cell autofluorescence .

      The authors use the RNA mean and RNA-FISH distributions and combine this data to build and compare different models. How do you know that the given data fulfils the central limit so that a model describing the mean is an adequate approach? To test this point, the authors should show through subsampling from the data and the model that indeed their data sets have enough cells to fulfil the central limit theorem.

      This comment reflects a misunderstanding of our approach, which we now try to better explain. In our inference framework we use a negative binomial (NB) distribution (and mixtures of NBs) to model the full distribution of mRNA counts, and our approach is therefore not based exclusively on the mean of the distribution. The estimation of model parameters and comparison of models is performed using the PSIS-LOO optimisation procedure (see below). The mixture model of NB binomials makes a few assumptions which we had clearly stated. In fact it captures both bursty transcription (in the limit of short bursts as is biologically plausible, which yields the NB distribution), and cell-to-cell variability (extrinsic noise) captured by the mixture. The suitability of the NB to model bursty transcription is established (Raj et al., 2006), and it is parameterized by a mean and a dispersion coefficient, such that the CV of the distribution is the inverse of the burst frequency (Zoller et al., 2015). Therefore the mean is indeed an important parameter of the model, but we do not see the relationship with the CLT. The used probabilistic inference (PSIS-LOO: Pareto-Smoothed Importance Sampling Leave-One-Out, Vehtari et al. 2017, see below) is established and state-of-the-art for selecting models of the appropriate complexity and we are not aware of a similar previous quantitative model for smFISH analysis.

      We have now added significantly more explanations both on the general approach as well as the methodological details in a fully-revised Methods section to avoid further misunderstanding.

      A strength of the manuscript is that several competing and biologically meaningful models have been generated. However, the manuscript lacks rigor in terms of how fitting and model selection is performed. It is not clear how good the models fit the data. To address this point, the authors should visually compare the model fits to the data and plot their fit errors as a function of model complexity.

      We fully agree that comparing different models using a model selection approach is a powerful methodology, in fact it is arguably the most systematic way to approach modeling problems in quantitative biology. Model selection is an active research area and there have been significant developments recently. Here, we used a state-of-the-art and established Bayesian approach (PSIS-LOO: Pareto-Smoothed Importance Sampling Leave-One-Out, Vehtari et al. 2017), which is certainly rigorous and more objective than visual comparison. The PSIS-LOO is conceptually similar to other approaches of model performance such as AIC or WAIC, and the entire field of model selection aims at establishing rigorous methods to assess the tradeoff between fit errors and model complexity. In PSIS-LOO, this is done by using pareto-smoothed importance sampling to estimate the expected log pointwise predictive density for a new dataset using leave-one-out cross-validation. The PSIS-LOO is the currently recommended metric for measuring model performance in Bayesian analysis (Vehtari et al., 2017) and is considered superior to other approaches such as computations of Bayes factors since it is less sensitive to model priors (Gelman et al. 2013). The performance of the models as measured with PSIS-LOO is shown in Figure 3B. As already mentioned, we have added further details as to how the fitting and model selection is performed in a revised Methods section. We agree that visual comparison is useful to gain intuition and this is why we showed the bivariate distributions in Figure 3D and in Supp Figure 11.

      Regarding the comment on “fit error”, note also that we probabilistically model the full mRNA distribution for each gene. In each cell, there is a likelihood score that measures the likelihood of observing the measured mRNA count given the modelled probability distribution. As our approach is based on this likelihood, the notion of “fitting error” needs to be replaced by the log likelihood (‘fitting error’ is mathematically equivalent to a log-likelihood when the noise model is Gaussian, which is not the case here).

      Another limitation is that the models have not been validated for example by using them to make predictions. One type of prediction could be to fit the model to one biological replica and then predict the other replica (cross validation). Another prediction would be to take the distribution fitted to the experimental data and then compare the model mean to the experimental mean.

      Thank you for this comment. As explained above, we used the state-of-the-art PSIS-LOO to measure the predictive performance of the models, which approximates the result of leave-one-out cross-validation using the full data set. To further assess the predictive capabilities of the model, we have now also added a “leave-replicate-out” cross-validation, as the reviewer suggests (new Supp Figure 12). The aim of our “leave-replicate-out” cross-validation was to test how well the predictions of each model generalise to independent cells that are not in the training set. To do this, we trained each model while omitting the data from one gene on a test slide. We then calculated the likelihood score of the test slide using the parameters from the training set, and repeated this for all slides. Similarly to the PSIS-LOO, the results of the leave-replicate-out cross-validation convincingly show that model M4 has the highest predictive performance. This is now described in the updated text on lines 265-271.

      The results from fitting and prediction should be plotted as a function of model complexity. This kind of analysis will illustrate how model complexity is supported by the data.

      As already mentioned, we used state-of-the-art algorithms to analyze prediction vs. complexity. With the above addition, we now have two methods of calculating the predictive performance of each model: the approximate leave-one-out score as measured with PSIS-LOO and the leave-replicate-out cross-validation. For each model, the PSIS-LOO score is plotted in Figure 3B and the leave-replicate-out cross-validation score is shown in Supp Figure 12.

      In the method section on models, a biological motivation must be presented to justify the different model assumption.

      Thank you for pointing out that the biological justification of the models needed to be expanded. In addition to the improved justifications already provided in the Results section, we have now updated the Methods section such that a biological motivation is included for each model.

      How do the models that fit the distributions describe the mean?

      As explained above, the inference is performed on the entire distributions, using a family of distributions (mixtures of NBs) which are parameterized in a biologically relevant manner (transcriptional bursting + extrinsic noise). The mean and variance of the distribution are now described on lines 585-586 in addition to Figure 3A.

      It is necessary to list model parameters for each of the models, their description, their parameter values, their parameter uncertainty and units of each parameter.

      Thank you, this has now been added as Supplementary Tables 2-5.

      It is not clear to me how the joint probability in figures 2,4, S2 and S4 have been used to fit the model.

      Again, the joint distributions are modeled using mixtures of NBs and the inference is performed on the entire dataset at once using a log-likelihood approach. This uses all the data at once, and it is embedded in a Bayesian model selection method. The way that the joint probability is used is now clarified in the revised Methods section and in the Results section (lines 208-214):

      “For both models M1 and M2, the likelihood of observing the data given the parameters of the model is evaluated using the model-specific NB distribution and the mRNA counts for both genes in each cell. This is performed for both Bmal1/Cry1 and Nr1d1/Cry1 pairs across all time points, and this likelihood is combined with model priors to define the posterior parameter distribution for each model (Methods). We applied Hamiltonian Monte Carlo sampling within the STAN probabilistic programming language to sample the posterior distribution and infer model parameters 40.”

      How do the models make sense in the context of the fact that human genes exist as a diploids?

      This is a good point, although note though that the 3T3 cells are from mice and not humans. 3T3 cells are tetraploid, and it turns out that under the justified assumption that the bursts are short (Zoller et al., 2015; Suter et al., 2011), the number of alleles rescales the burst frequency, i.e. the effective (observed) burst frequency equals the number of alleles times the burst frequency per allele, but it does not change the shape of the distributions. On line 580-582 we have now written: “Since 3T3 cells are tetraploid, and, again assuming that the bursts are short, the inferred burst frequency for tetraploid cells will be approximately four times that of a single allele.”

      The variance decomposition is shortly described but no results are presented to show how this is done. This should be better explained.

      The variance decomposition we used is not a new result; in fact, we used the analytical results of Bowsher, C. G. & Swain, P. S. “Identifying sources of variation and the flow of information in biochemical networks” (PNAS, 2012). The mathematical proofs of the formula we use are contained within that reference; however, we have re-written this section to make it clearer to the reader (lines 688-718).

      \*Minor comments:** *

      In figure 3A, it is not clear to me what these different plots relate to the models. It is also not clear what are equations that describe each model.

      The Methods section has now been improved to show the full data-generating mechanism for each model, and each model has its own section title to make it easier to find. We have also improved the legend for Figure 3 to make the relationship to each model clearer.

      The legends in figure 3 are not very informative. More details need to be presented to understand this figure.

      Thank you for pointing this out, and we have now re-written the figure legend for Figure 3 to make the figure clearer.

      Reviewer #2 (Significance (Required)):

      This is an interesting and important topic with the potential to have general implication of how to model periodic single cell gene expression data and eventually better understand circadian clocks. This study will expand on other modeling studies of circadian clocks and has the potential to advance the field (PMCID: PMC7229691). I personally have done similar analysis and experiments in another system and biological context which has demonstrated the power of this approach if implemented rigorously. I am not an expert in circadian clocks in human cells.

      We thank the reviewer for appreciating the implications for the circadian and single cell gene expression community. Note that to our knowledge, modeling smFISH counts using mixtures of negative binomials combined with Bayesian model selection has not been done. It is both highly relevant biologically (combines intrinsic and extrinsic fluctuations in a rigorous way), general and its applicability extends far beyond the circadian oscillator. Therefore, this approach for quantitative smFISH data analysis also fills an important methodological gap.

      \*Referees Cross commenting** *

      Reviewer #1:

      I agree with the assessment that model fitting and model selection was not sufficient. But I disagreed that the data is enough. Although many cells and time points are analyzed, there is no evidence of how reproducible each mRNA distribution can be measured at each time point. I think reproducibility is key and will also help with the model fitting and identification.

      Regarding the point on reproducibility, we have made the following four changes:

      1. We have added an independent 4 time-point experiment to show that the oscillatory patterns of the distributions are reproducible (Supp Figure 7).
      2. In Figure 1 we now also show the mean of each replicate for the main experiment (Figure 1B).
      3. We also show the mRNA distributions of each replicate in Supp Figures 2-5.
      4. We have added the “leave-replicate-out” cross-validation to show that that the model performance of the preferred model generalises to independent slides that were not included in training (Supp Figure 12). In responding to Reviewer #1 regarding the modeling, we have now also added a simplified dynamical model of circadian clock expression to add mechanistic insight into our proposed models. Overall, we have significantly expanded the description of the model selection approaches to help readers who are less familiar with Bayesian model selection methods.

      Reviewer #3:

      Regarding the red background, my understanding is that this comes from the probe hybridization. This is maybe because the probe concentration has not been optimized or the number of probes per gene is low and the signal to noise is not so good.Or it could be auto fluorescent background. In this case a different fluorophore needs to be used to avoid this problem.

      Thank you for those comments, and we agree with all reviewers that the presentation of the images needed to be improved. It turned out that in Figure 1, we had shown the cell mask in red so it is clearly not related to probe concentration or autofluorescence. We have now removed the cell mask channel from the main images which allows highlighting better the smFISH signals. All smFISH images for Figures 1 and 2 have been much improved, and we’ve added a new Supp Figure 1 to show the performance of our cell segmentation.

      Reviewer #3 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity (Required)):

      In this paper Nicholas et al image mRNAs encoding the key controllers of circadian rhythms, Rev-erba, Cry and Bmal1 in single cells over time. It was shown earlier that single cells exhibit circadian rhythms using reporter genes. A large number of studies have shown that transcription is an inherently stochastic process, which raises a question as to how single cells are able to achieve their rhythms on the face of this noise. Their results show that the number of mRNAs for the three genes exhibit the expected periodicity, but this periodicity is associated with significant cell-to-cell variation. They also explore to what extent this variability derives from stochastic transcription vs other sources of variation that are extrinsic to the genes. The results are interesting and experimental and modeling results are important (however this reviewer is not able to judge the veracity of mathematics that underlay the models).

      We thank this reviewer for appreciating the importance of our work.

      \*Some of the concerns that arose are listed below:** *

      1.The images show an annoying red background. If the red is HCS cell mask, it should be removed, and RNA presented on grey scale. This will make a better presentation. The red hue also appears in fig 2 b but here it is one of the RNA. I suggest in Fig 2 one RNA can be presented in green and the other in red, while the nuclei in blue.

      Thank you for this comment. We had indeed shown the cell mask in the red channel and now removed it. Together with the other suggestions and comments from the reviewers, we implemented the following changes: 1) added the cell contours as requested; 2) use red/green for the smFISH signal in the pairs of genes; 3) we have improved the contrast to make it easier to distinguish the RNA FISH signals. The presentation of the images is now much improved.

      2.This paper and a few others talk about the cell size contributing to the cell-to-cell variability in mRNA numbers. Where does it come from physically? One can imagine based on the cell cycle stage there could be more than two copies of then gene in a cell, which will yield more RNAs, but they say that their cells don't have much cell cycle variability. Perhaps a clearer discussion is called for rather than just being polite to other investigators.

      The referee is right that several studies observed empirically that larger cells show more mRNA molecules in smFISH experiments (Padovan et al., 2015; Kempe et al., 2015). In Padovan et al. (2015), the authors found that transcriptional burst size changes with cell volume and burst frequency with cell cycle. The main theory for transcription scaling with cell volume is to maintain transcript concentration. Using cell fusion experiments, they showed that cellular size can directly and globally affect gene expression by modulating transcription. Furthermore, they proposed that the mechanism underlying the global regulation integrates both DNA content and cellular volume to produce the appropriate amount of RNA for a cell of a given size, which is consistent with a model whereby a factor limiting for transcription is sequestered to the DNA. We used these results to propose a model whereby burst size scales with area, and we found an increase in predictive performance (compare M2 with M1 in Figure 3B). While our model selection supported the inclusion of cell area, the variance decomposition showed that the fraction of variance due to cell area ranged from 4.2% for Nr1d1 to 17.6% for Bmal1. We have now expanded the introduction to discuss this in more depth (lines 73-80) as requested.

      3.References 26 and 27 are cited for 10-80% of variance due to gene extrinsic sources. These references actually deny that there is a significant transcriptional noise in most genes. Again, stronger discussion is called for.

      As mentioned in the reply to Reviewer 1, previous work from our lab is also nuancing the conclusions from references 26 and 27. Specifically, buffering effects are expected to be highly gene-specific (3’UTR), and in fact we have not seen those with our unstable construct during live-cell imaging (Suter et al., 2011; Zoller et al., 2015). We have also added text in order to explicitly state that subsequent papers have nuanced the general claims in references 26 and 27. In the text we write (lines 335-342):

      “One explanation for the low intrinsic fluctuation in these studies is that transcriptional fluctuations are filtered by nuclear retention, though other reports suggest that Fano factors (variance/mean, a measure of overdispersion compared to the Poisson distribution) can be even larger in the cytoplasm than in the nucleus [38]. In the cells used here, the strong signature of transcriptional bursting and high intrinsic noise is consistent with live imaging of a Bmal1transcriptional reporter in the same cell line under similar growth conditions, where intrinsic noise was estimated to be 4-times larger than extrinsic noise [23].”.

      4.The results raise a very important question, whether and to what extent the transcriptional noise propagates to the next step of gene regulation and are there buffering mechanisms in the cell. For example, Raj et al, Variability in gene expression underlies incomplete penetrance, Nature 2010, show that alternative pathways serve to buffer the impact of gene expression noise. Similarly, Shah and Tyagi, Barriers to transmission of transcriptional noise in a c-fos c-jun pathway, Mol Syst Biol, 2013, show that variability in mRNA is buffered at protein level and the level of protein-protein complexes. Furthermore, they show that to the extent those vary, the chromatin intrinsically buffers against the fluctuations in numbers of transcription factors. Mention of these and other studies will enrich the paper.

      We have modified the Discussion section and now discuss these papers (and a few more). We thank the reviewer for the suggestions, which will help the reader to have a broader overview of noise buffering in gene expression and indeed enrich the paper.

      Reviewer #3 (Significance (Required)):

      Significance is high. Quality is high.

      \*Referees Cross-Commenting** *

      I agree with the comments made by other reviewers particularly about references 26 and 27. The major conclusions of reference 26 were questioned by Hansen et al 2018. At the bottom of page 7 the authors are qualifying their results in the light of references 26 and 27. Perhaps now there is less of a need to do so.

      As mentioned above, we have added the following sentence citing the Hansen paper to make it clear to the reader that key conclusions of the references 26 and 27 are disputed (lines 335-342):

      “One explanation for the low intrinsic fluctuation in these studies is that transcriptional fluctuations are filtered by nuclear retention, though other reports suggest that Fano factors (variance/mean, a measure of overdispersion compared to the Poisson distribution) can be even larger in the cytoplasm than in the nucleus [38].

      References

      Gelman A, Carlin JB, Stern HS, Dunson DB, Vehtari A, Rubin DB. 2013. Bayesian Data Analysis, 3rd edn. CRC Press, London.

      Hughes ME, DiTacchio L, Hayes KR, Vollmers C, Pulivarthy S, Baggs JE, Panda S, Hogenesch JB. 2009. Harmonics of circadian gene transcription in mammals. PLoS Genet 5. doi:10.1371/journal.pgen.1000442

      Kempe H, Schwabe A, Cremazy F, Verschure PJ, Bruggeman FJ. 2015. The volumes and transcript counts of single cells reveal concentration homeostasis and capture biological noise. Mol Biol Cell 26:797–804. doi:10.1091/mbc.E14-08-1296

      Padovan-Merhar O, Nair GP, Biaesch AG, Mayer A, Scarfone S, Foley SW, Wu AR, Churchman LS, Singh A, Raj A. 2015. Single Mammalian Cells Compensate for Differences in Cellular Volume and DNA Copy Number through Independent Global Transcriptional Mechanisms. Mol Cell 58:339–352. doi:10.1016/j.molcel.2015.03.005

      Raj A, Peskin CS, Tranchina D, Vargas DY, Tyagi S. 2006. Stochastic mRNA synthesis in mammalian cells. PLoS Biol4:e309. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0040309

      Relógio A, Westermark PO, Wallach T, Schellenberg K, Kramer A, Herzel H. 2011. Tuning the mammalian circadian clock: Robust synergy of two loops. PLoS Comput Biol 7:1–18. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002309

      Saini C, Morf J, Stratmann M, Gos P, Schibler U. 2012. Simulated body temperature rhythms reveal the phase-shifting behavior and plasticity of mammalian circadian oscillators. Genes Dev 26:567–580. doi:10.1101/gad.183251.111

      Suter DM, Molina N, Gatfield D, Schneider K, Schibler U, Naef F. 2011. Mammalian Genes Are Transcribed with Widely Different Bursting Kinetics. Science (80- ) 332:472–474. doi:10.1126/science.1198817

      Ukai-Tadenuma M, Yamada RG, Xu H, Ripperger JA, Liu AC, Ueda HR. 2011. Delay in feedback repression by cryptochrome 1 Is required for circadian clock function. Cell 144:268–281. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2010.12.019

      Vehtari A, Gelman A, Gabry J. 2017. Practical Bayesian model evaluation using leave-one-out cross-validation and WAIC. Stat Comput 27:1413–1432. doi:10.1007/s11222-016-9696-4

      Wu C, Simonetti M, Rossell C, Mignardi M, Mirzazadeh R, Annaratone L, Marchiò C, Sapino A, Bienko M, Crosetto N, Nilsson M. 2018. RollFISH achieves robust quantification of single-molecule RNA biomarkers in paraffin-embedded tumor tissue samples. Commun Biol 1:1–8. doi:10.1038/s42003-018-0218-0

      Zoller B, Nicolas D, Molina N, Naef F. 2015. Structure of silent transcription intervals and noise characteristics of mammalian genes. Mol Syst Biol 11:823. doi:10.15252/msb.20156257

    2. Note: This preprint has been reviewed by subject experts for Review Commons. Content has not been altered except for formatting.

      Learn more at Review Commons


      Referee #1

      Evidence, reproducibility and clarity

      The authors generated and analyzed a great amount of single-cell RNA FISH data over time on circadian genes (Nr1d1, Cry1, Bmal1), and performed model selection/fitting to explain the observed mRNA distributions. They decomposed the mRNA variability into distinct sources, and showed that intrinsic noise (transcription burst) dominates the variance. Therefore, looking at transcript counts may not be feasible to estimate single-cell circadian phase. However, the study is quite descriptive and ends up being a bit dissatisfying, so if the authors could improve this aspect by perhaps analyzing a mechanism on cell-specific burst size (F5), gene-specific dependence on cell size (beta), or the positive/negative gene-pair correlations (rho), it would help quite a bit in this regard. The model selection/fitting itself was not really sufficient to compensate for this, as it stands .

      Specific comments:

      1.It is hard to distinguish the RNA FISH signals (Figure 1A, 2B). It is probably technically challenging as the mRNAs are of low abundance. I think it may help if they adjust the contrast for the cytoplasm stain or just delineate the cell boundaries.

      2.In Figure 2C, the authors showed gene-pair correlations with cells of all sizes. Could the authors do a size-dependent extrinsic-noise filtering (Padovan-Merhar, Dev. Cell, 2015; Hansen et al., 2018, Cell Systems) to better dissect the correlations?

      3.For fitting model M3, as the authors pointed out, there are many local minima. Is the fitting score truly sufficient to eliminate the possibility for partial synchrony especially considering that the authors didn't show how effective the Dex treatment was to synchronize the circadian phase?

      4.Regarding model M4, the authors added a cell-specific noise term without specifying the contributing factors. Typically adding degrees of freedom should improve fitting and make it easier for a model to fit, why not in this case? Can the authors provide some explanations/mechanisms.

      5.The authors should include the number (range) of cells analyzed in the figure legends.

      Significance

      Overall, we felt conflicted about the manuscript. On one hand, the authors generated and analyzed a great amount of single-cell RNA FISH data over time on circadian genes. On the other hand, the manuscript was a bit dissatisfying/descriptive. If the authors could provide and analyze some sort of mechanisms on cell-specific burst size (F5), gene-specific dependence on cell size (beta), or the positive/negative gene-pair correlations (rho) it should help improve the manuscript.

      Referees cross-commenting

      I agree with Reviewer #3 regarding expanding the discussion to include the Shah & Tyagi and Raj et al citations on buffering. However caution should be exercised regarding ref 26 as it is quite controversial and subsequent analyses came to different conclusions (PMID: 30359620 and 30243562). The general consensus is that nuclear buffering of transcript noise (proposed in ref 26) is not a general phenomenon (ref 27 is specific to the calcium response pathway). In fact, the presence and evolution of specific pathways to buffer transcriptional noise, such as protein-protein mechanisms (Shah & Tyagi) or extended half-life proteins (Raj et al. and others), argues that transcript fluctuations are not probably buffered in general.

    1. mm 58 Better than Human "communist man" won't work, no matter how vigorous the indoc-trination program is. A culturally produced characteristic could sat-isfy all four criteria if it was inculcated early enough in the individual's development and was strongly supported by peer pressure and social practices. So traits that are the product of nurture, if they satisfy (l)-(4), could be considered part of human nature. Given how important culture has become for defining who we are and how we differ from other animals, this makes good sense. Using a notion of human nature that makes room for cultural traits is useful for evaluating worries about biomedical enhancements changing or destroying human nature. Sometimes, those who have these worries are concerned about biological changes per se, but some-times they worry about biological changes destroying cultural traits that they think are very valuable. For example, as we'll see later, Bush's Council on Bioethics and its chairman, the physician-bioethicist Leon Kass, think of human nature as including certain very specific relationships between men and women and between children and parents. They worry that if biomedical enhancements become wide-spread, these valuable relationships will be damaged. We needn't read them as saying that these relationships are purely biological; they may be culturally evolved relationships, though they're based in biology. Bush's Council apparendy thinks these relationships are so vital to a good human life that they are in effect part of our nature or what's natural for us. They worry that biomedical enhancements, especially genetic enhancements, will destroy these relationships and replace them with relationships that are unnatural, not really human. The Moral Imperialist Bait and Switch There's something fishy about the way the Council proceeds. Why do they think the way to stress that something's highly valuable is CHANGING HUMAN NATURE? 59 I Co say that it's part of human nature or natural human relations? ' That would only make sense if human nature or natural human I relations are always good. If human nature and natural human rela-tions are simply what we are like because of our evolutionary his-• tory, then there's no reason to believe they're good. In fact, what we ; learned about evolution in chapter 2 should make us think that at \ least some of nature, including the part of nature we call human I nature, isn't good. Why should we think it's any different with cultural traits? Anthropologists provide plenty of evidence that some deeply entrenched social practices are not only morally disgusting, but downright destructive. Here's one example among many. Among the Ilahita Arapesh, a tribe in New Guinea, there's a deeply entrenched I social practice requiring men to gorge themselves even when this means that their wives and children are chronically hungry and mal-I nqurished. This behavior is very stressful for the men, who sometimes I become physically sick as a result. But the social taboo on sharing food equitably with your wife and children is so strong that men con-tinue to act in a way that literally makes them ill and condemns their families to misery. Whether this practice was ever valuable seems dubious, but it certainly isn't now. It's an abomination. Female genital mutilation (female genital cutting, for the politically correct) may be easier to explain as an adaptation. Perhaps the first women to undergo this ghastiy procedure achieved a gain in reproductive fitness through the mechanism of sexual selection. In evolutionary terms, the excision of the clitoris served as a signal to the male that this woman was not likely to spread other guys' genes (mainly because she wouldn't enjoy sex enough to fool around). But once the practice became universal, it obviously couldn't play this role in sexual selection: If all women have it, it can't signal that any particular woman is special. Yet a woman who refrains from the practice would be at a reproductive disadvantage, because no one

      "human nature"'s definition usually refers to learned cultural values which can be harmful

    Annotators

    1. ³Ma\bHVHULRXV. DRFWRU WHOO PH, aW P\ aJH, PHaVOHV FaQ NLOO. ́TKLV ZaV WKH ILUVW WLPH I UHaOL]HG WKaW P\ IaWKHU FRXOG GLH. I ORRNHG XS WKHZRUG ³NLOO ́ LQ HYHU\ GLFWLRQaU\ aQG HQF\FORSHGLa aW VFKRRO, WU\LQJ WRXQGHUVWaQG ZKaW LW UHaOO\ PHaQW, WKaW P\ IaWKHU FRXOG bH HUaGLFaWHG IURPP\ OLIH

      I feel like these few lines, in a way, frame the whole relationship of Ka and her father, a way that many people can relate to. Although the "natural" way of life is for children to be alive for the passing of their parents, it is an idea that seems so far away to most lucky children, that when they hear of the possibility, they shudder. They think it could never truly happen. What is then more interesting is that it kind of seems like the daughter is in that same denial. She (a few sentences up) admits that her father may even be mentally ill, but has not seen the signs, besides the prison nightmares, but that alone could be a sign enough. This type of denial we have as kids, follows us. It is what keeps us in denial about our biggest fears.

    1. Reviewer #3:

      In this manuscript, Sachella et al examine the contributions of the lateral habenula (LHb) to fear conditioning. They use 3 different paradigms: (1) a contextual fear conditioning paradigm, (2) a cued fear conditioning paradigm, (3) a combination paradigm where both context and cues can predict shocks. They also manipulate the LHb in several ways: (1) using muscimol, (2) using inhibitory optogenetics, (3) using excitatory optogenetics. The results are thought-provoking and would represent a novel contribution to the field, but I am left confused about some of the major points. My suggestions for improvement/clarification of the manuscript are as follows:

      Major Comments:

      1) Some important points need to be brought up in the introduction in order to frame the problem the authors are addressing and motivate the study. First, the introduction needs more background on separate circuits controlling cued vs contextual fear conditioning (hippocampus, amygdala). This only comes up in the discussion. Readers also need more background on connections between known structures for fear conditioning and the LHb. There should also be explicit discussion of the well characterized connections between LHb and dopamine neurons, including how LHb inputs help generate reward prediction errors that may be important for fear conditioning. The idea that prediction errors contribute to the authors' observations could be foreshadowed here.

      2) In general, the muscimol experiments are nicely done. However, muscimol is always administered during training. I am left wondering whether LHb activity is required during the initial learning of the association or for consolidation later. It would be ideal to also include a test of muscimol infusion immediately following the FC training, during a memory consolidation period. This is important because the authors at times seem to argue that the LHb is important specifically for memory consolidation, but later in the discussion claim that activity during the training (related to prediction errors) is an explanation for their results.

      3) I'm struggling with the interpretation of the experiments in Figures 3 + 4 using the cue + context FC paradigm and talking about "reconsolidation." These are really key to the paper so making sure the experiments are clear is a must. From the cue + context test, it seems that having both cues + contexts available for memory provides a much stronger memory. I am uncertain about why the authors think this is so and whether the effect is independent of the LHb? For the "reconsolidation" experiment, I can't figure out what's new. The no-reconsolidation group should look like Figure 2 muscimol group, and it mostly does. The reconsolidation group should look like the Figure 3 muscimol group, and it mostly does. So this looks to me more like a replication of Figures 2+3 (with no vehicle control) than anything else. What did we learn that could not be learned from the experiments in Figures 1-3? The suggestion is that "FC training under inactivation of the LHb creates a cued memory whose retrieval depends on contextual information." (lines 154-155). I don't disagree with this interpretation necessarily but it seems vague, and there is no circuit-level insight as to the mechanism.

      4) The ArchT experiments, as the authors already recognize, are problematic because of potential heating and other artifacts. 25s of continuous 10mW green light is a lot. I am not left with much confidence in interpreting these experiments and therefore I am not sure why they are included in the paper. There are other methods of optogenetic inhibition that would be better suited perhaps, or the results could be replicated with chemogenetics, where the authors could ensure DREADD viruses did not spread into the medial habenula.

      5) The oChIEF experiments are interesting, but again very difficult to interpret. There is no data showing what the stimulation does to LHb firing, which is a concern given the very long light stimulation (through the whole experiment). Therefore, it is unclear whether the authors' hypothesis that the light stimulation interferes with normal function is correct. The design here also does not take advantage of the temporal precision of optogenetics.

    1. Examples: "John suffers from cerebral palsy." "John is inflicted with cerebral palsy." "John is physically challenged." Impact: These terms immediately suggest that the person with the condition has a poor quality of life. While having a disability may have an impact on one's well-being, many people have still managed to live productive and fulfilling lives. Some have even turned their situations into a positive, such as Rick Hansen, Christopher Reeve, Temple Grandin, and more. With this in mind, it makes little sense to automatically generalize all people with disabilities as "suffering." Instead, saying something simple like "John has cerebral palsy" or "John uses a wheelchair" would be a drastic improvement. It treats the person's condition as-is, without any judgment or assumptions about quality of life.

      the language that we use to describe disabilities can influence both how others see people with disabilities, but also impact how people view themselves.

    1. Without question, we don't have the same old Antigua in mind.)

      At face value, one may think the Mill Reef Club missing the old Antigua might be a good thing, but it does not help native Antiguans at all. While the natives miss the culture and appreciate the aspects of daily life, the Mill Reef Club misses the amenities and sense of escapism Antigua provided for them. Like tourists, they glorify the dirt roads and other "raw" aspects of Antigua, but they do not appreciate it enough to bring about improvement. They do not see Antigua as a home, but as a place that serves to benefit themselves.

    1. Readers and consumers of texts will have to get used to not knowing whether the source is artificial or human. Probably they will not notice, or even mind—just as today we could not care less about knowing who mowed the lawn or cleaned the dishes. Future readers may even notice an improvement, with fewer typos and better grammar. Think of the instruction manuals and user guides supplied with almost every consumer product, which may be legally mandatory but are often very poorly written or translated. However, in other contexts GPT-3 will probably learn from its human creators all their bad linguistic habits, from ignoring the distinction between “if” and “whether”, to using expressions like “beg the question” or “the exception that proves the rule” incorrectly.

      Getting habituated to text production as just another task that machines can do

    1. All refl ect incidents in which African-Americans were mistreated, assaulted or arrested for conduct that would be ignored if they were white

      we like to think we're behind slavery and racial inequality but we're not. while we may not have the same black codes as they did hundred years ago, they are still built into our system one way or another

    1. On the other hand it is possible that human control over the machines may be retained. Inthat case the average man may have control over certain private machines of his own, suchas his car or his personal computer, but control over large systems of machines will be inthe hands of a tiny elite—just as it is today, but with two differences.

      I still think that we have control over this type of stuff today.

    Annotators

    1. These plants are poorly understood by landscape architects and by those who love gardens.

      Contextualize

      Introducing exotic plants to a natural landscape can bring a different texture level to the site and bring an unexpected habitat to the existing landscape. This habitat may be useful or harmful to the existing condition, human has a plan to it but no power to control. One crucial element to remember when bringing in exotic plant species to the natural landscape is respecting the natural landscape and species. In the article, Wilde, Gandhi, and Colson state that “Recent ecological studies have found that landscaping with exotic plant species can reduce biodiversity on multiple trophic levels. To support biodiversity in urbanized areas, the increased use of native landscaping plants has been advocated by conservation groups and US federal and state agencies. A major challenge to scaling up the use of native species in landscaping is providing ornamental plants that are both ecologically functional and economically viable. Depending on ecological and economic constraints, accelerated breeding approaches could be applied to ornamental trait development in native plants. This review examines the impact of landscaping choices on biodiversity, the current status of breeding and selection of native ornamental plants, and the interdisciplinary research needed to scale up landscaping plants that can support native biodiversity”. There are chances of having negative impacts on the natural landscape when bringing in exotic species. Paying respect to the natural landscape is one of the keys to work on landscape design.

      Wilde, H., Gandhi, K., & Colson, G. (2015, January 28). State of the science and challenges of breeding landscape plants with ecological function. Retrieved November 12, 2020, from https://www.nature.com/articles/hortres201469

      Relate

      Marx believes that humans need to respect nature during landscape design because there are so many uncertainties when bringing in exotic plant species into an environment. That is the right way to consider the relationship between the natural environment and outcoming plants. However, as a landscape architect, they should think about what the role of landscape architecture is? How does it communicate with the general public? What is the value of it? There are a handful of elements that landscape architects need to consider on the design to lead to the question: is it necessary to bring exotic plant species? After researching, studying, and discovering about the area site and alien plant species, whether bring in exotic plant species will be an easy decision to make. Also, evaluated the risk of exotic plant species is necessary. Ferrell It is hard to identify the line Cooper described between human development and nature. Only humans can learn and discover through it to figure out what the line is? How are we balance out the relation with nature? Getting human, educated, and informed is something fundamental to do. Therefore, enough knowledge could help humans to figure out the balance and have respect for nature.

    1. “We may disagree with some of their tactics, but there probably isn’t a single Indian organization anywhere that would disagree with those 20 points. A lot of Indians out there are watching the protest and saying ‘right on!’”

      "Pigeonholing" is endemic to the media and human nature, and I think this is definitely something to be aware of today. When a group of people act out with a set of goals or ideas that go against a media outlet's agenda, the media outlet will take advantage on any potentially controversial actions (or actions that they can spin as controversial) done by those people and use it to discredit the goals or ideas of that group. Media outlets aren't the only ones guilty of this; we as individuals and even governments are! That being said, I am glad that the New York Times article included this quote. "Ideas are bulletproof," and creating discomfort has been used throughout history by underrepresented voices to be heard.

    1. We shouldn’t be cruel to animals, i.e. we shouldn’t harm animals unnecessarily
      1. I completely disagree with this statement as I once read in an article to which I totally agree too. A living thing needs to have certain characteristics to be counted as a living thing. These may include, speech, movement, reproduction, breathing, etc. As for animals, they have all these characteristics. The voice they take out makes us realize weather they're in pain or not. Moreover they're able to move if in danger. As for plants, the only 2 qualities they have are reproduction and breathing in one way or the other. So rather than considering cruelty towards animals, we should think twice before plucking out a plant and eating it.
    1. Doctors and nurses have much more expertise in managing cases even in using nonmedical interventions like proning, which can improve patients’ breathing capacity simply by positioning them facedown. Health-care workers are also practicing fortified infection-control protocols, including universal masking in medical settingsOur testing capacity has greatly expanded, and people are getting their results much more quickly. We may soon get cheaper, saliva-based rapid tests that people can administer on their own, itself a potential game changer.

      Similarly good news, but long-term effects persist in some patients, I think. Again, people may see all the good news as reason to ignore social distancing and mask rules.

    1. In the last 2–3 years, I’ve observed the phrase “Diversity of Thought” gain momentum among many tech leaders and beyond. Proponents of this approach argues the way we think and express our opinions, our eclectic personalities, the myriad of leadership styles and Myer-Briggs results, are just as, if not more, important than demographic diversity that focuses on one’s gender, race/ethnicity, sexual orientation, religion, disability, etc.

      while the idea of [[diversity of thought]] may have started off a little more innocent (or not...), it's changed over the years to uphold the status quo, while distracting from [[DEI]] efforts that are aimed at improving representation, and lived experience

    1. We account for it by the supposition that his metaphysical views, carefully excluded from his scientific work, are the results of an earlier and less severe training than that which has secured to us his valuable positive contributions to the theory of Natural Selection.   Mr. Wallace himself is fully aware of this contrast, and anticipates a scornful rejection of his theory by many who in other respects agree with him. The doctrines of the special and prophetic providences and decrees of God, and of the metaphysical isolation of human nature, are based, after all, on barbaric conceptions of dignity, which are restricted in their application by every step forward in the progress of science.  And the sense of security they give us of the most sacred things is more than replaced by the ever-growing sense of the universality of inviolable laws, -- laws that underlie our sentiments and desires, as well as all that these can rationally regard in the outer world.  It is unfortunate that the prepossessions of religious sentiment in favor of metaphysical theories should make the progress of science always seem like an indignity to religion, or a detraction from what is held as most sacred; yet the responsibility for this belongs neither to the progress of science nor to true religious sentiment, but to a false conservatism, an irrational respect for the ideas and motives of a philosophy which finds it more and more difficult with every advance of knowledge to reconcile its assumptions with facts of observation.

      This sums up the entire point of the article. Wright says that Wallace makes good points on Natural Selection, however wavers when he begins to define things he does not understand as non-scientific or metaphysical. This article seems to not just be an argument for Darwin and Wallace’s natural selection theories, but a rebuke of Wallace’s ideas on will and his assertion of those things he does not understand like consciousness and feeling being placed under the banner of “will”. This may be the greatest contribution of this article, the defense of science in the time where science and religion were still intermingled and becoming frayed. Psychology should always be used in scientific terms, things like feeling and consciousness are difficult to explain as every individual experiences these things differently but they should be looked at through the lens of science and not as some mystical force which has predetermined/preprogrammed how we feel, act, and think.

    2. which seems to us erroneous, that all causation is reducible to the conversions of equivalent physical energies.  It may be trite (at least we are not prepared to dispute the assumption) that every case of real causation involves such conversions or [p. 122] changes in forms of energy, or that every effect involves changes of position and motion.  Nevertheless, every case of real causation may still involve also another mode of causation. A much simpler conception than our author's theory, and one that seems to us far more probable is that the phenomena of conscious volition involve in themselves no proper efficiencies or forces coming under the law of the conservation of force, but are rather natural types of causes, purely and absolutely regulative, which add nothing to, and subtract nothing from, the quantities of natural forces.   No doubt there is in the actions of the nervous system a much closer resemblance than this to a machine.  No doubt it is automatically regulated, as well as moved, by physical forces; but this is probably just in proportion as its agency -- as in our habits and instincts -- is removed from our conscious control. 

      This seems to be one of the key points Wright is trying to make with this article. It is almost as if he was influenced by those before him which worked with the nervous system. Thinking about the nervous system as a machine (very William James-ian) he basically says here that our nervous systems have evolved to the point which we do not have to consciously think about them for them to do their jobs, sort of in the way which we automatically breathe and have a heartbeat. Giving agency to parts of the body which it seems that humans have evolved to not need to regulate and it moves and operates by physical forces not metaphysical ones. Does this idea still hold true, it would seem correct as electrical impulses sent out from the brain do operate our entire body almost without any “will” from the user.

    3. We may not be able to understand how such regulation is possible; how sensations and other mental conditions can restrain, excite, and combine the conversions of physical forces in the cycles into which they themselves do not enter; though there is a type of such regulation in the principles of theoretical mechanics, in the actions of forces which do not affect the quantities of the actual or potential energies of a system of moving bodies, but simply the form of the movement, as in the rod of the simple pendulum.  Such regulation in the sensitive organism is more likely to be an ultimate inexplicable fact; but it is clear that even in a machine the amounts of the regulating forces bear no definite relations to the powers they control, and might, so far as these are directly concerned, be reduced to nothing as forces; and in many cases they are reduced to a minimum of the force of friction.  They must,[p. 121 however, be something in amount in a machine, because they are physical, and, like all physical forces, must be derived in quantity from pre-existing forms of force.  To infer from this that the Will must add something to the forces of the organism is, therefore, to assume for it a material  nature. But  Mr. Wallace escapes, or appears to think (as others think who hold this view) that he escapes, from complete materialism by the doctrine of the freedom of the Will.  Though he makes the Will an efficient physical force, he does not allow it to be a physical effect.  In other words, he regards the Will as an absolute source of physical energy, continually adding, though in small amounts, to the store of the forces of nature; a sort of molecular leakage of energy from an absolute source into the nervous system of animals, or, at least, of men.

      Wright talking here about materialism over what Wallace hypothesized is being compared to a machine here. After the thoughts on sensation which most materialist say are just atoms discharging against one another, Wright begins to focus on this idea that Wallace theorized was “Will”. Wright is correct in saying that Will is just another name for materialism and Wallace doesn’t really escape what materialism is by citing it as “Will”.

    4. We say, and say truly, that a stone has no sensation, since it exhibits none of the signs that indicate the existence of sensations.   It is not only a purely objective existence, like everything else in nature, except our own individual self-consciousness, but its properties indicate to us no other than this purely objective existence, unless it be the existence of God.  To suppose that its properties could possibly result in a sensitive nature, not previously existing or co-existing with them, is to reason entirely beyond the guidance and analogies of experience. It is a purely gratuitous supposition, not only metaphysical or transcendental, but also materialistic; that is, it is not only asking a foolish question,[p. 119] but giving a still more foolish answer to it.  In short, the metaphysical problem may be reduced to an attempt to break down the most fundamental antithesis of all experience, by demanding to know of its terms which of them is the other. To this sort of fatuity belongs, we think, the mystical doctrine which Mr. Wallace is inclined to adopt, "that FORCE is a product of MIND"; which means, so far as it is intelligible, that forces, or the physical antecedents and conditions of motion (apprehended, it is true, along with motion itself through our sensations and volitions), yet bear to our mental natures the still closer relation of resemblance to the prime agency of the Will; or it means that "all force is probably will-force." Not only does this assumed mystical resemblance, expressed by the word "will-force," contradict the fundamental antithesis of subject and object phenomena (as the word "mind-matter" would), but it fails to receive any confirmation from the law of the correlation of the physical forces.

      This point that Wright is making, trying to use scientific reasoning to understand and ultimately cut through Wallace’s argument for the more “mystical” explanations in evolutionary science is very interesting. While a bit hard to process, it does seem as if the idea here is to discount any argument of the supernatural using what Wright knows as the order of nature. For example: we know what a rock is made of, we know it is not sentient, and therefore has a purely objective existence. All of these things can be observed and scientifically studied, which is what Wright is trying to hammer on here. Stating that the question of if the rock has sensation is not only foolish but any answer to explain sensation in a rock would be equally if not more foolish. This is important as Wright is seemingly being a scientific purest here, almost saying that there is no room for God in scientific pursuit taking all of the metaphysical out of psychology.

    1. As a consequence, the organization soon noticed the familiar set of symptoms indicating deeper problems. The most visible symptom was a surge in the number of support issues. Maybe that could be resolved by hiring more testers or even expand with a first line support?

      While the business may see the bugs come in, churn, unhappy customers - and think that we may need to solve by adding more engineering people, we should be considering other options.

    1. It is true that moral rules are often enforced much more strictly than the rules of etiquette, and our reluctance to press the non-hypothetical "should" of etiquette may be one reason why we think of the rules of etiquette as hypothetical imperatives. But are we then to say that there is nothing behind the idea that moral judgments are categorical imperatives but the relative stringency of our moral teaching

      6

    2. In writing about imperatives Kant seems to be thinking at least as much of statements about what ought to be or should be done, as of injunctions expressed in the imperative mood. He even describes as an imperative the assertion that it would be "good to do or refrain from doing something"' and explains that for a will that "does not always do something simply because it is presented to it as a good thing to do" this has the force of a command of reason. We may therefore think of Kant's imperatives as statements to the effect that something ought to be done or that it would be good to do it. The distinction between hypothetical imperatives and cate- gorical imperatives, which plays so important a part in Kant's ethics, appears in characteristic form in the following passages from the Foundations of the Metaphysics of Morals

      2

    1. (Hypothesis didn't recognize the text so I couldn't add a normal annotation): "Good evening. I'm sorry to -- bother you, but we just thought we'd better let you know that we haven't got anything left. We sent up all we had. There's no more food down here (111)."

      In Ben's communication with someone on the upper level, he shows a clear subordination to whoever it is. The pause after saying "sorry" made me think he had a momentary thought of: "I didn't sign up for this, why am I apologizing?" This is a fleeting thought though, if one at all, because he goes on to explain their inability to provide anything more. Without even knowing why, he's taken on the responsibility of giving Gus' things to an unknown "higher power." This bit of dialogue made it very obvious that the play is highlighting a blind adherence to authority, even if it's nonsensical to the viewer. We may even question our own judgement of it -- the hidden authority doesn't give us the space to clearly judge it. It can come off as humorous or frustrating that these orders have made the men panic.

    1. “If that girl got out of the seat when she was told, there’d be no problem. But apparently she had no respect for the school, no respect for her teacher, probably has no respect at home or on the street, and that’s why she acted the way she did”

      This may be controversial but I think think there should be a perspective citing both her actions and how she was victimized but placing emphasis on the way she was victimized as a result. If she would have gotten out of her seat, there would have been a strong likelihood we would have never heard about this story. However, due to the fact that she did not and the officer used excessive force to remove her, clearly making her a victim in the situation.

      I could equate it to driving. If you stop at a stop sign for less than three seconds, that's against the law. Say one stops at a stop sign for less than three seconds but due that action they get involved in a hit-and-run accident that leaves them inquired. Could stopping at that stop sign for the full three seconds changed the outcome? Possibly, but being involved in the accident doesn't make them any less of a victim.

    1. Three Stages of Existence

      1. State of Nature

      • Man's nature is not shaped by the society.
      • Understanding this state sheds light on social experience can shape men.
      • Man is unsocial; it has native knowledge and has not acquired other knowledge.
      • Man is independent, cannot be influenced by others.
      • Man is innocent, is leaning to self-preservation, is lacking interest, his
      • Man cannot return to its primeval state.

      2. Social Dependence

      • As new needs arise, man needs the service of other men.
      • Possessions & division of labor = evils of social life.

        > A man with possessions is a man with something to lose, and a man who is dependent on the activities of others is also dependent upon their dispositions.

      • Everyone is connected in circumstantial dependence & undesirable comparison

        • unequal circumstances may cause the "state of war"; it is where the government must exist to protect the rich's property and the poor's rights.
      • Social existence stay and cannot return to primitive nature.

      3. Social Contract/ Community

      • Men should overcome conflicts that may dissemble men.
      • Men must think that the goal is not to outdo each other but is to establish mutual connection.
      • General will must manifest thru democracy, only by abolishing unequal privileges and partial combinations

        > all institutions and practices that mediated the direct relationship of man to community or that divided man's loyalty—were declared illegitimate.

      • Better constitution = better influence

      • Men as we know them are formed by society, they are malleable, and their social and political participation is potentially inherently re-warding.

    Annotators

    1. Author Response

      Summary: This study tackles a difficult problem of understanding the basis for hippocampal theta rhythms through reduction of a highly detailed model, seeking to validate a reduced model that would be more amenable to analysis. The reviewers appreciated the attention to this challenging problem and the substantial work that went into it, but had several fundamental concerns about the methodology, interpretation, and reporting.

      We appreciate the detailed feedback provided to us by the reviewers and editors and we are pleased that there was an appreciation for the attention we have given to this challenging problem and the substantial work that went into it. We would like to thank the reviewers for their efforts.

      This feedback helped us realize that there was possibly too much presented in this single paper and moving forward, we will split the work into two papers. While we agree with some of the feedback, we think that some aspects were misunderstood, which may be partially due to the extensiveness of the submitted paper. Below we provide general responses to the points raised, leaving specifics for elsewhere.

      Reviewer #1:

      This study takes two existing models of hippocampal theta rhythm generation, a reduced one with two populations of Izhikevich neurons, and a detailed one with numerous biophysically detailed neuronal models. The authors do some parameter variation on 3 parameters in the reduced model and ask which are sensitive control parameters. They then examine control of theta frequency through a phase response curve and propose an inhibition-based tuning mechanism. They then map between the reduced and detailed model, and find that connectivity but not synaptic weights are consistent. They take a subset of the detailed model and do a 2 parameter exploration of rhythm generation. They compare phenomenological outcomes of the model with results from an optogenetic experiment to support their interpretation of an inhibition-based tuning mechanism for intrinsic generation of theta rhythm in the hippocampus.

      This statement summarizes our work to a certain extent but it misses a key aspect – the ‘mapping’ between the minimal (that this reviewer refers to as ‘reduced’) and detailed model is what is used to rationalize and motivate the subsequent extensive 2-parametric exploration in a ‘piece’ of the detailed model (which we termed the segment model). We will aim to write this more clearly in an edited version.

      General comments:

      1) The paper shows the existence of potential rhythm mechanisms, but the approach is illustrative rather than definitive. For example, in a very lengthy section on parameter exploration in the reduced model, the authors find some domains which do and don't exhibit rhythms. Lacking further exploration or analytic results, it is hard to see if their interpretations are conclusive.

      We agree that these are interpretations (not meant to be conclusive), but the goal was to use the minimal model to develop further insight as we did with a hypothesis development presented in the middle of the paper.

      2) The authors present too much detail on too few dimensions of parameters. An exhaustive parameter search would normally go systematically through all parameters, and be digested in an automated manner. For reporting this, a condensed summary would be presented. Here the authors look at 3 parameters for the reduced model and 2 parameters in the detailed one - far fewer than the available parameter set. They discuss the properties of these parameter choices at length, but then pick out a couple of illustrative points in the parameter domain for further pursuit. This leaves the reader rather overwhelmed on the one hand, and is not a convincing thorough exploration of all parameters of the system on the other.

      See above.

      3) I wonder if the 'minimal' model is minimal enough. Clearly it is well- supplied with free parameters. Is there a simpler mapping to rate models or even dynamical systems that might provide more complete insights, albeit at the risk of further abstraction?

      We agree that models can be even more minimal, but the goal here was not to further analyse the minimal model through simpler mappings or otherwise. Rather, it was to exploit linkages between the minimal model and detailed models to help understand how theta rhythms could be generated in the biological system (Goutagny et al. 2009 intrinsic theta), using a piece of the detailed model as a ‘biological proxy’.

      4) Around line 560 and Fig 12 the authors conclude that only case a) is consistent with experiment. While it is important to match data to experiment, here the match is phenomenological. It misses the opportunity to do a quantitative match which could be done by taking advantage of the biological detail in the model.

      5) The paper is far too long and is a difficult read. Many items of discussion are interspersed in the results, for example around line 335 among many others.

      We will split the paper into two.

      Reviewer #2:

      In this work Chatzikalymniou et al. use models of hippocampus of different complexities to understand the emergence and robustness of intra-hippocampal theta rhythms. They use a segment of highly detailed model as a bridge to leverage insights from a minimal model of spiking point neurons to the level of a full hippocampus. This is an interesting approach as the minimal model is more amenable to analysis and probing the parameter space while the detailed model is potentially closer to experiment yet difficult and costly to explore.

      We completely agree.

      The study of network problems is very demanding, there are no good ways to address robustness of the realistic models and the parameter space makes brute force approaches impractical. The angle of attack proposed here is interesting. While this is surely not the only approach tenable, it is sensible, justified, and actually implemented. The amount of work which entered this project is clear. I essentially accept the proposed reasoning and the hypotheses put forward. The few remarks I have are rather minor, but I think they merit a response.

      1) l. 528-530 "This is particularly noticeable in Figure 9D where theta rhythms are present and can be seen to be due to the PYR cell population firing in bursts of theta frequency. Even more, we notice that the pattern of the input current to the PYR cells isn't theta-paced or periodic (see Figure 10Bi)."

      This is a loose statement. When you look at the raw LFP theta is also not apparent (e.g. Figure 9.Ei or Fi). What happens once you look at the spectrum of the activity shown in 10.Bi? Do you see theta or not?

      We agree – to be done.

      2) l. 562 "This implies that the different E-I balances in the segment model that allow LFP theta rhythms to emerge are not all consistent with the experimental data, and by extension, the biological system."

      This is speculative. We do not know how generic the results of Amilhon et al. are. They showed what you can find experimentally, not what you cannot find experimentally. I agree with the statement from l.581, though : "Thus, from the perspective of the experiments of Amilhon et al. (2015) theta rhythm generation via a case a type pathway seems more biologically realistic ..."

      We agree – to edit accordingly.

      3) There are several problems with access to code and data provided in the manuscript.

      l. 986, 1113 - osf.io does not give access l. 1027 - bitbucket of bezaire does not allow access l. 1030 - simtracker link is down l. 1129, 1141 - the github link does not exist (private repo?)

      Our apologies that all of these were not made public as intended.

      4) l. 1017 - Afferent inputs from CA3 and EC are also included in the form of Poisson-distributed spiking units from artificial CA3 and EC cells.

      Not obvious if Poisson is adequate here - did you check on the statistics of inputs? Any references? Different input statistics may induce specific correlations which might affect the size of fluctuations of the input current. I do not think this would be a significant effect here unless the departure from Poisson is highly significant. Any comments might be useful.

      We were simply using the same input protocol setup done by Bezaire et al. 2016.

      5) l. 909 - "Euler integration method is used to integrate the cell equations with a timestep of 0.1 msec."

      This seems dangerous. Is the computation so costly that more advanced integration is not viable?

      Our apologies as the timestep was erroneously reported. At initial stages of the project, larger stepsizes were attempted to speed up computation. The stepsize/integration used were as in minimal model of Ferguson et al. (2017). That is, Euler integration with a 0.04ms stepsize for the cell simulations and Runge-Kutta for network simulations.

      Reviewer #3:

      [...] I have a number of methodological issues with the paper. First, both models should be validated against experimental evidence given that the experimental results exist. The validation of a "minimal" model with data from another model is circumstantial and useful to link two models, but in no way is a scientific validation, in my opinion. Second, the model reduction arguments are simply taken as a piece of a large model. This is in now way a systematic reduction, which the authors should provide. In the absence of that, the two models are simply two different models. Third, it is not clear what aspects of the mechanisms cannot be investigated using the larger models that require the reduced models, given that the models do not necessarily match. Fourth, the concept of a minimal model should be clearly explained. They used caricature (toy) models of (2D quadratic models, aka Izhikevich models) combined with biophysically plausible descriptions of synapses. The model parameters in 2D quadratic models are not biophysical as the authors acknowledge, but they can be related to biophysical parameters through the specific equations provided in Rotstein (JCNS, 2015) and Turquist & Rotstein (Encyclopedia of Computational Neuroscience, 2018). In fact, they can represent either h-currents or M-currents. I suggest the authors determine this from these references. In this framework, the dynamics would result from a combination of these currents and persistent sodium or fast (transient) sodium activation. Fifth, from the original paper (Ferguson et al., 2017) their minimal model has 500 PV and 10000 PYR cells (I couldn't find the number of PV cells in this paper, but I assumed they were as in the original paper). This is not what I would call a minimal model. It is minimal only in comparison with the more detailed model. While this is a matter of semantics, it should be clarified since there are other minimal model approaches in the literature (e.g., Kopell group, Erdi group). Related to these models, it is typically assumed that the relationship between PYR to PV is 5/1. This is certainly not holy, but seems to have been validated. Here it is 20/1. Is there any reason for that? Sixth, the networks are so big that it is very difficult to gain some profound insight. What is it about the large networks and their contribution to the generation of theta activity that cannot be learned from "more minimal" networks?

      The published minimal model (Ferguson et al. 2017) used experimental data constraints on EPSC and IPSC ratios to come up with the prediction of connectivity. As this connectivity was found in the detailed model (with empirically determined connections), this can be considered a form of validation for the minimal model’s predictions if one considers the detailed as a ‘biological proxy’.

      We agree that the segment model is not a systematic reduction of the detailed model. The segment model reasonably represents a ‘piece’ of the CA1 microcircuit that was experimentally shown to be possible to be able to generate oscillations on its own (see Goutagny et al. 2009 Supplementary figure 11). This was the assumption in determining the network size of the previously published minimal model. A large network is needed in order to appropriate capture the very large EPSCs relative to IPSCs that are present in the experiment. This is the essence of why smaller network sizes cannot be justified.

      Because of these concerns and the development of the paper, I believe the paper is about the comparison between two existing models that the authors have constructed in the past and the parameter exploration of these models.

      We do not fully agree with this statement. The minimal model was constructed by us (Ferguson et al. 2017), but the detailed model was painstakingly constructed in a state-of-the-art fashion by Bezaire et al. 2016. We used a ‘piece’ of this detailed model (see above) so that we could make ‘links’ with the minimal model in understanding the generation of intrinsic theta rhythms. This ‘piece’ also allowed us to do the extensive exploration for the additional results presented. The paper is about taking advantage of the comparison and linkage of minimal and detailed models to show how theta rhythms are generated and their frequencies controlled.

      I find the paper extremely difficult to read. It is not about the narrative, but about the organization of the results and the lack (or scarcity) of clear statements. I can't seem to be able to easily extract the principles that emerge from the analysis. There are a big number of cases and data, but what do we get out of that?. Perhaps creating "telling titles" for each section/subsection would help, where the main result is the title of the section/subsection. I also find an issue with the acronyms. One has to keep track of numbers, cases, acronyms (N, B), etc. All that gets in the way of the understanding. I believe figures would help.

      Another confusing issue in the paper is the use of the concept of "building blocks". I am not opposed to the use of these words, on the contrary. But building blocks are typically associated with the model structure (e.g., currents in a neuron, neurons in a network). PIR, SFA and Rheo are a different type of building blocks, which I would call "functional building blocks". They are building blocks in a functional world of model behavior, but not in the world of modeling components. For example, PIR can be instantiated by different combinations of ionic currents receiving inhibitory inputs. Also, the definitions of the building blocks and how they are quantified should be clearly stated in a separate section or subsection.

      The concept of building blocks was directly taken from Gjorgjieva et al. 2016 as cited in Ferguson et al. 2017 when we first used it, but also cited in the present paper, but for a different point.

      I disagree with the authors' statement in lines 214-216, related to Fig. 4. They claim that "From them, we can say that the PYR cell firing does not speci1cally occur because of their IPSCs, as spiking can occur before or just after its IPSCs." Figure 4 (top, left panel) suggests the opposite, but instead of being a PIR mechanism, it is a "building-up" of the "adaptation" current in the PYR cell. (By "adaptation" current I mean the current corresponding to the second variable in the model. If this variable were the gating variable of the h-current, it would be the same type of mechanism suggested in Rotstein et al. (2005) and in the models presented in Stark et al. (2013).) The mechanism operates as follow: the first PV-spike (not shown in the figure) causes a rebound, which is not strong enough to produce a PYR spike before a new PV spike occurs (the first in the figure), this second PV-spike causes a stronger rebound (it is super clear in the figure), which is still not strong enough to produce a PYR-spike before the new PV-spike arrives, this third PV spike produces a still stronger rebound, which now causes a PYR spike. The fact that this PYR spike occurs before the PV spike is not indicative of the authors' conclusions, but quite the opposite.

      The authors should check whether the mechanistic hypothesis I just described, which is consistent with Fig. 4 (top, left panel), is also consist with the rest of the panels and, more generally, with their modeling results and the experimental data and whether it is general and, if not, what are the conditions under which it is. If my hypothesis ends up not being proven, then they should come up with an alternative hypothesis. The condition the authors' state about the parameter "b" and PIR is not necessarily general. PIR and other phenomena are typically controlled by the combined effect of more than one parameter. As it stands, their basic assumption behind the PRC is not necessarily valid.

      The subsequent hypothesis (about PYR bursting) is called to question in view of the previous comments. The experimental data should be able to provide an answer.

      See above.

      The authors should provide a more detailed explanation and justification for the presence of an inhibitory "bolus". What would the timescale be? Again, the data should provide evidence of that. In their discussion about the PRC, the authors essentially conclude what they hypothesis, but this conclusion is based on the "bolus" idea. The validity of this should be revised.

      The discussion about degeneracy of the theta rhythm generation is interesting. However, because of the size and complexity of the models, this degeneracy is expected. Their minimal modeling approach does not help in shedding any additional light. In addition, the authors' do not discuss the intrinsic sources of degeneracy and how they interact with the intrinsic ones.

      The last two sections were difficult to follow and I found them anecdotal. I was expecting a deeper mechanistic analysis. However, I have to acknowledge that because of my difficulty in following the paper, I might have missed important issues.

      These last sections are where the ‘piece’ of the detailed model (that we termed the segment model) - a ‘biological proxy’ - essentially shows that the theta rhythm is initiated from the pyramidal cells and that the frequency is controlled by the net input to the pyramidal cells.

      The discussion is extensive, exhaustive and interesting. But it is not clear how the paper results are integrated in this big picture, except for a number of generic statements.

      The proposal that the hippocampus has the circuitry to produce theta oscillations without the need of medial septum input has been proposed before by Gillies et. (2003) and the models in Rotstein et al. (2005) and Orban et al. (2005). But the idea from this work is not that the hippocampus (CA1) is a pacemaker, but rather what we now call a "resonator". To claim that the MS is simply an amplificatory of an existing oscillator is against the existing evidence.

      We agree that many models show theta generation without explicit mention of the medial septum. However, what our modelling work shows is how the intrinsic theta rhythm is generated – it is initiated by the pyramidal cells (large enough network size with some recurrent connections) and the control of the theta frequency (LFP) is due to the net input to the pyramidal cells – this is the main claim of the paper. This is explicitly in reference to an intrinsic theta rhythm experimental context. From there, we suggest that MS and other inputs could amplify an already existing intrinsic rhythm in the CA1 microcircuit.

      References:

      Bezaire, M. J., Raikov, I., Burk, K., Vyas, D., & Soltesz, I. (2016). Interneuronal mechanisms of hippocampal theta oscillation in a full-scale model of the rodent CA1 circuit. ELife, 5, e18566. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.18566

      Ferguson, K. A., Chatzikalymniou, A. P., & Skinner, F. K. (2017). Combining Theory, Model, and Experiment to Explain How Intrinsic Theta Rhythms Are Generated in an In Vitro Whole Hippocampus Preparation without Oscillatory Inputs. ENeuro, 4(4). https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0131-17.2017

      Gjorgjieva, J., Drion, G., & Marder, E. (2016). Computational implications of biophysical diversity and multiple timescales in neurons and synapses for circuit performance. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 37, 44–52. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2015.12.008

      Goutagny, R., Jackson, J., & Williams, S. (2009). Self-generated theta oscillations in the hippocampus. Nature Neuroscience, 12(12), 1491–1493. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2440

    2. Reviewer #2:

      In this work Chatzikalymniou et al. use models of hippocampus of different complexities to understand the emergence and robustness of intra-hippocampal theta rhythms. They use a segment of highly detailed model as a bridge to leverage insights from a minimal model of spiking point neurons to the level of a full hippocampus. This is an interesting approach as the minimal model is more amenable to analysis and probing the parameter space while the detailed model is potentially closer to experiment yet difficult and costly to explore.

      The study of network problems is very demanding, there are no good ways to address robustness of the realistic models and the parameter space makes brute force approaches impractical. The angle of attack proposed here is interesting. While this is surely not the only approach tenable, it is sensible, justified, and actually implemented. The amount of work which entered this project is clear. I essentially accept the proposed reasoning and the hypotheses put forward. The few remarks I have are rather minor, but I think they merit a response.

      1) l. 528-530 "This is particularly noticeable in Figure 9D where theta rhythms are present and can be seen to be due to the PYR cell population firing in bursts of theta frequency. Even more, we notice that the pattern of the input current to the PYR cells isn't theta-paced or periodic (see Figure 10Bi)."

      This is a loose statement. When you look at the raw LFP theta is also not apparent (e.g. Figure 9.Ei or Fi). What happens once you look at the spectrum of the activity shown in 10.Bi? Do you see theta or not?

      2) l. 562 "This implies that the different E-I balances in the segment model that allow LFP theta rhythms to emerge are not all consistent with the experimental data, and by extension, the biological system."

      This is speculative. We do not know how generic the results of Amilhon et al. are. They showed what you can find experimentally, not what you cannot find experimentally. I agree with the statement from l.581, though : "Thus, from the perspective of the experiments of Amilhon et al. (2015) theta rhythm generation via a case a type pathway seems more biologically realistic ..."

      3) There are several problems with access to code and data provided in the manuscript.

      l. 986, 1113 - osf.io does not give access<br> l. 1027 - bitbucket of bezaire does not allow access l. 1030 - simtracker link is down l. 1129, 1141 - the github link does not exist (private repo?)

      4) l. 1017 - Afferent inputs from CA3 and EC are also included in the form of Poisson-distributed spiking units from artificial CA3 and EC cells.

      Not obvious if Poisson is adequate here - did you check on the statistics of inputs? Any references? Different input statistics may induce specific correlations which might affect the size of fluctuations of the input current. I do not think this would be a significant effect here unless the departure from Poisson is highly significant. Any comments might be useful.

      5) l. 909 - "Euler integration method is used to integrate the cell equations with a timestep of 0.1 msec."

      This seems dangerous. Is the computation so costly that more advanced integration is not viable?

    1. PDF version <img class="" alt="Supporters attend a campaign rally for U.S. President Donald Trump" src="//viahtml3.hypothes.is/proxy/im_///media.nature.com/lw800/magazine-assets/d41586-020-02948-4/d41586-020-02948-4_18498916.jpg"> A New Jersey campaign rally for US President Donald Trump, who has espoused herd immunity as a strategy to deal with the pandemic.Credit: Spencer Platt/Getty In May, the Brazilian city of Manaus was devastated by a large outbreak of COVID-19. Hospitals were overwhelmed and the city was digging new grave sites in the surrounding forest. But by August, something had shifted. Despite relaxing social-distancing requirements in early June, the city of 2 million people had reduced its number of excess deaths from around 120 per day to nearly zero.In September, two groups of researchers posted preprints suggesting that Manaus’s late-summer slowdown in COVID-19 cases had happened, at least in part, because a large proportion of the community’s population had already been exposed to the virus and was now immune. Immunologist Ester Sabino at the University of São Paulo, Brazil, and her colleagues tested more than 6,000 samples from blood banks in Manaus for antibodies to SARS-CoV-2.“We show that the number of people who got infected was really high — reaching 66% by the end of the first wave,” Sabino says. Her group concluded1 that this large infection rate meant that the number of people who were still vulnerable to the virus was too small to sustain new outbreaks — a phenomenon called herd immunity. Another group in Brazil reached similar conclusions2.Such reports from Manaus, together with comparable arguments about parts of Italy that were hit hard early in the pandemic, helped to embolden proposals to chase herd immunity. The plans suggested letting most of society return to normal, while taking some steps to protect those who are most at risk of severe disease. That would essentially allow the coronavirus to run its course, proponents said. Rethinking herd immunity But epidemiologists have repeatedly smacked down such ideas. “Surrendering to the virus” is not a defensible plan, says Kristian Andersen, an immunologist at the Scripps Research Institute in La Jolla, California. Such an approach would lead to a catastrophic loss of human lives without necessarily speeding up society’s return to normal, he says. “We have never successfully been able to do it before, and it will lead to unacceptable and unnecessary untold human death and suffering.”Despite widespread critique, the idea keeps popping up among politicians and policymakers in numerous countries, including Sweden, the United Kingdom and the United States. US President Donald Trump spoke positively about it in September, using the malapropism “herd mentality”. And even a few scientists have pushed the agenda. In early October, a libertarian think tank and a small group of scientists released a document called the Great Barrington Declaration. In it, they call for a return to normal life for people at lower risk of severe COVID-19, to allow SARS-CoV-2 to spread to a sufficient level to give herd immunity. People at high risk, such as elderly people, it says, could be protected through measures that are largely unspecified. The writers of the declaration received an audience in the White House, and sparked a counter memorandum from another group of scientists in The Lancet, which called the herd-immunity approach a “dangerous fallacy unsupported by scientific evidence”3.Arguments in favour of allowing the virus to run its course largely unchecked share a misunderstanding about what herd immunity is, and how best to achieve it. Here, Nature answers five questions about the controversial idea.What is herd immunity?Herd immunity happens when a virus can’t spread because it keeps encountering people who are protected against infection. Once a sufficient proportion of the population is no longer susceptible, any new outbreak peters out. “You don’t need everyone in the population to be immune — you just need enough people to be immune,” says Caroline Buckee, an epidemiologist at Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health in Boston, Massachusetts.Typically, herd immunity is discussed as a desirable result of wide-scale vaccination programmes. High levels of vaccination-induced immunity in the population benefits those who can’t receive or sufficiently respond to a vaccine, such as people with compromised immune systems. Many medical professionals hate the term herd immunity, and prefer to call it “herd protection”, Buckee says. That’s because the phenomenon doesn’t actually confer immunity to the virus itself — it only reduces the risk that vulnerable people will come into contact with the pathogen.But public-health experts don’t usually talk about herd immunity as a tool in the absence of vaccines. “I’m a bit puzzled that it’s now used to mean how many people need to get infected before this thing stops,” says Marcel Salathé, an epidemiologist at the Swiss Federal Institute of Technology in Lausanne.How do you achieve it?Epidemiologists can estimate the proportion of a population that needs to be immune before herd immunity kicks in. This threshold depends on the basic reproduction number, R0 — the number of cases, on average, spawned by one infected individual in an otherwise fully susceptible, well-mixed population, says Kin On Kwok, an infectious-disease epidemiologist and mathematical modeller at the Chinese University of Hong Kong. The formula for calculating the herd-immunity threshold is 1–1/R0 — meaning that the more people who become infected by each individual who has the virus, the higher the proportion of the population that needs to be immune to reach herd immunity. For instance, measles is extremely infectious, with an R0 typically between 12 and 18, which works out to a herd-immunity threshold of 92–94% of the population. For a virus that is less infectious (with a lower reproduction number), the threshold would be lower. The R0 assumes that everyone is susceptible to the virus, but that changes as the epidemic proceeds, because some people become infected and gain immunity. For that reason, a variation of R0 called the R effective (abbreviated Rt or Re) is sometimes used in these calculations, because it takes into consideration changes in susceptibility in the population. A guide to R — the pandemic’s misunderstood metric Although plugging numbers into the formula spits out a theoretical number for herd immunity, in reality, it isn’t achieved at an exact point. Instead, it’s better to think of it as a gradient, says Gypsyamber D’Souza, an epidemiologist at Johns Hopkins University in Baltimore, Maryland. And because variables can change, including R0 and the number of people susceptible to a virus, herd immunity is not a steady state.Even once herd immunity is attained across a population, it’s still possible to have large outbreaks, such as in areas where vaccination rates are low.

      I think this is super important. There is a large population of people in the United States who are against vaccinations. And we need to keep this in mind when looking at the backlash that this could cause when determining herd immunity. If there are enough people not getting vaccinations, the spread of the virus could be prolonged and outbreaks could be increased. I have even heard rumors and gossip about how once a vaccination is created, our government is going to force its citizens to take it. It will definitely be interesting to see what happens in the coming months to years.

    1. The question arises as to whether utilitarian objects communicate meaning in theway great artworks do:does a wooden chair designed by the 20thcentury architect Frank Lloyd Wright carry meaningin the same way a painting by Rembrandt does? Theanswer is probably not, but this does notmean that Wright’s chair is devoid of meaning. Rather, its meaning is of a different kind. Rembrandt’s painting may speak tous of a mother’s love for her child, or the poignancy of the painter observing his face as he ages. Wright’s chair, in turn, speaks to us on its most basic level of a certainbody posture and way of sitting, its form and style suggesting perhaps a certain style of human interaction. If we dig into history, we may discover that Wright’s designs reflected the social climate of his moment in history, and his relation to the Arts and Crafts movement that responded against the perceived sterility of industrially-produced furniture and other goods.

      Do a little research on Rembrandt's paintings and Frank Lloyd Wright's designs as described in https://flwright.org/researchandexplore/furnitureanddecorativearts. Do you think Wright would welcome a Rembrandt painting into his interior designs? Why or why not?

    1. I thought this was extremely interesting. To begin, I agree greatly with the line in the beginning that states, "Whenever new approaches or techniques are being advocated, a very understandable ill-humor overcomes those who feel they may have to modify or to reconsider well-established pedagogical habits." I think this is accurate because many times, I've had English teachers complain or explain how new techniques or way of teaching literature are difficult to adapt to and requires them to change their logic on a lot and break old habits. I also thought that noting that their response feeds off of " moral indignation." Truthfully, whether they believe it or not, I find that a lot of teachers do teach the way they do or explain things the way they do based off of their morals and values. They aren't on the Supreme Court, so it is okay. Another thing that stuck out to me in this piece was the line, " Perhaps the most difficult thing for students and teachers to realized is that their appreciation is measured by the analytical rigor of their own discourse about literature, a criterion that is not primarily or exclusively aesthetic." Basically what I get form that after a bit of deciphering is that we appreciate the literature we are able to accurately discuss, and the more interested we are in our discussion about certain literature, the greater appreciation we have for it. It is not always an aesthetic, because it doesn't have to be something that necesarrily draws you in to imply an "aesthetic," yet it is merely a piece of literature that you are able to discuss based off of how much effort you put into analyzing it. I also liked how the author discussed how teaching English is not a "substitution for the teaching of theology, ethics, psychology, or intellectual history." I think it does indeed have to do with close reading. I've personally always thought of those subjects as completely seperate, however, it's interesting to see another viewpoint and understand why and how those possibilities could arise.

    1. Summary: In this well done manuscript, the authors examine the bHLH transcription factor TWIST1 and its interacting proteins in neural crest cell development using an unbiased screen. Given the important role of neural crest cells in craniofacial and cardiac developmental defects, the data are both useful and important.

      The major problem is the claim that the regulation reported here is important for neural crest specification / induction. This cannot be the case, as Twist 1 starts to be expressed in mouse only during the delamination step according to published single cell data. The premigratory Zic/Msx positive neural crest shows no expression of Twist1 before EMT markers kick in. The authors need to deal with this. It would be important to show in vivo expression data analysis and bring the conclusions in line with the timing in neural crest development.

      Reviewer #1:

      This excellent study is focused on the mechanisms of action of Twist1 in the neural crest cells and on the identification of core components of the Twist1 network. The authors performed an in-depth experimental study and sophisticated analysis to identify Chd7/8 as the key partners of Twist 1 during NCC development. This identification and corresponding predictions later appeared consistent with experimental in vivo data including single and combinatorial gene knockout mouse models with phenotypes in the cranial neural crest. Overall, this study is important for the field. However, I disagree with some secondary interpretations the authors give to their results. At the same time, the major conclusions stay solid. Below I discuss the most critical points.

      1) Chd7, Chd8 and Whsc1 are ubiquitously expressed. Thus, the specificity of regulation is achieved via interactions with other, more cell type- and stage-specific, factors. This would be good to mention.

      2) The authors suggest: "The phenotypic data so far indicate that the combined activity of TWIST1-chromatin regulators might be required for the establishment of NCC identity. To examine whether TWIST1- chromatin regulators are required for NCC specification from the neuroepithelium and to pinpoint its primary molecular function in early neural differentiation, we performed an integrative analysis of ChIP-seq datasets of the candidates".

      • This is a strange assumption, given that Twist1 is expressed only starting from the NCC delamination stage in mouse cranial neural crest (Soldatov et al., 2019). It does not seem to correlate with premigratory NCC identity and the situation inside of the neural tube. The authors conclude: "Therefore, combinatorial binding sites for TWIST1, CHD7 and CHD8 may confer specificity for regulation of patterning genes in the NECs." Or, alternatively, they may confer the control of mesenchymal phenotype, downstream migration and fate biasing etc. I do not think the authors have good arguments to bring up induction or patterning of NCCs at the level of the neural tube.
      • I have a good suggestion for the authors: I would extract the regulons from Soldatov et al. single cell data and run the binding site proximity check for the individual genes belonging to the gene modules /regulons specific to delamination and early NCC migration stages. I am curious, if the proximity of binding sites of Twist1-related crowd would rather correlate with genes from these specific regulons as compared to randomly selected regulons from the entire published single cell dataset. Randomization/bootstrapping analysis are welcomed. So far, being an excellent study, this paper does not solve a problem of downstream (of Twist1) gene expression program in the neural crest cells. At the same time, this is what the author can try to obtain with their DNA binding data in combination with published single cell data. Repression of Sox2 and upregulation of Pdgfra (reported in Figure 4) might be a part of this downstream program being in line with the published single cell gene expression data (Soldatov et al., 2019).
      • The authors conclude the paragraph: "Therefore, combinatorial binding sites for TWIST1, CHD7 and CHD8 may confer specificity for regulation of patterning genes in the NECs". Again, this is not a good or plausible explanation based on specificity of expression of suggested patterning genes (or visualized genes are poorly selected). Additionally, although I believe the obtained results are important and of a good quality, I would not call them "developmentally equivalent to ectomesenchymal NCCs" or other NCCs. This is because the in vitro system will never reflect the embryonic in vivo development with high accuracy (especially when it comes to patterning and positional identity). This might explain that some prominent binding positions and interpretations the authors give do not correspond to the gene expression logic during neural crest development. Besides, Twist1 and Chd7/8 are naturally expressed in many other cell types and might target non-NCC genes (Vegfa?). This does not reduce the value of the data, but it is good to mention for the community.

      3) Figure 2: Twist1-/+ Chd8-/+ is repeated two times in panel B (but the embryos look differently), although the authors most likely meant to show Twist1-/+ Chd7-/+ in the second case. If this is indeed the case, the authors should also show a phenotype of Chd7 KO.

      4) The authors write: "Impaired motility in Twist1, Chd8 and Whsc1 knockdowns was accompanied by reduced expression of EMT genes (Pdgfrα, Pcolce, Tcf12, Ddr2, Lamb1 and Snai2) (Figure 6D, S3D) and ectomesenchyme markers (Sox9, Spp1, Gli3, Klf4, Snai1), while 375 genes that are enriched in the sensory neurons located in the dorsal root ganglia (Ishii et al., 2012) were upregulated (Sox2, Sox10, Cdh1, Gap43; Figure 6E).

      • From the list of genes characterizing EMT, I can agree only on Pdgfra and Snai2, the rest is unspecific for EMT, and appears rather ubiquitous or specific to different cell populations (non-EMT).
      • From the list of suggested ectomesenchyme markers, I cannot pick any gene that would be a bit specific for ectomesenchyme (within neural crest lineage) except for Snai1. Sox9 is broadly expressed also in the trunk neural crest, Spp1 and Klf4 are not expressed in early mouse ectomesenchyme, Gli3 is too broad and non-selective. I suggest to select other gene sets (check the expression with online PAGODA app from Soldatov et al): http://pklab.med.harvard.edu/cgi-bin/R/rook/nc.p63-66.85-87.dbc.nc/index.html
      • The choice of DRG genes is also non-optimal, as Sox10 is pan-NCC, Sox2 is expressed in early migrating crest and satellite glial cells of DRG and Schwann cell precursors, Gap43 and Cdh1 are not specific enough. These genes clearly suggest the beginning of neuro-glial fates or trunk neural crest bias. To be more precise and for claiming sensory neurons, the authors should come up with pro-neuronal genes such as neurogenins, NeuroD, Isl1, Pou4f1, Ntrk and many others.

      Still, overall, I agree with the author's main conclusions.

      5) The authors write: "The genomic and embryo phenotypic data collectively suggest a requirement of TWIST1- chromatin regulators in the establishment of NCC identity in heterogeneous neuroepithelial 403 populations". Again, I do not think the authors can claim anything related to the establishment of NCC identity. NCC identity, in broad sense, includes NCC induction within the neural tube, at both trunk and cranial levels. In mice, Twist1 is not expressed in trunk NCCs at all. At a cranial level, Twist1 is expressed too late to be a NCC inducing or patterning gene. As I mentioned earlier, it comes up during delamination.

      6) Figure 7G only partly corresponds to the positioning of the NCC markers in a mouse embryo. Id1 and Id2 are broadly expressed throughout all phases of NCC development and in the entire dorsal neural tube beyond the NC region. Mentioning Otx2 as a NCC specifier is strange. At the same time, Msx1, Msx2, Zic1 are excellent genes! Tfap2 is a bit too late, but still ok. Please keep in mind, Msx1/2, Zic1 are expressed before Twist1, and, thus, Twist1 can be downstream of this gene expression program. Also, these genes become downregulated quite soon upon delamination, whereas Twist1/Chd7/8 expression stays (in vivo). Expression pattern of Tfap2a better corresponds to Twist1, although Tfap2a comes a bit before Twist1, and, besides, Tfap2a is expressed independently of Twist1 in trunk NCC. Despite such gene expression divergence, Twist1-based networks might provide positive feedback loops stabilizing the expression of other transcriptional programs that were originally induced by other factors. It might be good mentioning this to the readers. This "stabilizing role" of the Twist1 network can be a really important one. Given the incremental and combinatorial nature of the phenotype in vivo - this is most likely the case. I believe these points are important to reflect in the discussion section.

      Reviewer #2:

      This manuscript, by Fan et. al, is a comprehensive look into the bHLH protein TWIST1 and its interacting proteins in neural crest cell differentiation. The study employs an unbiased screen where a TWIST1-BirA fusion is used in conjunction with biotin linking to collect Twist protein transcriptional complexes. (BioID-Proximity-labeling, TWIST1-CRMs). The work appears carefully done and the data and impact of this study are high given the nature of NCCs being involved as key players in craniofacial and cardiac developmental defects. The association of TWIST1 with the chromatin helicases CHD7 & 8 is important to understand as numerous TWIST1 loss-of-function studies indicate that its role in NCCs clearly is required for normal NCC function.

      The NCC cell line O9-1 is used to collect the data. Genetic interactions between TW1, Chd7, Chd8 and Whsc1 are tested in genome edited ESCs. Overall, this is a well-executed, interesting and important study.

      Reviewer #3:

      Using BioID, the authors identified more than 140 proteins that potentially interact with transcription factor Twist1 in a neural crest cell line. Most of these 140 Twist1-interactomes do not overlap with the 56 known Twist1 binding partners during neural crest cell development (see below). By focusing on several strong Twist1 binding partner candidates (particularly a novel candidate CHD8), the authors found:

      1) Twist1 interacts with these proteins via its N-terminal protein domain as demonstrated by co-IP.

      2) Compound heterozygous mutation of Chd8, Chd7 or Whsc and Twist1 displayed more severe phenotype compared to heterozygous mutation of Twist1 alone, for example, more significant reduction of the cranial nerve bundle thickness.

      3) ChIPseq analysis of Twist1 and CHD8 and key histone modifications revealed that the binding of Chd8 strongly correlates with those of Twist1, to active enhancers that are also labeled by H3K4me3 and H3K27ac.

      4) The binding of CHD8 requires the binding of Twist1, but not vice versa.

      5) Twist1-Chd8 regulatory module represses neuronal differentiation, and promotes neural crest cell migration, and potentially their differentiation into the non-neuronal cell types.

      The authors use an impressive array of different techniques, both in vitro and in vivo, and yield consistent results. The manuscript is nicely written. The findings are nuanced, but the major conclusions are largely expected.

      Critiques:

      • As the title states, the three key TWIST interacting factors that most of the study focuses on are chromatin regulators. However, the consequence of mutating these factors at the epigenetic level was not directly addressed, including the level of active histone modification, the accessibility of the Twist1/CDH co-bound promoters/enhancers, and the position of nucleosomes.
      • CRISPR-generated ESCs and chimera technology were used effectively to generate mutants. In comparison, the analysis of the phenotypes was rather cursory and can benefit from more in-depth molecular analysis. The altered genes found in mutant NEC and NCC in the last section of the study, especially, should be validated in mutants.
      • Across the manuscript, there were jumps from NCC to NEC and back. It will be important to justify why a certain cell type is selected for each analysis, focusing on the biological question at hand.
      • Using BioID, the authors detected 140 different proteins that interact with Twist1. However, only 4 of them overlap with the 56 known Twist1 partners (Figure 1A). This result suggests that BioID identified almost a distinct set of Twist1-interacting proteins, compared to the published results. The authors need to discuss the discrepancy, and the underlying reasons.
      • The authors show that Twist1 colocalizes with Cdh8, and is required for the binding of Cdh8, thus suggesting that Twist1-Cdh8 form a regulatory module. Given the degenerate nature of bHLH factor binding motifs, it is likely that the binding of Twist1, and subsequently the binding of Cdh8, are dictated by other transcription factors. Therefore, a motif enrichment analysis should be done among the Twist1/Cdh8 co-binding sites, and compare those motifs enriched in Twist1-only and Cdh8-only binding sites.
      • The increasing expression of DRG neurons genes in Twist1/Cdh8 mutants suggests a possible transition from cranial NC to trunk NC. Therefore, the authors should examine the expression of marker genes accordingly.
    1. ventually, a different fixation overtook extreme weather, and another after that. Such is the pattern of categorical learners. It may have been sharks before the Titanic, or the other way around—I’ve forgotten. Two years have passed since we saw “Nature’s Fury”; a year and a half since our president led the US to withdraw from the Paris climate accords. The boy is seven now, what Jesuits call “the age of reason.” The girl is five and learning to read. If current trends continue, the world is projected to be 1.5 degrees Celsius warmer than pre-industrial levels by the time they reach their late twenties. The scientific community has long held two degrees Celsius to be an irreversible tipping-point. Two degrees of global warming, according to the UN’s Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change (IPCC), marks climate catastrophe.

      I was surprised that this paragraph is exactly regarding the topic I wrote for my social commentary essay. Writing my essay, I learned so much about what disaster would happen and how the end of the world comes as result of global warming. Intuitionally, we could think the increase of 1 or 2 degree is not a big deal. We could think it is 'just' 2 degrees but is isn't just marginal increase. Our earth is very sensitive to climate change so if the increase of climate really happens, weather anomalies such as heat waves, heavy rains, and fine dust will occur more and more frequently and drought will be everywhere resulting us to lose all the food resources. Unless any food-substitution is invented, we are all put the death because of famine. This is very serious problem. Since I am reading this article after writing my essay, my fury and anxiety penetrate deeply into my heart. Now I once more awakened why we should confront the global warming issue.

    1. In Global Catastrophic Risks 2016, we referred to a number used in the Stern Review on the Economics of Climate Change: a 0.1% annual chance of human extinction. Stern uses this as a modelling assumption for discussing discount rates. There is a small amount of discussion of this figure in the Stern Review. It is clear that Stern did not intend the figure as an estimate. We’ve had a critique of our use of the figure forwarded to us, and we think its analysis is useful. We had no intention of using this figure in a misleading way, and we agree that we made a mistake in how we presented this figure. We should have been clearer about what the status of the number in the Stern Review was and about how we intended to use the comparison. Throughout the rest of the report, we are very explicit that we do not believe it is possible to make robust probability estimates of extinction or catastrophic risk and do not attempt to (except for asteroid and super-volcano risk). This mistake does not affect the validity of the main points of the report – that global catastrophic risks are worth addressing and that there are things we can do to address them. In our report, we originally wrote that: “It is easy to be misled by the apparently low probabilities of catastrophic events. The UK’s Stern Review on the Economics of Climate Change suggested a 0.1% chance of human extinction each year, similar to some rough estimates of accidental nuclear warfare. At first glance, this may seem like an acceptable level of risk. Moreover, small annual probabilities compound significantly over the long term. The annual chance of dying in a car accident in the United States is 1 in 9,395. However, this translates into an uncomfortably high lifetime risk of 1 in 120. Using the annual 0.1% figure from the Stern Review would imply a 9.5% chance of human extinction within the next hundred years.” We were aware that the Stern Review used this figure merely as a modelling assumption, and were trying to give a concise accurate statement. Our intention in using the figure from the Stern Review was not to try to pin down an accurate estimate of the likelihood of global catastrophe, but to demonstrate that existing serious analysis treats the 0.1% probability as a plausible modeling assumption, which would have consequences that are interesting and non-intuitive. We also had a full-page summary pull-quote, which said: “The UK’s Stern Review on the Economics of Climate Change suggested a 0.1% chance of human extinction each year. If this estimate is correct, a typical person is more than five times as likely to die in an extinction event as in a car crash.” This implies more confidence in the 0.1% figure than either we felt or expect the Stern Review to have felt, and more than our argument required. The car crash comparison was picked up in The Atlantic, which reported it as an unconditional claim and emphasised it in their article. We did not intend to argue that the 0.1% figure was an accurate estimate of extinction risk (as we did not plan to offer an estimate of extinction risk), so this was inadvertently misleading to Atlantic readers. We believe that in general The Atlantic stood out by doing an excellent job of engaging constructively with our work. We are also sorry in particular that we allowed the word ‘estimate’ to enter the soundbite on the full page. This error occurred at a late stage in the editing; the word was introduced to avoid an ambiguity, but not subjected to proper review. We have carefully reviewed our language concerning the Stern Review, and written to our partners at the Global Challenges Foundation who published the report to change this to: “The probabilities of these catastrophic events are low but not negligible. Moreover, small annual probabilities compound significantly over the long term. We do not know of a robust estimate of the annual probability of global catastrophic risk. Nor do we believe that we are able to create a robust estimate because the uncertainties in key parameters are so large. However, for extinction risks some experts have suggested that a 0.1% annual chance of extinction is within the range of plausible orders of magnitude. A 2008 Oxford survey of expert judgement on the topic implied an average annual extinction risk over the next century of around 0.2%. [1] The UK’s Stern Review on the Economics of Climate Change used 0.1% as an upper bound modeling assumption for annual extinction risk. Now let’s suppose that the chance of extinction were 0.1% per year and consider the consequences. It may seem at first glance that this would be an acceptable level of risk. However, that would mean an individual would be more than five times as likely to die in an extinction event than a car crash. Moreover, these small annual probabilities add up, so that the chance of extinction within the next century under this scenario is 9.5%.  A global catastrophe, which involves the death of 10% of the global population, is more likely than an event that involves human extinction. As a result, even if 0.1% were on the high side for extinction risk, it might be of the appropriate order of magnitude for global catastrophic risk.” We are also correcting a citation and adding a citation to [1] Sandberg, A. & Bostrom, N. (2008): “Global Catastrophic Risks Survey”, Technical. Report #2008-1, Future of Humanity Institute, Oxford University: pp. 1-5. We are also making the corresponding changes in the one-page soundbite and will also write to The Atlantic to inform them of the inadvertent inaccuracy in the article, and offer to help in correcting the nuance of the article.

      Beautiful example of a detailed (and reasonable) correction after the horse has bolted i.e. the report is out there, being used in media etc. I came across via 3 hyperlink trail from the report of an otherwise very admirable foundation where i found the claim "a typical person today is five times more likely to die in an extinction event than a car crash." There link took me to the Atlantic which had added an errata at the top (no doubt some time after their article was out) which took me to here.

      I also find the correction somewhat dubious:

      We do not know of a robust estimate of the annual probability of global catastrophic risk. Nor do we believe that we are able to create a robust estimate because the uncertainties in key parameters are so large. However, for extinction risks some experts have suggested that a 0.1% annual chance of extinction is within the range of plausible orders of magnitude. A 2008 Oxford survey of expert judgement on the topic implied an average annual extinction risk over the next century of around 0.2%. [1] The UK’s Stern Review on the Economics of Climate Change used 0.1% as an upper bound modeling assumption for annual extinction risk.

      The experts they cite are, guess what, Sandberg and Bostrom from the very same organization they are associated with (Future of humanity). Their number seems IMO too high. A 0.2% risk per annum => 20% chance of total extinction in the next 100y (and where do these point estimates come from anyway!).

      Furthermore, this is not a constant risk as with say a car crash (where the risk exists every time i drive).

    1. Many students believe that intelligence is fixed, that each person has a certain amount and that's that. We call this a fixed mindset, and, as you will see, students with this mindset worry about how much of this fixed intelligence they possess. A fixed mindset makes challenges threatening for students (because they believe that their fixed ability may not be up to the task) and it makes mistakes and failures demoralizing (because they believe that such setbacks reflect badly on their level of fixed intelligence

      I too used to believe this as a student in high school, and in high school i did not try things i thought i was gonna fail, i loved this passage, its also sad to think that there a lot of students out there that do not try something they could be good at.

    1. Note: This rebuttal was posted by the corresponding author to Review Commons. Content has not been altered except for formatting.

      Learn more at Review Commons


      Reply to the reviewers

      We thank the reviewers for their feedback and constructive comments to our work. We provide here a point-by-point response to the comments of Reviewers #1, #2 and #3 (text in grey and italic).

      Responses written in plain text correspond to Reviewer comments that have been addressed in the revised version of the manuscript provided at this stage of the review process (referred-to as “revised version I” below).

      Reponses written in bold text correspond to comments that need further experiments. The list of experiments we intend to perform to address these comments is provided in a separate document (Revision plan). The results of these additional experiments will be included in a later revised version of the manuscript referred-to as “revised version II” below.

      Reviewer #1

      The manuscript addresses an important topic, the posttranscriptional maturation of ribosomes. This topic is inherently interesting because we normally think of ribosome biogenesis as a sequential series of steps that automatically proceeds and cannot be "accelerated" in physiological conditions, but only "delayed" in the presence of genetic mutations. In short, the manuscript proposes that RIOK2 phosphorylation by the action of RSK, below the Ras/MAPK pathway promotes the synthesis of the human small ribosomal subunit.

      I honestly admit that I have some difficulties in reviewing this manuscript. The quality of the presented data is, in generally, good. However, overall I find the whole manuscript preliminary and I am not much convinced of the conclusions. Several aspects are superficially analyzed. In short, I think that most of the conclusions are not fully supported by the data because shortcuts are present. A list of all the aspects that I found wrong are listed.

      Biological issue

      1. _The authors claim that the effects of the inhibition of the maturation of ribosomes by acting on a pathway upstream of RIOk2 are limited to the 40S subunit. This is far from being a trivial point, for the following reason. RIOK2 is known to affect the maturation of 40S ribosomes. Hence, the fact that using an upstream inhibitor of the MAPK pathway such as PD does not inhibit 60S processing in reality would argue against a biologically relevant control in ribosome maturation (of the MAPK patheay). Have the authors considered this? In a way, also, given the fact that the mutants confirm a role in 18S final maturation, it is a bit complex to put all the data in a clear biological context.

      We agree that we put more emphasis on the effects on the pre-40S pathway than on the pre-60S pathway in the original manuscript but we did not claim that the effects of PD or LJH inhibitors of the MAPK pathway are restricted to the 40S subunit. We described that the effect of PD or LJH on the 32S was less severe than on the 30S, and we did mention variations of the 12S intermediate. These changes are in the same range of amplitude as the changes in the 21S and 18S-E intermediates in the small subunit pathway. The Northern blot data concerning the pre-60S pathway were placed in the supplementary material of the original manuscript, which may have left the reader with an impression of lesser emphasis. We rephrased this part in the present revised version I of the manuscript (Page 6, Line 26) and we now show the pre-40S and pre-60S intermediates on the same figures (Figures 1A and 1C).

      In addition, we will probe more exhaustively the intermediates of the pre-60S pathway in the revised version II of the manuscript as described in the revision plan. These data will be complemented with metabolic labeling experiments to provide a more dynamic analysis of the pre-rRNA processing defects resulting from inactivation of the MAPK pathway. Furthermore, as requested by Reviewer #2 (see below), we will quantify more accurately these data.

      A number of specific issues will be concisely described.

      Manuscript very well written. Data do not always support the strong conclusions. Low magnitude of the observed effects.

      In introduction the authors make a general claim that ribosome biogenesis is one of the most energetically demanding cellular activities. This statement lingers in the literature since 15 years but in reality it has never been formally proved for mammalian cells, and certainly not for HEK293 cells. The original statement, to my knowledge, can be traced by some obscure statement referred to the yeast case and then repeated as a truth. In conclusion, beside being a very banal observation, it should be referenced.

      We agree with this comment of Reviewer #1. The original statement has been proposed by Jonathan R. Warner (Warner, 1999, TiBS and references therein) and data from the Bähler group also supported this statement (Marguerat et al., 2012, Cell). However, these data were indeed referring to yeast (S. cerevisiae and S. pombe). In the present revised version I of the manuscript, we introduced the reference of a review providing quantitative data of ribosome biogenesis in human cells (Lewis & Tollervey, 2000, Science) and we modified the problematic sentence as follows:” Growing human cells produce around 7500 ribosomal subunits per minutes (Lewis and Tollervey 2000), which represents a significant expenditure of energy.” (Page 4, Line 1).

      Growth factors, energy status are not cues but are proteins or metabolites (introduction).

      We agree with this comment of Reviewer #1. We changed the text accordingly in the revised version I of the manuscript (Page 4, Line 8).

      Authors write about mTOR without making statements on mTORC1/2. This is very obsolete. Also I am not sure that the choice of Geyer et al., 1982, and subsequent papers makes much sense. At the very minimum TOP mRNA concepts and mTORC1 must be defined.

      We provide more details on the mTOR pathway in the revised version I of the manuscript according to Reviewer #1’s suggestions (Page 4, Line 13 and Page 5, Line 3).

      The authors claim that their work fills a major gap between known functions of MAPK and cytoplasmic translation. I would not be so sure about it.

      Our original sentence stated that “our work fills a major gap between currently known functions of MAPK signaling in Pol I transcription and cytoplasmic translation”. Indeed, although MAPK signaling was known to regulate Pol I transcription and cytoplasmic translation, the impact of the pathway on the post-transcriptional steps of ribosome synthesis, namely pre-ribosome assembly and maturation, has been very little investigated and remains poorly understood. Our data provides the first example of a detailed mechanism of regulation of the maturation of pre-ribosomal particles by the MAPK pathway. Reviewers #2 and #3 seem to agree with this point:

      Reviewer #2: “However, there is a lacking mechanistic connection of signaling pathways to pre-rRNA processing and maturation steps of ribosome biogenesis. The authors set out to provide a specific example of a direct target of MAPK signaling, RSK that regulates pre-rRNA maturation through the phosphorylation of a ribosome assembly factor (RIOK2), offering for the first time providing mechanistic insight into MAPK regulation of pre-rRNA maturation.

      Reviewer #3: “With these provisos, the work is technically good and will be of considerable interest to the field. The post-transcriptional regulation of ribosome synthesis is increasingly recognized a significant topic.

      Results. Authors start with a major mistake, i.e. that PMA selectively stimulates the MAPK pathway. Perhaps it stimulates, certainly it does not do it selectively.

      We agree with this comment of Reviewer #1. We removed the term “selectively” in the problematic sentence (Page 6, Line 8).

      RIOK2 phosphosites are first found by bioinformatics analysis. It should be noted that the predicted phosphosite (S483) is found only in a limited set of datasets from MS databases. The actual importance of this site would not emerge from unbiased studies. Also, there are many other phosphosites that were not analyzed in this study.

      We agree with Reviewer #1 that phosphorylation of S483 of RIOK2 has been detected in a limited number of mass spectrometry datasets, but these datasets have been reported in high impact journals (Nature Methods, Mol Cell Proteomics, Science), attesting of the quality of these studies

      As mentioned by Reviewer #1, there are several other phosphosites within RIOK2 that were not analyzed in our study. We provided the list of these phosphosites in Supplementary Table S1 of the original manuscript. Besides T481 and S483, none of the other sites belong to consensus motifs recognized by ERK or RSK at medium and high stringency. They are therefore less relevant to our study. We only analyzed phosphorylation at S483 because: (i) our mass spectrometry analysis revealed that S483 is the only phosphosite in RIOK2 whose level increases upon MAPK activation but not in the presence of the MAPK inhibitor PD184352 (Figure 2B); (ii) our in vitro kinase assay showed that the phosphorylation level of RIOK2 by RSK is residual when S483 is replaced by a non-phosphorylatable alanine (Figure 3D); (iii) our data presented in Figure 2C further show that mutation of T481 to an alanine does not prevent RIOK2 phosphorylation on RxRxxS/T motifs upon stimulation of the MAPK pathway.

      We clarified this point in the relevant part of the result section of the revised version I of the manuscript (Page 7, Lines 16 and 24, Page 8, Line 17 and Page 9, Line 5).

      Throughout the paper the authors use the word strongly, significantly, but the actual effects seem in general quite marginal.

      We agree with Reviewer #1 that some of the phenotypes described in the manuscript are modest, in particular the phenotypes resulting from the S483A mutation of RIOK2, which is not aberrant for a point mutation. We rephrased several sentences throughout the manuscript to soften the formulation in the description and interpretation of the data and in the conclusions.

      Discussion. The authors claim that they provide solid evidence on MAPK signalling to ribosome maturation. At the very best this is circumstantial evidence for the 40S maturation.

      We rephrased the sentence accordingly (Page 16, Line 5): “Our study provides evidence that MAPK signaling applies another level of coordination during ribosome biogenesis, by directly regulating pre-40S particle assembly and maturation.

      Figure 1.

      Unclear why LJH should increase P-ERK.

      A negative feedback loop has been described in the MAPK pathway whereby RSK activation partially inhibits ERK phosphorylation (Saha et al., 2012, Horm Metab Res; Dufresne et al., 2001, MCB; Schneider et al., 2011, Neurochem; Re Nett et al., 2018, EMBO Rep). Inactivation of RSK with LJH alleviates this inhibition, which results in increased phosphorylation levels of ERK.

      We added this information in the revised version of the manuscript along with the corresponding references (Page 6, Line 17).

      General lack of quantitation (sd, replicates, bars). Experiment done only on a single cell line in a single experimental setup.

      As also requested by Reviewer #2 (Major comment 1.), we applied in the revised version I of the manuscript RAMP quantifications to all Northern blot data. We included error bars corresponding to biological replicates.

      Furthermore, in order to validate the impact of the MAPK pathway on pre-ribosome assembly and maturation, we plan to perform the same experiments using PD inhibitors in different cell lines and we will provide a figure with accurate RAMP quantifications, error bars and statistical significance, in the revised version II of the manuscript (see revision plan).

      Very different effects on 21S by LJH, PMA and siRNA for RIOK2. Overall the message given by the authors is to me mysterious.

      We assume that the reviewer wanted to point out the difference between PMA, PMA+LJH and shRNA for RSK since we did not perform RNAi targeting RIOK2. We agree with this comment. We believe that this difference is likely due to experimental setups that are different between both experiments. In the experiment using inhibitors, we assessed short-term effects of RSK inhibition after acute stimulation of the MAPK pathway (starved cells stimulated with PMA), while in the experiment using shRSK, we monitored long term effects of RSK depletion in serum-growing cells in which other signaling pathways are also active. Prolonged RSK depletion is likely to induce pleiotropic cellular effects, which would interfere with ribosome biogenesis both directly and indirectly. These differences probably explain the variable effects on the 21S intermediate. However, in both experiments we do observe an accumulation of the early 30S intermediate, consistent with the phenotype observed when ERK is inactivated (PD inhibitor), therefore indicating that RSK regulates some post-transcriptional stages of ribosome biogenesis.

      To make our results clearer we have withdrawn the experiments using shRSK to avoid the risk of showing indirect effects due to the prolonged absence of RSK. Instead, we included RAMP analyses with error bars from 2 biological replicates using PD and LJH inhibitors (Figure 1B).

      Figure 2.

      Several red flags. For instance in 2C the loaded levels of RIOK2-HA loaded are clearly less than the ones of the other genotypes, hence the conclusion on P-RIOK2 is not convincing.

      Our aim in this experiment was to compare the impact of PMA treatment on the phosphorylation levels of different RIOK2 mutants (T481A, S483A, double mutant). For a given mutant, the levels of RIOK2 loaded in the two conditions (i.e. not stimulated and PMA stimulated) are very similar and we therefore assume that our conclusions are valid.

      We nevertheless plan to repeat these experiments and quantify the data for the revised version II of the manuscript.

      Staining with anti-P RIOK2 lacks controls, how can be sure that the signal is due to the phosphate? Phosphatase treatment?

      We fully agree with Reviewer #1 and we did perform an experiment showing that the phosphorylation signal disappears following treatment of the protein extracts with λ-phosphatase. We did not show these data in the original version of the manuscript because of space limitations. We added these data in the supplementary material of the revised version I of the manuscript (Supplementary Figure S2B) and amended the text accordingly (Page 7, Line 24)

      Why FBS does not lead to ERK staining in HEK293? There are plenty of growth factors in FBS that should lead to ERK phosphorylation. I do not understand this experiment.

      We agree with this comment. Addition of serum to starved cells does lead to ERK and RSK phosphorylation but with a much lesser efficiency compared to stimulation by EGF and PMA. ERK phosphorylation is barely visible on the exposure shown in Figure 2D but RSK-phosphorylation is clearly observed, although the signal is much weaker compared to EGF and PMA treatments. It is common to observe a stronger response with purified PMA and EGF (see Carrière et al., 2011, JBC ; Ray et al., 2013, Oncogene). There are indeed several growth factors in the serum, but the most abundant (Insulin, IGF1, TGF) are present at ng/ml concentration, while EGF is used at 25 µg/ml in Figure 2D. Moreover, they are not very strong activators of the Ras/MAPK pathway, and it is also possible that after 20 min of FBS treatment the phosphorylation is in the decreasing phase.

      In the present revised version I of the manuscript, we included a set of western blots from another experiment showing the same results but of better quality to make the effects more visible (Fig. 2D). We also provided quantifications of phosphorylation of RIOK2 and associated statistical analyses (Fig. 2E).

      Figure 3. In vitro phosphorylation, if I understood, it relies on a truncated version of RIOK2. Why? Is the folding of the full length protein not permissive to in vitro phosphorylation?

      We did not test phosphorylation of the full length RIOK2 protein in vitro because RIOK2 has been reported to auto-phosphorylate (Zemp I. et al., 2009, JCB) and we were concerned that this auto-phosphorylation activity of RIOK2 in addition to RSK phosphorylation may render this experiment inconclusive.

      HA-RSK3 is less?

      It was reported that RSK3 is insoluble when over-expressed (Zhao et al., 1996, JBC), which explains the lower levels of protein recovered in our soluble extract. The information was present in the legend of Figure but we transferred it to the main text of the result section in the present revised version I of the manuscript (Page 10, Line 3).

      Figure 4. Immunofluorescence is low mag, difficult to understand.

      We agree with Reviewer #1. We modified the FISH experiment figure to show cells with a higher magnification and we provided more details in the text (Page 12, Lines 20-25) to facilitate the understanding of the data.

      I really like the experiments with RIOK2 mutants, however I wonder what about protein levels after the knock-in? Given the 18S phenotype overlap between the phenotype of the RIOK2 loss of function with the S483A, testing protein level becomes of the utmost importance.

      We checked RIOK2 protein levels and observed that the mutations do not decrease the level of RIOK2. On the contrary, the mutations slightly increase RIOK2 levels. Therefore, we are pretty confident that the phenotypes resulting from expression of RIOK2 mutants do not result from defects in the global accumulation of the protein. These data have been added to Figure 4C of the revised version I of the manuscript and we amended the text accordingly (Page 12, Line 5).

      Figure 5. Low quality IFL.

      Our aim in preparing this figure was to show many cells in the different images to show that the effect of our mutation was homogenous at the level of cell populations. The drawback is that cells are small and look blurred. We improved the quality of the figure in this revised version I of the manuscript with new images from the same experiment, showing less cells with a higher magnification.

      Hard to think that histogram quantitation of nuclear versus cytoplasmic staining are reliable in the absence of fractionation, better quantitation, experiment done in other cell lines and so on.

      We provide in this revised version I of the manuscript a supplementary figure explaining the procedure we used to quantify the fluorescence data (Supplementary Fig. S7).

      Furthermore, to confirm this result using other experimental conditions and cell lines, we will transfect HEK293 and HeLa cells with plasmids expressing GFP-tagged RIOK2 WT or the S483S mutant and we will compare the kinetics of nuclear import of both proteins upon inhibition of pre-40S particle export by leptomycin B using fluorescence microscopy and GFP quantifications. Second, we will transfect HeLa cells with plasmids expressing HA-tagged RIOK2 WT or S483A and perform fractionation assays to monitor their presence in both cytoplasmic and nuclear compartments. We will include these data in the revised version II of the manuscript.

      However, very beautiful Fig. 5E perhaps the best of the paper shows also mobility shift driven by S483, thus supporting posttranslational modifications.

      We thank Reviewer #1 for this comment. We added the note on the evidence of RIOK2 post-translational modification in the result section (Page 14, Line 9).

      Fig. 6. IFL studies are really impossible to interpret.

      We improved the quality of the figure with new images from the same experiment, showing less cells with a higher magnification. NOB1 IF data and quantifications have been transferred to the supplemental material (Supplemental Fig. S4A and S4B) to clarify the figure. In addition, we provided more explanations on the principle of this experiment and expected results in the text (Page 15, Line 9).

      The effects on RIOK2 release (this figure) and 18S maturation (Fig. 5) are very clear and of great quality.

      We thank Reviewer #1 for this comment.

      Overall conclusions. The manuscript tends to overinflate the meaning of several experiments. What to me is very clear and interesting is that the the authors provide clear evidence that S483A mutants have a defect in 40S maturation. Whether this is due to MAPK signalling, is only circumstantial. I would suggest to build up on the strong findings and eliminate ambiguous data.

      We do not fully agree with this comment of Reviewer #1. If mutation S483A were simply a partial loss of function mutation, this would not be of strong interest for the subject of this manuscript. It would just indicate that S483 is important for RIOK2 function independently of its phosphorylation status. Our data show that the impact of S483 mutation on pre-rRNA processing and other phenotypes is different depending on whether the serine is converted to an alanine (phosphorylation mutant) or to an aspartic acid (phospho-mimetic mutation). These data are a strong indication that what matters is not simply the serine residue by itself but its phosphorylation status.

      Reviewer #1 (Significance (Required)):

      The paper deals with an important topic, namely whether a regulation of ribosome maturation exists, and how it is mechanistically regulated. In this context, the analysis of the ERK pathway is highly needed considered that most works deal with effects of the PI3K-mTOR pathway, and the parallel, yet important RAS-ERK pathway, is less understood.

      As a final note, we should consider that S6K downstream of mTOR, and ribosomal S6K, downstream of ERK have been considered to share some substrates.

      We introduced this information in the revised version of the manuscript (Page 19, Line 20). A related comment has been raised by Reviewer #3 (see below, Caveat #2).

      The manuscript is interesting, but several statements given by the authors are rather superficial. An example, listed in the previous section, relates to the linguistic usage of mTOR kinase, instead of detailing whether we are dealing with mTORc1 or mTORc2.

      We agree with this comment of Reviewer #1. Given that the main focus of this manuscript is the regulation by the MAPK pathway, we had chosen to put less emphasis on mTOR in the introduction. However, we added more precise information on mTOR in the present revised version I of the manuscript to address this comment (Page 4, Line 13 and Page 5, Line 3).

      A second gross mistake is the definition of PMA as a stimulator of the ERK pathway. If this is certainly true, this is historically not correct as seminal papers by the group of Parker define this drug as a stimulator of conventional PKC kinases. In short, this paper is a step back in knowledge from the perspective of the literature context.

      We are a bit confused by this comment because seminal papers from the Parker group clearly state that PMA activates the MAPK pathway via PKC (Adams and Parker, 1991, FEBS Lett.; Ways et al., 1992, JBC; Whelan et al., 1999, Cell Growth Differ.). We agree, as mentioned earlier by Reviewer #1, that PMA is not specific to MAPK, a comment that has been addressed above.

      All people interested to the crosstalk between ribosome maturation and signaling pathways will be certainly read this manuscript.

      My expertise is within the ribosome biology and signalling field.

      Reviewer #2 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity (Required)):

      There have been mechanistic connections of various signaling pathways to regulation ribosome biogenesis steps including rDNA transcription by RNA polymerase I and III, ribosomal protein transcription, and differential mRNA translation efficiency. However, there is a lacking mechanistic connection of signaling pathways to pre-rRNA processing and maturation steps of ribosome biogenesis. The authors set out to provide a specific example of a direct target of MAPK signaling, RSK that regulates pre-rRNA maturation through the phosphorylation of a ribosome assembly factor (RIOK2), offering for the first time providing mechanistic insight into MAPK regulation of pre-rRNA maturation.

      The authors observe slight pre-rRNA processing defects upon the use of RSK inhibitors and RSK depletion. They identified several candidate ribosome assembly and modification factors containing the canonical RSK substrate motif, including the RIOK2 kinase. Phosphorylation at this motif was verified to be specifically phosphorylated by RSK1 and 2 isoforms in cells and in an in-vitro kinase assay. The authors produced RIOK2 knock-in eHAP1 cell lines expressing non-phosphorylatable or phosphomimetic versions of RIOK2, observing slowed cellular proliferation, decreases in global translation, slight pre-rRNA processing abnormalities, but not changes in overall mature 18S rRNA levels. More specifically, the authors defined the inability of RIOK2 to be phosphorylated leads to defects in RIOK2 dissociation from the pre-40S ribosomal subunit in an in-vitro assay, and inability for it to be recycled for reuse in pre-ribosome export from the nucleus to the cytoplasm by immunofluorescence.

      Overall, the authors provide an interesting mechanism of MAPK regulation of a ribosome assembly factor RIOK2. However, they fail to provide the necessary reproducibility, controls, quantification, and consistent results between experiments to support their hypotheses.

      Major Comments:

      1. The northern blots reported throughout the manuscript are lacking proper reproducibility and quantification. First, the northern blots are lacking a loading control, which is necessary to report fold changes that are being measured across treatments. Please include a proper loading control (i.e. 7SL or U6 RNAs). Additionally, more rigorous analysis of the pre-rRNA precursor levels through ratio analysis of multiple precursors (RAMP) (Wang et al 2014) can be completed to provide a clearer depiction on which precursor(s) are accumulating. It is unclear for the Figure 1 northern blots if there were replicates completed and what the error bars represent in Figure 1B. Please report replicates, so that statistical analysis can be completed on the differences in precursor relative abundance. This need is emphasized by the small changes observed in pre-rRNA levels (less than 2 fold) between conditions.

      As mentioned above (Reviewer #1), we applied in the revised version I of the manuscript RAMP quantifications to all Northern blot data. These quantifications are shown as separate panels in the figures of the revised manuscript.

      Furthermore, we are planning to repeat the Northern blot experiments of Figure 1 to obtain biological replicates in other cell lines. We will probe the membranes to detect the 7SL RNA as a loading control in all these experiments. We will perform RAMP analyses on all these Northern blot experiments to provide more accurate quantifications of the pre-rRNA levels in the different conditions. These data will be included in the revised version II of the manuscript.

      1. The western blots reported throughout the manuscript are lacking proper reproducibility and quantification. For example, the western blots validating RSK1 and RSK2 depletion in Figure 1C lack a proper loading control. Additionally, it is unclear if there are replicates completed and there is lack of statistical analysis to determine if the changes are significant. Please include loading controls, replicates, and quantification of the western blots throughout the manuscript.

      We have included actin levels as loading controls in several figures (Figures 2D, 3A, 3C, 3E, 4C) of the revised version I of the manuscript. We also added phosphorylated Rps6 at Ser235/36 to monitor RSK activity in Figures 1A, 2D, 3A.

      We provided quantifications and associated statistical analyses of phosphorylation of RIOK2 presented in Figures 3A and 3C of the revised version I of the manuscript. We also included quantifications of the in vitro phosphorylation assays presented in Figures 3F and 3G.

      We are nevertheless planning to repeat and quantify more accurately the western blot experiments presented in Figures 2A, 2C and 3E of the revised version I of the manuscript. These data will be included in the revised version II of the manuscript.

      1. Please report the full bioinformatic analysis of the RSK substrate motif search among human AMFs including other AMFs found in this search. A sorted list format would be valuable for the reader to understand other potential RSK substrates involved in ribosome biogenesis.

      We understand the request of Reviewer #2. Providing the full list of AMFs identified in our bioinformatic screen would be valuable for the reader, mostly because it would make clearer that RSK seems to be regulating multiple stages of the pre-ribosome maturation pathway, therefore that RSK inhibition induces pleiotropic defects in ribosome synthesis. However, we are currently working on a more global study of the impact of MAPK regulation on the post-transcriptional steps of ribosome synthesis that we would like to publish in a near future.

      1. The authors report that RSK inhibition/depletion leads to accumulation of the 30S pre-rRNA, yet mutation of its target site on RIOK2 or RIOK2 depletion leads to an accumulation of the 18S-E pre-rRNA. Additionally, the phosphomimic mutation of RIOK2 leads to an accumulation of 30S, the opposite of the expected result. Please elaborate on this discrepancy in processing defects observed across experiments.

      In contrast to RIOK2 which is specifically involved in the late, cytoplasmic stages of the maturation of the pre-40S particles, RSK regulates ribosome biogenesis at multiple levels. Upon activation of the MAPK pathway, RSK activates Pol I transcription in the nucleoli and promotes translation of mRNAs encoding ribosomal proteins and AMFs. In addition, our bioinformatic screen identified several AMFs at different stages of the maturation pathway of both ribosomal subunits as potential targets of RSK. These considerations imply that RSK inhibition is expected to impact ribosome biogenesis at multiple levels (Pol I transcription, availability of RPs and AMFs, export of the pre-ribosomal particles, probably several maturation steps) whereas RIOK2 inactivation more specifically delays 18S-E processing in the cytoplasm. In terms of processing, RSK inhibition induces a significant accumulation of the 30S intermediate. This is another evidence that RSK regulates pre-rRNA processing at several stages. This phenotype might result, as recently described in yeast (Yerlikaya et al., 2016, MCB), from an inhibition of RPS6 phosphorylation which affects its early incorporation into pre-ribosomes, although this has not been demonstrated in human cells. This 30S precursor accumulation affects production of the downstream intermediates and we strongly believe that this precludes accumulation of 18S-E even if the activity of RIOK2 is affected. Given the broad implication of RSK at different stages of ribosome biogenesis, it is biologically relevant to observe that inactivation of RSK does not result in the same processing defects as inactivation of RIOK2.

      We nevertheless tried to make this point clearer in the present revised version I of the manuscript. We added in the supplementary material a diagram (Supplementary Fig. S1C) showing all the known and hypothetical targets of ERK and RSK in ribosome synthesis to provide the readers with a global view of the function of RSK in this process and refer to this figure in the introduction and results. In the introduction, we also emphasize more on the multiple aspects of the regulation of ribosome synthesis by ERK and RSK (Page 4, Line 18).

      Concerning the phospho-mimetic mutant, it does accumulate slightly the 45S and 30S intermediates contrary to the non-phosphorylatable mutant but this is not totally unexpected. RIOK2 is incorporated into pre-ribosomes in the nucleus, at a stage that remains unclear, and constitutive RIOK2 phosphorylation may interfere with this recruitment and affect processing at an earlier stage. This point has been addressed in the discussion of the revised version I of the manuscript (Page 18, Line 7).

      Are there similar results for RSK depletion/inhibition and RIOK2 release from the pre-40S and inability to import into the nucleus? If so, this could provide phenotypic consistency between these two proteins in the proposed pathway to further support the hypothesis.

      We performed the same experiments as reported in Figure 6C to try to demonstrate a cytoplasmic retention of RIOK2 after leptomycin B treatment upon ERK inhibition (PD treatment). We also performed IF and cell fractionation experiments upon PD treatment. In all cases, we failed to observe the expected result. We strongly believe that we are facing here the same problem as described above for the previous comment of Reviewer #2. ERK and thus RSK inhibition leads to accumulation of the early, nucleolar 30S intermediate, indicating that the processing pathway is significantly blocked at an early stage preceding formation of the pre-40S particles in which RIOK2 is recruited. This early blockage most likely explains why we do not see the same phenotypes. We discussed this comment in the discussion section of the revised version I of the manuscript (Page 18, Line 19).

      1. Mature levels of 18S rRNA are not altered in the RIOK2 mutant cell lines. This could be due to compensation in these mutant cell lines since RIOK2 is essential.

      We agree with Reviewer #2 that compensation mechanisms may operate to restore mature 18S rRNA levels despite RIOK2 mutation. On the other hand, although RIOK2 is indeed essential, we may expect that the point mutation of S483 only partially affects RIOK2 function and delays the maturation of pre-40S particles but not to a sufficient extent to impact the mature 18S rRNA levels. This has been observed by others (Montellese et al., 2017, NAR; Srivastava et al., 2010, MCB).

      We added this point in the discussion section of the revised version I of the manuscript (Page 19, Line 9).

      Please report the mature 18S rRNA levels upon shRNA depletion and RSK inhibitors to provide insight into if this pathway significantly alters mature 18S rRNAs as a mechanism for the altered translation and proliferation observed.

      We will probe the levels of the mature 18S and 28S rRNAs in these experiments and the results will be included in Figure 1 of the revised version II of the manuscript.

      Minor Comments:

      1. Figure 1A lower: The authors use an RSK inhibitor LJH685, that does not inhibit RSK phosphorylation S380. Therefore, another verification of RSK inhibition must be used besides RSK-pS380 abundance as for PD184352 inhibition. Please validate the usage of this RSK inhibitor in the experiments by inclusion of quantification of a direct downstream substrate of RSK, such as YB1-pS102 quantification.

      We agree with Reviewer #2. We have probed the membrane with anti-RPS6 and anti-phosho-RPS6 antibodies to show the effect of LJH treatment on RPS6 phosphorylation. These data have been added to Figure 1A in the revised version I of the manuscript and the text has been updated (Page 6, Line 16).

      1. Page 7, Lines 8-12: The authors state that RSK knockdown led to increases in the 45S, while the LJH685 treatment led to no changes in 45S levels due to differences in growth conditions. Please elaborate more on how growth conditions would alter 45S pre-rRNA levels. It would be expected that stimulation of the MAPK pathway would increase pre-rRNA transcription compared to steady state growth conditions. However, pre-rRNA processing northern blots are only measuring steady state levels of the precursors. Thus, an rDNA transcription assay would need to be completed to evaluate these differences.

      We do observe that PMA treatment of starved cells induces an increase in 45S precursor levels, consistent with an increase in transcription but we agree that northern blot experiments measure the steady-state levels of the intermediates.

      To address this comment, we propose to perform short pulse labelings with ortho-phosphate to assess synthesis of the 45S precursor independently of its processing in the different conditions. These data will be included in the revised version II of the manuscript.

      1. Figure 2C: Please quantify these results to properly evaluate the role of these two phosphorylation sites in MAPK signaling.

      We will repeat these experiments and quantify the results in the new version of Figure 2C.

      1. Please include the RIOK2 pS483 antibody generation methodology used in this study.

      We added this information in the Materials and Methods section of the revised version I of the manuscript (Page 21, Line 22).

      1. In vitro kinase assay methods: Is the recombinant RSK1 the human version of the protein? Please clarify in methods.

      Human recombinant RSK1 has been purchased from SignalChem. The information has been added in the revised version I of the manuscript (Page 30, Line 5).

      1. Figure 4B: Please include statistical analysis of the puromycin incorporation assay.

      We performed a statistical analysis of this assay out of 3 replicates. This analysis has been included in the present revised version I of the manuscript (Figure 4B).

      1. Page 13, Line 18: Please explain why RIOK2 co-IP with NOB1 is important.

      We added this explanation in the result section of the revised version I of the manuscript (Page 14, Line 3).

      1. In vitro dissociation assay: There is no control for pulldown of entire pre-40S particles and not just NOB1 protein. Thus, it is unclear if RIOK2 is dissociating from NOB1 or entire pre-40S particles. Please reference previous literature of the methodology of this experiment if applicable. Additionally, please include controls, such as western blotting of ribosomal proteins or northern blotting of rRNA in the pulldown fraction used.

      We agree with Reviewer #2. We have probed the membranes with antibodies detecting LTV1 and ribosomal protein RPS7 to show that the entire pre-40S particle is indeed pulled down. These additional data have been added in Figure 6A of the revised version I of the manuscript and the text has been amended accordingly (Page 14, Line 20).

      1. Page 16, Lines 10-12: The authors state "RSK facilitates the release of RIOK2 and other AMFs", however the only other AMF in this study was NOB1. Please reword appropriately that most likely facilitates release of RIOK2 and other AMFs in a RIOK2 dependent or independent manner if it also phosphorylates other AMFs which possess the motif.

      We agree with Reviewer #2 and we changed the text accordingly (Page 16, Line 11) but we did not introduce the hypothesis that RIOK2 may target directly other AMFs of late pre-40S particles which possess the motif because our in silico screen did not identify consensus RXRXXS/T motifs in any of these factors.

      Reviewer #2 (Significance (Required)):

      This manuscript is significant due to the lack of mechanistic connection of cellular signaling pathways to pre-rRNA processing. There have been, for the most part, no mechanistic connection of signaling pathways to pre-rRNA processing regulation and none for direct targets of MAPK signaling (Reviewed in Gaviraghi et al 2019). They provide the groundwork for analysis of MAPK signaling in regulation of an assembly factor and inclusion of their motif analysis could provide RSK signaling targets' regulation of specific steps of ribosome biogenesis that remain to be elucidated.

      Although the research delves into a specific mechanism, its audience could be far reaching as it is in the ribosome biogenesis field and MAPK signaling, which have broad implications in cancer and developmental diseases.

      Reviewer #3 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity (Required)):

      The authors report that inhibition of MAPK signaling via RSK is associated with modest alterations in the relative abundance of human pre-rRNA species, that are most marked for 30S but also visible for 21S - although not clearly shown for 18S-E.

      RIOK2 has two closely spaced sites predicted as RSK targets, one of which was confirmed to be MAPK sensitive and shown to be an RSK substrate in vitro. Substitution of Ser483 with Ala was associated with reduced growth and 18S-E accumulation, consistent with impaired NOB1 cleavage activity. RIOK2-S483A also showed greater pre-ribosome association in vivo and consistent with this, more stable association in vitro and increase cytoplasmic residence. These effects are clear, although the data do not directly demonstrate their linkage to loss of RSK phosphorylation.

      The mutations were apparently generated directly in the genome of haploid cells, potentially raising concerns that the introduction of a deleterious mutation might have been accompanied by compensatory mutations elsewhere. However, three cells line gave similar results, mitigating this concern.

      Specific comments:

      1. To help the reader, the authors should directly discuss why they think the data on MAPK inhibition did not reveal a clearer pre-18S cleavage phenotype, as would have been expected for loss of RIOK2 activity.

      This comment is similar to major comment #4 of Reviewer #2.

      Please refer to the above response.

      1. Fig. S3: The degree of RSK depletion with the siRNAs appears very modest, as are the effects on RIOK2-P. Moreover, the double depletion is not clearly better than single depletions. These data should probably be supported by quantitation or withdrawn._

      We agree with Reviewer #3 that the effects shown in this figure are modest but we originally chose to show these data because their further supported the role of RSK in RIOK2 phosphorylation at S483 in complement to Figure 3.

      We have withdrawn this figure from the present revised version I of the manuscript.

      1. Fig. 5D: For 18S-E recovery with RIOK2, is the ratio adjusted for the increase in 18S-E abundance in the mutant - ie is recovery increased when adjusted for the increased pre-rRNA abundance?_

      In these experiments, the tagged versions of RIOK2 WT and S483A have been expressed ectopically from plasmids in cells expressing the endogenous wild-type protein. RIOK2 S483A does not behave as a dominant negative mutant in these conditions and does not induce 18S-E accumulation, as shown in the northern blot analysis of the 18S-E levels in the cell lysates (lower panel). This information is indicated in the revised version I of the manuscript (Page 13, Line 26).

      Reviewer #3 (Significance (Required)):

      Overall, the analyses on the phenotype of RIOK2-S483A, and the demonstration that this site is an RSK target, appear convincing.

      Caveats are

      1) the phenotype seen on inhibition of RSK, would not have implicated RIOK2 as the obvious candidate for the factor responsible for the observed processing defects;

      We agree with this comment, which has also been raised by Reviewer #2 (Major comment 4.). We provide several evidence in the manuscript that RSK phosphorylates RIOK2 on S483 in vivo and in vitro (Figure 3). However, as explained above in response to Reviewer #2, we cannot correlate the in vivo phenotypes resulting from RSK or RIOK2 inactivation for biological reasons. As mentioned in the introduction, RSK regulates multiple substrates at different stages of ribosome biogenesis (Translation of RPs and AMFs, Pol I transcription, pre-ribosome maturation and export), whereas RIOK2 is specifically implicated in the cytoplasmic maturation of pre-40S particles. Inactivation of RSK is therefore expected to induce pleiotropic defects in ribosome biogenesis, and in particular early defects (Reduced Pol I transcription, 30S precursor accumulation) that preclude observation of the expected phenotype linked to RIOK2 inactivation, i.e. 18S-E accumulation.

      We nevertheless tried to clarify this point as described in the response to Reviewer #2, major comment 4.

      2) the RIOK2-S483A phenotype is not demonstrated to be RSK dependent. This raises the possibility that, although RSK can phosphorylate S483, the effects of the mutation are not due to the loss of this modification.

      As mentioned by Reviewer #3, our data show that RSK can phosphorylate RIOK2 S483 in vitro and in vivo (Figure 3). We believe that Figure 4C strongly suggests that the accumulation of the 18S-E in cells expressing RIOK2 S483A mutant is due to the loss of S483 phosphorylation, since mutation of S483 to an aspartic acid (S483D), generally considered as a mutation mimicking a phosphorylated serine, does not affect 18S-E maturation. However, although our manuscript provides many lines of evidence identifying RSK as the kinase responsible for RIOK2 phosphorylation at S483, we cannot formally exclude that other AGC kinases involved in growth and proliferation, such as S6K or Akt, may also be involved redundantly or alternatively. Our data presented in Figure 3A showing that treatment of cells with the RSK inhibitors LJH decrease RIOK2 phosphorylation at S483 support a specific role of RSK.

      We developed this point in the discussion section (Page 18, from Line 25).

      With these provisos, the work is technically good and will be of considerable interest to the field. The post-transcriptional regulation of ribosome synthesis is increasingly recognized a significant topic.

    1. Author Response

      Summary:

      This work is of interest because it increases our understanding of the molecular mechanisms that distinguish subtypes of VIP interneurons in the cerebral cortex and because of the multiple ways in which the authors address the role of Prox1 in regulating synaptic function in these cells.

      The authors would like to thank the reviewers for their constructive comments. In response, we would like to clarify a number of issues, as well as outline how we plan to resolve major concerns.

      Reviewer #1:

      Stachiak and colleagues examine the physiological effects of removing the homeobox TF Prox1 from two subtypes of VIP neurons, defined on the basis of their bipolar vs. multipolar morphology.

      The results will be of interest to those in the field, since it is known from prior work that VIP interneurons are not a uniform class and that Prox1 is important for their development.

      The authors first show that selective removal of a conditional Prox1 allele using a VIP cre driver line results in a change in paired pulse ratio of presumptive excitatory synaptic responses in multipolar but not bipolar VIP interneurons. The authors then use RNA-seq to identify differentially expressed genes that might contribute and highlight a roughly two-fold reduction in the expression of a transcript encoding a trans-synaptic protein Elfn1 known to contribute to reduced glutamate release in Sst+ interneurons. They then test the potential contribution of Elfn1 to the phenotype by examining whether loss of one allele of Elfn1 globally alters facilitation. They find that facilitation is reduced both by this genetic manipulation and by a pharmacological blockade of presynaptic mGluRs known to interact with Elfn1.

      Although the results are interesting, and the authors have worked hard to make their case, the results are not definitive for several reasons:

      1) The global reduction of Elfn1 may act cell autonomously, or may have other actions in other cell types. The pharmacological manipulation is less subject to this interpretation, but these results are not as convincing as they could be because the multipolar Prox1 KO cells (Fig. 3 J) still show substantial facilitation comparable, for example to the multipolar control cells in the Elfn1 Het experiment (controls in Fig. 3E). This raises a concern about control for multiple comparisons. Instead of comparing the 6 conditions in Fig 3 with individual t-tests, it may be more appropriate to use ANOVA with posthoc tests controlled for multiple comparisons.

      The reviewer’s concerns regarding non-cell-autonomous actions of global Elfn1 KO are well founded. Significant phenotypic alterations have previously been reported, both in the physiology of SST neurons as well in the animals’ behavior (Stachniak, Sylwestrak, Scheiffele, Hall, & Ghosh, 2019; Tomioka et al., 2014). The homozygous Elfn1 KO mouse displays a hyperactive phenotype and epileptic activity after 3 months of age, suggesting generalcortical activity differences exist (Dolan & Mitchell, 2013; Tomioka et al., 2014). Nevertheless, we have not observed such changes in P17-21 Elfn1 heterozygous (Het) animals.

      Comparing across different experimental animal lines, for example the multipolar Prox1 KO cells (Fig. 3 J) to the multipolar control cells in the Elfn1 Het experiment (controls in Fig. 3E), is in our view not advisable. There is a plethora of examples in the literature on the effect of mouse strain on even the most basic cellular functions and hence it is always expected that researchers use the correct control animals for their experiments, which in the best case scenario are littermate controls. For these reasons, we would argue that statistical comparisons across mouse lines is not ideal for our study. Elfn1 Het and MSOP data are presented side by side to illustrate that Elfn1 Hets (3C,E) phenocopy the effects of Prox1 deletion (3G,H,I,J). (See also point 3) MSOP effect sizes, however, do show significant differences by ANOVA with Bonferroni post-hoc (normalized change in EPSC amplitude; multipolar prox1 control: +12.1 ± 3.8%, KO: -8.4 ± 4.3%, bipolar prox1 control: -5.2 ± 4.3%, KO: -3.4 ± 4.7%, cell type x genotype interaction, p= 0.02, two way ANOVA).

      2) The isolation of glutamatergic currents is not described. Were GABA antagonists present to block GABAergic currents? Especially with the Cs-based internal solutions used, chloride reversal potentials can be somewhat depolarized relative to the -65 mV holding potential. If IPSCs were included it would complicate the analysis.

      No, in fact GABA antagonists were not present in these experiments. The holding voltage in our evoked synaptic experiments is -70 mV, which combined with low internal [Cl-] makes it highly unlikely that the excitatory synaptic responses we study are contaminated by GABA-mediated ones, even with a Cs MeSO4-based solution. Nevertheless, we have now performed additional experiments where glutamate receptor blockers were applied in bath and we observe a complete blockade of the synaptic events at -70mV proving that they are AMPA/NMDA receptor mediated. When holding the cell at 0mV with these blockers present, outward currents were clearly visible, suggesting intact GABA-mediated events.

      3) The assumption that protein levels of Elfn1 are reduced to half in the het is untested. Synaptic proteins can be controlled at the level of translation and trafficking and WT may not have twice the level of this protein.

      We thank reviewer for pointing this out. Our rationale for using the Elfn1 heterozygous animals is rather that transcript levels are reduced by half in heterozygous animals, to match the reduction we found in the mRNA levels of VIP Prox1 KO cells (Fig 2). The principle purpose of the Elfn1 KO experiment was to determine whether the change in Elfn1 transcript levels could be sufficient to explain the synaptic deficit observed in VIP Prox1 KO cells. As the reviewer notes, translational regulation and protein trafficking could ultimately result in even larger changes than 0.5x protein levels at the synapse. This may ultimately explain the observed multipolar/bipolar disparity, which cannot be explained by transcriptional regulation alone (Fig 4).

      4) The authors are to be commended for checking whether Elfn1 is regulated by Prox1 only in the multipolar neurons, but unfortunately it is not. The authors speculate that the selective effects reflect a selective distribution of MgluR7, but without additional evidence it is hard to know how likely this explanation is.

      Additional experiments are underway to better understand this mechanism.

      Reviewer #2:

      Stachniak et al., provide an interesting manuscript on the postnatal role of the critical transcription factor, Prox1, which has been shown to be important for many developmental aspects of CGE-derived interneurons. Using a combination of genetic mouse lines, electrophysiology, FACS + RNAseq and molecular imaging, the authors provide evidence that Prox1 is genetically upstream of Elfn1. Moreover, they go on to show that loss of Prox1 in VIP+ cells preferentially impacts those that are multipolar but not the bipolar subgroup characterized by the expression of calretinin. This latter finding is very interesting, as the field is still uncovering how these distinct subgroups emerge but are at a loss of good molecular tools to fully uncover these questions. Overall, this is a great combination of data that uses several different approaches to come to the conclusions presented. I have suggestions that I think would strengthen the manuscript:

      1) Can the authors add a supplemental table showing the top 20-30 genes up and down regulated in their Prox1 KOS? This would make these, and additional, data more tenable to readers.

      We would be happy to provide supplementary tables with candidate genes at both P8 and P12.

      2) It is interesting that loss of Prox1 or Elfn1 leads to phenotypes in multipolar but are not present or mild in bipolar VIP+ cells. The authors test different hypotheses, which they are able to refute and discuss some ideas for how multipolar cells may be more affected by loss of Elfn1, even when the transcript is lost in both multipolar and bipolar after Prox1 deletion. If there is any way to expand upon these ideas experimentally, I believe it would greatly strengthen the manuscript. I understand there is no perfect experiment due to a lack of tools and reagents but if there is a way to develop one of the following ideas or something similar, it would be beneficial:

      We thank the reviewer for the note.

      a) Would it be possible to co-fill VIPCre labeled cells with biocytin and a retroviral tracer? Then, after the retroviral tracer had time to label a presynaptic cell, assess whether these were preferentially different between bipolar and multipolar cell types, the latter morphology determined by the biocytin fill? This would test whether each VIP+ subtype is differentially targeted.

      Although this is a very elegant experiment and we would be excited to do it, we do feel that single-cell rabies virus tracing is technically very challenging and will take many months to troubleshoot before being able to acquire good data. Hence, we think it is beyond the scope of this study.

      b) Another biocytin possibility would be to trace filled VIP+ cells and assess whether the dendrites of multipolar and bipolar cells differentially targeted distinct cortical lamina and whether these lamina, in the same section or parallel, were enriched for mGluR7+ afferents.

      We thank the reviewer for their suggestion and we are planning on doing these kinds of experiments.

      Reviewer #3:

      In this work Stachiak and colleagues investigate the role of Prox1 on the development of VIP cells. Prox1 is expressed by the majority of GABAergic derived from the caudal ganglionic eminence (CGE), and as mentioned by the authors, Prox1 has been shown to be necessary for the differentiation, circuit integration, and maintenance of CGE-derived GABAergic cells. Here, Stachiak and colleagues show that removal of Prox1 in VIP cells leads to suppression of synaptic release probability onto cortical multipolar VIP cells in a mechanism dependent on Elfn1. This work is of interest for the field because it increases our understanding of differential synaptic maturation of VIP cells. The results are noteworthy, however the relevance of this manuscript would potentially be increased by addressing the following suggestions:

      1) Include histology to show when exactly Prox1 is removed from multipolar and bipolar VIP-expressing cells by using the VIP-Cre mouse driver.

      We can address this by performing an in-situ hybridization against Prox1 from P3 onwards (when Cre becomes active).

      2) Clarify if the statistical analysis is done using n (number of cells) or N (number of animals). The analysis between control and mutants (both Prox1 and Elfn1) need to be done across animals and not cells.

      Statistics for physiology were done across n (number of cells) while statistics for ISH are done across number of slices. We will clarify this point in the text and update the methods.

      Regarding the statistics for the ISH, these have been done across n (number of slices) for control versus KO tissue (N = 3 and N = 2 animals, respectively). We will add more animals to this analysis to compare by animal instead, although we do not expect any change in the results.

      Regarding the physiology, we would provide a two-pronged answer. We first of all feel that averaging synaptic responses for each animal would hide a good deal of the biological variability in PPR present in different cells (response Fig 1), the characterization of which is integral to the central findings of the paper. Secondly, to perform such analysis asked by the reviewer one would need to obtain recordings from ~10 animals or so per condition for each condition, which, to our knowledge, is something that is not standard when utilizing in vitro electrophysiological recordings from single cells. For example, in these very recent studies that have performed in vitro electrophysiological recordings all the statistics are performed using “n” number of cells and not the average of all the cells recorded per animal collapsed into a single data point. (Udakis, Pedrosa, Chamberlain, Clopath, & Mellor, 2020) https://www.nature.com/articles/s41467-020-18074-8

      (Horvath, Piazza, Monteggia, & Kavalali, 2020) https://elifesciences.org/articles/52852

      (Haas et al., 2018) https://elifesciences.org/articles/31755

      Nevertheless, we have now re-run the analysis grouping the cells and averaging the values we get per animal, since we have obtained our data from many animals. The results are more or less indistinguishable from the ones presented in the original submission, except for on p value that rose to 0.07 from 0.03 due to the lack of the required number of animals. We hope that the new plots and statistics presented herein address the concern put forward by the reviewer.

      *Response Fig 1: A comparison of cell wise versus animal-wise analysis of synaptic physiology. Some cell to cell variability is hidden, and the reduction in numbers impacts the P values.*

      (A) PPR of multipolar Prox1 Control for 14 cells from 9 animals (n/N=14/9) under baseline conditions and with MSOP, cell-wise comparison p = 0.02 , t = 2.74 and (B) animal-wise comparisons (p = 0.04, t stat = 2.45). Statistics: paired t-test.

      (C) PPR of multipolar Prox1 KO cells (n/N=9/8) under baseline conditions and with MSOP, cell-wise comparison p = 0.2, t = 1.33 and (D) animal-wise comparisons (p = 0.2, t stat = 1.56). Statistics: paired t-test. Comparisons for PPR of bipolar Prox1 Control (n/N=8/8) and KO cells (n/N=9/9) did not change.

      (E) PPR for Prox1 control (n/N=18/11) and KO (n/N=13/11) bipolar VIP cells, cell-wise comparison p = 0.3, t = 1.1 and (F) animal-wise comparisons (p = 0.4, t stat = 0.93). Statistics: t-test.

      (G) PPR of Elfn1 Control (n/N=12/4) and Het (n/N=12/4) bipolar VIP cells, cell-wise comparison p = 0.3, t = 1.06 and (H) animal-wise comparisons (p = 0.4, t stat = 0.93)

      (I) PPR of Prox1 control (n/N=33/18) and KO (n/N=19/14) multipolar VIP cells, cell-wise comparison p = 0.03, t = 2.17. and (J) animal-wise comparisons (p = 0.07, t stat = 1.99).

      (K) PPR of Elfn1 Control (n/N=14/6) and Het (n/N=20/8) multipolar VIP cells, cell-wise comparison p = 0.008, t = 2.84 and (L) animal-wise comparisons (p = 0.007, t stat = 3.23).

      3) Clarify what are the parameters used to identify bipolar vs multipolar VIP cells. VIP cells comprise a wide variety of transcriptomic subtypes, and in the absence of using specific genetic markers for the different VIP subtypes, the authors should either include the reconstructions of all recorded cells or clarify if other methods were used.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. The cell parameter criteria will be amended in the methods: “Cell type was classified as bipolar vs. multipolar based on cell body morphology (ovoid vs. round) and number and orientation of dendritic processes emanating from it (2 or 3 dendrites perpendicular to pia (for bipolar) vs. 3 or more processes in diverse orientations (for multipolar). In addition, the laminar localization of the two populations differs, with multipolar cells found primarily in the upper layer 2, while bipolar cells are found throughout layers 2 and 3. Initial determination of cell classification was made prior to patching fluorescent-labelled cells, but whenever possible this initial assessment was confirmed with post-hoc verification of biocytin filled cells.”

      Reference:

      Dolan, J., & Mitchell, K. J. (2013). Mutation of Elfn1 in Mice Causes Seizures and Hyperactivity. PLOS ONE, 8(11), e80491. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0080491

      Haas, K. T., Compans, B., Letellier, M., Bartol, T. M., Grillo-Bosch, D., Sejnowski, T. J., … Hosy, E. (2018). Pre-post synaptic alignment through neuroligin-1 tunes synaptic transmission efficiency. ELife, 7, e31755. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.31755

      Horvath, P. M., Piazza, M. K., Monteggia, L. M., & Kavalali, E. T. (2020). Spontaneous and evoked neurotransmission are partially segregated at inhibitory synapses. ELife, 9, e52852. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.52852

      Stachniak, T. J., Sylwestrak, E. L., Scheiffele, P., Hall, B. J., & Ghosh, A. (2019). Elfn1-Induced Constitutive Activation of mGluR7 Determines Frequency-Dependent Recruitment of Somatostatin Interneurons. The Journal of Neuroscience, 39(23), 4461 LP – 4474. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2276-18.2019

      Tomioka, N. H., Yasuda, H., Miyamoto, H., Hatayama, M., Morimura, N., Matsumoto, Y., … Aruga, J. (2014). Elfn1 recruits presynaptic mGluR7 in trans and its loss results in seizures. Nature Communications. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms5501

      Udakis, M., Pedrosa, V., Chamberlain, S. E. L., Clopath, C., & Mellor, J. R. (2020). Interneuron-specific plasticity at parvalbumin and somatostatin inhibitory synapses onto CA1 pyramidal neurons shapes hippocampal output. Nature Communications, 11(1), 4395. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-18074-8

    1. Reviewer #3:

      This manuscript reports results from an eye tracking study of humans walking in natural terrain. These eye movements together with images simultaneously obtained by a head-fixed camera are used to calculate optic flow fields as seen by the retina and as seen by the head-fixed camera. Next, the structure of these flow fields is described. It is noted that this structure is somewhat stable in the retinal image, due to compensatory gaze stabilisation reflexes, but varies wildly in the head-centric image. Then, the authors estimate the focus of expansion in the head-centric flow and argue that it cannot be used for locomotor control, because it also varies wildly during walking. In a second, more theoretical section of the manuscript, they calculate retinal flow for a movement over an artificial ground plane, given the locomotor and eye movements recorded previously. They describe the structure of the retinal flow and compute the distribution of curl and divergence across the retina as well as in a projection onto the ground plane. They argue that curl around the fovea and the location of the maximum of divergence can be used to estimate the direction of walking relative to the direction of gaze and in relation to the ground plane.

      I really like the experimental part of the study. However, I see fundamental issues in the theoretical part, in the general framing of the presentation, and in misrepresentations of previous literature.

      The simultaneous measurement of head-centric image and gaze with sufficient temporal resolution to calculate retinal flow during natural walking provides a beautiful demonstration of retinal flow fields, and confirms many known aspects of retinal flow. The calculation of head-centric flow from the head camera images provides a compelling, though not unexpected, demonstration that the FOE in head-centric flow is not useful for locomotor control. It is not unexpected since one of the most well-known issues in optic flow is that the FoE is destroyed when self-motion contains rotational components (Regan and Beverley, 1982, Warren and Hannon, 1990, Lappe et al. 1999). Although this is often presented as an issue of eye movements in retinal flow, it applies to all rotations and combinations of rotations that exist on top of any translational motion of the observer. Thus, the oscillatory bounce and sway motion of the head during walking is expected to render any use of the FOE in a head-centric image futile.

      Yet, the first part of the manuscript is very much framed as a critique of the idea of a stable FoE in head-centric flow, presuming that this is what previous researchers commonly believed. This argument contains a logical fallacy. Previous research argued that there is no FoE in retinal flow because of eye rotations (e.g. Warren and Hannon, 1990). This does not predict, inversely, that there is an FoE in head-centric flow. In fact, it does not provide any prediction on head-centric flow. The authors often suggest that a stable FoE in head-centric flow is tacitly implied, commonly believed, etc without providing reference. In fact, the only paper I know that specifically proposed a head-centric representation of heading is by van den Berg and Beintema (1997).

      Instead, the fundamental problem of heading perception is to estimate self-motion from retinal flow when the self-motion that generates retinal flow combines all kinds of translations and rotations. The present study shows, consistent with much of the prior literature, that the patterns of retinal flow are sufficiently stable and informative to obtain the direction of one's travel in a retinal frame of reference, and, via projection, with respect to the ground plane. This is due to the stabilising gaze reflexes that keep motion small near the fovea and produce (in case of a ground plane) a spiralling pattern of retinal flow. This is well known from theoretical and lab studies (e.g. Warren and Hannon, 1990, Lappe et al., 1998, Niemann et al., 1999, Lappe et al. 1999) and, to repeat, beautifully shown for the natural situation in the present data. The presentation should link back to this work rather than trying to shoot down purported mechanisms that are obviously invalid.

      The second part of the manuscript presents a theoretical analysis of the retinal flow for locomotion across a ground plane under gaze stabilisation. This has two components: (a) the structure of the retinal flow and the utility of gaze stabilisation, and (b) ways to recover information about self-motion from the retinal flow. Both aspects have a long history of research that is neglected in the present manuscript. The essential circular structure of the retinal flow during gaze stabilisation is long known (Warren and Hannon, 1990, van den Berg, 1996, Lappe et al., 1998, Lappe et al. 1999). Detailed analyses of the statistical structure of retinal flow during gaze stabilisation have shown the impact and utility of gaze stabilisation (Calow et al., 2004; Calow and Lappe, 2007; Roth and Black, 2007) and provided links to properties of neurons in the visual system (Calow and Lappe, 2008). These studies included simulated motions of the head during walking, as in the current manuscript, and extended to natural scenes other than a simple ground plane.

      Given the structure of the retinal flow during gaze stabilisation the central question is how to recover information about self-motion from it. The authors investigate a proposal originally made by Koenderink and van Doorn (1976; 1984) that relies on estimates of curl and divergence in the visual field. They propose that locomotor heading may be determined directly in retinotopic coordinates (l. 314). This is true, but it fails to mention that other models of heading perception during gaze stabilisation similarly determine heading in retinotopic coordinates (e.g. Lappe and Rauschecker, 1993; Perrone and Stone, 1994; Royden, 1997). In fact, as outlined above, the mathematical problem of self-motion estimation is typically presented in retinal (or camera) coordinates (e.g. Longuet-Higgins and Prazdny, 1980). The problem with the divergence model in comparison to the other models above is threefold. First, it really only works for a plane, not in other environments. Second, it requires a local estimate of divergence at each position in the visual field. The alternative models above combine information across the visual field and are therefore much more robust against noise in the flow. One would need to see whether the estimate of the divergence distribution is sufficient to work with the natural flow fields. Third, being a local measure it requires a dense flow field while heading estimation from retinal flow is known to work with sparse flow fields (Warren and Hannon, 1990). Thus, the theoretical part of the manuscript should either provide proof that the maximum of divergence is superior to these other models or broaden the view to include these models as possibilities to estimate self motion from retinal flow.

      The case is similar for the use of curl. It is true that the rotational or spiral pattern around the fovea in retinal flow provides information about the direction of self motion with respect to the direction of gaze, as has been noted many times before. This structure is used by many models of heading estimation. However, curl is, like divergence, a local property and thus not as robust as models that use the entire flow field. It may be interesting to note that neurons in optic flow responsive areas of the monkey brain can pick up this rotational pattern and respond to it in consistency with their preference for self-motion across a plane (Bremmer et al., 2010; Kaminiarz et al. 2014).

      I think what the authors may want to draw more attention to is the dynamics of the retinal flow and the associated self-motion in retinal (or plane projection) coordinates. The movies provide compelling illustrations of how the direction of heading (or the divergence maximum, if you want to focus on that) sways back and forth on the retina and on the plane with each step. This requires that the analysis of retinal flow (and the estimation of self-motion) has to be fast and dynamic, or maybe should include some form of temporal prediction or filtering. Work on the dynamics of retinal flow perception has indeed shown that heading estimation can work with very brief flow fields (Bremmer et al. 2017), that the brain focuses on instantaneous flow fields (Paolini et al. 2000) and that short presentations sometime provide better heading estimates than long presentations (Grigo and Lappe, 1999). The temporal dynamics of retinal flow is an underappreciated problem that could be more in the focus of the present study.

      Additional specific comments:

      Footnote on page 2: It is not only VOR but also OKN (Lappe et al., 1998, Niemann et al., 1999) that stabilises gaze in optic flow fields.

      Line 55: Natural translation and acceleration patterns of the head have been considered by (Cutting et al., 1992; Palmisano et al. 2000; Calow and Lappe, 2007, 2008; Bossard et al., 2016)

      Line 59: The statement is misleading that the key assumption behind work on the rotation problem is that the removal of the rotational component of flow will return a translational flow field with a stable FoE. Only one class of models, those using differential motion parallax (Rieger and Lawton, 1985, Royden, 1997) explicitly constructs a translational flow field and aims to locate the FoE in that field. Other models (Koenderink and van Doorn, 1976, 1984; Lappe and Rauschecker, 1993; Perrone and Stone, 1994) do not subtract the rotation but estimate heading in retinal coordinates from the combined retinal flow. This also applies to line 109.

      Last paragraph on page 5: Measures of eye movement during walking in natural terrain were also taken by Calow and Lappe (2008) and 't Hart and Einhäuser (2012).

      Lines 140 to 163: This paragraph is problematic and misleading as pointed out before.

      Line 193: The lack of stability is expected, as outlined above. The use of a straight line motion in psychophysical experiments reflects an experimental choice to investigate the rotation problem in retinal flow, not an implicit assumption that bodily motion is usually along a straight line.

      Line 200: That gaze stabilization may be an important component in understanding the use of optic flow patterns has also long been assumed (Lappe and Rauschecker, 1993; 1994; 1995; Perrone and Stone, 1994; Glennerster et al. 2001; Angelaki and Hess, 2005; Pauwels et al., 2007).

      Line 314: Locomotor heading may be determined directly in retinotopic coordinates. Yes, and this is precisely what the above mentioned models do.

      Line 334: What is meant by "robust" here? The videos seem to show simulated flow for a ground plane, not the real flow from any of the terrains. It is not clear whether the features can be extracted from the real terrain retinal flow.

      First paragraph on page 15: This is an important discussion about the dynamics of retinal flow in conjunction with the dynamics of the gait cycle. It should be expanded and better balanced with respect to previous work and other models. It is true that any simple inference of an FoE would not work. However, models that estimate heading (not FoE) in the retinal reference frame would be consistent with the discussion. Oscillations of the head during walking affect the location of the divergence maximum and curl as much as the direction of heading in retinal coordinates. In fact, the videos nicely show how these variables oscillate with each step. This applies to all retinal flow analyses, and is a problem for any model. It requires a dynamical analysis. The speed of neural computations is an issue, of course, but it applies to divergence and curl in the same way as to other models. There is some indication, however, that neural computations on optic flow are fast, deal with instantaneous flow fields, and respond consistently to natural (spiral) retinal flow, as described above.

      Line 393: This paragraph is misleading in suggesting that naturally occurring flow fields have not been used in psychophysical and electrophysiological experiments.

      Line 516: This has been done by Bremmer et al. (2010) and Kaminiarz et al. (2014). Their results are consistent with computing heading directly in a retinal reference frame as predicted by several models of retinal flow analysis (e.g. Lappe et al. 1999).

      References:

      Angelaki, D. E. and Hess, B. J. M. (2005). Self-motion-induced eye movements: effects an visual acuity and navigation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 6:966-976.

      Bossard, M., Goulon, C., and Mestre, D. R. (2016). Viewpoint oscillation improves the perception of distance travelled based on optic flow. J Vis, 16(15):4.

      Bremmer, F., Kubischik, M., Pekel, M., Hoffmann, K. P., and Lappe, M. (2010). Visual selectivity for heading in monkey area MST. Exp. Brain Res., 200(1):51-60.

      Calow, D., Krüger, N., Wörgötter, F., and Lappe, M. (2004). Statistics of optic flow for self-motion through natural scenes. In Ilg, U., Bülthoff, H. H., and Mallot, H. A., editors, Dynamic Perception, Workshop of the GI Section 'Computer Vision', pages 133-138, Berlin. Akademische Verlagsgesellschaft Aka GmbH.

      Calow, D. and Lappe, M. (2007). Local statistics of retinal optic flow for self- motion through natural sceneries. Network, 18(4):343-374.

      Calow, D. and Lappe, M. (2008). Efficient encoding of natural optic flow. Network Comput. Neural Syst., 19(3):183-212.

      Cutting, J. E., Springer, K., Braren, P. A., and Johnson, S. H. (1992). Wayfinding on foot from information in retinal, not optical, flow. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen., 121(1):41-72.

      Grigo, A. and Lappe, M. (1999). Dynamical use of different sources of information in heading judgments from retinal flow. JOSA A, 16(9):2079-2091.

      't Hart, B. M. and Einhäuser, W. (2012). Mind the step: complementary effects of an implicit task on eye and head movements in real-life gaze allocation. Exp. Brain Res., 223(2):233-249.

      Kaminiarz, A., Schlack, A., Hoffmann, K.-P., Lappe, M., and Bremmer, F. (2014). Visual selectivity for heading in the macaque ventral intraparietal area. J. Neurophys. 112(10):2470-80

      Lappe, M., Pekel, M., and Hoffmann, K. P. (1998). Optokinetic eye movements elicited by radial optic flow in the macaque monkey. J. Neurophysiol., 79(3):1461-1480.

      Lappe, M. and Rauschecker, J. P. (1993). A neural network for the processing of optic flow from ego-motion in man and higher mammals. Neural Comp., 5(3):374-391.

      Lappe, M. and Rauschecker, J. P. (1994). Heading detection from optic flow. Nature, 369(6483):712-713.

      Lappe, M. and Rauschecker, J. P. (1995). Motion anisotropies and heading detection. Biol. Cybern., 72(3):261-277.

      Niemann, T., Lappe, M., Büscher, A., and Hoffmann, K. P. (1999). Ocular responses to radial optic flow and single accelerated targets in humans. Vision Res., 39(7):1359-1371.

      Pauwels, K., Lappe, M., and Hulle, M. M. (2007). Fixation as a mechanism for stabilization of short image sequences. Int. J. Comp. Vis., 72(1):67-78.

      Perrone, J. A. and Stone, L. S. (1994). A model of self-motion estimation within primate extrastriate visual cortex. Vision Res., 34(21):2917-2938.

      Regan, D. and Beverley, K. I. (1982). How do we avoid confounding the direction we are looking and the direction we are moving? Science, 215:194-196.

      Rieger, J. H. and Lawton, D. T. (1985). Processing differential image motion. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A, 2(2):354-360.

      Roth, S. and Black, M. J. (2007). On the spatial statistics of optical flow. Int. J. Comp. Vis., 74(1):33-50.

      Royden, C. S. (1997). Mathematical analysis of motion-opponent mechanisms used in the determination of heading and depth. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A, 14(9):2128-2143.

      van den Berg, A. V. (1996). Judgements of heading. Vision Res., 36(15):2337-2350.

      van den Berg, A. V. and Beintema, J. A. (1997). Motion templates with eye velocity gain fields for transformation of retinal to head centric flow. NeuroReport, 8(4):835-840.

  2. Oct 2020
    1. He highlights the Memex’s killer feature of associative linking and how trails of links have never been implemented in the way the Memex envisioned: It is associative indexing though, that is the essential feature of the memex, “the process of tying two items together is the important thing.” Bush describes a hypertext like mechanism at this point, but most interesting from my perspective is his emphasis on a trail as a fundamental unit — something we largely seem to have lost today. […] Documents and links we have aplenty. But where are our trails?
  3. covidandreopeningcolleges.weebly.com covidandreopeningcolleges.weebly.com
    1. Son, Changwon, et al. “Effects of COVID-19 on College Students’ Mental Health in the United States: Interview Survey Study.” Journal of Medical Internet Research, vol. 22, no. 9, Sept. 2020. EBSCOhost, doi:10.2196/21279.    This article demonstrates the effect online learning has had on college students. The author notes that COVID-19 has placed large amounts of stress and anxiety on students, leading many to fall behind in school and other areas in life. From a study performed on college students in Texas, 91% of the students reported they were negatively impacted by the pandemic, with 86% citing lack of social interactions due to online learning. The author suggests schools look into this issue and recommends they look to find alternatives that can help students through this hard time, which, if the situation allows, could point to bringing students back on campus. Online learning has proven to be very lonely and isolating, and many students need to be back in a social environment where they can interact with their peers. Prolonged isolation may prove to be detrimental to students' mental health as time goes on. Bradley EH, An M, Fox E. Reopening Colleges During the Coronavirus Disease 2019 (COVID-19) Pandemic—One Size Does Not Fit All. JAMA Netw Open. 2020;3(7):e2017838. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.17838This article highlights the different ways schools can safely reopen during the COVID-19 pandemic. Obviously every school is different, and different strategies can be applied to different situations. For example, bigger schools may need to employ stricter restrictions and more social distancing measures than a smaller school. Some schools may decide they cannot safely reopen at full capacity, while others can and predict they will have minimal cases. The way in which a school reopened depends on its size, population, location, and local laws regarding the pandemic. As time goes on, schools can learn from each other and determine what is best for them and their students. This article will provide insight on the morality of opening schools, and show that while every school has the opportunity to bring students on campus, not all of them should. Coryton, Demitri. “What Does the Research Evidence Tell Us about the Effect of Closing and Reopening Schools during the Coronavirus Pandemic?” 14-19 Learning & Skills Bulletin, no. 331, Apr. 2020, pp. 18–27. EBSCOhost, search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=eue&AN=142954038&site=ehost-live.        This article gives an international perspective to the issue of having schools closed during the pandemic. Using data collected from the United Kingdom at the start of their lockdown, the author highlighted some of the key effects shutting down all schools in the UK had on society. It looked into economic impacts, whether or not the lockdown was effective, as well as how successful the switch to online learning was compared to other European countries. This article will provide insights to the effects of having campuses closed, and the possible negative effects we may begin to see in our own societies if schools do not begin to reopen. Andersen, M., Bento, A., Basu, A., Marsicano, C., & Simon, K. (2020, September 23). College Openings, Mobility, and the Incidence of COVID-19 Cases. Retrieved October 28, 2020, from https://www.medrxiv.org/content/10.1101/2020.09.22.20196048v1.full.pdf html    This article studied two separate colleges and the COVID-19 cases on campus and in the surrounding areas. The researchers carefully monitored cases starting two weeks before classes started up until two weeks after. There was a noticeable link between spikes in cases in areas where students were coming from COVID-19 “hotspots”, pointing to the idea that bringing students back to school from around the country is not exactly safe nor smart. The data found in this study can be used to predict the pattern of coronavirus spread in other campuses across the country. This article shows the effects of opening campus on the local community. Yamey, G., & Walensky, R. (2020, September 01). Covid-19: Re-opening universities is high risk. Retrieved from https://www.bmj.com/content/370/bmj.m3365This article looks at schools that have reopened for the fall semester, and looks deeper into why they have increased spread of COVID-19. Looking at other countries’ successful reopening of schools, researchers saw that the most effective way to stop the spread on college campuses was to first stop community spread. This has also shown to have been true in the United States, with case counts being relatively low in areas that have stopped community COVID-19 spread. Researchers then went on to say that in order to have a safe reopening, schools should have a thorough and efficient testing strategy which allows cases to be caught before they can spread. This article sheds light on some of the issues facing the reopening of schools in the United States.

      These all look good - I don't think you need to change anything.

    1. But why should we think these histories are in any normative or descriptive sense alike?

      This rhetorical question is a good response to the claims of Bloom's argument. The author suggest that even though a good may be historically relevant, it may not describe the worth of the good entirely. Instead, the author suggests more reasoning than simply history defining something as expensive.

    1. The history of science teaches us how difficult this renun-ciation is. How we come to hypotheses, theories, systems, or whatever other modes of thought may exist through which we try to grasp the infinite, will be the topic of the second part of this short essay. In the first part I will consider how we proceed when we aim to understand the forces of nature. My current studies of the history of physics often provide the opportunity to think about these matters and give rise to this little essay. I strive to show in what way many great indi-viduals have furthered, and also harmed, science.As soon as we consider a phenomenon in itself and in relation to others, neither desiring nor disliking it, we will in quiet attentiveness be able to form a clear concept of it, its parts, and its relations. The more we expand our consider-ations and the more we relate phenomena to one another, the more we exercise the gift of observation that lies within us. If we know how to relate this knowledge to ourselves in our actions, we earn the right to be called intelligent.

      En ese párrafo el autor nos habla sobre el primer tema que abordará. Básicamente dice que intentará demostrar en el ensayo como a lo largo del tiempo muchos grandes individuos han fomentado correcta e incorrectamente la ciencia. Y de que el don de la observación cuanto más ampliamos nuestras consideraciones y cuanto más relacionamos los fenómenos entre sí es ejercitándolo. Teniéndolo y sabiéndolo relacionar con nosotros mismos en nuestras acciones podremos ganar el derecho a ser llamados inteligentes.

    1. Author Response

      Summary:

      The strengths of the study are the findings that a single oxytocin level measured from saliva or plasma is not meaningful in the way that the field might currently be measuring. The reviewers appreciated this finding, and the careful attention to detail, but felt that the results fell short.

      Reviewer #1:

      This article describes the investigation of a valuable research question, given the interest in using salivary oxytocin measures as a proxy of oxytocin system activity. A strength of the study is the use of two independent datasets and the comparison between intranasal and intravenous administration. The authors report poor reliability for measuring salivary oxytocin across visits, that intravenous delivery does not increase concentrations, and that salivary and blood plasma concentrations are not correlated.

      Line 77-78: While it's true that saliva collection provides logistical advantages, there are also measurement advantages (e.g., relatively clean matrix) that are summarised in the MacLean et al (2019) study, which has already been cited.

      Thanks for the suggestion. We added this advantage:

      Line 101Compared to blood sampling, saliva collection presents several logistical and measurement advantages (i.e. relatively clean matrix)(1).”

      Line 86: It is important to note that the 1IU intravenous dose in this study led to equivalent concentrations in blood compared to intranasal administration.

      The reviewer is right that 10 IU (over 10min) in our case increased the concentrations of plasmatic oxytocin beyond those observed for the spray or nebuliser (we reported the full time-course of variations in plasmatic oxytocin in another manuscript we published earlier this year)(2). This was an intentional aspect of our study design. We decided to use the highest intravenous dose (at the highest rate of 1IU/min) that we could get permission to administer safely in healthy volunteers as a proof of concept, so as to achieve a robust and prolonged increase in plasmatic oxytocin over the course of our full testing session. In this manner, we demonstrate that even when plasmatic levels of OT are maintained substantially increased throughout the observation interval, we cannot detect increases in salivary oxytocin. In this aspect, we believe that our manuscript goes one step beyond the important findings described in of Quintana et al. 2018(3), showing that this phenomenon is not linked to dosage (or to amount of increase in plasmatic levels of exogenous OT), as far as we can determine given the current safety standards for the administration of OT IV.

      Please see also response to Reviewer 2, point 1.

      Line 158: When using both ELISA and HPLC-MS, extracted and unextracted samples are correlated when measuring oxytocin concentrations in saliva, at least in dogs. (https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2017.08.033).

      Thanks for pointing out this study. Indeed, in this specific study the authors found correlations between extracted and unextracted saliva samples. Such associations in humans have nevertheless been rare. In humans, the body of evidence suggests that the measurements obtained when comparing extracted samples to unextracted samples, or when comparing samples obtained using different methods of quantification (for instance, ELISA versus radioimmunoassay), do not correlate or show very low correlations (4, 5). Furthermore, most ELISA kits and HPLC-MS protocols to measure oxytocin have so far fallen short on sensitivity to detect the typical concentrations observed in humans at baseline (0-10pg/ml)(6). The current gold-standard method for quantifying oxytocin in biological fluids is the radioimmunoassay we used in this study(4). This method has shown superior sensitivity and specificity when compared to other quantification methods, when combined with extracted samples; therefore, it was our primary choice. We now highlight this advantage in the revised version of the manuscript more explicitly.

      Line 129For all analyses, we followed current gold-standard practices in the field and assayed oxytocin concentrations using radioimmunoassay in extracted samples, which has shown superior sensitivity and specificity when compared to other quantification methods(7).

      Statistical reporting: I ran the article through statcheck R package (a web version is also available) and found a number of inconsistencies with the reported statistics and their p values. For example, on Line 302 the authors reported: t(123) = 1.54, p = 0.41, but this should yield a p value of 0.13. The authors should do the same and fix these errors.

      Thanks very much for taking the time to check our statistical reporting thoroughly. We apologize if we were not sufficiently clear in the previous version of the manuscript, but the p-values we reported are corrected for multiple comparisons using Tukey correction. Currently, statcheck can only evaluate inconsistencies when the results are reported in the standard APA style and does not take into consideration corrections for multiple comparisons of any kind. We did check all of our statistical reporting and the p-values and correspondent statistics are correct (we only corrected an inadvertent error in reporting the degrees of freedom for these tests). In any case, we have now clarified in the manuscript when the reported p-values have been adjusted for multiple comparison to avoid any further confusion.

      Line 305: The confidence intervals for these correlations should be reported.

      We have now added the confidence intervals, estimated using bootstrapping, in our results section.

      Line 348: This is an important point, but it's important to note that the vast majority of these studies use plasma or saliva measures. Perhaps CSF measures are more reliable, but the question wasn't assessed in the present study, and I'm not sure if anyone has looked at this question.

      We are not aware of any study evaluating the stability of measurements of oxytocin in the CSF. Indeed, there are only a few studies sampling CSF to measure oxytocin in clinical patients and it is unlikely that CSF will become a widely used fluid to measure oxytocin in humans, given the invasiveness of the procedure to obtain CSF samples. Here, we wanted to refer specifically to saliva and plasma, which remain as the most popular options for measuring oxytocin in humans and which we investigated specifically in the current study. We have changed the text accordingly for clarity.

      Line 466 “Our data poses questions about the interpretation of previous evidence seeking to associate single measurements of baseline oxytocin in saliva and plasma with individual differences in a range of neuro-behavioural or clinical traits.”

      Line 423: I broadly agree with this conclusion, but it should be added that "single measurements of baseline levels of endogenous oxytocin in saliva and plasma are not stable under typical laboratory conditions" Perhaps these measures can be more stable using other means (i.e., better standardising collection conditions). But the fact remains, under typical conditions these measures do not demonstrate reliability.

      Thanks for the suggestion. We have revised the text accordingly throughout the manuscript (examples below). Our study is a pharmacological study, which means that it is conducted in a highly controlled setting and adheres to strict protocols (i.e. we tested participants at the same time of the day, we instructed participants to abstain from alcohol and heavy exercise for 24 h and from any beverage or food for 2 h before scanning). These exclusion criteria were stricter than those applied in a large number of studies sampling saliva and plasma for measuring oxytocin for the purposes estimating possible associations with various traits associating. Most of these studies do not control, for instance, for fluid or food ingestion. Therefore, we expected our reliability calculations to represent an optimistic estimate of the reliabilities of the salivary and plasmatic oxytocin concentration used in most studies.

      For now, it remains unclear to us what factors might be driving the within-subject variability in salivary and plasmatic concentrations we report in this study. Thanks to Reviewer 3, we are now confident that this is unlikely to represent measurement error (see response to Reviewer 3, point 3).

      Line 117 “Here, we aimed to characterize the reliability of both salivary and plasmatic single measures of basal oxytocin in two independent datasets, to gain insight about their stability in typical laboratory conditions and their validity as trait markers for the physiology of the oxytocin system in humans.

      Line 567 “In summary, single measurements of baseline levels of endogenous oxytocin in saliva and plasma as obtained in typical laboratory conditions are not stable and therefore their validity as trait markers of the physiology of the oxytocin system is questionable.”

      Reviewer #2:

      Summary:

      To test questions whether salivary and plasmatic oxytocin at baseline reflect the physiology of the oxytocin system, and whether salivary oxytocin index its plasma levels, the authors quantified baseline plasmatic and/or salivary oxytocin using radioimmunoassay from two independent datasets. Dataset A comprised 17 healthy men sampled on four occasions approximately at weekly intervals. In the dataset A, oxytocin was administered intravenously and intranasally in a triple dummy, within-subject, placebo-controlled design and compared baseline levels and the effects of routes of administration. With dataset A, whether salivary oxytocin can predict plasmatic oxytocin at baseline and after intranasal and intravenous administrations of oxytocin were also tested. Dataset B comprised baseline plasma oxytocin levels collected from 20 healthy men sampled on two separate occasions. In both datasets, single measurements of plasmatic and salivary oxytocin showed insufficient reliability across visits (Intra-class correlation coefficient: 0.23-0.80; mean CV: 31-63%). Salivary oxytocin was increased after intranasal administration of oxytocin (40 IU), but intravenous administration (10 IU) does not significantly change. Saliva and plasma oxytocin did not correlate at baseline or after administration of exogenous oxytocin (p>0.18). The authors suggest that the use of single measurements of baseline oxytocin concentrations in saliva and plasma as valid biomarkers of the physiology of the oxytocin system is questionable in men. Furthermore, they suggest that saliva oxytocin is a weak surrogate for plasma oxytocin and that the increases in saliva oxytocin observed after intranasal oxytocin most likely reflect unabsorbed peptide and should not be used to predict treatment effects.

      General comments:

      The current study tested research questions relevant for the study field. The analyses in two independent datasets with different routes of oxytocin administrations is the strength of current study. However, the limited novelty of findings and several limitations are noticed in the current report as described below.

      Specific and major comments:

      1) Previous study with similar results has already revealed that saliva oxytocin is a weak surrogate for plasmatic oxytocin, and increases in salivary oxytocin after the intranasal administration of exogenous oxytocin most likely represent drip-down transport from the nasal to the oral cavity and not systemic absorption (Quintana 2018 in Ref 13). Therefore, the novelty of current findings is limited. The authors should more clearly state the novelty of current results and the replication of previous findings.

      We apologize for not describing the novelty and impact of our findings with sufficient clarity, and thanks for the opportunity to do so. Our study had two major goals. The first was to investigate whether single measurements of salivary and plasmatic concentrations of oxytocin can be reliably estimated within the same individual when collected at baseline conditions (i.e. without any experimental manipulation). As the reviewer highlighted, this is an important methodological question given the wide use of these measurements in a large and increasing number of studies to establish associations between the physiology of the oxytocin system and a number of brain and behavioural phenotypes in both clinical and non-clinical samples. However, to our knowledge, no previous study has appropriately conducted a thorough investigation of the reliability of these measurements (see also response to Reviewer 3, point 5). Thanks to our study, we now know that when single measurements are collected at baseline, salivary and plasmatic oxytocin cannot provide a sufficiently stable trait marker of the physiology of the oxytocin system in humans. As we highlight in the manuscript, this finding should deter the field from making strong claims based exclusively on associations of phenotypes with single measurements of peripheral oxytocin concentrations. Furthermore, our study also describes two very concrete implications of our findings which we believe are very important for the field. First, if baseline level of OT is to be used as a trait marker, future studies should, as much as possible, rely on repeated measures within the same participant but collected on different days to maximize reliability. Second, this less than perfect reliability should be taken into consideration when calculating the sizes of the samples needed to detect a certain effect, if it exists, with sufficient statistical power.

      The second goal of our study was, as pointed out by the reviewer, to revisit the findings of Quintana et al. 2018(3), but this time with two major design modifications which could strengthen the conclusions from that study. The first modification was the dose of intravenous oxytocin administered, which was considerably higher (see response to Reviewer 1, point 2). The administration of a higher dose that resulted in substantial and sustained increases in plasmatic oxytocin throughout the two hours observation period can only strengthen the previous conclusion that increases in plasmatic oxytocin cannot be detected in salivary measurements, and that this is not a matter of dose (as far as we can ascertain by administering the maximum intravenous dose we could safely administer in healthy volunteers). We believe that this is an important addition to the literature.

      The second modification regarded the choice of the method we used to quantify oxytocin. In this study, we used radioimmunoassay, which is superior to ELISA in sensitivity and hence more appropriate to measure the low concentrations of oxytocin in saliva and plasma typically detected in humans at baseline conditions (1-10 pg/ml; for most individuals 1-5 pg/ml)(6). For instance, in Quintana et al. 2018(3) the limitations in the sensitivity of the ELISA kit used led the authors to discard around 50% of the collected saliva samples. Hence, our study replicates and extends the previous findings from Quintana et al. 2018 in important ways, demonstrating that the lack of an association between increases plasmatic oxytocin and salivary measurements is not limited by the dose of intravenous oxytocin administered or limitations of the sensitivity of the method used to quantify oxytocin.

      We have now made the novelty and contribution of our work more explicit:

      *Line 77 “Currently, we lack robust evidence that single measures of endogenous oxytocin in saliva and plasma at rest are stable enough to provide a valid trait marker of the activity of the oxytocin system in healthy individuals. Indeed, previous studies have claimed within-individual stability of baseline plasmatic and salivary concentrations of oxytocin in both adults and children based on moderate-to-strong correlations between salivary and plasmatic oxytocin concentrations measured repeatedly within the same individual over time using ELISA in unextracted samples(14-16). However, these studies have a number of methodological limitations that raise questions about the validity of their main conclusion that baseline plasmatic and salivary concentrations are stable within individuals. First, measuring oxytocin in unextracted samples has been postulated as potentially erroneous, given the high risk of contamination with immunoreactive products other than oxytocin(4). It is conceivable that these non-oxytocin immunoreactive products might constitute highly stable plasma housekeeping proteins (17) that masked the true variability in oxytocin concentrations. Second, a simple correlation analysis cannot provide information about the absolute agreement of two sets of measurements – which would be a more appropriate approach to study within-subject reliability/stability. Third, it is not clear whether these findings generalize beyond the early parenting(14) or early romantic(15) periods participants were in when the studies were conducted, since these periods engage the activity of the oxytocin system in particular ways(18). Hence, establishing the validity of salivary and plasmatic oxytocin as trait markers of the activity of the oxytocin system in humans remains as an unmet need. Such evidence is urgently required, given reports that plasma and saliva levels of oxytocin are frequently altered during neuropsychiatric illness and that they co-vary with clinical aspects of disease(13).

      Line 509 “Our findings were not consistent with these expectations. We could replicate previous evidence that intravenous oxytocin does not increase salivary oxytocin(3) and extended it by showing that the lack of increase in salivary oxytocin is not limited to the specific low dose of intravenous OT that was previously used (1IU) and that it is not driven by the insufficient sensitivity of the OT measurement method (which had resulted in more than 50% of the saliva samples being discarded in the previous study(3).”*

      2) As authors discussed in the limitation section of discussion, the current study has several limitations such as analyses only in male participants and non-optimized timing of collection of saliva and blood due to the other experiments. These limitations are understandable, because the current study was the second analyses on the data of the other studies with the different aims. However, these limitations significantly limit the interpretations of the findings.

      Here, we would like to highlight two aspects. First, most studies in the field are indeed conducted in men to avoid potential confounding from fluctuations in oxytocin concentrations across the menstrual cycle in women. Therefore, our study is representative of the typical samples used in most human studies. Second, we did not optimize our study to collect repeated samples of saliva. Indeed, it would have been interesting to describe the full-time course of variations of oxytocin concentrations in saliva after intranasal and intravenous administration. However, this does not detract the importance of our findings in respect to our first aim (which was our main goal).

      We agree with the reviewer though that it is at least theoretically possible that we could have missed the window for increases in salivary oxytocin after intravenous oxytocin if it existed, given that we only sampled one post-administration time-point. However, we believe this was unlikely for one reason. Despite the sustained increase (throughout the two-hour observation interval) in plasmatic oxytocin following the intravenous administration of oxytocin, we observed no increase in salivary oxytocin post-dosing (at ~115 min). Unless the half-life of oxytocin is shorter in saliva than in the blood (which we do not know yet), we expected the levels of salivary oxytocin to mirror the changes in plasma – potentially with a slight delay given the time that it might take for oxytocin concentrations to build up in saliva through ultrafiltration from the blood, but this was not the case. Most likely the half-life of oxytocin in the saliva is not shorter than in the blood, since a previous study found increased concentrations of oxytocin in saliva up to 7h after administration of intranasal oxytocin (as the reviewer pointed out below, in our study we no longer could detect significant increases in plasmatic oxytocin after the intranasal administration of 40 IU with two different methods at around 115 mins post-administration). Therefore, while we acknowledge these limitations we also believe they do not detract from the importance of our main findings and the potential they hold to influence the field towards a more rigorous use of these measurements. Please see below for the implemented changes in the text.

      Line 554 “It is possible that we may have missed peak increases in saliva oxytocin after the intravenous administration of exogenous oxytocin if they occurred between treatment administration and post-administration sampling. This is unlikely given that the dose we administered intravenously resulted in sustained increases in plasmatic oxytocin over the course of two hours. Unless the half-life of oxytocin in saliva is much shorter than in the plasma, it would be surprising to not find any increases in salivary oxytocin after intravenous oxytocin given that concentrations of oxytocin in the plasma were still elevated at the specific time-point of our second saliva sample. Currently, we have no estimate for the half-life of oxytocin in saliva; however, given that previous studies have found evidence of increased salivary oxytocin after single intranasal administrations of 16IU and 24IU oxytocin up to seven hours post-administration(19), it is unlikely that the half-life of oxytocin is shorter in the saliva than in the plasma.

      3) As reported in page 6, the dataset A comprises administrations approximately 40 IU of intranasal oxytocin and 10 IU on intravenous. The rationale to set these doses should be described. Since the 40IU is different from 24 IU which is employed in most of the previous publications in the research field, potential influence associated with the doses should be tested and discussed.

      Thank you for the opportunity to clarify this aspect of our work. With respect of our primary aims (to investigate whether single measurements of salivary and plasmatic oxytocin at baseline can be reliably measured within individuals across different days), the choice of doses is of course not relevant.

      With respect to our secondary aim, namely, to investigate whether salivary oxytocin can be used to index concentrations of oxytocin in the plasma, particularly after the administration of synthetic oxytocin using the intranasal and intravenous routes, the administered doses are relevant.

      The data reported here were collected as part of a larger project – which determined the choice of both intranasal and IV doses (2). As explained in our response to Reviewer 1, point 2, the selection 10IU (over 10min) was the highest intravenous dose that we could get permission to administer safely in healthy volunteers as a proof of concept, so as to achieve a robust and prolonged increase in plasmatic oxytocin over the course of our full testing session. In this manner, we demonstrate that even when plasmatic levels of OT are maintained substantially increased throughout the observation interval, we cannot detect increases in salivary oxytocin.

      Regarding the intranasal OT dose, it is worth noting that the 24 IU is indeed popular in oxytocin studies, but not exclusive, and generally the selection of dose in oxytocin studies has not been informed by detailed dose-response characterizations. Our choice of 40IU was made for the purposes of matching our previous work on the pharmacodynamics of OT in healthy volunteers(20), and is a dose we (21-29) and others (e.g. (30)) have commonly used with patients.

      A potentially important implication if dose variations also imply variation in the total volume of liquid administered (as is usually the case with standard nasal sprays – but not with the nebuliser), then it is likely that the potential for drip-down might increase for higher volumes and decrease for lower volumes. As far as we know, no study has ever investigated the impact of administered volume on salivary oxytocin after the intranasal administration of synthetic oxytocin, but we agree this would be an important point to look at. We have now expanded our discussion to accommodate this point.

      Line 519 “We expect this phenomenon to be particularly pronounced for higher administered volumes. Further studies should examine the impact of different administered volumes on increases in salivary oxytocin.”

      4) It is difficult to understand that no significant elevations in plasma oxytocin levels were observed after intranasal spray or nebuliser of oxytocin. From figure 4A, the differences between levels at baseline and post administration are similar between nebuliser, spray, and placebo. Please discuss the potential interpretation on this result.

      The plasmatic concentrations of oxytocin we report in this study refer solely to the samples acquired at around 2h after the administration of intranasal oxytocin. We reported the full-time course of changes in plasmatic oxytocin in a paper published earlier this year(2) – which we now refer the reader to. We did find increases in plasmatic oxytocin after administration of oxytocin with the spray and nebuliser (around 3x the baseline concentrations) that did not differ between intranasal methods of administration. Plasmatic oxytocin reached a peak within 15 mins from the end of the intranasal administrations. Given the short half-life of oxytocin in the plasma, we believe it is not surprising that at 115 mins after the end of our last treatment administration the concentrations of oxytocin in the plasma are no longer different from the placebo condition.

      Line 166 “The full time course of changes in plasmatic oxytocin after the administration of intranasal and intravenous oxytocin in this study has been reported elsewhere(2).”

      5) In page 12, the reason why not to employ any correction for multiple comparisons in the statistical analyses should be clarified.

      We apologize that this was not sufficiently clear, but we did correct for multiple testing using the Tukey procedure in our analyses investigating the effects of treatment on salivary and plasmatic oxytocin (this was described in page 9 – Treatment effects). If the reviewer meant something else, we would be glad to follow any further advice on multiple testing correction he/she might have.

      Line 250 “Treatment effects: The effect of treatment on blood/saliva oxytocin concentration were assessed using a 4 x 2 repeated-measures two-way analysis of variance Treatment (four levels: Spray, Nebuliser, Intravenous and Placebo) x Time (two levels: Baseline and post-administration). Post-hoc comparisons to clarify a significant interaction were corrected for multiple comparisons following the Tukey procedure.

      Reviewer #3:

      In the current study, baseline samples of salivary and plasma oxytocin were assessed in 13, respectively, 16 participants, to assess intra-individual reliability across four time points (separated by approximately 8 days). The main results indicate that, while as a group, average salivary and plasma samples were not significantly different across time points, within-subject coefficient of variation (CV) and intra-class correlation coefficient (ICC) showed poor absolute and relative reliability of plasma and salivary oxytocin measurements over time. Also no association was established between plasma and salivary levels, either at baseline or after administration of oxytocin (either intranasally, or intravenously). Further, salivary/ plasma oxytocin was only enhanced after intranasal, respectively intravenous administration.

      The study addresses an important topic and the paper is clearly written. While the overall multi-session design seems solid, sample collections were performed in the context of larger projects and therefore there appear to be several limitations that reduce the robustness of the presented results and consequently the formulated conclusions.

      General comments

      1) A main conclusion of the current work is that 'single measures of baseline oxytocin concentrations in saliva and plasma are not stable within the same individual'. It seems however that the study did not adhere to a sufficiently rigorous approach to put forward this conclusion. It lacks a control for several important factors, such as timing of the day at which saliva/ plasma samples were obtained, as well as sample volume. Particularly while it is indicated that all visits were identical in structure, important information is missing with regard to whether or not sampling took place consistently at a particular point of time each day, to minimize the influence of circadian rhythm. Without this information it is not possible to draw any firm conclusions on the nature of the intra-individual variability as demonstrated in the salivary and plasma sampling.

      Thanks for pointing this out. Indeed, we were not sufficiently explicit on how strict we were in controlling for some potential sources of variability that could have contributed to the lack of reliability we report here. Our data was acquired in the context of two human pharmacological studies, which by design were strict on a number of aspects to minimize unwarranted noise. All participants were tested in the same period of the day (morning) to avoid the potential contribution of circadian fluctuations of oxytocin. In dataset A, we tried, as much as possible, to match the exact time participants were tested between visits, using the start time of the first visit as a reference. With the exception of one participant, where one session was conduct 1h and 30 mins later than the other three, all the remaining participants from study A were tested within 1h of the exact start time of session 1. Further, we also instructed participants to abstain from alcohol and heavy exercise for 24 h and from any beverage or food for 2 h before scanning. Hence, we believe our sampling protocol was strict enough to discard any potential contribution of major known sources of variability in oxytocin levels.

      The reviewer also inquiries about the volume of the samples. For the plasma samples, we used a standardized protocol and collected the same blood volume in all participants, visits and time-points (1 EDTA tube of approximately 4 ml). The saliva samples were collected using Salivettes. Participants were instructed to place the swab from the Salivette kit in their mouth and chew it gently for 1 min to soak as much saliva as possible. After this, the swab was then returned back to the Salivette and centrifuged. In both cases, to avoid degradation of the peptide in the collected sample, we followed a strict protocol where all samples were put immediately in iced water until centrifugation, which happened within 20 mins of sample collection. Samples were then immediately stored at -80C until analysis. Hence, differences in degradation of the peptide related to the processing of the sample are also unlikely to justify the poor reliabilities we report here.

      For completeness, we have now added all of these further details to our Methods section.

      Line 169 “**All visits were conducted during the morning to avoid the potential confounding of circadian variations in oxytocin levels(31, 32). In addition, we also made sure that each participant was tested at approximately the same time across all four visits (all participants were tested in sessions with less than one hour difference in their onset time, except for one participant where the difference in the onset of one session compared to the other three sessions was 1.5h). “*

      Line 192 “Blood was collected in ethylenediaminetetraacetic acid vacutainers (Kabe EDTA tubes 078001), placed in iced water and centrifuged at 1300 × g for 10 minutes at 4°C within 20 minutes of collection and then immediately pipetted into Eppendorf vials. Samples were immediately stored -80C until analysis. Saliva samples were collected using a salivette (Sarstedt 51.1534.500). Participants were instructed to place the swab from the Salivette kit in their mouth and chew it gently for 1 min to soak as much saliva as possible. After this, the swab was then returned back to the Salivette, centrifuged and stored in the same manner as blood samples. For both saliva and plasma, we stored the samples in aliquots of 0.5 ml, following the RIAgnosis standard operating procedures. We followed this strict protocol, putting all samples in iced water until centrifugation with immediate storage at -80C until analysis to minimize the impact putative differences in degradation of the peptide related to differences in the processing of the samples might have on the reliability of the estimated concentrations of oxytocin.” *

      Correspondingly, a deeper discussion is needed on the reason why ICC's were considerably variable across pairs of assessment sessions, with some pairs yielding good reliability, whereas others yielded (very) poor reliability.

      Currently we have no insightful hypothesis on why this could have been the case. Indeed, we found higher ICCs for only 2 out of 6 pairs of visits for the plasma. However, it is plausible that this might have occurred by chance. In any case, we should note that the 95% confidence intervals for the ICCs of our different pairs of samples overlap; this suggests that there is no evidence that the ICCs we estimated for the specific two pairs where we found higher reliabilities are significantly higher than those observed in the remaining pairs.

      Line 431 “If there are specific reasons explaining the higher reliability indices observed for the specific pairs of sessions, these reasons remain to be elucidated. However, it is not implausible that we might have found higher reliabilities for these specific two pairs by chance, since the 95% confidence intervals for the ICCs for all pairs of samples overlapped.

      More detailed descriptions regarding sampling procedures (timing and sampling intervals) are necessary. Also, more information is needed on the volume of saliva collected at each session, to control for possible dilution effects.

      This information has been added to the revised version of the manuscript (please see response to your point number 1). As a further clarification, oxytocin concentrations were measured in plasma and saliva aliquots of 0.5 ml, following the standard operating procedures of RIAgnosis. This volume was used for all participants, sessions and time-points. Furthermore, for measuring cortisol, the salivettes were shown to allow for an almost 100% recovery, regardless of cortisol concentration, volume of the sample or method of quantification(33), suggesting that the sampling method is robust.

      2) It is indicated that the initial sample would allow to detect intra-class correlation coefficients (ICC) of at least 0.70 (moderate reliability) with 80% of power. Is this still the case after the drop-outs/ outlier removals? Since the main conclusions of the work rely on negative results (conclusions drawn from failures to reject the null hypothesis) it is important to establish the risk for false negatives within a design that is possibly underpowered.

      We understand the concern of the reviewer. However, according to the power calculations provided by Bujang and Baharum, 2017(34), the four repeated samples we collected in Dataset A would have allowed us to detect an ICC of 0.5 with 80% of statistical power even with only 13 subjects (which is the lowest sample size we used for the analysis on saliva in dataset A). The two samples we collected in Dataset B would allow us to detect an ICC of 0.6 with 80% of statistical power even with only 19 subjects. Hence, both datasets were powered to detect an ICC of 0.7 with acceptable power, if it existed, even after the exclusion of outliers.

      3) Did the authors also assess within-session reliability? For example, by assessing ICC between pre and post-measurements in the placebo session.

      Thanks for the suggestion. Indeed, we had not performed this analysis before but we agree it would be informative. We calculated the ICC and CV for the two samples acquired before any treatment administration and the intravenous infusion of saline during the placebo session. These samples where acquired with an approximate 15 min interval in between them. In this analysis, we found that the ICC was excellent 0.92 and the CV 20%. This additional analysis strengthens our findings by supporting the idea that our poor reliabilities across different days reflect true biological variability and cannot be attributed to measurement error. These new findings have now been included in the revised version of the manuscript.

      Abstract

      Line 44 "Results: Single measurements of plasmatic and salivary oxytocin showed poor reliability across visits in both datasets. The reliability was excellent when samples were collected within 15 minutes from each other in the placebo visit.”

      Line 240 “Within-visit reliability analysis: To investigate the reliability of salivary and plasmatic oxytocin concentration within the same visit, we calculated the ICC and CV as described above for two samples acquired before any treatment administration and the intravenous infusion of saline during the placebo session. These samples where acquired with an approximate 15 minutes interval in between them.

      Line 405 “Furthermore, in a further analysis assessing the within-session stability of plasmatic oxytocin using two measurements collected 15 minutes apart from each other in the placebo visit (one sample collected at baseline and the other after the intravenous administration of saline), we found excellent within-session reliability (ICC=0.92, CV=20%). Together, this suggests that the low reliability of endogenous oxytocin measurements across visits in the current study results from true intrinsic individual biological variability and not technical variability/error in the method used for oxytocin quantification.“*

      4) It is indicated that the intra-assay variability of the adopted radioimmunoassay constitutes <10%. Were analyses of the current study run on duplicate samples? Was intra-assay variability assessed directly within the current sample?

      We reported the intra-assay variability determined by RIAgnosis during the development of this assay(35). This was not specifically assessed for the current study.

      Introduction & Discussion

      5) The introduction and discussion is missing a thorough overview of previous studies assessing intra-individual variability in oxytocin levels.

      Thanks for the suggestion. We have now included in our introduction/discussion an overview of previous studies attempting to tackle this question, which unfortunately do not address this question with sufficient detail or using the appropriate methods and statistical analyses (see response to Reviewer 2, point 1). Hence, from the available evidence, it is not possible to draw robust conclusions about the validity of concentrations of oxytocin in saliva and plasma as valid trait markers of the activity of the oxytocin system. With this manuscript, we hope we can prompt further discussion and guide the field towards a more rigorous use of these measurements. A thorough discussion of this literature has now been added to the Introduction and Discussion.

      Line 434 “Our observation of poor reliability questions the use of single measurements of baseline oxytocin concentrations in saliva and plasma as valid trait markers of the physiology of the oxytocin system in humans. Instead, we suggest that, at best, these measurements can provide reliable state markers within short time-intervals (5 mins in our study). Our data does not support previous claims of high stability of plasmatic and salivary oxytocin within individuals over time. For instance, in one study, Feldman et al. (2013) assessed plasmatic oxytocin in recent mothers and fathers at two time-points spaced six months apart during the postpartum period. The authors found strong correlations between the two assessments for both mothers and fathers(14). In another study, Schneiderman et al. (2012) found strong correlations between plasmatic oxytocin concentrations measured at two different instances spaced six months apart in both single and individuals recently involved in a new romantic relationship(15). Two important differences between these studies and ours are i) the method used for oxytocin quantification, and ii) the particular states participants were in when the studies were conducted. Regarding the first difference, these previous studies used ELISA without extraction, reporting concentrations of plasmatic oxytocin well above the typical physiological range of 1-10 pg/ml detected in extracted samples (in their studies, the authors report concentrations above 200 pg/ml). The inclusion of extraction has been postulated as a critical step for obtaining valid measures of oxytocin in biological fluids(4). Unextracted samples were shown to contain immunoreactive products other than oxytocin(4), which contribute largely to the concentrations of oxytocin estimated by this method. It is possible that these non-oxytocin products might represent highly stable plasma housekeeping molecules(17) that masked the true biological variability in oxytocin concentrations between assessments in these previous studies that we could detect in extracted samples in our study. Regarding the second difference, these previous studies on within-individual stability were conducted during the early parenting(14) or early romantic(15) periods, which engage the activity of the oxytocin system in particular ways(18). Instead, we used a normative sample that did not specify these inclusion criteria. Hence, we cannot exclude that during these specific periods the reliability of salivary and plasmatic oxytocin concentrations might be higher. We note though that our sample more closely resembles the samples used the vast majority of studies in the field (which sometimes even exclude participants during early parenthood(36)). Hence, our estimates of reliability are a better starter point for all studies where specific circumstances potentially affecting the activity of the oxytocin system have not been specified a priori.

      6) The paper misses a discussion of previous studies addressing links between salivary/ plasma levels and central oxytocin (e.g. in cerebrospinal fluid). I understand the claim that salivary oxytocin cannot be used to form an estimate of systemic absorption, although technically, a lack of a link between salivary and plasma levels, does not necessarily imply a lack of a relationship to e.g. central levels. The lack of effect is limited to this specific relationship.

      In this study, we did not intend to investigate whether salivary and plasmatic oxytocin are valid proxies for the activity of the oxytocin system in the brain. Our data does not address that question and a thorough discussion of these studies falls, in our opinion, out of the scope of the manuscript. Instead, we focused on whether measurements of oxytocin in saliva and plasma (by far the most commonly used biological fluids to measure oxytocin) are sufficiently stable to provide valid indicators of the physiology of the oxytocin system in humans. Additionally, we also investigated whether salivary oxytocin can index plasmatic oxytocin at baseline and after the administration of synthetic oxytocin using different routes of administration.

      A previous meta-analysis of studies correlating peripheral and CSF measurements of oxytocin has shown that most likely peripheral and CSF measurements do not correlate at baseline; significant correlations could be found after intranasal administration of oxytocin or specific experimental manipulations, such as stress(37). We believe that currently we still do not have a clear answer about the extent to which these peripheral fluids can actually index oxytocin concentrations in the brain (even if associations with CSF are evident in specific instances). For instance, no study has ever shown that CSF oxytocin actually predicts the concentrations of oxytocin in the extracellular fluid of the brain. Given what we currently know about the synaptic release of oxytocin in the brain(38) (in contrast with former theories of exclusive bulk diffusion in the CSF(39)), we think we have good reasons to suspect this might not be the case.

      The only contribution our study can make in that respect is highlighting our current lack of understanding of how oxytocin reaches saliva if not from the blood. Currently there is no evidence of direct secretion of oxytocin to the saliva (not from acinar secretion or nerve terminals release). Hence, as it stands, the most likely mechanism for oxytocin to entry the saliva is from the blood (for instance, by ultrafiltration). If increases in plasmatic oxytocin after intravenous oxytocin cannot produce any significant increases in salivary oxytocin (shown in ours and in a previous study), how does oxytocin reach the saliva and why might it be able to predict concentrations in the CSF, if it does? In this respect, we hope our study highlights the need for further research shedding light on the mechanisms underlying these potential saliva – CSF relationships, if they exist. We would be glad to accommodate any other hypothesis the reviewer might have on this respect.

      Line 522 “The lack of increase in salivary oxytocin after the intravenous administration of exogenous oxytocin that was consistently found in our study and in a previous study(3) also raises the question of how oxytocin reaches the saliva if not from the blood. Currently there is no evidence of direct acinar secretion or direct nerve terminals release of oxytocin to the saliva; therefore, transport from the blood remains as the most plausible mechanism of appearance of oxytocin in the saliva. Clarifying these mechanisms of transport is paramount, given the current hypothesis that salivary oxytocin might be superior to plasma in indexing central levels of oxytocin in the CSF(40).

      Methods

      7) Related to the general comment, the variability in days between sessions is relatively high (average 8.80 days apart (SD 5.72; range 3-28). However, it appears that no explicit measures were taken to control the conducted analyses for this variability.

      Thanks for point this out. Indeed, we were not sufficiently thorough in exploring the impact of this potential variability in the time gap between visits on our estimated ICCs. Thanks to the reviewer we now acknowledged this limitation of our analysis and decided to explore this further. We decided to run the following sensitivity analysis. First, we went back to our dataset A and identified all pairs of consecutive measures that were collected with an exact time interval of 7 days between visits. We could retrieve 15 examples of these pairs from 15 different participants for both saliva and plasma. Then, we recalculated the ICC and CV on this subset of our initial sample. In line with our main analysis, we found poor reliabilities for both salivary and plasmatic oxytocin; in both cases the ICCs were not significantly different from 0 and the CVs were 49% and 40%, respectively. This further analysis has been added to the revised version of the manuscript. We hope the reviewer shares our vision that our main conclusion of poor reliabilities of single measurements of baseline oxytocin in saliva and plasma cannot be simply attributed to the variability in the number of days between visits.

      Line 229 “Since there was considerable variability in the time-interval between visits across participants, we conducted a sensitivity analysis where we repeated our reliability analysis focusing on 15 pairs of consecutive measures that were collected with an exact time interval of 7 days between visits in 15 participants. Here, we recalculated the ICC and CV on this subset of our initial sample, using the approach described above.

      Line 399 “These poor reliabilities are unlikely to be explained by variability in the time-interval between visits of the same individual, since we also found poor reliability indexes for both saliva and plasma when we restricted our analysis to a subset of our sample controlling for the exact number of days spacing visits.”*

      8) A rationale for the adopted dosing and timing (115 min post administration) of the sample extraction is missing. Additionally, it seems that intravenous administrations were always given second, whereas intranasal administrations were given third, with a small delay of approximately 5 min. Hence, it seems that the timing of 115 min post-administration is only accurate for the intranasal administration.

      We collected saliva samples before any treatment administration and after the end of our scanning session (collection of saliva samples in between was just not possible because the participants were inside the MRI machine and could not have moved their heads). For the plasma, we collected samples before any treatment administration, after each treatment administration and at other five time-points during the scanning session. Here, we only report the plasma data that was acquired concomitantly with the saliva samples (the full-time course of plasma changes in plasmatic oxytocin has been reported elsewhere(2)). In the manuscript, we report post-administration times from the end of the full treatment administration protocol. Hence, as the reviewer highlights our post-administration sample was collected at around 115 mins from the last intranasal administration and 120 mins from the end of the intravenous administration. We have now made this aspect explicit in the revised version of the manuscript.

      Line 162 “For the purposes of this report, we use the plasmatic and salivary oxytocin measurements that were obtained at baseline and at 115 minutes after the end of our last treatment administration (this means that our post-administration samples were collected 115 mins after the intranasal administrations and 120 mins after the intravenous administration of oxytocin).

      9) Since the ICC of baseline samples showed poor reliability, it seems suboptimal to pool across sessions for assessing the relationship between salivary and blood measurements. It should be possible to perform e.g. partial correlations on the actual scores, thereby correcting for the repeated measure (subject ID). Further, since the sample size is relatively small (13 subjects), it might be recommended to use non-parametric (e.g. Spearmann correlations) instead of Pearson. The additional reporting of the Bayes factor is appreciated; it is very informative.

      Thanks for the suggestion. In fact, for the correlation the reviewer mentions we indeed used a multilevel approach where we specified subject as a random effect (please see pages 9-10). This allowed us to deal with the dependence of measurements coming from the same subject in different visits. Furthermore, since we also had concerns about the sample size, we calculated Pearson correlations but used bootstrapping (1000 samples) to obtain the 95% confidence intervals and assess significance. Bootstrapping is a robust statistical technique which allows significance testing independently of any assumptions about the distribution of the data and is robust to outliers. Please see page 12 of the manuscript, section “Association between salivary and plasmatic oxytocin levels”.

      10) Now, the authors only compared relationships between salivary and plasma levels, either at baseline or post administration. I'm wondering whether it would be interesting to explore relationships between pre-to-post change scores in salivary versus plasma measures.

      Thanks for the suggestion. We have now conducted this further analysis and we could not find any significant correlation between changes from baseline to post-administration in any of our treatment conditions. As for our other correlation analyses, here we also conducted Bayesian inference, which supported the idea that the null hypothesis of no significant correlation between changes in saliva and plasma from baseline to post-administration is at least 4x more likely than the alternative hypothesis. This further analysis strengthens our confidence that changes in salivary oxytocin after administration of oxytocin using the intranasal and intravenous routes should not be used to predict systemic absorption to the plasma.

      Line 260 “*As a final sanity check, we also investigated correlations between the changes from baseline to post-administration in saliva and plasma in each of our treatment conditions separately.

      Line 485 “Furthermore, we could not find any significant correlation between changes in salivary or plasmatic oxytocin from baseline to 115 mins after the end of our last treatment administration in any of our four treatment conditions. The lack of significant associations between salivary and plasmatic oxytocin (and respective changes from baseline) was further supported through our Bayesian analyses which demonstrated that given our data the null hypotheses were at least three times more likely than the alternative hypothesis.”*

      11) Please provide more information on the outlier detection procedure (outlier labelling rule).

      This information has now been added to the revised version of the manuscript.

      Line 271 “Outliers were identified using the outlier labelling rule(41); this means that a data point was identified as an outlier if it was more than 1.5 x interquartile range above the third quartile or below the first quartile.”*

      12) Please indicate how deviations from a Gaussian distribution were assessed.

      We used the combined assessment of i) differences between mean and median; ii) skewness and kurtosis; iii) histogram; iv) Q-Q plots; and v) the Kolmogorov-Smirnov and Shapiro-Wilk normality tests. Deviations from a normal distribution is common in the concentration of several analytes in the saliva (42), including oxytocin (15); hence, following the current recommendations, we used log transformations of the raw concentrations but plot the raw concentrations to facilitate the interpretation of our plots.

      Results

      13) Please verify the degrees of freedom for the post-hoc tests performed to assess pre-post changes at each treatment level (e.g. baseline vs Post administration: Spray - t(122) = 7.06, p < 0.001) . Why is this 122? Shouldn't this be a simple paired-sample t-test with 13 subjects?

      We apologize for this oversight. Indeed, we did a mistake in copying the values of the degrees of freedom from SPSS. We have now corrected these values. All the other p-values and F or T values were reported correctly and hence are not changed in the revised version of the manuscript (please see also response to Reviewer 1, question 4 regarding inconsistencies in the reported p-values).

    1. Note: This rebuttal was posted by the corresponding author to Review Commons. Content has not been altered except for formatting.

      Learn more at Review Commons


      Reply to the reviewers

      Please note that the authors have provided a formatted PDF version of this rebuttal, including additional figures and references, via the Open Science Framework: https://osf.io/5acqp/

      Reviewer #1

      This is an interesting and thorough study characterising human iPSC with hetero or homozygous mutation in pi3k pathway that lead to its hyper-activation. They prove that the increased stemness results from enhanced autocrine responsiveness to TGF signalling pathway. The main conclusions are well supported by the presented data. Cutting edge tools and bioinformatic analysis are adequately applied. I have only one important point:

      1) Western blot based validation of TGF pathway activation in wt and mutant iPSCs will be helpful to strengthen the results based on bioinformatic data.

      AUTHORS’ RESPONSE__:__ We thank the Reviewer for the positive evaluation of our work.

      Functional validation of the signalling hypothesis is indeed important, and we did in fact already present supportive data. Current evidence suggests that SMAD2 is the main transcription factor mediating actions of the TGFb/NODAL pathway in an early developmental context [1,2], and we have shown increased phosphorylation of SMAD2 (S465/S467) in PIK3CAH1047R/H1047R iPSCs using RPPA in the two datasets shown in Fig.2.

      We have attempted to demonstrate increased NODAL protein directly in PIK3CAH1047R/H1047R cells, but have been unsuccessful due to poor signal on immunoblotting. We thus opted for functional testing of our hypothesis using the experiment presented in Fig. 5, wherein TGFb (a surrogate for NODAL) is removed from the culture medium. Human iPSCs depend strictly on TGFb/NODAL for maintenance of NANOG expression and thus pluripotency [3,4]. Upon exclusion of TGFb/NODAL from the culture medium of normal human iPSCs, the early responses (prior to overt differentiation) are expected to be: (A) decreased NODAL expression, due to well-established autoregulation [2], then (B) a decrease in NANOG and ultimately POU5F1 (OCT3/4) mRNA levels (see also Introduction, lines 80-90). The evidence in Fig. 5 that PIK3CAH1047R/H1047R fail to exhibit these responses upon exogenous TGFb/NODAL removal supports the notion that these cells autonomously sustain TGFb/NODAL signalling.

      For improved clarity, we have also added the following information to the revised manuscript:

      lines 202-205: “This is consistent with strong NODAL mRNA upregulation and increased pSMAD2 (S465/S467) in PIK3CAH1047R/H1047R iPSCs in the current study (Dataset S2 and RPPA data in Fig. 2, respectively), and with prior evidence of activation of the NODAL/TGFb pathway in homozygous PIK3CAH1047R iPSCs.”

      Reviewer #2

      In this manuscript, Madsen et al have investigated the role of heterozygous versus homozygous PIK3CAH1047R gain-of-function mutation at maintaining stemness of induced pluripotent stem cells (iPSCs). The authors have performed high-depth RNAseq, proteomic, and RPPA analyses to show that biallelic PIK3CA alterations induce stronger activation of the PI3K signaling axis, compared to monoallelic mutations. The authors claim that a higher PI3K signaling dose activates the NODAL/TGF-b pathway, which in turn supports stemness in an autocrine fashion. These are important findings, however, the manuscript and its conclusions can be improved.

      AUTHORS’ RESPONSE__:__ We thank the Reviewer for acknowledging the importance of the work and for their constructive suggestions for improvements.

      The authors have described the role of PIK3CAH-1047R gain-of-function mutation in cancer and overgrowth syndromes. However, cancer associated somatic mutations in PIK3CA are mostly heterozygous. Similarly, PIK3CA related overgrowth syndromes (PROS) are caused by post-zygotic mosaic PIK3CA activating mutation. As such, the relevance of homozygous PIK3CA alterations to these pathological conditions is unclear. The authors should elaborate on the biological implications of their findings.

      AUTHORS’ RESPONSE__:__ We disagree with the Reviewer’s comment which implies that homozygous PIK3CA mutations are not relevant to many cancers. In our previous work [5], we provided evidence that many human cancers harbour multiple PIK3CA mutant alleles. Specifically, among cancers with a unique PIK3CA mutation, approximately 50% exhibit multiple copies according to allele copy number analysis. We further demonstrated that a substantial proportion of cancers have multiple different PIK3CA variants or additional oncogenic ‘hits’ within the pathway. These findings have been supported by other recent high-profile papers [6–8]. Such multiple alterations increase activity of the PI3K pathway beyond the level seen with heterozygosity alone [5,6]. This substantial body of literature renders our PIK3CAH1047R iPSC model system highly relevant for studying disease-relevant, dose-dependent oncogenic PIK3CA activation.

      The Reviewer is correct, however, that PROS is caused by postzygotic heterozygous PIK3CA mutations almost exclusively. Observations in homozygous cells are therefore not directly relevant to the pathogenesis of PROS. On the other hand, the heterozygous cells are closely relevant, being human, carefully matched with isogenic controls, and unperturbed by further manipulations such as artificial immortalisation. Our prior studies demonstrated no clear phenotypes in heterozygous cells in the iPSC differentiation paradigm, despite the rock solid causal nature of heterozygous mutations in PROS. This negative finding, surprising given the dramatic PROS phenotypes, is very important in understanding how best to create disease-relevant PROS models. One intent of the current study was to increase the sensitivity of our transcriptomic analysis, and to combine this with proteomic studies to determine if heterozygous cells really do not exhibit a phenotype. We now show that there are indeed faint echoes in heterozygous cells of the dramatic changes in homozygous cells. We believe that the human growth phenotype is a summative consequence of such small differences in growth behaviours sustained over months and years, highlighting how subtle difference in signalling can lead to dramatic human growth consequences across the lifecourse. Similar observations were also recently made following systematic analyses of oncogenic RAS mutations [9]. The new information we present about heterozygous PIK3CAH1047R cells, while much less “showy” than the cancer-relevant behavious of homozygous cells, we thus contend is very important for understanding of the PROS phenotype and its experimental modelling. To emphasise this point, we have added the following statements to the abstract and discussion, respectively.

      • lines 56-57: “This work illustrates the importance of allele dosage and expression when artificial systems are used to model human genetic disease caused by activating PIK3CA mutations.”
      • lines 104-106: “We discuss the implications of our findings for understanding and modelling developmental disorders and cancers driven by genetic PI3K activation.”
      • lines 333-340: “Finally, our observations are important for future studies seeking to model human PIK3CA-related diseases. The modest changes observed in heterozygous PIK3CAH1047R cells, in sharp contrast to the radical transcriptional alterations in homozygous cells, emphasise the importance of careful allele dose titration when artificial overexpression systems are used to model disorders caused by genetic PIK3CA activation. Our findings in heterozygous cells are also a reminder that very small effect sizes in cellular systems may summate and result in major human phenotypes over a life course. That such minor changes are found in a cellular study of a rare and severe disorder emphasises the challenges of modelling much more subtle disease susceptibility conferred by GWAS-detected genetic associations, where cellular effect sizes are likely to be smaller still.”

        The role of biallelic PIK3CA mutation is reminiscent of compound mutations in PIK3CA which have also been shown to increase PI3K signaling output. However, double PIK3CA mutations confer enhanced sensitivity to PI3K inhibition (Toska et al. Science 2019). Could the authors kindly speculate on this discrepancy.

      AUTHORS’ RESPONSE: We emphasise first that PIK3CAH1047R/H1047R cells do respond to BYL719 at the signalling level, as demonstrated previously [5] and in the manuscript (revised Figure S5; see also additional Western blot below). Our point is that the cells have undergone a switch to self-sustained stemness. That is, while PIK3CA activation was the driver of the initial change in cell state, the induced stemness phenotype is no longer reversed by removal of that trigger, with our data suggesting that this is now driven by self-sustained TGFb/NODAL signalling. This is in line with the role of this pathway in the maintenance of the pluripotent state. We speculate that this may be important in a cancer context where surviving stem cells may permit cancer persistence after toxic therapies, even if short term growth of tumours is reduced by agents such as PI3K inhibitors.

      Our data are not directly comparable to prior cellular data, for example in Vasan et al. [6], due to: (a) use of different cell model system and (b) assessment of different functional responses. We would also sound some methodological notes of caution re some of the prior studies alluded to, as potentially confounding differences in growth rate in the cells studied was not corrected for. It is well-established that IC50 and Emax values depend on cell division rates, and failure to correct for this can result in artefactual correlations between genotype and drug sensitivity (see, e.g., Hafner et al. Nature Methods 2016: “Growth rate inhibition metrics correct for confounders in measuring sensitivity to cancer drugs” [10]**).

      Similarly, the p110 alpha specific inhibitor, alpelisib, is highly effective against PIK3CA-mutant ER+ breast cancer and PROS. As such, the clinical relevance of the insensitivity of homozygous PIK3CA mutation to PI3K inhibitors is unclear.

      AUTHORS’ RESPONSE__:__ Efficacy of Alpelisib in PROS is currently supported only by unregistered observational studies, but is nevertheless striking. It is not relevant to our findings in homozygous cells, as the Reviewer has previously observed, however.

      As for cancer, in a randomised phase 3 trial that compared Alpelisib/BYL719 with fulvestrant to fulvestrant alone, the overall response (irrespective of PIK3CA mutant status) was indeed greater with the combination treatment (26.6 % vs 12.8 %), with a hazard ratio of 0.65 (95% CI, 0.5 to 0.85) in patients with PIK3CA-mutant caners versus a hazard ratio of 0.85 (95% CI, 0.58 to 1.25) in those without a PIK3CA mutation [11]. This trial demonstrated the utility of additional PIK3CA mutant-centric stratification, but a substantial proportion of patients with PIK3CA-mutant tumours (>50%) did not benefit from the BYL719 and fulvestrant combination [11]. However, these observations are not directly relevant to this manuscript and are instead included in a separate manuscript focused on PI3K signalling and stemness in human breast cancers (preprint [12]**).

      Figure 2: The authors have performed RPPA analysis in the presence of 100 nM BYL719. Alpelisib is commonly used at 1 uM concentration for in-vitro experiments, and has a cMax of ~5 uM. We suggest the authors perform western blot analysis to confirm the results of RPPA.

      AUTHORS’ RESPONSE__:__ We carefully chose the optimal concentration of BYL719 to preserve inhibitor selectivity, and to avoid undue toxicity and confounding off-target effects, rather than copying the dose “commonly used”. The Cmax is not relevant to our use of BYL719 in the current study as a precise tool compound. We refer the Reviewer to the known pharmacological characteristics of this compound [13,14]. According to available evidence, it is only a selective PI3Kα inhibitor at concentrations 250 nM (Table below adapted from Ref. **[13]; for formatted version, please see PDF version: https://osf.io/ecmhr/)

      Enzyme

      In vitro IC50 for NVP-BYL719 (nM)

      PI3Kα

      4.6 +/- 0.4

      PI3Kα-H1047R

      4.8 +/- 0.4

      PI3K**b

      1156 +/- 77

      PI3K**d

      290 +/- 180

      PI3K**g

      250 +/- 140

      PI4K**b

      571 +/- 42

      We have previously demonstrated (Fig. 2C in Ref. [5]) that 100 nM BYL719 is sufficient to restore pAKT (S473) levels in both heterozygous and homozygous PIK3CAH1047R to levels observed in WT cells. This is consistent with the RPPA data reported in the current work (Fig. 2B). Of note, while 500 nM BYL719 completely ablates pAKT irrespective of genotype, we previously noted substantial toxicity [5], precluding use of this or higher doses of BYL719 in our model system. This is in line with a recent Nature Cell Biology study by Yilmaz et al. ([15]) which demonstrated the essential growth-promoting role of the PI3K pathway in human pluripotent stem cells; Yilmaz et al. also demonstrate that compared to somatic cells (fibroblasts), human pluripotent stem cells suffer dramatic effects on growth/survival in response to Torin1/rapamycin [15], overall suggesting that this cell type is exquisitely sensitive to inhibition of the PI3K/AKT/mTOR pathway.

      In the present study we have also confirmed that 250 nM BYL719, used for Fig. 5 experiments, has worked as expected at the level of pAKT (S473) as shown in the below Western blot (see also revised Fig. S5; please access PDF version to view Western blot: https://osf.io/ecmhr/)

      Figures 3 and 4: The authors should expand their RNAseq analysis to demonstrate enrichment of stemness and TGFb signaling in homozygous mutant cells compared to heterozygous cells.

      AUTHORS’ RESPONSE__:__ We thank the Reviewer for this suggestion. The unsupervised MDS plot (Fig. 1A) clearly demonstrates the overlap between wild-type and heterozygous cells, strongly suggesting functional concordance and consistent differences to homozygous counterparts. Indeed, the below count table illustrates that the majority of differentially expressed genes in homozygous versus wild-type cells are also differentially expressed in homozygous versus heterozygous cells, including the direction of the change (please access the PDF version for formatted table: https://osf.io/ecmhr/)

      Comparison

      Differentially expressed gene count

      HOMvsWT

      5644

      HOMvsHET

      5764

      HOMvsWT AND HOMvsHET

      4825 (2300 upregulated; 2525 downregulated; 1 discordant)

      We have now performed additional fast gene set enrichment analyses (fgsea; shown below - please access PDF version to view figure: https://osf.io/ecmhr/) using the R package fgsea ([16]) and 14 of the Broad Institute’s 50 Hallmark Gene Set Collection [17], including manual addition of the PLURINET signature [18]. The 14 gene sets were chosen based on their relevance to answering the Reviewer’s question as well as their connection to PI3K signalling. Fold changes for all expressed genes were included in the analyses, without further thresholding in order to minimise bias.

      The results for homozygous vs wild-type comparisons are concordant with our upstream regulator analyses using IPA; as expected, TGFb signalling and PI3K signalling are among the top positively enriched (NES > 1) in comparison between homozygous and heterozygous cells. Unsurprisingly, however, the strength of the enrichments are lower when comparing the two PIK3CAH1047R genotypes.

      We are not convinced that including these surplus data will add value to the manuscript and its main message, however we will leave this decision to the discretion of the Editor (please also refer to our response to the subsequent question from Reviewer 2). Moreover, these data will remain visible in the publicly available rebuttal document.

      The authors should confirm the results of pathway analysis in vitro to show that homozygous PIK3CA mutation confers increased stemness compared to heterozygous mutation.

      AUTHORS’ RESPONSE__:__ This was a key finding in our previous publication [5]. The aim of the current study was to interrogate this phenomenon further through high-depth transcriptomic/signalling analyses.

      Figure 5: Kindly provide direct evidence demonstrating that increased PIK3CA signaling output induces NODAL expression in this experimental setting.

      AUTHORS’ RESPONSE__:__ We have consistently demonstrated increased NODAL mRNA expression (RNAseq data, Fig. S4 and Ref. [5]). Unfortunately, we have been unsuccessful in attempts to obtain good quality immunoblots for NODAL protein in PIK3CAH1047R/H1047R cells (as noted in response to Reviewer 1). We note, in fact, that such documentation of NODAL protein levels, while not unprecedented, is fairly rare.

      Also, please normalize gene expression data to WT cells so it is easy to visualize the changes in NODAL and NANOG expression in homozygous and heterozygous mutants compared to WT iPSCs.

      AUTHORS’ RESPONSE__:__ It is arithmetically more precise to normalise to the highest expression (i.e. that of PIK3CAH1047R/H1047R cells) – thereby avoiding artificial inflation of fold-changes when normalising to very low levels of expression. Ultimately, the relative levels calculated – and the increased expression of NODAL in PIK3CAH1047R/H1047R cells – are identical visually. Only the entirely arbitrary units change. Thus we do not deem normalisation to WT to be necessary or to add value to the analysis.

      Kindly quantify Fig. S5.

      AUTHORS’ RESPONSE__:__ These brightfield micrographs were taken as part of routine practice to monitor cell health during maintenance and experimentation, and are suboptimal for direct quantitation due to uneven illumination background and lack of whole-well imaging. Nevertheless, we have now undertaken quantification as the Reviewer suggests, using individual images taken during independent experimental replicates. The results have been added to Fig. S5 and support our assertion that 250 nM BYL719 had a growth inhibitory effect in homozygous PIK3CAH1047R iPSCs. All raw images and associated data have been uploaded to the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/hbf7x/). The following short method section detailing the image analysis algorithm has also been included in the revised supplementary material:

      “Colony size quantitation from light micrographs

      Routine cell culture light micrographs were acquired on an EVOS FL digital inverted microscope (AMF4300, Thermo Fisher Scientific) using the 4X or 10X objective (final magnification 40X and 100X, respectively). For quantitation, 4X images were used for colony segmentation with Definiens Developer XD software. Background was detected using a contrast threshold; for this each pixel was compared to those in the surrounding 24 pixels (i.e. a 5x5 pixel box), and pixels with low contrast (between -50 and +50) were classified as background. Remaining pixels were classified as colonies, and any holes (pixels that were not initially classified as being part of the colony due to low contrast) were filled. Edges of the resulting colonies were smoothened by shrinking and then growing the colonies by 2 pixels. Finally, colonies less than 2000 pixels in size were reclassified as background. The area of the resulting colonies could then be measured and averaged over each field of view.”

      Reviewer #3

      In this manuscript by Madsen et al., a comparison of the transcriptome and proteome in heterozygous and homozygous PIK3CAH1047R human pluripotent stem cells mutants is presented. The authors demonstrate marked alterations in expression at both the protein and RNA level of homozygous mutants compared to wildtype, while heterozygous lines exhibit only minor changes. Multiple analytical approaches are employed to investigate network alterations, leading the authors to suggest a TGFβ-mediated rewiring of key pluripotent genes to induce a state of sustained stemness. Madsen et al. conclude with a set of experiments to functionally implicate NODAL/TGFβ autocrine signalling in PIK3CAH1047R dose-dependent stemness. The key conclusions are not convincing. While the unbiased omics approach sets up this study well, the study suffers from a lack of convincing functional assays (cell biological assays) to test their model and tease apart a phenotype for the het cells. More robust functional experiments are required to support the finding the NODAL/TGFβ signalling mediates the self-sustained stemness, particularly because this is the major novel finding distinguished from the authors previous work.

      AUTHORS’ RESPONSE__:__ We thank the Reviewer for their detailed critique. Our perspective on the robustness and novelty of our findings diverges from that of the Reviewer, however, as we elaborate on in more detail below.

      While the authors present a comprehensive omics investigation into alterations between wild type, homozygous, and heterozygous mutants, the critical functional experiments are lacking. In Figure 5, the authors seek to support the role of TGFβ in mediated stemness in the homozygous mutants, however, are not able to directly deplete TGFβ due to technical limitations of the culture conditions. Consequentially, the experiments are primarily built on the use of NODAL withdrawal and stimulation. The data presented thus implicate NODAL in the stemness phenotype, but it's not obvious TGFβ is substantially involved, particularly considering the inhibitor subsequently employed also inhibits NODAL type 1 receptors.

      AUTHORS’ RESPONSE__:__ NODAL and TGFb activate shared signalling pathways downstream from their respective receptors, and indeed they (as well as Activin) can be used interchangeably in stem cell culture, which is common practice [19–21]. Commercially available Essential 8/TeSR-E8 is supplemented with TGFb not NODAL; therefore the factor we have removed is TGFb, prior to any controlled introduction of NODAL (based on strong upregulation of its mRNA in PIK3CAH1047R/H1047R). Any residual TGFb-like ligands will be contributed by Matrigel as outlined in the text (lines 247-251). It is well-established that “NODAL/TGFb signalling” denotes signalling through SMAD2/3/4 (as opposed to BMP signalling through SMAD1/5/8), and this is how we use the term throughout the manuscript. Accordingly, it is functional activation of the “NODAL/TGFb signalling pathway” that we investigate (see also response to Reviewer 1, p.1).

      In summary, we seek not to make a distinct point about TGFb, but rather refer to NODAL/TGFb signalling as a matter of biochemical correctness. For clarity, we now replace mentions of “TGFb signalling” with “NODAL/TGFb signalling” throughout the revised manuscript. We have also revised the legend for Figure 3 to make this clearer.

      Furthermore, there is a paucity of readouts for stemness. For example, a more convincing narrative would include additional expression markers of the core pluripotency network (e.g. OCT4, SOX2, etc.) as well as functional readouts (e.g. NODAL withdrawal and assessment of differentiation) after NODAL stimulation/depletion and comparing across genotypes. Overall, the primary conclusions of this work are not well-evidence by the presented data and the authors should consider additional functional experiments or reframing the narrative.

      AUTHORS’ RESPONSE__:__ We chose the current strategy because we wanted to capture the earliest changes after depletion of NODAL/TGFb/ signalling, prior to any signalling rewiring triggered by differentiation. In fact, we believe that a strength of this study is our observation of differences in critical stemness markers in spite of the short time course. To aid non-expert readers we offered a primer on stemness genes and rationale for the markers chosen in the existing introduction (lines 80-90).

      We have further assessed additional stemness and differentiation marker genes in two independent homozygous PIK3CAH1047R cell lines using a high-throughput pluripotent stem cell scorecard (Fig. S4). This replicates the effect on cell marker genes documented by RT-qPCR in Fig.5, while also showing additional reductions in genes that were upregulated in homozygous PIK3CAH1047R cells (MYC, GDF3, FGF4) and which have previously been shown to be highly expressed in pluripotent stem cells (we have now added this additional clarification to the legend of Fig. S4) [22]. Despite the short term treatment, these data also show that no other treatment but SB431542 is capable of triggering expression of early neuroectoderm markers (CDH9, MAP2 and PAPLN) [23], prior to overt morphological changes in the cultures (Fig. S5; higher resolution images are also available via The Open Science Framework: https://osf.io/hbf7x/). Neuroectodermal gene expression is expected upon inhibition of TGFb signalling in human pluripotent stem cells [24,25].

      A key conclusion of this study is there is a dose-dependent stemness phenotype. As this is not explicitly defined, to this reader, it would imply a graded response between wild type, heterozygotes, and homozygotes in the phenotypic and molecular characteristics. However, as is noted particularly in the omics components of the manuscript, there is in fact "near-binary" alteration in the assayed characteristics. Again, this should be qualified more explicitly, but it is more consistent with the data, which suggests the heterozygotes behave very similarly to the wild types, while homozygotes have substantial alterations. I would suggest the authors consider renaming their descriptions, removing "near-binary" and "dose-dependent" to something like "dose-threshold." This suggests after X threshold of oncogenic PI3K signalling, substantial alterations occur; under this threshold (e.g. hets), changes are marginal. In the event however that there may be a more "dose-dependent" effect, I would expect the transcriptomic and proteomic changes observed in the heterozygous cell lines should be seen in the homozygous cell lines (of which they are likely in greater in magnitude in addition to other changes).

      AUTHORS’ RESPONSE__:__ This appears to us to be largely a matter of semantics. In talking of “dose dependency” we were certainly not implying a graded affect (as the Reviewer points out, our are findings are far from this, suggesting a sharp threshold of dose which triggers widespread changes), and indeed nothing in these words strictly suggests this interpretation. Nevertheless we are sensitive to the fact of the Reviewer’s interpretation of the term, and mindful that this might be shared by other readers. On the other hand talking of a “near-binary” effect seems to us to be an accurate description of our findings. We have edited the manuscript to minimise ambiguity with the following changes:

      • line 49 “dose” replaced with “strength”: “We demonstrate signalling rewiring as a function of oncogenic PI3K signalling strength, and provide experimental evidence that self-sustained stemness is causally related to enhanced autocrine NODAL/TGFb
      • line 102: “This work provides in-depth characterisation of the near-binary PI3K signalling effects seen in hPSCs ….”
      • lines 195, 198, 317: inserted “allele dose-dependent We would also like to take issue with the case that the Reviewer seems to be making that a more graded change in gene expression across heterozygotes and homozygotes is to be expected. As mentioned in the manuscript (lines 206-210), there is evidence for NODAL/TGFb pathway activation in heterozygous cells. Nevertheless given the known temporal, context- and dose-dependent effects of this pathway [1,2,26,27] and, importantly, the widely described biological properties of developmental systems (featuring positive feedback loops, bistability and hysteresis; see Ref. [28,29]), we have no reason to expect that transcriptomic and proteomic changes observed in homozygous cell lines will be reproduced in heterozygous cell lines.

      The manuscript would benefit from more direct comparisons between the heterozygotes and homozygotes.

      AUTHORS’ RESPONSE__:__ Please refer to the additional data provided in response to a similar question by Reviewer 2.

      Further to the above point, as the marginal phenotype observed in heterozygotes is a critical point in this paper, the authors would benefit from including heterozygote lines in the functional experiments presented in Fig 5. Inclusion of the hets in these experiments would instill confidence in this reader that the marginal molecular alterations characterized at the proteomic and transcriptomic level is reflected in the lack of functional stemness-sustaining behaviour.

      AUTHORS’ RESPONSE__:__ The lack of stemness-sustaining behaviour in the heterozygous clones was demonstrated across multiple different experiments in our previous work, and further functional studies of early differentiation in these cells seemed a poor use of resource and very unlikely to give useful insights. Given the major disease phenotype associated with the same genetic change (PROS), the relative lack of phenotype in heterozygous cells was surprising and holds obvious implications for disease modelling (see also response to Reviewer 2, pp.2-3), and for how model systems are “calibrated” against human developmental disease. The aim of the current work was to:

        • Determine whether increasing the depth of signalling and transcriptomic analyses would unmask small but important changes in heterozygous mutants that might have been missed in prior studies (i.e. we actively aimed to increase the power of the study for identification of subtle changes) and *
        • To characterise in greater depth the signalling and transcriptional changes underpinning the robust threshold effect observed for self-sustained stemness driven by PIK3CAH1047R/H1047R. We would further observe that PROS does not feature obvious qualititative errors in tissue specification, but rather excessive growth of more or less normally differentiated tissues. We conceptualise this as reflecting a small incremental growth advantage in normally differented tissues of certain lineages that summates to create a major disease phenotype over months and years.*

      Thus, without the functional and mechanistic experiments alluded to above, the claims/ conclusions are speculative. In particular, the cancer narrative is irrelevant to the study. Considering both the lack of conclusive differentiation experiments or relevant breast cancer experiments, the discussion on differentiation therapy for breast cancer should be removed.

      AUTHORS’ RESPONSE__:__ The reference to cancer links to a computational study of human breast cancers where we specifically looked at the relationship between strength of PI3K signalling and ‘stemness’ [12], both measured using established transcriptional indices. We have included the bioRxiv reference in our revised manuscript (see l.337). While there is an element of speculation in this cancer observation, we do feel it is important and grounded in this and the BioRXiv study, and would prefer to maintain it. However, if editors take a different view it can be removed.

      Reproducibility is a concern for this study. The authors should perform more replicates on their experiments (focusing on technical replicates of the lines employed to discern technical vs biological variability). A challenge in reading this manuscript is understanding which replicates were used for which experiments, and whether they are technical or biological (i.e. different lines). While some of the figure legends note this information, it would be helpful to provide clarity throughout the text. In addition, it should be noted that some experiments (e.g. the RPPA analysis in Fig 2B and Fig S3B) show substantial variability between replicates, but because it appears only a single technical replicate from two different cell lines was used, it is impossible to distinguish whether the variability is of a biological or technical nature. The authors would do well to focus on collecting more technical replicates of fewer biological replicates, and then expand to include more biological replicates if initial biological variation is observed.

      AUTHORS’ RESPONSE__:__ We strenuously disagree with the Reviewer on this point. Throughout this manuscript, we have been transparent and thorough in reporting how experiments were performed, including the number of both biological and technical replicates. Representative examples include:

      Legend to Figure 2A (RPPA dataset in growth-replete conditions): “The data are based on 10 wild-type cultures (3 clones), 5 PIK3CAWT/H1047R cultures (3 clones) and 7 PIK3CAH1047R/H1047R cultures (2 clones) as indicated.”

      Legend to Figure 5: “The data are from two independent experiments, with each treatment applied to triplicate cultures of three wild-type and two homozygous iPSC clones.

      Specifically to address the RPPA studies, and as is clear from the Figure 2 legend, we initially performed RPPA analyses in growth factor-replete conditions with extensive technical and biological replication, arguing against the Reviewer’s point. To aid interpretation, we opted for summarising this large dataset in Venn diagrams (following extensive limma-based statistical analysis, including correction for multiple comparisons and sample interdependence as advised in Ref. [30]). If the Reviewer deems it valuable, we could include a heatmap overview as shown below:

      [To view figure, please access PDF version of this rebuttal on https://osf.io/ecmhr/]

      We took the view that the above representation, while comprehensive, is not particularly informative to the reader. All individual data points for both total and phosphoproteins – with and without normalisation – are plotted as part of separate barplots in the accompanying RNotebook (https://osf.io/d9tca/). These clearly demonstrate that the technical and biological variability in canonical PI3K signalling responses at the level of AKT and immediately downstream of AKT is very low. The same applies to the increased phosphorylation of SMAD2 (S465/S467) in PIK3CAH1047R iPSCs. We include two examples below, and would be happy to include the link to the above RNotebook in the respective Figure legend if the Reviewer deems this helpful.

      [To view figure, please access PDF version of this rebuttal on https://osf.io/ecmhr/]

      The interpretation of the second RPPA experiment (Fig. 2B) in growth factor-depleted conditions is focused entirely on these responses due to their consistency across both datasets (further supported by low-throughput signalling analyses in the previous PNAS publication).

      We had made all raw data and guided analysis scripts for the above RPPA dataset publicly available, and the same is true for all original data as highlighted in the Materials & Methods section. Thus we strongly believe that readers have the opportunity to assess our work and reproduce our analyses/conclusions fully should they wish to do so.

      • Finally, we noted in the initial PNAS paper describing these models that we derived and worked with up to 10 independent homozygous PIK3CAH1047R clones, as well as with 3 and 4 independent heterozygous and wild-type clones, respectively. This exceeds the common use of 2 clones (if at all mentioned) in many similar studies in the stem cell literature (e.g. Ref. [31–34]). In our view, derivation of more than two independent clones is crucial for reproducibility in gene editing studies given substantial variability arising from genetic drift [35,36]. We have consistently shown the phenotypic robustness of our mutant clones across the two studies; note, for example, the low technical and biological variability in both heterozygous and homozygous mutants in the transcriptomic data in Fig. 1A. As noted in the manuscript, the high-depth RNAseq data analysis was performed in different clones and independently of the RNAseq reported in Ref. [5], yet yields highly similar results and confirms transcriptional rewiring of PIK3CAH1047R/H1047R iPSCs.*

      Throughout the text, the authors frequently reference their previous study in PNAS and often the lines of what is novel in this paper vs. reproduction of previous findings is blurred. The authors would benefit from reducing the frequency of referencing their previous study and focusing on emphasizing the novelty of the present findings.

      AUTHORS’ RESPONSE__:__ We have carefully reviewed all instances of citation of our previous study in the manuscript and have reduced their numbers to improve focus on the current findings as suggested. As noted above, however, the current study builds closely upon the findings of the previous work, and referring to these to put the current work in context is important. Indeed, this is reflected in some of the reviewers’ collective comments and questions which are answered by the prior study. We have carefully reviewed the places in which we have cited our previous study and note that except for 2 citations in the Introduction and 3 more in the Discussion, all remaining citations are in the context of linking new and old data, which we believe is important for clarity as suggested by the reviewers. However, if editors take a different view we can minimise this and reduce the number of citations.

      Without functional assays to complement and test their models, this manuscript is not a significant advance.

      AUTHORS’ RESPONSE__:__ While we take the Reviewer’s point that further studies could have strengthened robustness of the evidence supporting a mediating role of NODAL/TGFb signalling in PI3K-driven stemness, we think this assertion is far too sweeping, and neglects numerous facets of the study of use and interest to several fields (as agreed by the other reviewers). To recapitulate some key points of interest/use of this study:

      • Using a carefully derived PIK3CAH1047R iPSC model system and pharmacologically relevant doses of a recently approved PI3Ka-selective inhibitor, we demonstrate that the efficacy of the latter can depend on the strength of PI3K pathway activation and phenotype under investigation – despite expected downregulation of PI3K signalling by Alpelisib, the stemness phenotype is not reversed.
      • We link this to self-sustained TGFb signalling in cells with strong PI3K activation by homozygous PIK3CAH1047R The link between the two pathways and the underlying rewiring are likely to be relevant in other contexts, as observed recently in a breast epithelial model system [37]. Given similarity between human pluripotent stem cells and cancer cells, our findings are of wider relevance.
      • Aberrant PI3K activation has been associated with numerous pathologies, so it is important for the field to have well-characterised model systems with endogenous expression of one of the most common PIK3CA mutations. Our thorough characterisation of PIK3CAH1047R iPSCs validates one such model.
      • To our knowledge, this is the first study to provide a comprehensive and integrated characterisation of isoform-specific PI3K signalling and transcriptomic changes in human pluripotent stem cells. This is important because current knowledge of PI3K signalling in human PSCs is largely based on extrapolation of findings from mouse embryonic stem cells, with many previous studies relying on high concentrations of the non-specific pan-PI3K inhibitor LY294002 (the use of which has been discouraged by the PI3K signalling community [38]).

        I believe the narrative was written for pluripotent stem cell biologists but without robust functional and quantitative cell biological assays to test their models, I don't anticipate stem cell biologists will be very interested.

      AUTHORS’ RESPONSE__:__ The Reviewer is incorrect in his/her assertion about the target audience. PI3K signalling plays a key role in numerous disease and physiological processes as well as in development, and is of broad interest to cancer biologists, genetecists, rare disease biologists, biochemists, cell signallers, and endocrinologists among many others. Indeed we started with a primary focus on disease modelling (cancer, PROS) rather than stem cell biology, but because our findings are significant for the role of PI3K in stem cell biology as well as for these diseases, we aimed to make findings accessible across many of these readers. We refer the Reviewer to our previous response with regards to the significance of this work.

      **Minor Comments:**

      Consider adding gridlines to the MDS plots for clarity of read

      AUTHORS’ RESPONSE__:__ This is a matter of taste, and as we honestly can not see how it would enhance appreciation of the very clear clustering, we have decided to leave the plot in its current form.

      In Fig S2, some of the in-figure labelling is incorrect

      AUTHORS’ RESPONSE__:__ We thank the Reviewer for spotting this. We believe the labelling error to be corrected now and we have further tried to streamline the plot headings, but please do let us know if there is something else which we may have missed.

      In Fig S1C, the authors note poor correlation in the heterozygotes between this and a previous study. It would be helpful to qualify this discrepancy, as it is potentially concerning.

      AUTHORS’ RESPONSE__: The sensitivity to detect differential gene expression is high for large fold changes (as seen in PIK3CAH1047R/H1047R mutants) in transcriptomic studies, but declines rapidly for fold changes in expression lines 126-131: “The magnitudes of gene expression changes in PIK3CAH1047R/H1047R cells correlated strongly with our previous findings (Spearman’s rho = 0.74, p WT/H1047R iPSCs (Fig. S1C), as expected given the smaller number and lower magnitude of observed gene expression changes in heterozygous cells, and the lower depth of previous transcriptomic studies__.”*

      Line 208, the authors state that the small p-value for the homozygotes is suggestive of a dose-dependent effect. This is not the case; it simply suggests a greater probability of the effect being non-random.

      AUTHORS’ RESPONSE__:__ The Reviewer is formally correct, and we apologise for the imprecision of our language. Nevertheless biological effect size is pertinent to the p value determined, and so our statement, while requiring an inductive leap from the reader, is not wholly invalid. To tidy this up and improve precision we have reworded as follows:

      lines 215-217: “This is in keeping with the much lower effect size in heterozygous cells, and consistent with a critical role for the TGFbeta pathway in mediating the allele dose-dependent effect of PIK3CAH1047R in human iPSCs.”

      What does the height in Fig 4B correspond to? It would perhaps be of value to scale nodes based on the significance value.

      AUTHORS’ RESPONSE__:__ 4B illustrates hierarchical clustering of the module eigengenes - the height corresponds to similarity of gene expression. We clarify this in the revised manuscript.

      References

      1 Lee, K. L. et al. (2011) Graded Nodal/Activin signaling titrates conversion of quantitative phospho-Smad2 levels into qualitative embryonic stem cell fate decisions. PLoS Genet. 7.

      2 Hill, C. S. (2018) Spatial and temporal control of NODAL signaling. Curr. Opin. Cell Biol. 51, 50–57.

      3 Xu, R. H. et al. (2008) NANOG is a Direct Target of TGFβ/Activin-Mediated SMAD Signaling in Human ESCs. Cell Stem Cell 3, 196–206.

      4 Vallier, L. et al. (2009) Activin/Nodal signalling maintains pluripotency by controlling Nanog expression. Development 136, 1339–49.

      5 Madsen, R. R. et al. (2019) Oncogenic PIK3CA promotes cellular stemness in an allele dose-dependent manner. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 116, 8380–8389.

      6 Vasan, N. et al. (2019) Double PIK3CA mutations in cis increase oncogenicity and sensitivity to PI3Kα inhibitors. Science (80-. ). 366, 714–723.

      7 Saito, Y. et al. (2020) Landscape and function of multiple mutations within individual oncogenes. Nature 582, 95–99.

      8 Gorelick, A. N. et al. (2020) Phase and context shape the function of composite oncogenic mutations. Nature.

      9 Gillies, T. et al. (2020) Oncogenic mutant RAS signaling activity is rescaled by the ERK/MAPK pathway 1–19.

      10 Hafner, M. et al. (2016) Growth rate inhibition metrics correct for confounders in measuring sensitivity to cancer drugs. Nat. Methods 13, 521–527.

      11 André, F. et al. (2019) Alpelisib for PIK3CA-mutated, hormone receptor-positive advanced breast cancer. N. Engl. J. Med. 380, 1929–1940.

      12 Madsen, R. R. et al. (2020) Relationship between stemness and transcriptionally-inferred PI3K activity in human breast cancer. bioRxiv 2020.07.09.195974.

      13 Fritsch, C. et al. (2014) Characterization of the novel and specific PI3Ka inhibitor NVP-BYL719 and development of the patient stratification strategy for clinical trials. Mol. Cancer Ther. 13, 1117–1129.

      14 Furet, P. et al. (2013) Discovery of NVP-BYL719 a potent and selective phosphatidylinositol-3 kinase alpha inhibitor selected for clinical evaluation. Bioorganic Med. Chem. Lett.

      15 Yilmaz, A. et al. (2018) Defining essential genes for human pluripotent stem cells by CRISPR–Cas9 screening in haploid cells. Nat. Cell Biol. 20, 610–619.

      16 Sergushichev, A. A. (2016) An algorithm for fast preranked gene set enrichment analysis using cumulative statistic calculation. bioRxiv 060012.

      17 Liberzon, A. et al. (2015) The Molecular Signatures Database Hallmark Gene Set Collection. Cell Syst. 1, 417–425.

      18 Müller, F. J. et al. (2008) Regulatory networks define phenotypic classes of human stem cell lines. Nature 455, 401–405.

      19 James, D. et al. (2005) TGFbeta/activin/nodal signaling is necessary for the maintenance of pluripotency in human embryonic stem cells. Development 132, 1273–82.

      20 Vallier, L. et al. (2005) Activin/Nodal and FGF pathways cooperate to maintain pluripotency of human embryonic stem cells. J. Cell Sci. 118, 4495–4509.

      21 Chen, G. et al. (2011) Chemically defined conditions for human iPSC derivation and culture. Nat. Methods 8, 424–429.

      22 Adewumi, O. et al. (2007) Characterization of human embryonic stem cell lines by the International Stem Cell Initiative. Nat. Biotechnol. 25, 803–816.

      23 Tsankov, A. M. et al. (2015) A qPCR ScoreCard quantifies the differentiation potential of human pluripotent stem cells. Nat. Biotechnol. 33, 1–15.

      24 Smith, J. R. et al. (2008) Inhibition of Activin/Nodal signaling promotes specification of human embryonic stem cells into neuroectoderm. Dev. Biol. 313, 107–117.

      25 Vallier, L. et al. (2004) Nodal inhibits differentiation of human embryonic stem cells along the neuroectodermal default pathway. Dev. Biol. 275, 403–421.

      26 Sorre, B. et al. (2014) Encoding of temporal signals by the TGF-β Pathway and implications for embryonic patterning. Dev. Cell 30, 334–342.

      27 David, C. J. and Massagué, J. (2018) Contextual determinants of TGFβ action in development, immunity and cancer. Nat. Rev. Mol. Cell Biol. 19, 1–17.

      28 Alon, U. (2007) Network motifs: theory and experimental approaches. Nat. Rev. Genet. 8, 450–461.

      29 Sonnen, K. F. and Aulehla, A. (2014) Dynamic signal encoding-From cells to organisms. Semin. Cell Dev. Biol. 34, 91–98.

      30 Germain, P. L. and Testa, G. (2017) Taming Human Genetic Variability: Transcriptomic Meta-Analysis Guides the Experimental Design and Interpretation of iPSC-Based Disease Modeling. Stem Cell Reports 8, 1784–1796.

      31 Wang, L. et al. (2017) GCN5 Regulates FGF Signaling and Activates Selective MYC Target Genes during Early Embryoid Body Differentiation. Stem Cell Reports 10, 287–299.

      32 Zeng, H. et al. (2016) An Isogenic Human ESC Platform for Functional Evaluation of Genome-wide-Association-Study-Identified Diabetes Genes and Drug Discovery. Cell Stem Cell 0, 1660–1669.

      33 Ho, L. et al. (2015) ELABELA Is an Endogenous Growth Factor that Sustains hESC Self-Renewal via the PI3K/AKT Pathway. Cell Stem Cell 17, 435–447.

      34 Roudnicky, F. et al. (2019) Modeling the effects of severe metabolic disease by genome editing of HPSC-derived endothelial cells reveals an inflammatory phenotype. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 20, 1–10.

      35 Veres, A. et al. (2014) Low incidence of Off-target mutations in individual CRISPR-Cas9 and TALEN targeted human stem cell clones detected by whole-genome sequencing. Cell Stem Cell 15, 27–30.

      36 Ben-David, U. et al. (2018) Genetic and transcriptional evolution alters cancer cell line drug response. Nature 560, 325–330.

      37 Katsuno, Y. et al. (2019) Chronic TGF-b exposure drives stabilized EMT, tumor stemness, and cancer drug resistance with vulnerability to bitopic mTOR inhibition. Sci. Signal. 12, eaau8544.

      38 Manning, B. D. and Toker, A. (2017) AKT/PKB Signaling: Navigating the Network. Cell 169, 381–405.

    1. After I read this chapter, I think the author defined that language is used to express and impart meaning, and it is not pre-existing, even though language has existed for a long time. In fact, the meaning of language existence is given by human beings with various meanings, which makes the language have the necessary meaning. I think literature is not only about reading words, but also about what information and emotions the author want to convey to readers. Because just reading the text is rigid and without any emotion. In this chapter, as Culler mentions, that meaning should not be determined by words. As a conventional symbol, language has certain limitations on the author’s way of expression and the reader’s own understanding ability, so the thinking and imagination are limited by the rigidity of language. In my opinion, when reading literary works, the most important thing is how to analyze the author’s situation and views, because, without it, different meanings may be transmitted to readers and cause misunderstanding. For example, a good lecture in a university is more likely to elicit a positive reaction from the audience. However, it is normal to say that the audience may have different views on the same class because their understanding and opinions will be different. I think that just like commenting on a writer, readers have their own educational level, life experience, and values, which shape their unique personality, which makes a language have different meanings for them. For those of us who are a second language, reading some English books has different views because of the different educational concepts. Comparatively speaking, it is different from people who are English as a first language. Therefore, I think reading is like an unknown secret waiting to be explored and discovered. There will be resonance and opposition. We should not limit our thoughts and opinions so that whenever we read a book, we feel that the text is as fresh as ever. I think that’s the meaning of reading

      This is very interesting!

    1. One idea that strikes me as interesting about language and meaning is the idea of the language and though. As it is mentioned in chapter four, “the language we speak determines what we can think.” I agree with these phrases. First of all, there are many languages over the world, and every language has its structures, rules, vocabularies, etc. thus making us think differently. I think the structures and rules are key to determine the meaning of a text. Culler gives us an example when he says, “Speakers of English have ‘pets’ – a category to which nothing in French corresponds, though the French possess inordinate numbers of dogs and cats.” This is just one of the many examples we can have. For instance, if two people who speak different languages observe the same event, they may interpret it in different ways because of the differences in each language. I think that is very interesting for many reasons. It can make us aware of the diversity around us, but most importantly, I think it can help us to understand why others think differently from us and why we think the way we do. It is to say, why do we give things the meaning we do?

      Great observations

    1. Author Response

      We thank the Editor of eLife f or kindly considering our manuscript for publication and for soliciting three peer reviews. We note that the reviews were positive for the most part. We sincerely believe that the key criticisms arise regrettably from a seeming misunderstanding of the motivation and context of our work – one that we hoped was a candid presentation of available data for tarantulas and the methods used. We provide detailed responses to the reviewers’ concerns below. We further note that our manuscript has since been published with minimal changes (Foley et al. 2020 Proceedings of the Royal Society B 287: 20201688, doi:10.1098/rspb.2020.1688).

      Tarantulas belong to an enigmatic and charismatic group with a nearly cosmopolitan distribution and intriguingly show vivid coloration despite being mostly nocturnal/ crepuscular. Using a robust phylogeny based on a comprehensive transcriptomic dataset that includes nearly all theraphosid subfamilies (except Selenogyrinae), we performed both discrete and continuous ancestral state reconstructions of blue and green coloration in tarantulas using modern phylogenetic methods. Using phylogenetic correlation tests, we evaluated various possible functions for blue and green coloration, for instance aposematism and crypsis. Our results suggest green coloration is likely used in crypsis, while blue (and green) coloration show no correlation with urtication, stridulation or arboreality. Our findings also support a single ancestral origin of blue in tarantulas with losses being more frequent than gains, while green color has evolved multiple independent times but never lost. We comparatively assessed opsin expression from the transcriptomic data across tarantulas to understand the functional significance of blue and green coloration. Our opsin homolog network shows that tarantulas possess a rather diverse suite of regular arthropod opsins than previously appreciated.

      While color vision in (jumping) spiders is relatively well studied, to the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to comparatively consider the identity of opsin expression across tarantulas, and in relation to the evolution of coloration. Our study challenges current belief (e.g., Morehouse et al. 2017 doi: 10.1086/693977 and references therein; Hsiung et al. 2015 doi: 10.1126/sciadv.1500709) that tarantulas are incapable of perceiving colors, at least from a molecular perspective and suggests a role for sexual selection in their evolution. This also adds to the growing body of knowledge on the complexity of arthropod visual systems (e.g., see Futahashi et al. 2015 doi:10.1073/pnas.1424670112, Hill et al. 2002 doi:10.1126/science.1076196).

      In short, we believe our results are timely and pertinent broadly to sensory biologists, behavioural ecologists and evolutionary biologists as it is an exhortation for sorely needed behavioural and sensory experiments to understand proximate use of vivid coloration in this enigmatic group.

      Summary:

      This study offers some interesting data and ideas on colour evolution in tarantulas, building upon previous work on this topic. However, the reviewers judged that the insights are too taxon-specific and that several key conclusions are too speculative. There were also concerns about the methodology for trait scoring from photographs that the authors might consider going forward.

      Reviewer #1:

      This study investigates the evolution of blue and green setae colouration in tarantulas using phylogenetic analyses and trait values calculated from photographs. It argues that (i) green colouration has evolved in association with arboreality, and thus crypsis, and (ii) blue colouration is an ancestral trait lost and gained several times in tarantula evolution, possibly under sexual selection. It also uses transcriptome data to identify opsin homologs, as indirect evidence that tarantulas may have colour vision.

      Otherwise, a few comments:

      1) Given that data is limited for the family (only 25% of genera could be included in this study), it seemed a shame not to discuss further the variation in colour and habit within genera. Based on Figure 1 and supplementary tables, the majority of "blue" genera contain a mix of blue and not-blue (and not-photographed) species. Does this mean that blue has been lost many more times in recent evolutionary history? And how often are "losses" on your tree likely to be the result of insufficient sampling for the genus (i.e. you happen not to have sampled the blue species)?

      First, the taxa in our robust and well-resolved phylogeny are representative of the major lineages within Theraphosidae, i.e., we have sampled nearly all theraphosid subfamilies (except Selenogyrinae). Our ideal is also to work with a more complete genus-level molecular phylogeny and corresponding color dataset for theraphosidae. However, this group is generally not well represented in museum collections (let alone in digitized collections), while the pet trade is focussed on only a select number of taxa. While we appreciate the reviewer’s concern that adding more taxa and corresponding data could potentially change the results, we believe that with a strong backbone phylogeny recovering the major branches, the results should not change all that much (For instance, cf. Hackett et al. 2008 10.1126/science.1157704 vs. Prum et al. 2016 10.1038/nature19417, where the initial Hackett et al. backbone is robust to increased sampling). Although the way trait losses are concentrated towards the tip suggests that using a genus-level phylogeny would perhaps show a few more recent trait losses, but unlikely to contradict an ancient origin of blue coloration at the base of this group, especially given the way the outgroups are polarized (i.e., outgroups also exhibit blue).

      2) A key conclusion of the study is that sexual selection should not be discarded as a possible explanation for spider colour. However, there is very little detail given in the discussion to build this case. Do these spiders have mating displays that might plausibly include visual signals? How common are sexually-selected colours in spiders generally? Where on the body is the blue coloration (in cases where it is not whole body)? I also missed whether the images used are of males or females or both, or how many species show sexual dimorphism in colouration (mentioned briefly in the Discussion, but not summarised for species or genera).

      We agree with the reviewer that we should have provided more information regarding sexual dichromatism in tarantulas, and on the images we used in the study (whether male/female). However, the location of blue coloration varies wildly with species – some species have blue chelicerae, blue abdomens, or blue carapaces while others are entirely blue. We also know very little about mating (and selection, if any) strategies in tarantulas, let alone the sensory ecology of this group. However, there is intriguing anecdotal information from one species (Aphonopelma) that they can be active as early as 4pm (Shillington 2002 Canadian J. Zoology, 80: 251-259, doi: 10.1139/z01-227), while some species show an intensification of color upon maturation, often a hallmark of sexual selection. Indeed, we believe that our work will incite broad interest on these intriguing questions.

      3) A quick scroll through the amazing images on Rick West's site suggests that oranges and red/pinks are not rare in tarantulas. Perhaps the data is just not available, but it would be good to mention somewhere the rationale behind the blue/green focus, rather than examining all colours.

      We agree. However, in the present study, we focused on blue and green colors because the data is readily available and we wanted to build upon the previous work by Hsiung et al 2015. Given that violet/blue and likely also some green coloration are structural in origin (Saranathan et al. 2015 Nano Letters, doi: 10.1021/acs.nanolett.5b0020; Hsiung et al. 2015), these hues are unlikely to fade or vary between individuals unlike diet acquired pigmentary coloration. Hence, these colors perhaps better lend themselves to analyses using digital photographs.

      I suggest defining stridulating / urticating setae for non-specialist readers. I had to look these up to understand that they were involved in defence.

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion.

      I notice the Rick West website says species IDs should not be made from photos alone. Is there a risk of misidentification for any photos?

      We understand the reviewer’s concern. However, Rick West is an experienced arachnologist and quite knowledgeable in tarantula systematics and taxonomy (see https://www.tarantupedia.com/researchers/rick-c-west), which is why we endeavoured to use his website as extensively as possible without resorting to photos from hobbyists. We further validated the IDs with field guides, when in doubt.

      The Results section would benefit from some more clear statements of key results. For example, phrases like "AIC values to assess the relationships between greenness and arboreality are reported in Table 3" could be replaced instead with a summary statement indicating what this table shows.

      We agree and thank the reviewer for this suggestion.

      In the Figure 1 caption I think there is a typo: 'the proportions of species with images that possess blue colouration (grey = no available images)" but should this say "grey = not blue"?

      We apologize for the confusion. This is not a typo – this is in relation to Trichopelma, for which no images of described species were available, and so we cannot conclude that none of the taxa are blue/green.

      142 - the lengthy discussion here of whether there is one or more mechanisms by which blue is produced in tarantulas, and the detailed criticism of Hsuing SEMs, seems a bit out of place given that the current study does not investigate the proximate mechanism of blue colouration but merely its presence.

      We respectfully disagree. The core support for Hsiung et al.’s (2015) argument against sexual selection as a driver of color evolution in tarantulas comes from their structural diagnoses of the nanostructures responsible for the violet/blue structural coloration and their subsequent argument that a diversity of divergent nanostructures rather than convergence argues against sexual selection. While it is true that we did not investigate the proximate mechanism of blue coloration here, one of us (Saranathan et al. 2015) has already done so elsewhere. It appears that in insects and spiders, the bulk of the nanostructural diversity is across families and not within.

      Table S6 - It is not clear to me how the values for predicted N orthologs were calculated.

      This is mentioned in line 354 of our methods – “Per the ‘moderate’ criteria from the Alliance of Genome Resources (55), hits may be considered orthologous if three or more of the twelve tools in their suite converge upon that result”.

      The Table S7 caption states: "A * indicates currently undescribed species with blue or green colour that can be confidently attributed to corresponding genus. However, as the described species exhibit no blue or green colour, we conservatively scored these as 0." Is this a conservative approach though? If they have been confidently assigned to genus, I don't understand why they would not be included.

      This refers to the cases where a hitherto undescribed species possesses the blue or green color. However, even though the species has not formally been described, its placement in the genus is not in question. We have not included such undescribed species in our tabulated number of species per genus, as it is difficult to express any such undescribed species as a fraction of the total number of species in that genus.

      Reviewer #2:

      This paper presents a broad-ranging overview of tarantula visual pigments in relationship with the color of the spiders. The paper is interesting, well-written and presented, and will inspire further study into the visual and spectral characteristics of the genus.

      We thank the reviewer for her/his/their kind words.

      First a minor remark, Terakita and many others distinguish between opsin, being the protein part of the visual pigment molecule and intact light-sensing, so-called opsin-based pigment, often generalized as a rhodopsin. The statement of line 65, 'convert light photons to electrochemical signals through a signalling cascade' is according to that view strictly not correct. Furthermore, the presence of opsins in transcriptomes may be telling, but it is not at all sure that they are expressed in the eyes, if at all. As the authors well know, in many animal species some of the opsins are expressed elsewhere. It may be informative to mention that.

      We thank the reviewer for this clarification. As for the regions of opsin expression, we very much agree – were it not for constraints of sample availability, we would also have preferred to sequence only the eyes and brain of various tarantulas that were all exposed to similar lighting conditions. However, we encouragingly see that our “leg only” transcriptomes have far fewer (often no) opsins as compared to the whole-body data.

      The blueness or greenness feature prominently in the paper, but the criteria used for determining to which class a spider belongs are not at all sure. The Color Survey and Supplementary Table S2 refer to Birdspiders.com, but that requires a donation; not very welcoming. The other used sources are also not readily giving the insight or overview which material was sampled. I therefore think that the paper would considerably gain in palatability by adding a few exemplary photographs as well as measured spectra. Of course, I am inclined to trust the authors, but I would not immediately take color photographs from the web as the best material for assessing color data with 4-digit accuracy. Furthermore, the accessible photographs do not always show nice, uniform colors, so it might be sensible to mention which body part was used to score the animals. And finally, using CIE metric might infer to many readers that the spiders are presumably trichromatic, like us. Any further evidence?

      We refer to the detailed description of our method for scoring blue or green coloration in tarantulas (l. 277-303). Briefly, we calculated ΔE (CIE 1976) difference values using between the images of each taxa against a suitable reference (average of green leaves, or Haplopelma lividum, the bluest taxa in our survey based on the b value of its images). We use the ΔE Lab values to perform quantitative ancestral state reconstruction, while we use ΔE b (for blue) and ΔE a (for green) to discretize the data for understanding trait gains and losses.

      BirdSpiders.com only requires one to enter names of genera as search terms in order to see photos that we used. However, we agree could have provided some photos of exemplars. We do realise that using pictures is not ideal, as opposed to reflectance spectrophotometry (our ideal as well), which is why we limited ourselves to a single reputable source (BirdSpiders.com) for consistent images, whenever possible. However, acquiring sample material and reflectance of tarantulas is challenging. This group is generally not well represented in museum collections (let along in digitized collections), while the pet trade is focussed on only a select number of taxa and doing field work to collect specimens is fraught with moral and ethical issues (e.g., see https://www.nytimes.com/2019/04/01/science/poaching-wildlife-scientists.html). This study nevertheless represents a substantial improvement upon a recent high-profile work that used the OSX “color picker” function (Hsiung et al. 2015).

      Indeed, available evidence on tarantula vision (including our opsin sequences) suggests tarantulas are likely trichromats (Dahl and Granda 1989 J. Arachnol., Morehouse et al. 2017) similar to jumping spiders (e.g., Zurek et al. 2015, doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.03.033), so we consider CIE as an appropriate color space for a putative tristimulus system in tarantulas (see also our response to Reviewer 3). Again, this underscores the need for future studies on the sensory biology and psychophysics of this enigmatic group.

      Reviewer #3:

      This neat paper continues the story of structural colour evolution in a group that is rarely appreciated for their ornamentation. The study uses colour & ecological data to model their evolution in a comparative framework, and also synthesises transcriptomic data to estimate the presence and diversity of opsins in the group. The main findings are that the tarantulas are ancestrally 'blue' and that green colouration has arisen repeatedly and seems to follow transitions to arboreality, along with evidence of perhaps underappreciated opsin diversity in the group. It's well-written and engaging, and a useful addition to our understanding of this developing story. I just have a few concerns around methods and the interpretation of results, however, which I feel need some further consideration.

      We thank the reviewer for his/her/their kind words.

      As the authors discuss in detail, this work in many ways parallels that of Hsiung et al. (2015). The two studies seem to agree in the broad-brush conclusions, which is interesting (and promising, for our understanding of the question), though their results conflict in significant ways too. Differences in methodology are an obvious cause, and they are particularly important in studies such as this in which the starting conditions (e.g. the assumed phylogeny or decisions around mapping of traits) so significantly shape outcomes. The current study uses a more recent and robust phylogeny, which is great, and the authors also emphasise their use of quantitative methods to assign colour traits (blue/green), unlike Hsiung et al.

      We thank the reviewer for his/her/their appreciation.

      1) This latter point is my main area of methodological concern, and I am not currently convinced that it is as useful or objective as is suggested. One issue is that the photographs are unstandardised in several dimensions, which will render the extracted values quite unreliable. I know the authors have considered this (as discussed in their supplement), but ultimately I don't believe you can reliably compare colour estimates from such diverse sources. Issues include non-standardised lighting conditions, alternate white-balancing algorithms, artefacts introduced through image compression, differences in the spectral sensitivities of camera models, no compensation for non-linear scaling of sensor outputs (which would again differ with camera models and even lenses), and so on (the works of Martin Stevens, Jolyon Troscianko, Jair Garcia, Adrian Dyer offer good discussion of these and related challenges). Some effort is made to minimise adverse effects, such as excluding the L dimension when calculating some colour distances, but even then the consequences are overstated since the outputs of camera sensors scale non-linearly with intensity, and so non-standardised lighting will still affect chromatic channels (a & b values). So with these factors at play, it becomes very difficult to know whether identified colour differences are a consequence of genuine differences in colouration, or simply differences in white balancing or some other feature of the photographs themselves.

      We thank the reviewer for his/her/their carefully considered thoughts and for drawing our attention to the work of Martin Stevens, Jolyon Troscianko, Jair Garcia, and Adrian Dyer in this regard (e.g. Stevens et al. 2007 Biol. J. Linn. Soc. Lond., doi: 10.1111/j.1095-8312.2007.00725.x). These are fair points raised by the reviewer. We are indeed aware that there are clear drawbacks in working solely with photographs from online sources as opposed to optical reflectance data (our ideal), but we are sure that the reviewer appreciates how challenging it is to source specimens of tarantulas. It is for this reason that we restricted ourselves to photographs from mostly only 1 reputable source (BirdSpiders.com). Furthermore, this is why we chose a perceptual model that permits device independent color representation, one that lets us separate chromatic variables from brightness, keeping in mind the underlying assumptions. However, some recent research suggests that CIELab space can perform reasonably well as compared to the latest algorithms for illuminant-invariant color spaces (Chong et al. 2008 ACM Transactions on Graphics, doi: 10.1145/1360612.1360660). Please also see our response below (to point #2) and also to Reviewer #2 above.

      Given the dearth of tarantula specimens and in the absence of spectrometry, future work will have to try and acquire uncompressed original images (with EXIF data) and could perform image processing such as homomorphic filtering and adaptive histogram equalization (Pizer et al. 1987 Computer Vision, Graphics, and Image Processing; Gonzalez and Woods 2018 Digital Image Processing, Pearson) in order to further mitigate artefacts such as those arising from differences in illumination, especially if using images from a diversity of sources.

      2) The justification for some related decisions are also unclear to me. The CIE-76 colour distance is used, and is described as 'conservative'. But it is not so much conservative as it is an inaccurate model of human colour sensation. It fails to account for perceptual non-uniformity and actually overestimates colour differences between highly chromatic colours (like saturated blues). The authors note they preferred this to CIE-2000, which is a much better measure in terms of accuracy, because the latter was too permissive (line 300). I understand the problem, and appreciate their honesty, but this decision seems very arbitrary. If the goal is to quantitatively estimate colour differences according to human viewers, then the metric which best estimates our perceptual abilities would strike me as most appropriate. Also, the fact that all species would be classified as 'blue' using the CIE-2000, when some of them are obviously not blue by simply looking at them, is consistent with the kinds of image-processing issues noted above. I only focus on this general point because it is offered as a key advance on previous work (L 40-41), but I don't think that is clearly the case (though I agree that the scoring methods of Hsiung et al. are quite vague). I'm generally in favour of this sort of quantitative approach, but here I wonder if it wouldn't be simpler and more defensible to just ask some humans to classify images of spiders as either 'blue' or 'green', since that seems to be the end-goal anyway.

      We agree that CIE 1976 is an inaccurate model of “human color sensation,” but at the same time the degree of their applicability or lack thereof to non-human tristimulus visual systems is not clear. In any case, the digital photographs do not preserve UV information anyway. We hasten to add CIE 1976 is still widely used in color science and engineering research for its simplicity and perceptual uniformity, as a simple Google Scholar search would attest. We believe that the reviewer is perhaps mistaken as to our motivation for choosing the CIE 1976 and the exact nature of the shortcomings of the CIE 1976 model, which it turns out to be an unintended advantage. Our goal was not, as the reviewer suggests, to just “quantitatively estimate color differences according to human viewers,” but to do so in a device independent fashion given the constraints of working with already available digital images, and for a putative trichromat visual system. Given there are technically no limits for a and b values in the CIE 76 space, color patches with high values of chroma are computed to have too strong a difference than in actual fact (Hill et al. 1997 ACM Transactions on Graphics, 16, 109-154). This is precisely the kind of situation that we do not face here, as we are essentially comparing shades of blue rather than for instance, chromatic contrasts between saturated blue vs. green or blue vs. red. Moreover, we only use the rectilinear rather than the polar coordinate representation of the colors (in other words, we do not compute the psychometric correlates, chroma Cab, or the hue angle hab). Contrary to the reviewer’s assertion that the CIE 1976 “overestimates color differences between highly chromatic colors (like saturated blues),” a quick perusal of Table S3 affirms that a comparison of highly saturated blues such as between our “standard” H. lividum and Poecilotheria metallica reveals they are quite close in terms of chromatic contrasts (i.e., small E values). Moreover, CIE 1994 and subsequent revisions rely on a von Kries-type transformation to account for non-uniformity of the perceptual space, but as the reviewer is well aware, without an accurate idea of the illumination conditions, use of CIE 2000 is not justified.

      Lastly, we are sure the reviewer appreciates that asking humans to manually score the colors of images (e.g. Hsiung et al. 2015) is neither reproducible nor enables quantitative analyses of trait evolution.

      3) L26-27, 53-56, 171-176: This is a more minor point than the above, but some of the discussion and logic around hypothesised functions could be elaborated upon, given it's presented as a motivating aim of the text (52-56). The challenge with a group like this, as the authors clearly know, is that essentially none of the ecological and behavioural work necessary to identify function(s) hasn't been done yet, so there are serious limitations on what might be inferred from purely comparative analyses at this stage. The (very interesting!) link between green colouration and arboreality is hypothesised and interpreted as evidence for crypsis, for example, but the link is not so straightforward. Light in a dense forest understory is quite often greenish (e.g. see Endler's work on terrestrial light environments) including at night which, when striking a specular, structurally-coloured green could make for a highly conspicuous colour pattern - especially achromatically (which is what nocturnal visual predators would often be relying on). This is particularly true if the substrate is brown rotten leaves or dirt, in which case they could shine like a beacon. Conversely, if the blue is sufficiently saturated and spectrally offset from the substrate it could be quite achromatically cryptic at dusk or night. To really answer these questions demands information on the viewers, viewing conditions, visual environment etc. The point being that it is a bit too simplistic to observe that, to a human, spiders are green and leaves on the forest floor may be green, and so suggest crypsis as the likely function (abstract L 22-23). So inferences around visual function(s) could either be toned down in places given the evidence at hand or shored up with further detail (though I'm not sure how much is available).

      We agree. Indeed, we are limited by the absence of rigorous behavioural studies. With this in mind, we have already made every effort to tone down and emphasize that our results might point towards a given function, but we do not claim it outright. It is our fervent hope that these findings will form the basis for future behavioural studies by giving researchers a starting point to test their hypotheses.

      We would like to point out that the association we uncovered is actually between arboreal taxa and the presence of green coloration and not as the reviewer says “spiders are green and leaves on forest floor may be green.” These taxa live in natural crevices on trees, shrubs and essentially spend their lives arboreally. Also, green coloration in tarantulas need not be structural in origin (see e.g., Saranathan et al. 2015) and this is why to test for crypsis against foliage, we used (pigmentary) leaves as the representative model for comparison to tarantula green colors. Although, certain lycaenid butterflies (Saranathan et al. 2010 10.1073/pnas.0909616107; Michielsen et al. 2010 10.1098/rsif.2009.0352), for instance, use structural coloration to better aid in crypsis against foliage.

      Minor comments:

      • I'm not familiar enough with with methods for creating homolog networks to comment in detail, but the use of BLASTing existing opsin sequences against transcriptomes seems straightforward enough. As do the methods for phylogenetic reconstruction.

      We agree this is straightforward.

      • L48: What constitutes a 'representative' species? And how reasonable is it to assign a value for such a labile trait to an entire genus? I understand we can only do our best of course and simplifications need to be made, but I can imagine many cases among insects (e.g. among butterflies and flies) where genus-level assignments would be meaningless due to the immense diversity of structural colouration among species (including in terms of simple presence/absence).

      Please see our response to Reviewer 2 above.

      • Line 168: Wouldn't this speak against a sexual function? Only in a tentative way of course, but the presence of conspicuous structural colouration in juveniles, which is absent in adults, would suggest a non-sexual origin to me.

      The reviewer’s inference is incorrect. We do not suggest that blue coloration is present in juveniles but absent in adults, but only that such conspicuous colors already appear in the penultimate moult right before the male creates a sperm web and is ready for mating.

    2. Reviewer #3:

      This neat paper continues the story of structural colour evolution in a group that is rarely appreciated for their ornamentation. The study uses colour & ecological data to model their evolution in a comparative framework, and also synthesises transcriptomic data to estimate the presence and diversity of opsins in the group. The main findings are that the tarantulas are ancestrally 'blue' and that green colouration has arisen repeatedly and seems to follow transitions to arboreality, along with evidence of perhaps underappreciated opsin diversity in the group. It's well-written and engaging, and a useful addition to our understanding of this developing story. I just have a few concerns around methods and the interpretation of results, however, which I feel need some further consideration.

      As the authors discuss in detail, this work in many ways parallels that of Hsiung et al. (2015). The two studies seem to agree in the broad-brush conclusions, which is interesting (and promising, for our understanding of the question), though their results conflict in significant ways too. Differences in methodology are an obvious cause, and they are particularly important in studies such as this in which the starting conditions (e.g. the assumed phylogeny or decisions around mapping of traits) so significantly shape outcomes. The current study uses a more recent and robust phylogeny, which is great, and the authors also emphasise their use of quantitative methods to assign colour traits (blue/green), unlike Hsiung et al.

      1) This latter point is my main area of methodological concern, and I am not currently convinced that it is as useful or objective as is suggested. One issue is that the photographs are unstandardised in several dimensions, which will render the extracted values quite unreliable. I know the authors have considered this (as discussed in their supplement), but ultimately I don't believe you can reliably compare colour estimates from such diverse sources. Issues include non-standardised lighting conditions, alternate white-balancing algorithms, artefacts introduced through image compression, differences in the spectral sensitivities of camera models, no compensation for non-linear scaling of sensor outputs (which would again differ with camera models and even lenses), and so on (the works of Martin Stevens, Jolyon Troscianko, Jair Garcia, Adrian Dyer offer good discussion of these and related challenges). Some effort is made to minimise adverse effects, such as excluding the L dimension when calculating some colour distances, but even then the consequences are overstated since the outputs of camera sensors scale non-linearly with intensity, and so non-standardised lighting will still affect chromatic channels (a & b values). So with these factors at play, it becomes very difficult to know whether identified colour differences are a consequence of genuine differences in colouration, or simply differences in white balancing or some other feature of the photographs themselves.

      2) The justification for some related decisions are also unclear to me. The CIE-76 colour distance is used, and is described as 'conservative'. But it is not so much conservative as it is an inaccurate model of human colour sensation. It fails to account for perceptual non-uniformity and actually overestimates colour differences between highly chromatic colours (like saturated blues). The authors note they preferred this to CIE-2000, which is a much better measure in terms of accuracy, because the latter was too permissive (line 300). I understand the problem, and appreciate their honesty, but this decision seems very arbitrary. If the goal is to quantitatively estimate colour differences according to human viewers, then the metric which best estimates our perceptual abilities would strike me as most appropriate. Also, the fact that all species would be classified as 'blue' using the CIE-2000, when some of them are obviously not blue by simply looking at them, is consistent with the kinds of image-processing issues noted above. I only focus on this general point because it is offered as a key advance on previous work (L 40-41), but I don't think that is clearly the case (though I agree that the scoring methods of Hsiung et al. are quite vague). I'm generally in favour of this sort of quantitative approach, but here I wonder if it wouldn't be simpler and more defensible to just ask some humans to classify images of spiders as either 'blue' or 'green', since that seems to be the end-goal anyway.

      3) L26-27, 53-56, 171-176: This is a more minor point than the above, but some of the discussion and logic around hypothesised functions could be elaborated upon, given it's presented as a motivating aim of the text (52-56). The challenge with a group like this, as the authors clearly know, is that essentially none of the ecological and behavioural work necessary to identify function(s) hasn't been done yet, so there are serious limitations on what might be inferred from purely comparative analyses at this stage. The (very interesting!) link between green colouration and arboreality is hypothesised and interpreted as evidence for crypsis, for example, but the link is not so straightforward. Light in a dense forest understory is quite often greenish (e.g. see Endler's work on terrestrial light environments) including at night which, when striking a specular, structurally-coloured green could make for a highly conspicuous colour pattern - especially achromatically (which is what nocturnal visual predators would often be relying on). This is particularly true if the substrate is brown rotten leaves or dirt, in which case they could shine like a beacon. Conversely, if the blue is sufficiently saturated and spectrally offset from the substrate it could be quite achromatically cryptic at dusk or night. To really answer these questions demands information on the viewers, viewing conditions, visual environment etc. The point being that it is a bit too simplistic to observe that, to a human, spiders are green and leaves on the forest floor may be green, and so suggest crypsis as the likely function (abstract L 22-23). So inferences around visual function(s) could either be toned down in places given the evidence at hand or shored up with further detail (though I'm not sure how much is available).

      Minor comments:

      -I'm not familiar enough with with methods for creating homolog networks to comment in detail, but the use of BLASTing existing opsin sequences against transcriptomes seems straightforward enough. As do the methods for phylogenetic reconstruction.

      -L48: What constitutes a 'representative' species? And how reasonable is it to assign a value for such a labile trait to an entire genus? I understand we can only do our best of course and simplifications need to be made, but I can imagine many cases among insects (e.g. among butterflies and flies) where genus-level assignments would be meaningless due to the immense diversity of structural colouration among species (including in terms of simple presence/absence).

      -Line 168: Wouldn't this speak against a sexual function? Only in a tentative way of course, but the presence of conspicuous structural colouration in juveniles, which is absent in adults, would suggest a non-sexual origin to me.

    3. Reviewer #1:

      This study investigates the evolution of blue and green setae colouration in tarantulas using phylogenetic analyses and trait values calculated from photographs. It argues that (i) green colouration has evolved in association with arboreality, and thus crypsis, and (ii) blue colouration is an ancestral trait lost and gained several times in tarantula evolution, possibly under sexual selection. It also uses transcriptome data to identify opsin homologs, as indirect evidence that tarantulas may have colour vision.

      Otherwise, a few comments:

      1) Given that data is limited for the family (only 25% of genera could be included in this study), it seemed a shame not to discuss further the variation in colour and habit within genera. Based on Figure 1 and supplementary tables, the majority of "blue" genera contain a mix of blue and not-blue (and not-photographed) species. Does this mean that blue has been lost many more times in recent evolutionary history? And how often are "losses" on your tree likely to be the result of insufficient sampling for the genus (i.e. you happen not to have sampled the blue species)?

      2) A key conclusion of the study is that sexual selection should not be discarded as a possible explanation for spider colour. However, there is very little detail given in the discussion to build this case. Do these spiders have mating displays that might plausibly include visual signals? How common are sexually-selected colours in spiders generally? Where on the body is the blue coloration (in cases where it is not whole body)? I also missed whether the images used are of males or females or both, or how many species show sexual dimorphism in colouration (mentioned briefly in the Discussion, but not summarised for species or genera).

      3) A quick scroll through the amazing images on Rick West's site suggests that oranges and red/pinks are not rare in tarantulas. Perhaps the data is just not available, but it would be good to mention somewhere the rationale behind the blue/green focus, rather than examining all colours.

      Minor comments:

      I suggest defining stridulating / urticating setae for non-specialist readers. I had to look these up to understand that they were involved in defence.

      I notice the Rick West website says species IDs should not be made from photos alone. Is there a risk of misidentification for any photos?

      The Results section would benefit from some more clear statements of key results. For example, phrases like "AIC values to assess the relationships between greenness and arboreality are reported in Table 3" could be replaced instead with a summary statement indicating what this table shows.

      In the Figure 1 caption I think there is a typo: 'the proportions of species with images that possess blue colouration (grey = no available images)" but should this say "grey = not blue"?

      142 - the lengthy discussion here of whether there is one or more mechanisms by which blue is produced in tarantulas, and the detailed criticism of Hsuing SEMs, seems a bit out of place given that the current study does not investigate the proximate mechanism of blue colouration but merely its presence.

      The Table S7 caption states: "A * indicates currently undescribed species with blue or green colour that can be confidently attributed to corresponding genus. However, as the described species exhibit no blue or green colour, we conservatively scored these as 0." Is this a conservative approach though? If they have been confidently assigned to genus, I don't understand why they would not be included.

      Table S6 - It is not clear to me how the values for predicted N orthologs were calculated.

    1. Your wrongs do set a scandal on my sex:We cannot fight for love, as men may do;We should be wood and were not made to woo.

      An example of a divide between the social standards for men and women. Helena talks about how women cannot "fight for love" which illustrates them not being able to peruse the ones they truly love, but I also think this comparison works to demonstrate the quite literal fight for "love" that Theseus peruses through taking Hippolyta in the midst of a battle. This quote describes a passive love that women are supposed to partake in, just allowing things to happen, while men are the ones who chose who and how they love. The words "scandal on my sex" demonstrates how not abiding by these terms is in no way acceptable and is a social disgrace.

    2. Away, you Ethiope!

      Lysander's referral to Hermia as an Ethiope is meant to be an insult regarding her darker complexion. We have talked about this sort of thing a lot, but again, we see how darker complexions were seen as negative qualities and could be used as an insult. Like in Othello, we see how these racial systems were already in place where white people thought less of black people. However, I think it may be of importance to note that Lysander, before the magic juice, was in love with Hermia despite this "negative" quality. I do not think this was intended, but the fact that Lysander only says this when he is in this trance makes me read it as a comment about how racial insults/hate/discrimination is the not the result of logical or empathetic thinking, but of animalistic, basic, and disgust driven thought processes. Again though, this is modern view of what I want this play to be saying, I do not think given the time period Shakespeare was arguing that racial biases are fickle and driven only by hate/ignorance.

    1. 5The Internet presents risks as well as opportunities. While many aspects of civic and political life increasingly occur online, these changes create risks and challenges as well as opportunities. For example, it is often difficult to judge the quality of information found online;individuals may choose primarily to read viewpoints and engage with those whose views align with their own; the distribution of media access and participatory habits may exacerbate inequalities in civic and political voice; and online communities may, at times, be characterized by a disturbing lack of civility. Thus, in addition to recognizing the many ways civic and political life rely on digital media, it is important for civic educators to think about risks and challenges as they consider ways to engage with the digital dimensions of civic education.EDUCATORS CAN TAKE ADVANTAGE OF DIGITAL MEDIA TO FOSTER DESIRED FORMS OF CIVIC AND POLITICAL ENGAGEMENT AND DEVELOPMENT While youth participation with many different dimensions of civic and political life IS low, youth engagement with digital media is high. Ninety-five percent of teenagers aged 14!17 and 93 percent of young adults aged 18!29 use the Internetand almost a quarter of the Smartphone market is held by teenagers and young adults, aged 13!24.10Civic educators can foster youth civic and political engagement bymeeting youth where they are: in online and digital spaces.

      The best social studies teacher I had was as good as he was, not just because he showed a lot of devotion and energy in his teaching, but also because he understood and used the tools that we used every day

    1. Author Response

      Reviewer #1:

      Köster and colleagues present a brief report in which they study in 9 month-old babies the electrophysiological responses to expected and unexpected events. The major finding is that in addition to a known ERP response, an NC present between 400-600 ms, they observe a differential effect in theta oscillations. The latter is a novel result and it is linked to the known properties of theta oscillations in learning. This is a nice study, with novel results and well presented. My major reservation however concerns the push the authors make for the novelty of the results and their interpretation as reflecting brain dynamics and rhythms. The reason for that is, that any ERP, passed through the lens of a wavelet/FFT etc, will yield a response at a particular frequency. This is especially the case for families of ERP responses related to unexpected event e.g., MMR, and NC, etc. For which there is plenty of literature linking them to responses to surprising event, and in particular in babies; and which given their timing will be reflected in delta/theta oscillations. The reason why I am pressing on this issue, is because there is an old, but still ongoing debate attempting to dissociate intrinsic brain dynamics from simple event related responses. This is by no means trivial and I certainly do not expect the authors to resolve it, yet I would expect the authors to be careful in their interpretation, to warn the reader that the result could just reflect the known ERP, to avoid introducing confusion in the field.

      We would like to thank the author for highlighting the novelty of the results. Critically, there is one fundamental difference in investigating the ERP response and the trial-wise oscillatory power, which we have done in the present analysis: when looking at the evoked oscillatory response (i.e., the TF characteristics of the ERP), the signal is averaged over trials first and then subjected to a wavelet transform. However, when looking at the ongoing (or total) oscillatory response, the wavelet transform is applied at the level of the single trial, before the TF response of the single trials is averaged across the trials of one condition trials (for a classical illustration, see Tallon-Baudry & Bertrand, 1999; TICS, Box 2). We have now made this distinction more salient throughout the manuscript.

      In the present study, the results did not suggest a relation between the ERP and the ongoing theta activity, because the topography, temporal evolution, and polarity of the ERP and the theta response were very dissimilar: Looking at Figure 2 (A and B) and Figure 3 (B and C), the Nc peaks at central electrodes, but the theta response is more distributed, and the expected versus unexpected difference was specific for the .4 to .6 s time window, but the theta difference lasted the whole trial. Furthermore, the NC was higher for expected versus unexpected, which should (due to the low frequency) rather lead to a higher theta power for unexpected, in contrast to expected events for the time frequency analysis for the Nc. To verify this intuition, we now ran a wavelet analysis on the evoked response (i.e., the ERP) and, for a direct comparison, also plotted the ongoing oscillatory response for the central electrodes (see Additional Figure 1). These additional analyses nicely illustrate that the trial-wise theta response provides a fundamentally different approach to analyze oscillatory brain dynamics.

      Because this is likely of interest to many readers, we also report the results of the wavelet analysis of the ERP versus the analysis of the ongoing theta activity at central electrodes and the corresponding statistics in the result section, and have also included the Additional Figure in the supplementary materials, as Figure S2.

      *Additional Figure 1. Comparison of the topography and time course for the 4 – 5 Hz activity for the evoked (A, B) and the ongoing (C, D) oscillatory response at central electrodes (400 – 600 ms; Cz, C3, C4; baseline: -100 – 0 ms). (A) Topography for the difference between unexpected and expected events in the evoked oscillatory response. (B) The corresponding time course at central electrodes, which did not reveal a significant difference between 400 – 600 ms, t(35) = 1.57, p = .126. (C) Topography for the same contrast in the ongoing oscillatory response and (D) the corresponding time course at central electrodes, which did likewise not reveal a significant difference between 400 – 600 ms, t(35) = -1.26, p = .218. The condition effects (unexpected - expected) were not correlated between the evoked and the ongoing response, r = .23, p = .169.*

      A second aspect that I would like the authors to comment on is the power of the experimental design to measure surprise. From the methods, I gathered that the same stimulus materials and with the same frequency were presented as expected and unexpected endings. If that is the case, what is the measure of surprise? For once the same materials are shown causing habituation and reducing novelty and second the experiment introduces a long-term expectation of a 50:50 proportion of expected/unexpected events. I might be missing something here, which is likely as the methods are quite sparse in the description of what was actually done.

      We have used 4 different stimuli types (variants) in each of the 4 different domains, with either an expected or unexpected outcome. This resulted in 32 distinct stimulus sequences, which we presented twice, resulting in (up to) 64 trials. We have now described this approach and design in more detail and have also included all stimuli as supplementary material (Figure S1). In particular, we have used multiple types in each domain to reduce potential habituation or expectation effects. Still, we agree that one difficulty may be that, over time, infants got used to the fact that expected and unexpected outcomes were to be similarly “expected” (i.e., 50:50). However, if this was the case it would have resulted in a reduction (or disappearance) of the condition effect, and would thus also reduce the condition difference that we found, rather than providing an alternative explanation. We now included this consideration in the method section (p. 7).

      Two more comments concerning the analysis choices:

      1) The statistics for the ERP and the TF could be reported using a cluster size correction. These are well established statistical methods in the field which would enable to identify the time window/topography that maximally distinguished between the expected and the unexpected condition both for ERP and TF. Along the same lines, the authors could report the spatial correlation of the ERP/TF effects.

      For the ERP analysis we used the standard electrodes typically analyzed for the Nc in order to replicate effects found in former research (Langeloh et al., 2020; see also, Kayhan et al., 2019; Reynolds and Richards, 2005; Webb et al., 2005). For the TF analyses we used the most conservative criterion, namely all scalp recorded electrodes and the whole time window from 0 to 2000 ms, such that we did not make any choice regarding time window or the electrodes (i.e., which could be corrected for against other choices). We have now made those choices clearer in the method section, and why we think that, under these condition a multiple comparison correction is not needed/applicable (p. 10). Regarding the spatial correlation of the ERP and TF effects, we explained in response to the first comment the very different nature of the TF decomposition of the ERP and ongoing oscillatory activity and also that these were found to be interdependent (i.e., uncorrelated). We hope that with the additional analysis included in response to this comment that this difference is much clearer now.

      2) While I can see the reason why the authors chose to keep the baseline the same between the ERP and the TF analysis, for time frequency analysis it would be advisable to use a baseline amounting to a comparable time to the frequency of interest; and to use a period that does not encroach in the period of interest i.e., with a wavelet = 7 and a baseline -100:0 the authors are well into the period of interested.

      The difficulty in choosing the baseline in the present study was two-fold. First, we were interested in the ERP and the change in neural oscillations upon the onset of an outcome picture within a continuous presentation of pictures, forming a sequence. Second, we wanted to use a similar baseline for both analyses, to make them comparable. Because the second picture (the picture before the outcome picture) also elicited both an ERP and an oscillatory response at ~ 4 Hz (see Additional Figure 2), we choose a baseline just before the onset of the outcome stimulus, from -100 to 0 ms. Also we agree that the possibility to take a longer and earlier baseline, in particular for the TF results would have been favorable, but still consider that the -100 to 0 ms is still the best choice for the present analysis. Notably, because we found an increase in theta oscillations and the critical difference relies on a higher theta rhythm in one compared to the other condition, the effects of the increase in theta, if they effected the baseline, this effect would counteract rather than increase the current effect. We now explain this choice in more detail (p.10).

      *Additional Figure 1. Display of the grand mean signals prior to the -100 to 0 baseline and outcome stimulus. (A) The time-frequency response across all scalp-recorded electrodes, as well as (B) the ERP at the central electrodes (Cz, C3, C4) across both conditions show a similar response to the 2. picture like the outcome picture. Thus a baseline just prior to the stimulus of interest was chosen, consistent for both analyses.*

      Reviewer #2:

      The manuscript reports increases in theta power and lower NC amplitude in response to unexpected (vs. expected) events in 9-month-olds. The authors state that the observed increase in theta power is significant because it is in line with an existing theory that the theta rhythm is involved in learning in mammals. The topic is timely, the results are novel, the sample size is solid, the methods are sound as far as I can tell, and the use of event types spanning multiple domains (e.g. action, number, solidity) is a strength. The manuscript is short, well-written, and easy to follow.

      1) The current version of the manuscript states that the reported findings demonstrate that the theta rhythm is involved in processing of prediction error and supports the processing of unexpected events in 9-month-old infants. However, what is strictly shown is that watching at least some types of unexpected events enhance theta rhythm in 9-month-old infants, i.e. an increase in the theta rhythm is associated with processing unexpected events in infants, which suggests that an increase in the theta rhythm is a possible neural correlate of prediction error in this age range. While the present novel findings are certainly suggestive, more data and/or analyses would be needed to corroborate/confirm the role of the observed infant theta rhythm in processing prediction error, or document whether and how this increase in the theta rhythm supports the processing of unexpected events in infants. (As an example, since eye-tracking data were collected, are trial-by-trial variations in theta power increases to unexpected outcomes related to how long individual infants looked to the unexpected outcome pictures?) If it is not possible to further confirm/corroborate the role of the theta rhythm with this dataset, then the discussion, abstract, and title should be revised to more closely reflect what the current data shows (as the wording of the conclusion currently does), and clarify how future research may test the hypothesis that the infant theta rhythm directly supports the processing of prediction error in response to unexpected events.

      We would like to thank the reviewer for acknowledging the merit of the present research.

      On the one hand, we have revised our manuscript and are now somewhat more careful with our conclusion, in particular with regard to the refinement of basic expectations. On the other hand, we consider the concept of “violation to expectation” (VOE), which is one of the most widely used concepts in infancy research, very closely linked to the concept of a prediction error processing, namely a predictive model is violated. In particular, we have made this conceptual link in a recent theoretical paper (Köster et al., 2020), and based on former theoretical considerations about the link between these two concepts (e.g., see Schubotz 2015; Prediction and Expectation). In particular, in the present study we used a set of four different domains of violation of expectation paradigms, which are among the best established domains of infants core knowledge (e.g., action, solidity, cohesion, number; cf. Spelke & Kinzler, 2007). It was our specific goal not to replicate, for another time, that infants possess expectations (i.e., make predictions) in these domains, but to “flip the coin around” and investigate infants’ prediction error more generally, independent of the specific domain. We have now made the conceptual link between VOE and prediction error processing more explicit in the introduction of the manuscript and also emphasize that we choose a variety of domains to obtain a more general neural marker for infant processing of prediction errors.

      Having said this, indeed, we planned to assess and compare both infants gaze behavior and EEG response. Unfortunately, this was not very successful and the concurrent recording only worked for a limited number of infants and trials. This led us to the decision to make the eye-tracking study a companion study and to collect more eye-tracking data in an independent sample of infants after the EEG assessment was completed, such that a match between the two measures was not feasible. We now make this choice more explicit in the method section (p. 7). In addition, contrary to our basic assumption we did not find an effect in the looking time measure. Namely, there was no difference between expected and unexpected outcomes. We assume that this is due to the specificities of the current design that was rather optimized for EEG assessments: We used a high number of repetitions (64), with highly variable domains (4), and restricted the time window for potential looking time effects to 5 seconds, which is highly uncommon in the field and therefore not directly comparable with former studies.

      Finally, besides the ample evidence from former studies using VOE paradigms, if it were not the unexpected vs. expected (i.e., unpredicted vs. predicted) condition contrast which explains the differences we found in the ERP and the theta response, there would need to be an alternative explanation for the differential responses in the EEG, which produce the hypothesized effects. (Please also note that there are many studies relying their VOE assumption on ERPs alone, here we have two independent measures suggesting that infants discriminated between those conditions.)

      2) The current version of the manuscript states "The ERP effect was somewhat consistent across conditions, but the effect was mainly driven by the differences between expected and unexpected events in the action and the number domain (Figure S1). The results were more consistent across domains for the condition difference in the 4 - 5 Hz activity, with a peak in the unexpected-expected difference falling in the 4 - 5 Hz range across all electrodes (Figure S2)". However, the similarity/dissimilarity of NC and theta activity responses across domains was not quantified or tested. Looking at Figures S1 and S2, it is not that obvious to me that theta responses were more consistent across domains than NC responses. I understand that there were too few trials to formally test for any effect of domain (action, number, solidity, cohesion) on NC and theta responses, either alone or in interaction with outcome (expected, unexpected). It may still be possible to test for correlations of the topography and time-course of the individual average unexpected-expected difference in NC and theta responses across domains at the group level, or to test for an effect of outcome (expected, unexpected) in individual domains for subgroups of infants who contributed enough trials. Alternatively, claims of consistency across domains may be altered throughout, in which case the inability to test whether the theta and/or NC signatures of unexpected event processing found are consistent across domains (vs. driven by some domains) should be acknowledged as a limitation of the present study.

      We agree that this statement rather reflected our intuition and would not surpass statistical analysis given the low number of trials. So we are happy to refrain from this claim and simply refer to the supplementary material for the interested reader and also mention this as a perspective for future research in the discussion (p. 12; p. 15).

      As outlined in our previous response, it was also not our goal to draw conclusions about each single domain, but rather to present a diversity of stimulus types from different core knowledge domains to gain a more generalized neural marker for infants’ processing of unexpected, i.e., unpredicted events.

      Reviewer #3:

      General assessment:

      In this manuscript, the authors bring up a contemporary and relevant topic in the field, i.e. theta rhythm as a potential biomarker for prediction error in infancy. Currently, the literature is rich on discussions about how, and why, theta oscillations in infancy implement the different cognitive processes to which they have been linked. Investigating the research questions presented in this manuscript could therefore contribute to fill these gaps and improve our understanding of infants' neural oscillations and learning mechanisms. While we appreciate the motivation behind the study and the potential in the authors' research aim, we find that the experimental design, analyses and conclusions based on the results that can be drawn thereafter, lack sufficient novelty and are partly problematic in their description and implementation. Below, we list our major concerns in more detail, and make suggestions for improvements of the current analyses and manuscript.

      Summary of major concerns:

      1) Novelty:

      (a) It is unclear how the study differs from Berger et al., 2006 apart from additional conditions. Please describe this study in more detail and how your study extends beyond it.

      We would like to thank the reviewers for emphasizing the timeliness and relevance of the study.

      The critical difference between the present study and the study by Berger et al. 2006 was that the authors applied, as far as we understand this from Figure 4 and the method section of their study, the wavelet analysis to the ERP signal. In contrast, in the present study, we applied the wavelet analysis at the level of single trials. We now explain the difference between the two signals in more detail in the revised manuscript and also included an additional comparison between the evoked (i.e., ERP) and the ongoing (i.e., total) oscillatory response (for more details, please see the first response to the first comment of reviewer 1).

      (b) Seemingly innovative aspects (as listed below), which could make the study stand out among previous literature, but are ultimately not examined. Consequently, it is also not clear why they are included.

      -Relation between Nc component and theta.

      -Consistency of the effect across different core knowledge domains.

      -Consistency of the effect across the social and non-social domains.

      -Link between infants looking at time behavior and theta.

      We are thankful for these suggestions, which are closely related to the points raised by reviewer 1 and 2. With regard to the relation between the Nc and the theta response, we have now included a direct comparison of these signals (see Additional Figure 1, i.e., novel Figure S2; for details, please see the first response to the first comment of reviewer 1). Regarding the consistency of effects across domains, we have explained in response to point 1 by reviewer 2 that this was not the specific purpose of the present study, but we aimed at using a diversity of VOE stimuli to obtain a more general neural signature for infants’ prediction error processing, and explain this in more detail in the revised manuscript. Having said this, we agree that the question of consistency of effects between conditions is highly interesting, but we would not consider the data robust enough to confidently test these differences given the limited number of trials available per stimulus category. We now discuss this as a direction for future research (p. 15). Finally, we also agree with regard to the link between looking times and the theta rhythm. As also outlined in response to point 1 by reviewer 2 (paragraph 2), we initially had this plan, but did not succeed in obtaining a satisfactory number of trials in the dual recording of EEG and eye-tracking, which made us change these plans. This is now explained in detail in the method section (p. 7).

      (c) The reason to expect (or not) a difference at this age, compared to what is known from adult neural processing, is not adequately explained.

      -Potentially because of neural generators in mid/pre-frontal cortex? See Lines 144-146.

      The overall aim of the present study was to identify the neural signature for prediction error processing in the infant brain, which has, to the best of our knowledge, not been done this explicitly and with a focus on the ongoing theta activity and across a variety of violations in infants’ core knowledge domains. Because we did not expect a specific topography of this effect, in particular across multiple domains, we included all electrodes in the analyses. We have now clarified this in the method section (p. 10).

      (d) The study is not sufficiently embedded in previous developmental literature on the functionality of theta. That is, consider theta's role in error processing, but also the increase of theta over time of an experiment and it's link to cognitive development. See, for example: Braithwaite et al., 2020; Conejero et al., 2018; Adam et al., 2020.

      We are thankful that the reviewer indicated these works and have now included them in the introduction and discussion. Closest to the present study is the study by Conejero et al., 2018. However, this study is also based on theta analyses of the ERP, not of the ongoing oscillatory response and it includes considerably older infants (i.e., 16-month-olds instead of 9-month-olds as in the present study).

      2) Methodology:

      (a) Design: It is unclear what exactly a testing session entails.

      -Was the outcome picture always presented for 5secs? The methods section suggests that, but the introduction of the design and Figure 1 do not. This might be misleading. Please change in Figure 1 to 5sec if applicable.

      Yes, the final images were shown for 5s in order to simultaneously assess infants’ looking times. However, we included trials in the EEG analysis if infants looked for 2s, so this is the more relevant info for the analysis. We now clarified this in the method section (p. 7) and have also added this info in the figure caption.

      -Were infants' eye-movements tracked simultaneously to the EEG recording? If so, please present findings on their looking time and (if possible) pupil size. Also examine the relation to theta power. This would enhance the novelty and tie these findings to the larger looking time literature that the authors refer to in their introduction.

      Yes, in response to the second reviewer (comment 1) we explained in more detail why the joint analysis of the EEG and looking time data was not possible: We planned to assess both, infants gaze behavior and EEG response. Unfortunately, this was not very successful and the dual recording only worked for a few infants and trials. This led us to collect more eye-tracking data after the EEG assessment was completed, such that a match between the two measures was not feasible. We now clarified this in the method section (p. 7).

      (b) Analysis:

      -In terms of extracting theta power information: The baseline of 100ms is extremely short for a comparison in the frequency domain, since it does not even contain half a cycle of the frequency of interest, i.e. 4Hz. We appreciate the thought to keep the baseline the same as in the ERP analysis (which currently is hardly focused on in the manuscript), but it appears problematic for the theta analysis. Also, if we understand the spectral analysis correctly, the window the authors are using to estimate their spectral estimates is largely overlapping between baseline and experimental window. The question arises whether a baseline is even needed here, or if a direct contrast between conditions might be better suited.

      Please see our explanation about the choice of the baseline in our response to reviewer 1, comment 2. Because our stimulus sequences were highly variable, likely leading to highly variable overall theta activity, and our specific interest was in the change in theta activity upon the onset of the unexpected versus unpredicted outcome, we still consider it useful to take a baseline here. Also because this makes the study more closely comparable to the existing literature. We now clarified this in the method section (p. 9)

      -In terms of statistical testing

      -It appears that the authors choose the frequency band that will be entered in the statistical analysis from visual inspection of the differences between conditions. They write: "we found the strongest difference between 4 - 5 Hz (see lower panel of Figure 3). Therefore, and because this is the first study of this kind, we analyzed this frequency range." ll. 277-279). This approach seems extremely problematic since it poses a high risk for 'double-dipping'. This is crucial and needs to be addressed. For instance, the authors could run non-parametric permutation tests on the time-frequency domain using FDR correction or cluster-based permutation tests on the topography.

      -Lack of examining time- / topographic specificity.

      Please also note the sentence before this citation, which states our initial hypothesis: “While our initial proposal was to look at the difference in the 4 Hz theta rhythm between conditions (Köster et al., 2019), we found the strongest difference between 4 – 5 Hz (see lower panel of Figure 3).” Note that the hypothesis of 4 Hz can be clearly derived from our 2019 study. We would maintain that the center frequency we took for the analysis 4.5Hz (i.e., 4 – 5Hz) is very close to this original hypothesis and, considering that we applied a novel design and analyses in very young infants, could indeed hardly have fallen more closely to this initial proposal. The frequency choice is also underlined, as the reviewer remarks, by the consistency of this peak across domains, peaking at 4Hz (cohesion), 4.5Hz (action), and 5Hz (solidity, number). Importantly, please note that we have chosen the electrodes and time window very conservatively, namely by including the whole time period and all electrodes, which we now explain in more detail on p. 10. Please also see our response to reviewer 1, comment “1)”.

      3) Interpretation of results:

      (a) The authors interpret the descriptive findings of Figure S1 as illustration of the consistency of the results across the four knowledge domains. While we would partly agree with this interpretation based on column A of that figure (even though also there the peak shifts between domains), columns B and C do not picture a consistent pattern of data. That is, the topography appears very different between domains and so does the temporal course of the 4-5Hz power, with only showing higher power in the action and number domain, not in the other two. Since none of these data were compared statistically, any interpretation remains descriptive. Yet, we would like to invite the authors to critically reconsider their interpretation. You also might want to consider adding domain (action, number etc.) as a covariate to your statistical model.

      We agree with the reviewers (reviewer 2 and reviewer 3) that our initial interpretation of the data regarding the consistency of effects across domains may have been too strong. Thus, in the revised version of the manuscript, we do not state that the TF analysis revealed more consistent results. Given that the analysis was based on a different subsample and highly variable in trial numbers, we did not enter them as a covariate in the statistical model.

    1. This article investigates the civic writing practices of more than 11,000 stu-dents writing letters to the next president in the lead up to the 2016 U.S. elec-tion. We analyze how letter topics are associated with socioeconomic factorsand reveal that 43 topics—including ones prevalent among students such asimmigration, guns, and school costs—were significantly associated withsocioeconomic and racial majority indicators. Furthermore, we conducteda qualitative analysis of the kinds of arguments and evidence developedin letters from five schools serving predominantly lower income studentsand/or students of color in different regions of the country. Student argu-ments and types of evidence used were site dependent, suggesting the impor-tance of teacher instruction. This analysis expands previous conceptions ofyouth civic learning

      This abstract is a great place to find topics that you may be interested in as you look for your 3 letters from the main website. Think about what topics were of interest to students. Also, how did this assignment give students, "choice and voice"? How does this type of assignment contrast to a typical assignment the could've been given on this topic but without student voice? Take note and find correlations that may be relevant and/or helpful to you as you write your final paper for this week.

    1.    I see that the most interesting idea in this chapter is the part that relates to “language and thought.” As Culler says that there is a theory that says “language expresses the existing ideas by providing methods for this.” So language and thought interact in many significant ways, thought comes first, while language is an expression of what we think. Also each specific language has its own influence on the thought and action of its speakers. Likewise, according to Culler, Expressing ideas that we think are easy and natural in our language may require a great effort from us to express them in another language. This brings us to an important point, anyone who has learned more than one language is struck by the many ways in which languages differ from one to another. I think that this part reminds me of myself when I started learning languages other than Arabic. Sometimes it was very difficult for me to express what I was thinking in English, I felt like my thoughts were frozen, and other times I couldn’t express them at all. Therefore, Culler mentions that literary works often try to reconfigure ideas in order to be able to express things that we did not expect to think of before. The relationship between language and thinking is often a strong one, but literature generates other ideas so we can see a different reality.

      Very insightful

    1. What caught my attention the most was actually the correlation Culler made between language and thought. Specifically, the “extreme view” that comes from the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis, that states what we think is governed by what language we speak. When he mentions, “Whorf argued that the Hopi Indians have a conception of time that can’t be grasped in English”(pg60), I was baffled. How hard could it be to translate seconds, days, and years? Just after that, Culler says that the French have no corresponding word to English’s “pets”. Does this mean that French people lack animal companions, furry and otherwise? Of course not! (I know, I’ve seen it.) Fun fact: the closest translation you can get in French would be “animal de compagnie”. Which when literally translated into English becomes “animal of company”; I personally find this a far more fitting description of, say, a dog. And there it is! To some, the term “pet” may seem demeaning – just some thing you own. In this context, it might appear odd to view “man’s best friend” as something you just happen to buy at the local pet store; like buying a t.v. from Best Buy. “Animal of company”, now there is a term that commands respect and shows appreciation. I’m beginning to understand exactly why translators often say “there’s no word for it in English”, or, “the closest it comes to is/ a rough translation is”. Language is heavily influenced by the speaker’s view of the world. I’m reminded of my frustration with Spanish, and its insistence of assigning genders to inanimate objects. Is a bicycle called a “bicicleta” (“a” being female) because it looks feminine?

      This is a very interesting account of how you related to ideas in the chapter.

    1. Author Response

      1) There were concerns about the normality tests and reanalysis to avoid pseudo-replication that must be addressed.

      We have now checked the data by two tests for normal distribution (Shapiro-Wilk and Kolmogorov_Smirnoff) and found that flight data do not follow a normal distribution. Therefore statistical analysis of flight data have now been performed using non-parametric tests. We have used the Kruskal-Wallace test followed by Dunn’s multiple comparison test for multiple comparisons and Mann-Whitney U-Test for pair wise comparisons. This information has been included in the statistical tests section in methods. Regarding pseudo-replication, as suggested imaging data have been replotted and calculated now to include just one cell, or one lobe per brain. In addition we have included individual brain traces for every experiment as supplemental data (Figure 5 - supplement F2, Figure 6 – supplement F1, F3 and F4).

      2) Discussion should be made clearer and expanded to encompass more of the literature. Specifically, the authors should expand upon the final section of the discussion to discuss more about 1) the potential context for cholinergic modulation of the PPL1-y2alpha'1 DANs (For example, consider where the acetylcholine signal onto DANs might come from. DANs may not be entirely presynaptic to Kenyon cells but might also receive input from Kenyon cells.), 2) the proposed role of these DANs (which have been studied in several contexts) and 3) modulation of innate behavior in general. The paper begins with the importance of modulating innate behavior, but the discussion on this topic is spare and focused almost entirely on research on the mushroom bodies of Drosophila. The discussion section leans heavily on summarizing the results, rather than making connections to work in other systems or networks.

      As suggested we have now addressed each of these points in greater detail in the last section of the discussion which has been expanded to two paragraphs. The possibility of cholinergic inputs from KC cells to DANs stimulating the IP3R have been included in the discussion and in the final model in Figure 7. Several other references that mention the role of PPL1-y2alpha'1 DANs in modulation of behaviour are now included – see last para of the discussion. We have expanded the last section of the discussion to include possible roles for other regions of the brain in modulating flight and references to other insect brains, where relevant.

      3) One common point raised by all reviewers was the need for expression of the itprDN during pupation which could have been due to either the perdurance of endogenous itpr vs. a developmental effect caused by the itprDN (the authors fully acknowledge the issue). This section raised many questions that aren't within the scope of this study, nor are easily resolved. Nevertheless, the authors must expand upon the implications of these results and suggest future studies will needed to resolve the issue.

      We are indeed unable to state equivocally if adult behavioural phenotypes, arising from expression of the IP3R^DN, are only pupal or both pupal and adult. We have expanded on the implications of these results both in the results (Page 9-10) and in the discussion (page 11). One way of addressing this is to express a tagged IP3R^DN specifically in late pupae and then follow it’s perdurance in adults. This experiment has now been suggested as a way to resolve this issue in the second paragraph of the discussion.

      Reviewer #1:

      The authors report experiments on Drosophila to show that the proper function of an IP3 receptor in a small subset of dopaminergic neurons is required for flight behavior. Most interesting is the fact that the requirement is restricted to a time point during pupal development. Technically, the authors report a novel dominant-negative mutant for of the IP3 receptor to interfere with its function. Physiologically, the IP3 receptor-dependent impairment in the function of the dopaminergic neurons affects both synaptic vesicle release and excitability, Also, muscarinic acetylcholine receptors are required for proper development of the flight-modulating circuit during development.

      The role of dopamine in the brain of Drosophila (as a model for general dopamine and brain function) is in the center of current research, and is studied by a large number of laboratories. More and more types of behavior are discovered that are modulated by dopaminergic neurons, and in particular those innervating the mushroom body. Therefore, the study is of very high interest for researchers working on Drosophila, but also to a broader readership.

      The experiments are well designed. with appropriate controls at place. The conclusions drawn are highly interesting and novel (dopaminergic modulation of flight behavior, perhaps in the context of food seeking behavior, molecular mechanisms of circuit maturation).

      Minor comments:

      1) A test for normal distribution of data is required to determine whether parametric statistical tests are actually appropriate.

      Done – please see response above.

      2) It is not clear to me why the authors conclude an acute requirement of IP3R during the adult state although the phenotype can arise through a genetic intervention during earlier time points in development (Page 9, lines 297ff). This has to be outlined much clearer. My interpretation of the data is: During a certain time window after pupal formation IP3 signaling is required for a proper formation of the neuronal circuit. This is likely to be not only a cell-intrinsic (i.e., cell autonomous) effect because the mAchR is also required during this time window. This provides an excellent example (there are actually only very few!) of circuit development that requires synaptic interactions between neurons. If one keeps in mind that dopaminergic neurons have reciprocal synapses with Kenyon cells (e.g. Cervantes-Sandova, elife 2017; should be included in schematic illustration!)), and these release acetylcholine onto dopaminergic neurons, a potential circuit maturation based on the concerted activity is most interesting. I suggest that the authors point out more precisely how they think the actual phenotype comes about, of course, with all due caution.

      The primary reason that we suggest an adult requirement for the IP3R in the DANs is that we see a Ca2+ response to carbachol in adult PPL1-y2alpha'1 DANs (Figure 5 – supplement 1). We put together this finding with the observation that carbachol stimulates dopamine release from PPL1-y2alpha'1 DANs (Figure 5) and that blocking vesicle release acutely in adults reduce durations of flight bouts (Figure 4) to suggest that there is likely to be an adult requirement. However, we agree that this is not conclusive and certainly does not negate a pupal requirement. As mentioned above we have addressed the pupal vs pupal+adult issue in greater detail in the results (page 9, 10) and discussion (page 11). We agree that there may be acetylcholine release from Kenyon cells at the MB synapse. This possibility has been included in the discussion and in Figure 7.

      3) Statistical tests should be done across independent brains, not across different cells in the same brains.

      We have done this. Thank you for pointing this out.

      Additional data files and statistical comments:

      A test for normal distribution of data is required to determine whether parametric statistical tests are actually appropriate.

      Done.

      Figure legend 5 C should be 5B. The scaling of the y-axis is not optimal.

      Done.

      Statistical tests should be done across independent brains, not across different cells in the same brains. This would cause a mixture of dependent and independent data. This is of importance!

      Done.

      Reviewer #2:

      The results of the individual experiments reported by the authors are convincing. The approach is rigorous and they take full advantage of the many powerful molecular genetic tools available in Drosophila. The identification of a mechanism by which a small subset of dopaminergic cells may control behavior is significant. My concerns about the manuscript are relatively minor.

      Minor comments:

      I have reviewed "Modulation of flight and feeding behaviours requires presynaptic IP3Rs in dopaminergic Neurons" by Sharma and Hasan. The authors first translated to Drosophila a dominant negative (DN) strategy first tested in mammalian cells to block the function of the fly IP3 receptor. Controls using westerns to test the expression in vivo and calcium imaging to assess inhibitory activity in an ex vivo prep were generally convincing. They then show that the DNA, RNAi and a wt transgene disrupts flight as they have shown previously using both genetic mutants and RNAi. They use genetic rescue to further show that alterations in the function of itpr in dopaminergic cells are likely to mediate at least some aspects of the flight deficit. The restricted distribution of the THD' driver was used to narrow down the identity of DA cell clusters responsible for this effect to PPL1 and/or PPL3. Additional split GAL4 lines identified a deficit when the DN was expressed in the PPL1-γ2α′1 subset of DA cells that project to the mushroom bodies. This is a key finding of the paper since it localizes the requirement of the IP3R to cells that have been implicated in other behaviors. Developmental tests using TARGET/GAL80 indicate a requirement for itpr during late development. Disruption of itpr only in the adult did not have a significant effect. This seems likely to be due to perdurance of itpr as suggested by the authors. However, these data make it difficult to determine which aspects of the phenotype are due to broad developmental deficits versus disruption of IP3R in the adult (see below). The authors next test the effects of mAhR with the idea that mAChR is likely to signal through IP3R. While it was known that developmental expression of mAcHR expression is required for adult flight, the current data more specifically that the PPL1-γ2α′1 DANs are required, enhancing the impact of the paper.

      To tie these results to vesicle recycling and release the authors use the shibere[ts] transgene in PPL1-γ2α′1. Flight bouts were disrupted via exposure to the non-permissive temperature both during late pupal development and the adult. The adult phenotype has been demonstrated previously but the developmental defect is novel. The demonstration of an effect in adults is important since it suggests loss of itpr during adulthood might also have an effect in adults even though this can't be tested due to perdurance. Expression of shibire[ts] in PPL1-γ2α′1 also disrupts feeding, and the authors next phenotype these effects with the itpr DN, indicating that IP3R expression in PPL1-γ2α′1 is required for both feeding and flight. However, here as with the flight experiments, it is not possible to directly demonstrate an effect in adults due to perdurance. They show that knockdown of mAChR also reduces feeding similar to its effects on flight and suggest that the deficits are due to disruption of the mAchR ->(Gq) ->IPR3 pathway. The suggestion of connections between mAchR and IPR3 within PPL1-γ2α′1 and the idea that PPL1-γ2α′1 controls two distinct behaviors are a significant finding and one of main contributions of the paper.

      To help link the shibire[ts] data set with and the results of perturbing mAchR and IPR3, the authors show that carbochol induced DA release is reduced, making excellent use of the relatively new GRAB-DA lines. As a control, they show that synapse density of PPL1-γ2α′1 in the γ2α′1 MB lobes are not altered. The demonstration that DA release is altered elevates the technical strength of the paper. Moreover, although further experiments might be needed to prove their model, these data support the argument that mAchR ->(Gq) ->IPR3 pathway is disrupted in the adult. The final set of experiments in Fig 6 indicate that excitability of the PPL1-γ2α′1 DANs is also disrupted by knock down or IP3R. Is it possible that this deficit contributes to the decrease in DA release by the mAchR ->(Gq) ->IPR3 and the authors nicely explain a possible mechanism and cite relevant references in the Discussion.

      The results of the individual experiments reported by the authors are convincing. The approach is rigorous and they take full advantage of the many powerful molecular genetic tools available in Drosophila. The generation of the DN transgene is a nice idea and in combination with other tools helped them to identify specific subsets of DA neurons important for the behaviors they test. However, they have previously demonstrated similar effects with mutants and RNAi, and again use them to help map the relevant cells. Since the use of the DN construct did not really go beyond the experiments using RNAi or genetic rescue, the emphasis on the importance of this reagent might be reduced in the abstract and introduction.

      Flight deficits have also been seen in other experiments on these the DANs identified by the authors. Thus, the major novel finding of this section is the demonstration that itpr is required in these cells for regulating flight. While it was previously shown that feeding behavior is also required by DAN projections to the MB, the idea that overlapping cells might control both flight and feeding is interesting. Although the idea that these two phenotypes are specifically related to each other seems somewhat speculative, one major strength of the paper lies in tying together prior observations on itpr and the DANs with their current experiments. They do this again at the cellular level using GRAB to show that carbachol induced release of DA (but not synapse density) is reduced by itpr knock-down, thus tying together data on shibere, AcHR and itpr.

      These connections make for an exciting story, and they have been cleverly woven together by the authors. On the other hand, they also represent a possible concern about the manuscript as a whole, since causal relationships between the deficits between the effects of blocking the effects of IP3R, mAcHR, neuronal excitability and vesicle release are not yet proven. It is therefore possible that all of these are relatively non-specific effects of disrupting the function of PPL1-γ2α′1 neurons. This modestly reduces the strength of the paper but is also a relatively minor concern. A second potential concern is that despite the interesting connections made by the authors as well as some exciting new data, some of the findings replicate previous data.

      It is indeed likely that loss of the IP3R in PPL1-y2alpha'1 DANs leads to both specific (acetylcholine signaling followed by neurotransmitter release) and non-specific changes (such as loss of excitability). Both are likely to have an effect on the behavioural phenotypes modulated by PPL1-y2alpha'1 DANs. We have previously shown a role for both mAchR and the IP3R in flight. However, in this work we have addressed cell specificity and mechanism, neither of which was known earlier.

      A third concern is the relationship between the effects of disrupting PPL1-γ2α′1 during development versus the adult. As the authors suggest, perdurance (of protein expression) and/or "perdurance" of previously formed tetramers could easily account for the failure of itpr and mAChR knock down in the adult to cause behavioral deficits. By the same token, it is difficult to parse out the contribution of developmental defects in the DA cells versus problems with signaling in the adult and the following issues should be addressed: the observation that synaptic bouton density is not disrupted is a good way to eliminate gross disruption of connectivity during development but does not rule out other more subtle developmental defects in neuronal function. The fact that shibire[ts] can cause effects in the adult is appreciated but does not really help us to understand what IP3R and perhaps mAcHR are doing during development.

      We agree and have tried to further address this issue in the text (see above).

      Additional Minor Concerns.

      To validate the decrease in the overall response to carbachol in Fig 1D and E, the authors show a statistically significant difference for area under the curve. A parallel metric and statistical test might be used to support the statement that the response is delayed in 1D but not 1E.

      Thank you for this suggestion. We performed the test and in fact found that both cellular and mitochondrial responses are delayed. In presence of IP3RDN. This part of the text has been modified (page 4).

      "Interestingly, the mitochondrial response did not exhibit a delay in reaching peak values." Why is that? A brief explanation might be useful.

      This is no longer the case. The sentence has been removed.

      The second explanation of how shibire[ts] works might be shortened.

      Done.

      Reviewer #3:

      General Assessment:

      This study demonstrates that IP3R signaling (triggered by muscarinic receptor activation) affects excitability and quantal content of a subset of dopaminergic neurons to modulate flight duration and food search. I had no technical concerns and am generally supportive. My only major concern was that the narrative was fragmented. I believe this is because the perspective shifted between the IP3Rs and the dopamine neurons themselves, and was too focused. I think that streamlining the narrative and providing a broader perspective for the results will remedy this issue.

      Major Comments:

      -I would like the authors to expand upon their final section of the discussion to discuss more about 1) the potential context for cholinergic modulation of the PPL1-y2alpha'1 DANs, 2) the proposed role of these DANs (which have been studied in several contexts) and 3) modulation of innate behavior in general. The paper begins with the importance of modulating innate behavior, but the discussion on this topic is spare and focused almost entirely on research on the mushroom bodies of Drosophila. The discussion section leans heavily on summarizing the results, rather than making connections to work in other systems or networks.

      We have expanded the last section of the discussion to include these suggestions (see above under consolidated review points).

      -The developmental section seemed somewhat tangential as the authors cannot distinguish between a developmental role for the IP3R from a need to express the ItprDN transgene prior to adulthood to overcome a potential slow turnover of endogenous IP3R. In essence, it was unclear how these results contributed to the overall narrative of state modulation of behavior. Is this section informative to the development of the mushroom bodies or rigorous validation of the novel transgene?

      The manuscript addresses how IP3R function impacts behaviour. In that context pupal (developmental) and adult contributions are both relevant.

    1. Reviewer #1:

      In this manuscript, Bosco et al. propose that DHX30 coordinates cytoplasmic translation and mitochondrial function to impact on cancer cell survival. They deplete DHX30 and report that this causes an enhancement of translation including those of mRNAs encoding for cytoplasmic ribosomal proteins, while paradoxically reducing the translation of mitoribosome protein mRNAs. There are cytoplasmic and mitochondrial isoforms of DHX30 and the authors assess the long-term consequences of knockdown of the cytoplasmic versus mitochondrial + cytoplasmic proteins. Some of the novelty of this paper has been preempted by a previous publication by Antonicka and Shoubridge showing that loss of DHX30 results in impaired mitochondrial ribosome assembly, impaired mitochondria OXPHOS assembly, impaired mitochondrial mRNA precursor processing, and a very severe decrease in mitochondrial translation. I think the work, while interesting, is preliminary and should aim to provide mechanistic insight for the phenotype associated with DHX30 knockdown.

      As far as I can see, none of the targets obtained from the polysome profiling are validated in this study. This is concerning since polysome profiling was previously reported in a Cell Report 2020 publication by the authors (GSE 95024; available at the GEO database), but the origin of the RNA-seq data in the current paper is not clear (GSE 154065; not available at the GEO database). We do not know if the RNA-seq data was generated from the same samples as the polysome profiling samples previously reported or completely independent of these (this information is lacking). Regardless, validation of any putative translation responsive genes predicted from polysome profiling data would appear to be a reasonable expectation these days.

      The authors claim that depletion of DHX30 leads to increased global translation (Figs 1f, g). They also provide evidence that translation of mRNAs encoding cytoplasmic ribosomal proteins is increased, while the translation of mRNAs encoding mitoribosome ribosomal proteins is decreased (Fig 1b). DHX30 is associated with ribosomal subunits, 80S monosone and low-molecular weight polysomes, and it also interacts with a CG-rich motif for p53-dependent death (CGPD) in 3' UTRs of mRNAs. What is lacking is a mechanism to explain these observations (if the data validates)? To this reviewer the lack of mechanistic insight is a serious shortcoming of the current submission. What is responsible for the general translational increase (including cytoplasmic rps encoding mRNAs), yet mitochondrial rp mRNA translation decrease, upon DHX30 knockdown? Many rp mRNAs have TOP motifs at their 5' ends, is this pathway affected?

      The authors previously identified DHX30 as a CGPD-motif interactor. They published this as a specific DHX30 binding motif, yet this motif is not enriched in the new data set established by the authors. I don't understand the statement put forth by the authors on line 286 that " While we cannot exclude that the CGPD motif can be implicated, only a subset of RP transcripts harbors instances of it". Either it is significantly enriched or it is not. In any event, there appears to be an inconsistency with previously published data.

      The ENCODE eCLIP data suggests that DHX30 can bind to 67 cytoplasmic ribosomal and 23 mitochondrial protein transcripts. Yet in their eCLIP validation experiments using RIP, the authors probe for the potential of DHX30 to bind to only MRPL11 and MRPS22 (Fig 2a). They write "These findings suggest that DHX30 directly promotes the stability and/or translation of mitoribosome transcripts." What about the cytoplasmic ribosome protein mRNAs, which according to the ENCODE data can also bind DHX30, yet their response to DHX30 depletion is the opposite of that of the mitoribosome protein mRNAs. I think it may be premature to correlate DHX30 with mitoribosome protein regulation.

      The comparison of the efficiency of knockdown using siRNAs targeting the cytoplasmic form versus the mitochondrial + cytoplasmic forms versus shRNA knockdown efficiency is confusing and, in my humble opinion doesn't add insight into mechanism of action. "Transient silencing of DHX30" (ie, using siRNAs) achieves ~50% mRNA reduction in HCT and U2OS cells 48-96s following transfection. On the other hand, silencing of DHX30 mRNA using shRNA achieved better levels of reduction (60-75% decrease) in U2OS and MCF7 cells (Fig S2e). The authors use these differences in knockdown efficiencies to correlate differences in expression response of several mitochondrial encoded genes. The authors need to show the extent to which DHX30 protein levels are reduced in the siRNA treated cells (only changes in mRNA levels are presented). As well, there should be a genetic rescue experiment to show that siRNA or shRNA resistant DHX30 cDNA can overcome this effect. Lane 3 of Fig 2h appears underloaded as assessed by the actin intensity. MRPL11 protein levels appear greater in lane 2 (siDHX30-C) compared to lane 1, why is that?

      Please provide details on the siRNA and shRNAs used. It appears that only one shDHX30 was used to target cytoplasmic DHX30 and one shRNA to target cytoplasmic + mito DHX30. I couldn't find information on this.

      If mutations in DHX30 are known to trigger stress granules formation, does knockdown of DHX30 do the same. Is eIF2 alpha phosphorylated upon HDX30 knockdown?

      There appears to be several DHX30 mRNAs made through alternative splicing (see https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/gene/22907). In this study, when the authors refer to cytoplasmic DHX30, is the equivalent function being attributed to these different potential isoforms?

      The pictures in Figs 1e, 2d, and S3g are quite difficult to appreciate and should be provided at higher magnification.

      Fig 2f. Why is there so much tubulin in the mitochondrial protein extract lane?

      Suppression of DHX30 mRNA leads to lowered proliferation rates in HCT116 cells. This however was not due to significant alterations in the cell cycle (Fig 4e). Apoptotic rates do not appear to be affected (compare HCT_shNT to HCT_shDHX30 in the DMSO samples of Fig 4g). Can the authors please provide an understanding into what is leading to the lowered proliferation rates if cell cycle progression and cell death are unaffected. Confusingly, "transient" silencing of DHX30 mRNA (protein levels were not assessed) in U2OS cells did not impact proliferation while in MCF7 cells it did. Although the authors attribute this difference in response to better depletion of DHX30 mRNA in MCF7 cells, they do not actually measure DHX30 protein levels and the use of different cell lines complicates the interpretation.

      Line 267 "none of the DHX30 closer homologs showed strong evidence of such localized translation". What homologs are being referred to here?

      Line 269. "Although our experiments did not enable us to confirm this in HCT116, a previous report also showed evidence for DHX30 interaction with mitochondrial transcripts in human fibroblasts by RIP-seq (Antonicka and Shoubridge, 2015). Our data instead point to a direct interaction with mitoribosome transcripts and their positive modulation as another means by which DHX30 can indirectly affect mitochondrial translation." DHX30 thus interacts with many different mRNAs and in my view it becomes difficult to ascribe a particular biological response to DHX30 to a particular set of transcripts based on interaction data.

    1. Note: This rebuttal was posted by the corresponding author to Review Commons. Content has not been altered except for formatting.

      Learn more at Review Commons


      Reply to the reviewers

      Response to the reviewers’ comments:

      We thank both the reviewer for their critical evaluation and excellent suggestion to improve the manuscript. We are making all the changes suggested by both the reviewers and performing the experiments to address all the concerns specifically from the reviewer #1. Please find below our response to the reviewers’ comments:

      Reviewer #1:

      This is an interesting study from the Rahaman group that identifies cardiolipin (CL) as a potential binding target for Drp6 recruitment to the nuclear membrane in Tetrahymena (that has a unique nuclear remodeling program). In addition, they identify a residue, I553 in the DTD region, which they claim is a key residue involved in specific CL interactions. While the experiments themselves are technically sound, and are well performed and controlled, I don't find the major conclusion that I553 is involved in direct CL interactions justified or well rationalized. By their own admission (in the discussion), the conservative mutation I553M may perturb local folding and may indirectly affect CL interactions. There is no test of DTD folding with and without the I553M mutation, nor are there other mutations (e.g. I553A and in the vicinity) tested. CD experiments in the absence and presence of CL-containing membranes will likely yield information on the impact of the I553 mutations, while DLS experiments would inform on the hydrodynamic properties (overall 3D fold) of the DTD and the impact of these mutations. CL interactions generally involve a combination of electrostatic and hydrophobic forces. Where do the electrostatic interactions come from? Why would an Isoleucine to Methionine mutation affect the hydrophobic component, even if I553 is the key hydrophobic residue?

      Response:

      We thank the reviewer for the comments that the experiments are sound, well performed with appropriate controls. While we agree that the exact mechanism of how I553 provides specificity to cardiolipin binding is not addressed in the present manuscript, our study clearly demonstrates that the isoleucine at 553 plays important role in determining cardiolipin specificity and nuclear recruitment. As pointed out by the reviewer, it is possible that changing isoleucine to methionine may affect the local conformation. However, there is no major conformational change in the DTD due to this mutation. This conclusion is based on clear loss of nuclear localization and cardiolipin interaction for the mutant without affecting other properties. The in vitro floatation assay clearly stablish that the effect is directly by inhibiting interaction specifically with cardiolipin containing membrane. It should be further noted that the same domain DTD interacts with other two lipids (PS and PA) and mutant retains interaction with them arguing that conformation of this domain is not significantly changed due to I to M mutation. Consistent with these results I553M mutant could be targeted to the nuclear membrane as a complex with wildtype Drp6 further confirming that I553 could form correct self-assembled structure with wildtype protein required for association with nuclear membrane. This is further substantiated by comparing all the known biochemical properties including GTPase activity, membrane binding via other two lipids, formation of helical spirals and ring structures. Hence it is clear that I553 provides specificity to bind cardiolipin and recruitment to the nuclear membrane. We will further confirm if there is any local conformation change due to the mutation I to M by fluorescence quenching experiments and will be incorporated in the revised manuscript.

      Regarding overall folding of the mutant, this is an excellent suggestion by the reviewer. We are planning to perform CD experiments of the I553M mutant and wildtype proteins to compare if there is any change in overall folding due to mutation. This result would be incorporated in the revised manuscript.

      Reviewer is right to point out that both electrostatic and hydrophobic interactions are important for interaction with cardiolipin. Electrostatic interaction is important for all the phospholipids while interacting with protein and is expected to come from other amino acid residues which are positively charged. Electrostatic interaction may contribute to the affinity of the interaction by providing additional binding energy. But considering its universal nature of interaction with all the phospholipids, it cannot give specificity for a specific lipid and hence would not discriminate among different phospholipids.

      Regarding affecting hydrophobic component, the reviewer is correct that both are strong hydrophobic amino acids and loss of I553M interaction with cardiolipin may not be due to change in hydrophobicity

      To address that the loss of cardiolipin interaction is not specific to methionine and is due to absence of isoleucine, the suggestion from the reviewer to replace I553 with A (alanine) is an excellent one. We are doing the experiments and we anticipate to incorporate these results in our revised manuscript.

      Reviewer #1 (Significance (Required)):

      The addressed phenomenon is restricted to Tetrahymena and may not have far reaching implications. Regardless, the identification of CL as a binding target for Drp6 at the nuclear membrane of this organism is in itself significant. The conclusion that I553 is the key CL binding residue is however not warranted. Additional experiments are needed to dissect how this residue impacts CL interactions and examine whether the observed effect is direct or indirect.

      Response:

      We thank the reviewer for appreciating the significance of this work. We agree that our data is Tetrahymena specific. However, we believe that the study is relevant for all the proteins whose association with target membranes depend on cardiolipin including many cardiolipin interacting DRPs (such as DRPs involved in biogenesis and maintenance of mitochondria).

      We really appreciate the reviewer for the excellent suggestions. Based on this we are performing the following experiments.

      1. CD experiments to assess overall folding of I553M and Wildtype protein
      2. Fluorescence quenching of Tryptophan (at amino acid position 548) residue in the vicinity of I553 to compare conformation of the mutant with that of wildtype protein.
      3. Evaluation of I553A in nuclear localization and cardiolipin binding. We anticipate these results to further confirm if I553 is the key CL binding residue and if the effect is direct.

      The writing is not clear in some parts and may require a round of language editing. There are no issues with reproducibility.

      Response

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out the language editing. We will edit the language wherever we find it appropriate. We would highly appreciate if reviewer can indicate the portions that need special attention.

      Reviewr #2:

      Reviewer #2 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity (Required)):

      Dynamin is a GTPase superfamily protein involved in membrane fusion and division. This paper focused on Drp6, one of the eight dynamin superfamily proteins of Tetrahymena, and analyzed its nuclear envelope localization mechanism by a combination of in vivo cytogenetical analysis and in vitro biochemical analysis for the various mutant Drp6 proteins. Results showed that a specific amino acid residue (isoleucine at the 553rd) in the membrane binding domain of Drp6 was required for its nuclear membrane localization, but this residue is not required for ER/endosome localization and GTPase activity. Furthermore, in vitro floating analysis using centrifugation indicated that Drp6 specifically bound to the cardiolipin at the 553rd isoleucine residue and this binding was required for Drp6's nuclear membrane localization. Finally, removal of cardiolipin from the conjugating cells using inhibitor treatment showed that cardiolipin was required for the new macronucleus formation (including the expansion of macronuclear envelope) through the function of Drp6. Based on these results, authors concluded that cardiolipin targets Drp6 to the nuclear membrane in Tetrahymena.

      \*Major comments:***

      The experimental data presented in this paper are reasonable and the results are solid, and therefore I think the deduced conclusions are convincing. However, to improve this paper, I have several minor comments to be revised before publication.

      \*Minor comments:***

      1. In the previous paper, it has been shown that GFP-Drp6 is localized in the inner nuclear membrane of both macronucleus and micronucleus. In this paper, however, this point is not clearly stated and is not shown in the figures --- I could not understand such localization pattern of GFP-Drp6 in Fig. 1C and Fig. 3b and the statements in the text. I suggest adding such statements somewhere in Introduction or Result section. Also, add adequate references to the corresponding statements in the text.
        • Related to the comment 1, I suggest replacing Fig. 1C (images of fixed cells) with Fig. S1B (images of live cells) because nuclear localization of GFP-Drp6 are much clearer in Fig. S1B (live cell) than Fig. 1C (fixed cell), and because fixation may cause artificial redistribution of the proteins. Please add arrows in those figures to point out the position of micronucleus in those figures if necessary.*
        • Similarly, I suggest replacing images of Fig. 5B (fixed cells) with those of Fig. S3 (live cells).*
        • page 7, line 224: GFP-Nup3 is used as a marker protein of the nuclear pore complex (NPC). However, there is no description of how GFP-Nup3 is obtained or made. Add description how this DNA plasmid was obtained or generated.*
        • Related to the comment 4, "Nup3" is first discovered in Malone et al., Eukaryotic Cells, 2009, but also soon after discovered as the name of "MicNup98B" in Iwamoto et al., Curr Biol, 2009 and used in several papers including Iwamoto et al., Genes Cells, 2010; JCS, 2015; JCS 2017; and more. Because Nup3 is the Tetrahymena paralogs of human Nup98 and the name of "Nup98" is well established to call these homologs in various eukaryotes, I suggest adding the name of "MacNup98B" after the word of "Nup3" for reader's better understanding. I also suggest adding appropriate references to refer to this protein as follows: Add Malone et al. 2009 for "Nup3" and Iwamoto et al., 2009 for "MacNup98B."*
        • page 9, line 295: I wonder if "Fig. 3b" may be a mistake of "Fig. 5C." If so, please correct this.*
        • page 10, the second paragraph (lines 311-322): This paragraph discussed the possible involvement of Drp6 in the nuclear envelope expansion of the post-zygotic nucleus. It may be interesting to point out that large-scale nuclear envelope reorganization including the formation of the redundant nuclear envelope and the type-switching of the NPC (from the MIC-type NPC to the MAC-type one) has been reported at this developmental stage (Iwamoto et al., JCS 2015). For example, the peculiar shaped nuclear envelope with the redundant/overlapping nuclear envelope structure can be seen and the MAC-type NPCs rapidly assembles to the expanding nuclear envelope. It may be interesting to point out that cardiolipin and Drp6 may be involved in these phenomena. But it is too speculative and therefore consider adding such a discussion as an option.*
        • page 13, line 412: Is the word "GFP-drp6-I553M" written in italics intended for the gene for the GFP-drp6-I553M protein? If so, protein may be acceptable here. Make sure there are no problems with italicized characters. Also, check if the lowercase letter "d" in "drp6" is OK because large letters are used in other cases.*
        • page 20, figure 1: I recommend switching the positions of HDyn1 and Drp6 in Figure 1a to keep the order in Figure 1b.*
        • page 21, line 671: Add the word "Tetrahymena" before "Drp 6" to pair with the word "human dynamin 1".*
        • page 23, line 729: Remove "and."*
        • page 23, lines 729 and 731: Unify the expression of "cardiolipin" and "Cardiolipin"*
        • page 23, line 732: Add "or" before "10% Phosphatidylserin."*
        • page 24, Figure 3a: Please mark the position of I553M in the figure if possible. Alternatively, indicate the range of amino acid residues after the words "red" and "green" in the figure legend.* Response:

      We thank the reviewer for the excellent comments that “the experimental data presented in this paper are reasonable and the results are solid, and therefore I think the deduced conclusions are convincing.” We also thank the reviewer for the minor comments which are thorough and very insightful. it will improve the manuscript substantially. We would incorporate all the changes in the revised manuscript.

      Reviewer #2 (Significance (Required)):

      The corresponding author and his colleagues have reported that Tetrahymena Drp6 is localized to the outer nuclear membrane of both macronucleus and micronucleus of Tetrahymena (Elde et al., 2005) and that Drp6 is required for the formation of new macronuclei during nuclear differentiation (Rahaman et al., 2008). Therefore, these parts are not novel.

      The novelty of this study is as follows:

      (1) The discovery of a specific amino acid residue (isoleucine at the 553rd) of Drp6 that is required for its nuclear membrane localization.

      (2) the discovery of a lipid molecule, cardiolipin, as a critical partner for Drp6's nuclear membrane targeting.

      (3) Discovery of involvement of cardiolipin in the new macronucleus formation (the expansion of macronuclear envelope) through the function of Drp6.

      *

      I think their findings are highly novel and will provide new insight into a field of cell biology. Especially, their findings will contribute to understanding how specific proteins targeted to the specific intracellular membranes. In addition, their methods (such as floatation assay) for analyzing the interaction between the protein of interest and lipid/liposomes will become an important tool.*

      Response:

      We are very happy to note that the reviewer has pointed out the significance of the present study. We fully agree with reviewer and appreciate thorough analysis and excellent conclusion from the reviewer.

    1. Americans are divided on the forthcoming coronavirus vaccine as just 51% say they would get vaccinated for the disease if a vaccine was available today, according to a Pew Research Center poll.That's a 21% drop from May, when 72% of Americans said they would definitely or probably get a vaccine.The percentage who say they would definitely get a vaccine has been cut in half to just 21%.Most of the concern stems from worries that the vaccine may not be safe, as 77% of Americans think it will be approved before the safety and effectiveness are fully understood.TRUMP VOWS ENOUGH CORONAVIRUS VACCINE DOSES FOR 'EVERY AMERICAN' BY APRILTo try to calm those fears, the CEOs of nine drug companies signed a pledge earlier this month to make clear their "commitment to developing and testing potential vaccines for COVID-19 in accordance with high ethical standards and sound scientific principles."President Trump said Friday that the United States will have at least 100 million doses of the vaccine by the end of this year and enough doses for "every American" by April 2021.“Hundreds of millions of doses will be available every month and we expect to have enough vaccines for every American by April and again I’ll say even at that later stage, the delivery will go as fast as it comes," he told reporters at the White House.CORONAVIRUS THERAPEUTICS 'MORE COMPLICATED' THAN VACCINE, EXPERT SAYSThat's a notably faster timeline than the one put forward by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. CDC director Robert Redfield said Wednesday that he thinks the general public won't get the vaccine until next summer, but it could be available in "very limited supply" later this year for first responders and others who need it most.“If you’re asking me when is it going to be generally available to the American public, so we can begin to take advantage of vaccine to get back to our regular life, I think we’re probably looking at third, late second quarter, third quarter 2021," Redfield told the Senate Appropriations Committee.Trump said Wednesday that Redfield "made a mistake" and had "incorrect information."CLICK HERE TO GET THE FOX NEWS APPDr. Anthony Fauci, the nation's top infectious disease expert, predicted this week that we would see a vaccine in November or December.The United States had 6,713,179 confirmed cases of coronavirus and 198,407 deaths as of Friday evening, according to Johns Hopkins University data.

      FOX NEWS SUCKS

    1. Before we can accept the Bible as a source of data, we need some reason for believing it to be true.

      SO TRUE-- "just because someone consistently believes something doesn't mean that it's likely to be true"... "even if a large number of people consistently believe something, its credibility may be negligible" 1.

      1) Theodore Schick Jr., Lewis Vaughn, How to Think About Weird Things: Critical Thinking for a New Age, (New York: McGraw-Hill Education, 2019), 84, e-book.

    Annotators

    1. Reviewer #2:

      The authors describe the dependence of the p-value on sample size (which is true by definition) and offer a solution, using simulated data and an applied example.

      I'm not sure that the introduction successfully motivates the paper. It is unclear whether this is due to misunderstandings by the authors of some key points, or rather is a matter of awkward communication, such that the authors' intentions are accurately conveyed.

      The authors note the link between the p-value and sample size. In particular, the authors suggest that statistical significance can be achieved by using a sufficiently large sample size, and they call this 'p-hacking'. I certainly don't recognise use of a large sample size as an example of p-hacking. Instead, this term refers to analytical behaviours which cause the p-value to lose its advertised properties (advertised type 1 error rate). Examples would include taking repeated looks at data without making any appropriate adjustment, trying tests on different groupings of data (and selecting results on the basis of significance), or trying different definitions of an outcome measure. The key point is that, when these actions are performed, reported p-values are no longer valid p-values - they do not behave as they are supposed to. So straight away the authors' argument becomes confusing. Are they criticising the behaviour of the valid p-value? Or are they trying to criticise behaviours that cause the p-value to lose its stated properties? This point remains very unclear. I believe the authors are attempting the former, but wrongly describe this as an example of p-hacking.

      But other statements in the introduction invite further confusion. The authors say " even when comparing the mean value of two groups with identical distribution, statistically significant differences among the groups can always be found as long as a sufficiently large number of observations is available using any of the conventional statistical tests (i.e., Mann Whitney U-test (Mann and Whitney, 1947), Rank Sum test (Wilcoxon, 1945), Student's ttest (Student, 1908)) (Bruns and Ioannidis, 2016)." Again, it is unclear what the authors are trying to say here, and the statement is clearly false under the most obvious interpretation. If the authors are saying that significance will always be found when the null is true and model assumptions are correct provided that the sample size is large, then this is clearly false. In this case, the test will reject the null 5% of the time, using a significance threshold of 5%. The authors can easily confirm this for themselves with a simple simulation. Are the authors trying to make the point that the error rate is conditional not only on the null, but also on the test assumptions (and so when they are violated the test may reject erroneously?) They certainly do not state this, and the fact that they refer to 'identical distribution' suggests otherwise. Another way the test assumptions could be violated is if actual p-hacking (see examples above) were present, such that the reported p-values were no longer valid. Again, the authors do not tell us that this is what they mean, if they in fact do, and this would be a criticism of p-hacking behaviours rather than of the p-value.

      When they write "big data can make insignificance seemingly significant by means of the classical p-value" they might be thinking of confusion between statistical and practical significance, which is a common misinterpretation made in the presence of large data size, but again, if this is what the authors are thinking of they should say it. The discussion by Greenland (Valid P-Values Behave Exactly as They Should: Some Misleading Criticisms of P-Values and Their Resolution With S-Values, especially section 4.3) seems to address the concerns raised by the authors fairly decisively. For a given parameter size, increasing sample size should produce stronger evidence against the null. The p-value does not tell you about the size of the parameter directly - it measures the discrepancy between the data and the null - interpreted correctly, there is no problem.

      So, with apologies to the authors, I don't think they are successful in convincing the reader that there is a problem to be solved, and the manner of presentation (which may just be an issue of communicating the authors' intentions) is such that it causes doubt about the authors' handling of the relevant concepts. Throughout the text, there are other confusing presentations around fundamental concepts. E.g. the authors write things like "Hence, we claim that whenever there exist real statistically significant differences between two samples..." I know what a real difference is, but what is a real statistically significant difference? There are no statistically significant differences in nature. Are the authors trying to refer to instances where the null is false and is rejected? Or, are they trying to say that a 'real significant difference' is where the difference exceeds some magnitude?

      For example - the authors write things such as "When 𝑁(0,1) is compared with 𝑁(0,1), 𝑁(0.01,1) and 𝑁(0.1,1), 𝜃 is null; so those distributions are assumed to be equal. In the remaining comparisons though, 𝜃 = 1, thus there exist differences between 𝑁(0,1) and 𝑁(𝜇,1) for 𝜇 ∈ [0.25,3]", highlighting the fact that perhaps the authors really want to address the practical significance vs statistical significance issue (although again, this is not explicitly stated). If the authors are interested in size of effect/ difference, then it is not clear that this proposal offers any advantage in that regard over the p-value (which, as noted, does not tell us about the size of a parameter). If interest is in size, then it is unclear why the authors do not direct the reader to consider the estimate and confidence interval, so that they may consider this explicitly in terms of magnitude and precision.

      With apologies to the authors, who have clearly spent a large amount of time on this - I would think that the best way forward here would be to post this as a preprint and to try to invite as much feedback as possible. The authors have lofty ambitions with this work. Maybe there is a good underlying idea here, obscured by the presentation? Unfortunately, it is difficult to assess this at present.

    2. Reviewer #1:

      The paper sets out to confront p-hacking and addressing the dependence of the p-value on the sample size. The paper sets out the motivation behind the problem and then proposes a solution using three examples.

      I have a major problem with this work in that I do not understand the motivation and hence cannot judge the value of the proposed solution.

      The authors need to set out some definitions which might help them framing the context. I outline below what I understand as the context and hence why I do not understand how their proposal will address the problem.

      Firstly 'p-hacking' is the term usually reserved for when researchers do not follow a pre-specified protocol on how a research question will be answered through the statistical analysis of a resource, single study or experiment, but instead analyse the data in many ways. Maybe they use slightly different assumptions, adjust the definition of an outlier or who is eligible for inclusion or adjust to a different outcome variable. In this manner they select to report the analysis that gives the smallest p-value. (Ioannidis referred to some of this as vibration effects) This is a major problem in science but it is not only the problem of the size of the data available. Although the bigger the dataset, the more subgroups that can be analysed. The main problem here is that we do not know how many ways the data have been analysed, we only know what researchers have selected to report. The manuscript does not address this problem at all.

      The p-value is defined as the probability of observing a result as or more extreme when the null hypothesis is true. In most settings the 'null' is that there are no differences between two or more groups, for example that all the means are the same or equal. Often this translates into the statement that we expect the distribution of p-values under the null to be uniformly distributed [0,1]. This can be demonstrated or checked by simulation. In the hypothesis testing framework we usually power our studies so we will be able to detect a (true) difference between two groups with some high probability. The specific difference we are interested in would be called the alternative hypothesis. Hence the p-value is used to reject the null, but under the alternative hypothesis the p-value will not be uniform [0,1]. It is well known that the larger your sample size the more precise estimates you will obtain and the smaller differences you will be able to detect. Sample size calculations require a specific alternative to be stated (e.g. a difference in means of 0.5 of a standard deviation) then a sample size that guarantees as specific power for the specific type 1 error can be calculated.

      This manuscript is confusing properties of the p-value when there are no differences and minimal differences between the two groups. I think the authors are trying to make the point that a statistically significant result is not necessarily a clinically or biologically meaningful result. They have done some simulations to show the distribution of the p-value when the true difference between the two means is 0.01. This is an example of an 'unimportant' difference, but it is not the null. This problem is best addressed by reporting effect sizes and 95% confidence intervals for quantities of interest rather than trying to adjust p-values in some way. Obviously when we have access to large datasets we may have a much larger sample than we needed to detect a meaningful effect though we may find small p-values. Adjusting the p-values will not really help as it is the effect sizes that are of interest.

      I feel the manuscript needs to be redrafted to be more clear about the problem they are trying to fix.

    1. As objective you may try to be, interpreting a text doesn’t happen in a vacuum. The hermeneutic circle captures the complex interaction between an interpreter and a text.

      This is the only useful idea in the text. Whatever we read has the context in which it was written and the context in whcih it is being read. Is this a hermeneutic circle as described earlier? Don't think so.

    1. Author Response

      We thank the editors and reviewers for taking the time to assess our paper. We note that the reviewers seemed generally supportive of the paper, including noting that the paper addressed important questions. For context, we reiterate here our main findings:

      • a prefrontal cortex population encodes the past and the present in its joint activity, but solves the interference problem by encoding all features on independent axes for their past and their present.
      • This encoding would in principle allow upstream regions to independently access representations of the past and present in mPfC populations. We go on to show this happens: we show that only the encoding of the present, and not the past, is reactivated in sleep after training.

      In this context, the main editorial objection that we “did not control for potential confounding of behavioral variables” is not explained in the reviews; we also note that there were no “concerns about the analytical methods used” that were pertinent to our main findings. We are thus unclear about the basis for rejection.

      We respond below to the main points of each reviewer; their suggestions on terminology and of separating literature citations on rodent and primate PfC are being given due consideration.

      Reviewer #1:

      Maggi and Humphries examined how the coding of the present and past choices in the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) of the rats during a Y-maze task overlaps and whether they can be reliably distinguished. They found that the neural signals related to the animal's choice in the present and past are distinct and as a result they can be recalled separately, for example, during post-training sleep. Although these are very important questions and an interesting set of analyses have been applied, the results in this report are not entirely convincing, because the analyses did not successfully exclude some alternative hypotheses.

      1) The authors analyzed the signals related to the choice, light cue, and outcome separately, and this is possible because the relationship between the animal's choices and cues were decoupled by testing the animals under at least two different rules. There were a total of 4 alternative rules and different sessions included different subsets of these rules. It is possible that at least some results reported in this paper might vary depending on which of these results were tested. For example, rules might affect how the animals learned the task. Therefore, the authors should provide more detailed information about how often different rules were used to collect the neural data reported in this paper, and whether any of the results change according to the rules used in a given session.

      In the paper we did examine mPfC encoding in the trials under the two qualitatively distinct types of rule (direction-based i.e. egocentric, and cue-based i.e. allocentric), and showed that encoding of the direction, light, and outcome occurred in both rule types (figure 1e). We gave the number of sessions for those rules in the legend for Figure 1e. (We could equally decode all 3 features in direction-based and cue-based rule sessions in the inter-trial interval as well, see Maggi et al 2018, Figure 9). Thus we compared the decoding vectors across all rule-types.

      Only 8 sessions contained more than 1 rule, in the sessions in which the rule was switched. In the full analysis underlying this paper, we had also separately examined the decoding in these 8 rule-switch sessions, and found equally good decoding of direction, choice, and cue. As the paper was already dense - see e.g. Reviewer 3’s comments - we elected to not show this null result in the current version of the manuscript - it is available in version 1 of this preprint - but it can be restored if desired.

      2) The authors claim that the neural coding identified in this study does not depend on the signals in individual neurons by showing comparable results after removing the neurons with significant modulations. This logic is flawed, because the neurons without "significant" modulations might still include meaningful signals due to type II errors. Furthermore, if individual neurons carry absolutely no signals, how can a population of neurons still encode any signals? This might suggest some kind of joint coding, and the authors should not merely implicate such a possibility without more thorough tests.

      The joint coding of information by a population of neurons is the basis for the whole paper, and is tested extensively: for example, Figure 1 is about establishing that joint coding exists in mPfC. Our point on lines 91-95 was simply to show that the decoding could not be trivially explained by one or two neurons that reliably and strongly differed in the firing rates between different labels (e.g. between left or right choice of direction). To do so, we found sessions in which there were neurons with significantly detectable tuning to the task feature, omitted those sessions, and then looked at the performance of the feature decoding in the remaining sessions - and found it was just as good. Indeed, our point is precisely that it is possible for individual neurons to carry no signals detectable by classic significance testing (potentially due to Type II errors), yet for the population to be able to perfectly encode the information.

      The explanation is simply that most, and sometimes all, individual neurons do not consistently covary their firing with the changes in a feature (e.g. choose left and choose right trials) across every trial of a session. In other words, no neuron need consistently participate in encoding information. But so long as when a neuron does change its firing it does consistently vary with the feature, then across a population there are enough intermittently participating neurons on a given trial to always decode the information.

      3) The authors analyzed the activity divided into 5 different epochs, where the position #3 corresponds to a choice point and #5 corresponds to the reward site. Therefore, it is surprising that the reliable outcome signals begin to emerge from the position #3 (i.e., choice point). Is this a false positive?

      No, this replicated a common finding of outcome-predictive signals in prefrontal cortex; e.g. Daw, N. D., O’Doherty, J. P., Dayan, P., Seymour, B. & Dolan, R. J. Cortical substrates for exploratory decisions in humans. Nature 441, 876–879 (2006).

      Fellows, L. K. Advances in understanding ventromedial prefrontal function: the accountant joins the executive. Neurology 68, 991–995 (2007).

      Sul, J. H., Kim, H., Huh, N., Lee, D. & Jung, M. W. Distinct roles of rodent orbitofrontal and medial prefrontal cortex in decision making. Neuron 66, 449–460 (2010).

      Kaplan, R. et al. The neural representation of prospective choice during spatial planning and decisions. PLoS Biol. 15, e1002588 (2017).

      We will add these references to the next version of the manuscript.

      4) The authors report that there is retrospective coding, i.e., no coding of the choice in the previous. By contrast, during the intertrial interval (while the animal's returning to the start position), the signals related to the "past" choice were still present but different from how this information was coding earlier during the trial. This is not surprising since during the intertrial interval, the animal's movement direction is opposite compared to that during the trial, so this coding change could reflect the animal's sensory environment. Whether the brain encodes the past and previous events using different coding schemes or not cannot be tested with such confounding.

      We note that the reviewer’s objection here only relates to the choice of arm direction, whereas we showed independent encoding of all three features: direction, outcome, and cue position. We can thus test how the past and present are differently encoded because we showed they are both encoded in the same set of neurons. We showed at length both here (Figure 2a&c, Supplementary Figure 5a) and in Maggi et al 2018 (Figs 5-6 and accompanying supplementary figures) that we could decode the past events from the population activity during the inter-trial interval. The information of the trial and the inter-trial interval can be decoded from the same neurons, so the question is: how can the same neurons encode both the present and the past?

      One interpretation of the reviewer’s comments is that they are concerned about the possible confounding of movement direction between the trial and the following inter-trial interval. Namely, that the turn directions are guaranteed to be opposite: e.g a left turn into the left-hand arm on the trial would mean a right-hand turn on the return journey of the inter-trial interval. However, that would mean the feature labels would be exactly complementary e.g. trial =[L L R L R] and ITI = [R R L R L]. So if the population was encoding the direction choice the same way in both the trial and ITI, then using the trial’s decoder of direction to decode direction choice in the ITI should result in a performance of 1-[proportion of correctly classified trials], meaning the classifier would be significantly below chance (and vice-versa for using the inter-trial interval’s decoder for the trials). However, we find the cross-decoding performs at chance (Fig 2).

      5) The authors tested whether the coding of present and past events is consistent using a transfer (cross-decoding) analysis. However, this is based on simply correlation, and does not exclude the possibility that neurons changing their activity similarly according to (for example) the animal's choice might also change their baseline activity between the two periods (as revealed by the analysis of "population activity" in Figure 3) or might additionally encode different variables. In this case, decoding based on simple correlation might not reveal consistent coding that might be present.

      It is unclear what the referee means by the cross-decoding analysis being “based on simple correlation”. The decoder is trained on vectors of firing rates (cf Figure 1b). The decoder assigns high weights to neurons whose activity differs most strongly between the two labels (e.g. left and right choice of direction). So a change in “baseline”, presumably meaning the average firing rate of a neuron across all trials or all ITIs, would not alter the decoder outcome. In addition to the two cross-decoding tests, we also showed the independent encoding by: (a) The angles formed by the decoding vectors trained solely on the trials and solely on the ITIs (Fig 2d-f) (b) The independence of the population rate vectors between trials and ITIs (Fig 3). Indeed, the change in population rates between trials and ITIs shown in Figure 3 is exactly those predicted by the cross-decoding results, as explained on pg 7.

      Reviewer #2:

      The study by Maggi and Humphries re-examines data by Peyrache et al. (2009), which the authors have themselves analysed previously (Maggi et al., 2018), recorded , in rat prelimbic/infralimbic cortex (see comment below on terminology). In particular, they look at the relationship between decoding of task events during performance of a trial, and during the subsequent intertrial interval. (n.b. in this study, unlike in many studies, the ITI is considerably longer than the trial period). They find that although task-relevant information can be decoded during these two periods, the information is encoded in orthogonal subspaces during trials ('the present') and ITIs ('the past'). They build on this to examine how information is encoded during sleep following training (vs a pre-training control period). They find that only the trial subspaces are reactivated during sleep, not the ITI subspaces, and more so if the rat received a higher rate of average reward.

      On the whole, I found this an interesting paper with a clear set of findings, and well-analysed data. Although the advance in some ways an incremental one on previous studies of sleep/replay, and on the authors' previous analyses of this dataset, the study will undoubtedly be of interest to researchers who are interested in consolidation of past experience during sleep. In particular, the study benefits from being able to look for two different types of information ('past' and 'present' decoders) in the same sleep recording sessions. There were a few things that I felt the authors could address:

      1) For the cross-decoding analysis in figure 2 b, it is not entirely clear from the main text which part of the trial and ITI coding is being used here. It seems to me like a more useful way of showing the cross-decoding analysis would be to show the 10x10 matrix of cross decoding accuracy for each of the 5 maze positions in both trials and ITIs. This is, I think, different from what the analysis in figure 3g is trying to show (which plots the classification error after dimensionality reduction to a 2D space).

      As we strived to explain in the text, for the cross-decoding analysis we used the decoder trained on the firing rates across the entire trial and separately across the entire ITI, in order to arrive at the most stable decoding vectors. We did not show the cross-decoding for the full 5x5 matrix of positions, as the results would be quite noisy. Nevertheless, this is a constructive suggestion, and we will add this analysis. (And indeed the analysis in Figure 3 already shows that the population activity is separable in 1 or 2 dimensions between the trials and ITIs at each maze position, so we would expect the decoder weight vectors to also be independent).

      2) It was surprising to me that the authors do not mention the finding in figure 4e anywhere in the abstract or introduction. It makes the reactivation story far more compelling if it can be linked to a change in behaviour during the preceding trials. I think this finding would benefit from not being buried deep in the results section.

      We are happy to make this result clearer. Our main finding is of the independent coding, and this result in Fig 4e does not speak directly to the independent coding results, but rather is a lovely little result to support the hypothesis that there really is reactivation of the population vectors in sleep. Because it did not speak to the main thrust of the paper, it was omitted from the abstract given the constraints on the number of words (150).

      3) The finding in figure 5 seems slightly extra-ordinary. It suggests that reactivation decoding during sleep is reliable even if very long bins of activity are used to calculate the firing rate (e.g. up to 10s). Does this relationship ever break down? Presumably with the sleep data, it would be possible to extend bins up to 1 minute, 5 minutes, etc. If there is still more reactivation at these extremely long time-bin lengths, does this mean that these neurons are essentially more persistently active? One possible way to test for this might be to project the data recorded during sleep through the classifier weights, and then calculate the autocorrelation function of this projected data (e.g. Murray et al., Nat Neuro 2014) - if this activity becomes more persistent, the shape of the ACF may change post-training.

      An excellent question. Rather than persistent activity, we interpreted the consistency of reactivation across orders of magnitude time-scales as showing that the correlations between the neurons were roughly consistent; and thus when active tended to be active in roughly the same relative order. Support for this comes from the findings in Appendix Fig A4e - the correlation matrix between neurons in the trial was more consistently found in post than pre-session sleep.

      Reviewer #3:

      This article asks the question if within trial (present) and ITI (past) task parameters are encoded in mPFC, and how encoding during these two trial epochs are encoded. They claim that firing in mPFC reflects past and present, but population encoding of past and present are independent. Further they show that the present is reactivated during sleep, not the past.

      On the face of it, this seems like an interesting paper. It is novel in that ITI encoding would be highly related to what was going on in the trial. The sleep finding is also interesting but I don't quite get the distinction between present and past for sleep. That could use some clarification.

      1) I'm not an expert in regards to this type of analysis, but throughout I was left with the feeling that I would prefer at least some single neuron data and firing rate analysis to complement the highly computational analysis, which frankly, was difficult to understand or critique by somebody who is not an expert.

      The goal of the paper is to assess the population coding in PfC of the same events in the past and the present. Indeed, as reported in the paper, we found 25-39 sessions which had no single neuron tuning at all to a given event in a trial (such as the choice of maze arm).

      2) I would have liked to see more analysis of firing correlations with behavior. It seems to me if animals were doing different things during the trial and the ITI, then it might not be a surprise that there is independent encoding.

      3) I also wonder if the finding is solely dependent on the task (which is poorly described). It seems like there should be independent coding of past and present in this circumstance because they do not feed into each other, and behavior during one is independent of behavior in the other.

      4) Relatedly, the authors suggest that independent encoding can explain how the brain resolves interference between past and present, but in this task there was no interference between past and present, and the authors do not show that when there is more or less dependent encoding that there is more or less interference. Without it is unclear how to know how important this finding is as it relates to performance and general mPFC function.

      We deal with these points together, as they are all on the behaviour in the trial and inter-trial interval in the task. Yes, the behaviour in the trial is independent of that in the inter-trial interval, so there is no “interference” of behaviour. But that is not of relevance to what is encoded in the PfC. The Introduction and Discussion both point out that the problem is interference of the encoding itself: the encoding of the past and present exists, as we show at length, so the question is: how can it co-exist in the same neurons? We indeed ask if there is no “interference” in the encoding simply because activity in the inter-trial interval is just a memory trace of activity in the trial, and rule that out.

      We cannot address when there is “more or less dependent” encoding, because the results are what they are: there is independent encoding of the same events (Figure 2).

      The task is described in detail in the Methods (pgs 20-21).

      5) Could activity reflect what the animal predicts will happen on the next trial, or what they are planning to do? It wasn't clear if that was examined.

      Whether activity in the inter-trial interval predicted what will happen in the next trial was examined in detail in Maggi et al 2018 (Fig 6), and shown here in Figure 2g. We found no encoding of the following trial’s choices, except for a very niche occurrence: an above chance decoding of the next trial’s direction choice when the rat had returned to the start position, during a learning session, and for a direction rule. In other words, as it turned to start the next trial, so there was decoding of the upcoming choice of arm.

    1. 9. Every man being presumed innocent until judged guilty, if it is deemed indispensable to arrest him, all rigor unnecessary to securing his person should be severely repressed by the law.

      I also agree with this statement because it should be only under the law that has been established by the people that someone who seems to have committed a crime be deemed guilty. Until that point in time when the jury decides on a guilty verdict, the person should be treated as though they are innocent for individuals can not be arbitrators of justice. Doing so may lead to false accusations and wrongful imprisonment, which we see far too often today. So although I agree with the statement, I do not think that it is applied as it should be in the modern society.

  4. learn-us-east-1-prod-fleet01-xythos.s3.amazonaws.com learn-us-east-1-prod-fleet01-xythos.s3.amazonaws.com
    1. "Advocating invading countries with mass forms of slow-motion toxicity, however, requires rethinking our accepted assumptions of violence to include slow violence." Because it's not what we typically think when we think of violence, its prioritization may be delayed in the news and our perception and response may not be as emergent as it should be.

    1. After all, those early days of computer history were sometimes military-oriented, sometimes popular (e.g. Vannevar Bush’s “As We May Think”), sometimes scholarly.

      Also the valences of publishing in these different areas has markedly changed over my parent's lifetime...

    1. Note: This rebuttal was posted by the corresponding author to Review Commons. Content has not been altered except for formatting.

      Learn more at Review Commons


      Reply to the reviewers

      This is a fascinating and beautifully written article about the possible evolutionary relationship between two major protein superfamilies - the P-loop NTPases and the Rossmans. Both are ancient and highly diverse superfamilies, containing a significant proportion of all extant domain sequences and were probably amongst the earliest enzyme superfamilies to emerge in evolution. No major evolutionary classification of proteins, such as SCOP, reports evolutionary relationships between them.

      Both share the same structural architecture of a beta-alpha-beta 3-layer sandwich and have an intriguing number of other shared structural features including the location of the binding site for phospho-ligands. However, whilst both bind phosphorylated ribonucleosides, the mode of binding differs and also the manner in which these compounds are exploited. Furthermore, there are differences in the topologies of the folds possibly suggesting distinct evolutionary trajectories. The Rossmanns appear to be more structurally conserved, whilst the P-Loops vary more in their topologies and possibly represent less stable arrangements of beta-sheets and alpha-helices. The authors have brought together several strands of evidence to explore possibly evolutionary relationships. Detailed structural analyses allow the authors to explicitly detail the significant shared structural features. For example, similarities in the mode of binding the phosphate moiety in the ligand. The structural features are well described and there are appropriate illustrations visualising key differences and similarities. The shared features of the phosphate binding site likely emerged and were favoured early in evolution, as supported by other analyses reported by Longo et al. However, as the authors point out there are other compelling similarities including the equivalent location of this site in the first beta-loop-alpha element in both superfamilies, which is not a necessary constraint of phosphate binding and the authors support this by giving examples of phosphate binding at the tip of alpha-4. In addition, they provide evidence supporting the common involvement of beta-2 which contains the conserved Asp in the Rossmanns common ancestor. The Walker-B Asp in the P-loops is also at the tip of the beta-strand adjacent to beta-1, as in the Rossmanns - although this is an inserted strand relative to the Rossmann topology. The authors propose feasible evolutionary scenarios for how the P-Loops and Rossmans may have diverged to acquire additional secondary structure elements extending the common beta-PBL-alpha-beta-Asp feature present in both superfamilies. Further compelling evidence is given by detection of a bridging protein - Tubulin - linking the two superfamilies. This has the distinct Rossmann topology but binds GTP in the P-loop NTPase mode. Furthermore, the GTP is hydrolysed by water activated by a ligated metal dication. Final support is given by reporting common sequence themes between the P-loop enzyme HPr kinase/phosphatase and some Rossmann proteins. The authors present further interesting and detailed analyses of similarities between the proteins sharing this unusual theme. The evidence provided by the authors for the shared beta-PBL-alpha-beta-Asp fragment seems very strong to me and has been presented in an interesting and informative way. Of course, it is not possible to know the subsequent evolutionary trajectories but the scenarios presented seem plausible.

      We thank the reviewer for their encouraging remarks on our manuscript.

      **I only have minor comments** 1) SCOP2 provides information on links between superfamilies based on rare sequence or structural features. Have the authors checked this resource for any details on beta-PBL-alpha-beta-ASP fragment? Or perhaps consulted with Alexey Murzin about this feature?

      The classification of Rossmann and P-Loop proteins in SCOP2 is consistent with the ECOD classification scheme. For further confirmation, we wrote Alexey Murzin and he replied that Rosmanns and P-Loops are annotated as two separate evolutionary lineages, termed “hyperfamilies” in SCOP2. He found our new evidence compelling, but that given the current criteria for shared ancestry, P-loops and Rossmanns are separate lineages.

      2) I was rather confused by the way in which EC annotations were collected for the two superfamilies ie via Pfam – wouldn’t it be better to use SUPERFAMILY as the domain structures would map directly to these sequence relatives. I’m also surprised that they only took the common EC from a Pfam family since the aim of this analysis was to identify how many different enzyme functions the two superfamilies supported. Pfam does not classify by function and so inevitably groups functionally diverse relatives. However, to get the full range of enzyme functions supported by these superfamilies I would have thought all non-redundant EC functions across these constituent Pfam families should be counted. Perhaps I have misunderstood.

      We have updated the analysis to make use of the SUPERFAMILY database and, as per your suggestion, we now count all non-redundant EC numbers. Although the EC number counts have somewhat changed, the major point – that these are exceptionally diverse evolutionary lineages – has not.

      3) The authors refer to a set of previously curated ‘themes’ and allude to a methodology that will be reported in a forthcoming manuscript. The idea of identifying rare themes and then using them to locate very distant homologues is appealing. However, I think some details should be provided here. For example, some brief details on the technology for detecting the themes and thresholds on significance. How rare are they and how conserved do these fragments need to be between superfamilies to join their curated list? Furthermore, how many of these curated themes are similar to the one reported in their article and do they get crosslinks to other superfamilies based on closely related themes? ie how unique is this theme to the P-loop and Rossmanns and are there closely related themes linking these two superfamilies to other superfamilies? I would imagine it is quite a distinct theme but I would have liked to see a few more details on this to reassure that there are no closely related themes.

      We have updated the manuscript to include a more detailed description of the methods used to detect bridging themes shared between the Rossmann and P-Loop evolutionary lineages. In addition, we now include a supplemental table (Table S2) with all of the initial hits from the theme analysis.

      4) The authors have built model structures to allow them to estimate ligand location in proteins with no structural characterisation. It would be helpful if they reported the degree of sequence similarity between the query and template proteins and also the model quality.

      We have updated this section to include more details. In addition, we have identified a structure from the same T-group to serve as our ligand donor. The updated ligand donor is more closely related to 1ko7 than the previous ligand donor, though the positioning of the ligand is effectively unchanged. We note that the global sequence identity to both the previous and new ligand donor is low (less than 30% sequence identity). However, the phosphate binding loops align well in both sequence and structure, as is detailed in the revised Methods section.


      The study by Longo et al. was devoted to evolutionary history of P-loop NTPases and Rossmann fold proteins. Although not related in sequence, the two protein families share some structural features that imply that they could be diverged from a common ancestor. Using bioinformatic analyses, the study under review identified some bridge proteins (of tubulin family) that share themes of both P-loops and Rossmanns, offering a possible support for the common ancestry. A minimum ancestral peptide structure is proposed based on the analysis and its possible diversification trajectory is hypothesized. Even though the divergence scenario is clearly outlined, the authors do not over-interpret the observations and admit that convergence could still explain the scenario. The methodology and results are sufficiently described and conclusions are explained in detail. Although it would be really interesting to design an experimental study to support the conclusion (and I suppose that the authors will do that), that is clearly outside the scope of this bioinformatic study.

      Obtaining experimental evidence for our hypothesis is far from trivial. Modern proteins, including the bridging ones identified here, may not be amenable to exchange due to differing contexts (epistasis). Still, we agree that highlighting experimental directions is a good idea. We have updated the sections From an ancestral seed to intact domains and Conclusion to include a brief discussion of experiments that may help test our hypotheses about the evolution of these protein lineages.

      I would not propose any major changes to the manuscript as I think that the message is very clear. **Minor comments:** (1)In the results section, the text is very clear but tends to be repetitive in places. I think the manuscript would be more easily readable if more to the point at some sections.

      We have edited the manuscript to remove cases of unnecessary repetition in the results section and throughout.

      (2)There is probably a few typos or unclear sentences, e.g. pg 5, mid-page, "The core, most common topology...); pg 12, three lines from the bottom "(where this element in canonical", probably should be "is canonical"; pg 11, mid page "the mode of binding of the catalytic dication of tubuling (often Ca2+)" - all the structures listed in Table S1 list Mg2+, so "often" is a bit misleading.

      We have corrected the unclear sentences and typos noted above, as well as a few others.

    1. Reserved Strings

      Reserved Strings

      #

      Strings which may be used elsewhere in code

      undefined undef null NULL (null) nil NIL true false True False TRUE FALSE None hasOwnProperty then constructor \ \

      Numeric Strings

      #

      Strings which can be interpreted as numeric

      0 1 1.00 $1.00 1/2 1E2 1E02 1E+02 -1 -1.00 -$1.00 -1/2 -1E2 -1E02 -1E+02 1/0 0/0 -2147483648/-1 -9223372036854775808/-1 -0 -0.0 +0 +0.0 0.00 0..0 . 0.0.0 0,00 0,,0 , 0,0,0 0.0/0 1.0/0.0 0.0/0.0 1,0/0,0 0,0/0,0

      --1

      -. -, 999999999999999999999999999999999999999999999999999999999999999999999999999999999999999999999999 NaN Infinity -Infinity INF 1#INF -1#IND 1#QNAN 1#SNAN 1#IND 0x0 0xffffffff 0xffffffffffffffff 0xabad1dea 123456789012345678901234567890123456789 1,000.00 1 000.00 1'000.00 1,000,000.00 1 000 000.00 1'000'000.00 1.000,00 1 000,00 1'000,00 1.000.000,00 1 000 000,00 1'000'000,00 01000 08 09 2.2250738585072011e-308

      Special Characters

      #

      ASCII punctuation. All of these characters may need to be escaped in some

      contexts. Divided into three groups based on (US-layout) keyboard position.

      ,./;'[]-= <>?:"{}|_+ !@#$%^&*()`~

      Non-whitespace C0 controls: U+0001 through U+0008, U+000E through U+001F,

      and U+007F (DEL)

      Often forbidden to appear in various text-based file formats (e.g. XML),

      or reused for internal delimiters on the theory that they should never

      appear in input.

      The next line may appear to be blank or mojibake in some viewers.

      ??????????????????????????

      Non-whitespace C1 controls: U+0080 through U+0084 and U+0086 through U+009F.

      Commonly misinterpreted as additional graphic characters.

      The next line may appear to be blank, mojibake, or dingbats in some viewers.

      €‚ƒ„†‡ˆ‰Š‹ŒŽ‘’“”•–—˜™š›œžŸ

      Whitespace: all of the characters with category Zs, Zl, or Zp (in Unicode

      version 8.0.0), plus U+0009 (HT), U+000B (VT), U+000C (FF), U+0085 (NEL),

      and U+200B (ZERO WIDTH SPACE), which are in the C categories but are often

      treated as whitespace in some contexts.

      This file unfortunately cannot express strings containing

      U+0000, U+000A, or U+000D (NUL, LF, CR).

      The next line may appear to be blank or mojibake in some viewers.

      The next line may be flagged for "trailing whitespace" in some viewers.

      ?? …             ​

   
      

      Unicode additional control characters: all of the characters with

      general category Cf (in Unicode 8.0.0).

      The next line may appear to be blank or mojibake in some viewers.

      ­؀؁؂؃؄؅؜۝܏᠎​‌‍‎‏‪‫‬‭‮⁠⁡⁢⁣⁤⁦⁧⁨⁩𑂽𛲠𛲡𛲢𛲣𝅳𝅴𝅵𝅶𝅷𝅸𝅹𝅺󠀁󠀠󠀡󠀢󠀣󠀤󠀥󠀦󠀧󠀨󠀩󠀪󠀫󠀬󠀭󠀮󠀯󠀰󠀱󠀲󠀳󠀴󠀵󠀶󠀷󠀸󠀹󠀺󠀻󠀼󠀽󠀾󠀿󠁀󠁁󠁂󠁃󠁄󠁅󠁆󠁇󠁈󠁉󠁊󠁋󠁌󠁍󠁎󠁏󠁐󠁑󠁒󠁓󠁔󠁕󠁖󠁗󠁘󠁙󠁚󠁛󠁜󠁝󠁞󠁟󠁠󠁡󠁢󠁣󠁤󠁥󠁦󠁧󠁨󠁩󠁪󠁫󠁬󠁭󠁮󠁯󠁰󠁱󠁲󠁳󠁴󠁵󠁶󠁷󠁸󠁹󠁺󠁻󠁼󠁽󠁾󠁿

      "Byte order marks", U+FEFF and U+FFFE, each on its own line.

      The next two lines may appear to be blank or mojibake in some viewers.

       ￾

      Unicode Symbols

      #

      Strings which contain common unicode symbols (e.g. smart quotes)

      Ω≈ç√∫˜µ≤≥÷ åß∂ƒ©˙∆˚¬…æ œ∑´®†¥¨ˆøπ“‘ ¡™£¢∞§¶•ªº–≠ ¸˛Ç◊ı˜Â¯˘¿ ÅÍÎÏ˝ÓÔÒÚÆ☃ Œ„´‰ˇÁ¨ˆØ∏”’ `⁄€‹›fifl‡°·‚—± ⅛⅜⅝⅞ ЁЂЃЄЅІЇЈЉЊЋЌЍЎЏАБВГДЕЖЗИЙКЛМНОПРСТУФХЦЧШЩЪЫЬЭЮЯабвгдежзийклмнопрстуфхцчшщъыьэюя ٠١٢٣٤٥٦٧٨٩

      Unicode Subscript/Superscript/Accents

      #

      Strings which contain unicode subscripts/superscripts; can cause rendering issues

      ⁰⁴⁵ ₀₁₂ ⁰⁴⁵₀₁₂ ด้้้้้็็็็็้้้้้็็็็็้้้้้้้้็็็็็้้้้้็็็็็้้้้้้้้็็็็็้้้้้็็็็็้้้้้้้้็็็็็้้้้้็็็็ ด้้้้้็็็็็้้้้้็็็็็้้้้้้้้็็็็็้้้้้็็็็็้้้้้้้้็็็็็้้้้้็็็็็้้้้้้้้็็็็็้้้้้็็็็ ด้้้้้็็็็็้้้้้็็็็็้้้้้้้้็็็็็้้้้้็็็็็้้้้้้้้็็็็็้้้้้็็็็็้้้้้้้้็็็็็้้้้้็็็็

      Quotation Marks

      #

      Strings which contain misplaced quotation marks; can cause encoding errors

      ' " '' "" '"' "''''"'" "'"'"''''"

      <foo val=“bar” />

      <foo val=“bar” />

      <foo val=”bar“ />

      <foo val=`bar' />

      Two-Byte Characters

      #

      Strings which contain two-byte characters: can cause rendering issues or character-length issues

      田中さんにあげて下さい パーティーへ行かないか 和製漢語 部落格 사회과학원 어학연구소 찦차를 타고 온 펲시맨과 쑛다리 똠방각하 社會科學院語學研究所 울란바토르 𠜎𠜱𠝹𠱓𠱸𠲖𠳏

      Strings which contain two-byte letters: can cause issues with naïve UTF-16 capitalizers which think that 16 bits == 1 character

      𐐜 𐐔𐐇𐐝𐐀𐐡𐐇𐐓 𐐙𐐊𐐡𐐝𐐓/𐐝𐐇𐐗𐐊𐐤𐐔 𐐒𐐋𐐗 𐐒𐐌 𐐜 𐐡𐐀𐐖𐐇𐐤𐐓𐐝 𐐱𐑂 𐑄 𐐔𐐇𐐝𐐀𐐡𐐇𐐓 𐐏𐐆𐐅𐐤𐐆𐐚𐐊𐐡𐐝𐐆𐐓𐐆

      Special Unicode Characters Union

      #

      A super string recommended by VMware Inc. Globalization Team: can effectively cause rendering issues or character-length issues to validate product globalization readiness.

      #

      表 CJK_UNIFIED_IDEOGRAPHS (U+8868)

      ポ KATAKANA LETTER PO (U+30DD)

      あ HIRAGANA LETTER A (U+3042)

      A LATIN CAPITAL LETTER A (U+0041)

      鷗 CJK_UNIFIED_IDEOGRAPHS (U+9DD7)

      Œ LATIN SMALL LIGATURE OE (U+0153)

      é LATIN SMALL LETTER E WITH ACUTE (U+00E9)

      B FULLWIDTH LATIN CAPITAL LETTER B (U+FF22)

      逍 CJK_UNIFIED_IDEOGRAPHS (U+900D)

      Ü LATIN SMALL LETTER U WITH DIAERESIS (U+00FC)

      ß LATIN SMALL LETTER SHARP S (U+00DF)

      ª FEMININE ORDINAL INDICATOR (U+00AA)

      ą LATIN SMALL LETTER A WITH OGONEK (U+0105)

      ñ LATIN SMALL LETTER N WITH TILDE (U+00F1)

      丂 CJK_UNIFIED_IDEOGRAPHS (U+4E02)

      㐀 CJK Ideograph Extension A, First (U+3400)

      𠀀 CJK Ideograph Extension B, First (U+20000)

      表ポあA鷗ŒéB逍Üߪąñ丂㐀𠀀

      Changing length when lowercased

      #

      Characters which increase in length (2 to 3 bytes) when lowercased

      Credit: https://twitter.com/jifa/status/625776454479970304

      Ⱥ Ⱦ

      Japanese Emoticons

      #

      Strings which consists of Japanese-style emoticons which are popular on the web

      ヽ༼ຈل͜ຈ༽ノ ヽ༼ຈل͜ຈ༽ノ (。◕ ∀ ◕。) `ィ(´∀`∩ _ロ(,,) ・( ̄∀ ̄)・:: ゚・✿ヾ╲(。◕‿◕。)╱✿・゚ ,。・::・゜’( ☻ ω ☻ )。・::・゜’ (╯°□°)╯︵ ┻━┻) (ノಥ益ಥ)ノ ┻━┻ ┬─┬ノ( º ºノ) ( ͡° ͜ʖ ͡°) ¯\(ツ)_/¯

      Emoji

      #

      Strings which contain Emoji; should be the same behavior as two-byte characters, but not always

      😍 👩🏽 👨‍🦰 👨🏿‍🦰 👨‍🦱 👨🏿‍🦱 🦹🏿‍♂️ 👾 🙇 💁 🙅 🙆 🙋 🙎 🙍 🐵 🙈 🙉 🙊 ❤️ 💔 💌 💕 💞 💓 💗 💖 💘 💝 💟 💜 💛 💚 💙 ✋🏿 💪🏿 👐🏿 🙌🏿 👏🏿 🙏🏿 👨‍👩‍👦 👨‍👩‍👧‍👦 👨‍👨‍👦 👩‍👩‍👧 👨‍👦 👨‍👧‍👦 👩‍👦 👩‍👧‍👦 🚾 🆒 🆓 🆕 🆖 🆗 🆙 🏧 0️⃣ 1️⃣ 2️⃣ 3️⃣ 4️⃣ 5️⃣ 6️⃣ 7️⃣ 8️⃣ 9️⃣ 🔟

      Regional Indicator Symbols

      #

      Regional Indicator Symbols can be displayed differently across

      fonts, and have a number of special behaviors

      🇺🇸🇷🇺🇸 🇦🇫🇦🇲🇸 🇺🇸🇷🇺🇸🇦🇫🇦🇲 🇺🇸🇷🇺🇸🇦

      Unicode Numbers

      #

      Strings which contain unicode numbers; if the code is localized, it should see the input as numeric

      123 ١٢٣

      Right-To-Left Strings

      #

      Strings which contain text that should be rendered RTL if possible (e.g. Arabic, Hebrew)

      ثم نفس سقطت وبالتحديد،, جزيرتي باستخدام أن دنو. إذ هنا؟ الستار وتنصيب كان. أهّل ايطاليا، بريطانيا-فرنسا قد أخذ. سليمان، إتفاقية بين ما, يذكر الحدود أي بعد, معاملة بولندا، الإطلاق عل إيو. בְּרֵאשִׁית, בָּרָא אֱלֹהִים, אֵת הַשָּׁמַיִם, וְאֵת הָאָרֶץ הָיְתָהtestالصفحات التّحول ﷽ ﷺ مُنَاقَشَةُ سُبُلِ اِسْتِخْدَامِ اللُّغَةِ فِي النُّظُمِ الْقَائِمَةِ وَفِيم يَخُصَّ التَّطْبِيقَاتُ الْحاسُوبِيَّةُ، الكل في المجمو عة (5)

      Ogham Text

      #

      The only unicode alphabet to use a space which isn't empty but should still act like a space.

      ᚛ᚄᚓᚐᚋᚒᚄ ᚑᚄᚂᚑᚏᚅ᚜ ᚛                 ᚜

      Trick Unicode

      #

      Strings which contain unicode with unusual properties (e.g. Right-to-left override) (c.f. http://www.unicode.org/charts/PDF/U2000.pdf)

      ‪‪test‪ ‫test‫ 
test
 test⁠test‫ ⁦test⁧

      Zalgo Text

      #

      Strings which contain "corrupted" text. The corruption will not appear in non-HTML text, however. (via http://www.eeemo.net)

      Ṱ̺̺̕o͞ ̷i̲̬͇̪͙n̝̗͕v̟̜̘̦͟o̶̙̰̠kè͚̮̺̪̹̱̤ ̖t̝͕̳̣̻̪͞h̼͓̲̦̳̘̲e͇̣̰̦̬͎ ̢̼̻̱̘h͚͎͙̜̣̲ͅi̦̲̣̰̤v̻͍e̺̭̳̪̰-m̢iͅn̖̺̞̲̯̰d̵̼̟͙̩̼̘̳ ̞̥̱̳̭r̛̗̘e͙p͠r̼̞̻̭̗e̺̠̣͟s̘͇̳͍̝͉e͉̥̯̞̲͚̬͜ǹ̬͎͎̟̖͇̤t͍̬̤͓̼̭͘ͅi̪̱n͠g̴͉ ͏͉ͅc̬̟h͡a̫̻̯͘o̫̟̖͍̙̝͉s̗̦̲.̨̹͈̣ ̡͓̞ͅI̗̘̦͝n͇͇͙v̮̫ok̲̫̙͈i̖͙̭̹̠̞n̡̻̮̣̺g̲͈͙̭͙̬͎ ̰t͔̦h̞̲e̢̤ ͍̬̲͖f̴̘͕̣è͖ẹ̥̩l͖͔͚i͓͚̦͠n͖͍̗͓̳̮g͍ ̨o͚̪͡f̘̣̬ ̖̘͖̟͙̮c҉͔̫͖͓͇͖ͅh̵̤̣͚͔á̗̼͕ͅo̼̣̥s̱͈̺̖̦̻͢.̛̖̞̠̫̰ ̗̺͖̹̯͓Ṯ̤͍̥͇͈h̲́e͏͓̼̗̙̼̣͔ ͇̜̱̠͓͍ͅN͕͠e̗̱z̘̝̜̺͙p̤̺̹͍̯͚e̠̻̠͜r̨̤͍̺̖͔̖̖d̠̟̭̬̝͟i̦͖̩͓͔̤a̠̗̬͉̙n͚͜ ̻̞̰͚ͅh̵͉i̳̞v̢͇ḙ͎͟-҉̭̩̼͔m̤̭̫i͕͇̝̦n̗͙ḍ̟ ̯̲͕͞ǫ̟̯̰̲͙̻̝f ̪̰̰̗̖̭̘͘c̦͍̲̞͍̩̙ḥ͚a̮͎̟̙͜ơ̩̹͎s̤.̝̝ ҉Z̡̖̜͖̰̣͉̜a͖̰͙̬͡l̲̫̳͍̩g̡̟̼̱͚̞̬ͅo̗͜.̟ ̦H̬̤̗̤͝e͜ ̜̥̝̻͍̟́w̕h̖̯͓o̝͙̖͎̱̮ ҉̺̙̞̟͈W̷̼̭a̺̪͍į͈͕̭͙̯̜t̶̼̮s̘͙͖̕ ̠̫̠B̻͍͙͉̳ͅe̵h̵̬͇̫͙i̹͓̳̳̮͎̫̕n͟d̴̪̜̖ ̰͉̩͇͙̲͞ͅT͖̼͓̪͢h͏͓̮̻e̬̝̟ͅ ̤̹̝W͙̞̝͔͇͝ͅa͏͓͔̹̼̣l̴͔̰̤̟͔ḽ̫.͕ Z̮̞̠͙͔ͅḀ̗̞͈̻̗Ḷ͙͎̯̹̞͓G̻O̭̗̮

      Unicode Upsidedown

      #

      Strings which contain unicode with an "upsidedown" effect (via http://www.upsidedowntext.com)

      ˙ɐnbᴉlɐ ɐuƃɐɯ ǝɹolop ʇǝ ǝɹoqɐl ʇn ʇunpᴉpᴉɔuᴉ ɹodɯǝʇ poɯsnᴉǝ op pǝs 'ʇᴉlǝ ƃuᴉɔsᴉdᴉpɐ ɹnʇǝʇɔǝsuoɔ 'ʇǝɯɐ ʇᴉs ɹolop ɯnsdᴉ ɯǝɹo˥ 00˙Ɩ$-

      Unicode font

      #

      Strings which contain bold/italic/etc. versions of normal characters

      The quick brown fox jumps over the lazy dog 𝐓𝐡𝐞 𝐪𝐮𝐢𝐜𝐤 𝐛𝐫𝐨𝐰𝐧 𝐟𝐨𝐱 𝐣𝐮𝐦𝐩𝐬 𝐨𝐯𝐞𝐫 𝐭𝐡𝐞 𝐥𝐚𝐳𝐲 𝐝𝐨𝐠 𝕿𝖍𝖊 𝖖𝖚𝖎𝖈𝖐 𝖇𝖗𝖔𝖜𝖓 𝖋𝖔𝖝 𝖏𝖚𝖒𝖕𝖘 𝖔𝖛𝖊𝖗 𝖙𝖍𝖊 𝖑𝖆𝖟𝖞 𝖉𝖔𝖌 𝑻𝒉𝒆 𝒒𝒖𝒊𝒄𝒌 𝒃𝒓𝒐𝒘𝒏 𝒇𝒐𝒙 𝒋𝒖𝒎𝒑𝒔 𝒐𝒗𝒆𝒓 𝒕𝒉𝒆 𝒍𝒂𝒛𝒚 𝒅𝒐𝒈 𝓣𝓱𝓮 𝓺𝓾𝓲𝓬𝓴 𝓫𝓻𝓸𝔀𝓷 𝓯𝓸𝔁 𝓳𝓾𝓶𝓹𝓼 𝓸𝓿𝓮𝓻 𝓽𝓱𝓮 𝓵𝓪𝔃𝔂 𝓭𝓸𝓰 𝕋𝕙𝕖 𝕢𝕦𝕚𝕔𝕜 𝕓𝕣𝕠𝕨𝕟 𝕗𝕠𝕩 𝕛𝕦𝕞𝕡𝕤 𝕠𝕧𝕖𝕣 𝕥𝕙𝕖 𝕝𝕒𝕫𝕪 𝕕𝕠𝕘 𝚃𝚑𝚎 𝚚𝚞𝚒𝚌𝚔 𝚋𝚛𝚘𝚠𝚗 𝚏𝚘𝚡 𝚓𝚞𝚖𝚙𝚜 𝚘𝚟𝚎𝚛 𝚝𝚑𝚎 𝚕𝚊𝚣𝚢 𝚍𝚘𝚐 ⒯⒣⒠ ⒬⒰⒤⒞⒦ ⒝⒭⒪⒲⒩ ⒡⒪⒳ ⒥⒰⒨⒫⒮ ⒪⒱⒠⒭ ⒯⒣⒠ ⒧⒜⒵⒴ ⒟⒪⒢

      Script Injection

      #

      Strings which attempt to invoke a benign script injection; shows vulnerability to XSS

      <script>alert(123)</script> <script>alert('123');</script> <svg><script>123<1>alert(123)</script> "><script>alert(123)</script> '><script>alert(123)</script> ><script>alert(123)</script> </script><script>alert(123)</script> < / script >< script >alert(123)< / script > onfocus=JaVaSCript:alert(123) autofocus " onfocus=JaVaSCript:alert(123) autofocus ' onfocus=JaVaSCript:alert(123) autofocus <script>alert(123)</script> <sc<script>ript>alert(123)</sc</script>ript> --><script>alert(123)</script> ";alert(123);t=" ';alert(123);t=' JavaSCript:alert(123) ;alert(123); src=JaVaSCript:prompt(132) "><script>alert(123);</script x=" '><script>alert(123);</script x=' ><script>alert(123);</script x= " autofocus onkeyup="javascript:alert(123) ' autofocus onkeyup='javascript:alert(123) <script\x20type="text/javascript">javascript:alert(1);</script> <script\x3Etype="text/javascript">javascript:alert(1);</script> <script\x0Dtype="text/javascript">javascript:alert(1);</script> <script\x09type="text/javascript">javascript:alert(1);</script> <script\x0Ctype="text/javascript">javascript:alert(1);</script> <script\x2Ftype="text/javascript">javascript:alert(1);</script> <script\x0Atype="text/javascript">javascript:alert(1);</script> '`"><\x3Cscript>javascript:alert(1)</script> '`"><\x00script>javascript:alert(1)</script> ABC<div style="x\x3Aexpression(javascript:alert(1)">DEF ABC<div style="x:expression\x5C(javascript:alert(1)">DEF ABC<div style="x:expression\x00(javascript:alert(1)">DEF ABC<div style="x:exp\x00ression(javascript:alert(1)">DEF ABC<div style="x:exp\x5Cression(javascript:alert(1)">DEF ABC<div style="x:\x0Aexpression(javascript:alert(1)">DEF ABC<div style="x:\x09expression(javascript:alert(1)">DEF ABC<div style="x:\xE3\x80\x80expression(javascript:alert(1)">DEF ABC<div style="x:\xE2\x80\x84expression(javascript:alert(1)">DEF ABC<div style="x:\xC2\xA0expression(javascript:alert(1)">DEF ABC<div style="x:\xE2\x80\x80expression(javascript:alert(1)">DEF ABC<div style="x:\xE2\x80\x8Aexpression(javascript:alert(1)">DEF ABC<div style="x:\x0Dexpression(javascript:alert(1)">DEF ABC<div style="x:\x0Cexpression(javascript:alert(1)">DEF ABC<div style="x:\xE2\x80\x87expression(javascript:alert(1)">DEF ABC<div style="x:\xEF\xBB\xBFexpression(javascript:alert(1)">DEF ABC<div style="x:\x20expression(javascript:alert(1)">DEF ABC<div style="x:\xE2\x80\x88expression(javascript:alert(1)">DEF ABC<div style="x:\x00expression(javascript:alert(1)">DEF ABC<div style="x:\xE2\x80\x8Bexpression(javascript:alert(1)">DEF ABC<div style="x:\xE2\x80\x86expression(javascript:alert(1)">DEF ABC<div style="x:\xE2\x80\x85expression(javascript:alert(1)">DEF ABC<div style="x:\xE2\x80\x82expression(javascript:alert(1)">DEF ABC<div style="x:\x0Bexpression(javascript:alert(1)">DEF ABC<div style="x:\xE2\x80\x81expression(javascript:alert(1)">DEF ABC<div style="x:\xE2\x80\x83expression(javascript:alert(1)">DEF ABC<div style="x:\xE2\x80\x89expression(javascript:alert(1)">DEF test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test `"'> `"'> `"'> `"'> `"'> `"'> `"'> `"'> `"'> `"'> "`'><script>\x3Bjavascript:alert(1)</script> "`'><script>\x0Djavascript:alert(1)</script> "`'><script>\xEF\xBB\xBFjavascript:alert(1)</script> "`'><script>\xE2\x80\x81javascript:alert(1)</script> "`'><script>\xE2\x80\x84javascript:alert(1)</script> "`'><script>\xE3\x80\x80javascript:alert(1)</script> "`'><script>\x09javascript:alert(1)</script> "`'><script>\xE2\x80\x89javascript:alert(1)</script> "`'><script>\xE2\x80\x85javascript:alert(1)</script> "`'><script>\xE2\x80\x88javascript:alert(1)</script> "`'><script>\x00javascript:alert(1)</script> "`'><script>\xE2\x80\xA8javascript:alert(1)</script> "`'><script>\xE2\x80\x8Ajavascript:alert(1)</script> "`'><script>\xE1\x9A\x80javascript:alert(1)</script> "`'><script>\x0Cjavascript:alert(1)</script> "`'><script>\x2Bjavascript:alert(1)</script> "`'><script>\xF0\x90\x96\x9Ajavascript:alert(1)</script> "`'><script>-javascript:alert(1)</script> "`'><script>\x0Ajavascript:alert(1)</script> "`'><script>\xE2\x80\xAFjavascript:alert(1)</script> "`'><script>\x7Ejavascript:alert(1)</script> "`'><script>\xE2\x80\x87javascript:alert(1)</script> "`'><script>\xE2\x81\x9Fjavascript:alert(1)</script> "`'><script>\xE2\x80\xA9javascript:alert(1)</script> "`'><script>\xC2\x85javascript:alert(1)</script> "`'><script>\xEF\xBF\xAEjavascript:alert(1)</script> "`'><script>\xE2\x80\x83javascript:alert(1)</script> "`'><script>\xE2\x80\x8Bjavascript:alert(1)</script> "`'><script>\xEF\xBF\xBEjavascript:alert(1)</script> "`'><script>\xE2\x80\x80javascript:alert(1)</script> "`'><script>\x21javascript:alert(1)</script> "`'><script>\xE2\x80\x82javascript:alert(1)</script> "`'><script>\xE2\x80\x86javascript:alert(1)</script> "`'><script>\xE1\xA0\x8Ejavascript:alert(1)</script> "`'><script>\x0Bjavascript:alert(1)</script> "`'><script>\x20javascript:alert(1)</script> "`'><script>\xC2\xA0javascript:alert(1)</script> <img\x47src=x onerror="javascript:alert(1)"> <img\x10src=x onerror="javascript:alert(1)"> <img\x13src=x onerror="javascript:alert(1)"> <img\x32src=x onerror="javascript:alert(1)"> <img\x47src=x onerror="javascript:alert(1)"> <img\x11src=x onerror="javascript:alert(1)"> <img[a][b][c]src[d]=x[e]onerror=[f]"alert(1)"> XXX <title onpropertychange=javascript:alert(1)></title><title title=> `><img src=x:x onerror=javascript:alert(1)></a> <script src="/\%(jscript)s"></script> <script src="\\%(jscript)s"></script> <SCRIPT>alert("XSS")</SCRIPT>"> perl -e 'print "";' > out <SCRIPT/XSS SRC="http://ha.ckers.org/xss.js"></SCRIPT> <BODY onload!#$%&()*~+-_.,:;?@[/|\]^`=alert("XSS")> <SCRIPT/SRC="http://ha.ckers.org/xss.js"></SCRIPT> <<SCRIPT>alert("XSS");//<</SCRIPT> <SCRIPT SRC=http://ha.ckers.org/xss.js?< B > <SCRIPT SRC=//ha.ckers.org/.j> Copy me</u> Scroll over me <plaintext> http://a/%%30%30 </textarea><script>alert(123)</script>

      SQL Injection

      #

      Strings which can cause a SQL injection if inputs are not sanitized

      1;DROP TABLE users 1'; DROP TABLE users-- 1 ' OR 1=1 -- 1 ' OR '1'='1 '; EXEC sp_MSForEachTable 'DROP TABLE ?'; --

      % _

      Server Code Injection

      #

      Strings which can cause user to run code on server as a privileged user (c.f. https://news.ycombinator.com/item?id=7665153)

      -

      --version --help $USER /dev/null; touch /tmp/blns.fail ; echo touch /tmp/blns.fail $(touch /tmp/blns.fail) @{[system "touch /tmp/blns.fail"]}

      Command Injection (Ruby)

      #

      Strings which can call system commands within Ruby/Rails applications

      eval("puts 'hello world'") System("ls -al /") ls -al / Kernel.exec("ls -al /") Kernel.exit(1) %x('ls -al /')

      XXE Injection (XML)

      #

      String which can reveal system files when parsed by a badly configured XML parser

      <?xml version="1.0" encoding="ISO-8859-1"?><!DOCTYPE foo [ <!ELEMENT foo ANY ><!ENTITY xxe SYSTEM "file:///etc/passwd" >]><foo>&xx;;</foo>

      Unwanted Interpolation

      #

      Strings which can be accidentally expanded into different strings if evaluated in the wrong context, e.g. used as a printf format string or via Perl or shell eval. Might expose sensitive data from the program doing the interpolation, or might just represent the wrong string.

      $HOME $ENV{'HOME'} %d %s%s%s%s%s {0} %.s %@ %n File:///

      File Inclusion

      #

      Strings which can cause user to pull in files that should not be a part of a web server

      ../../../../../../../../../../../etc/passwd%00 ../../../../../../../../../../../etc/hosts

      Known CVEs and Vulnerabilities

      #

      Strings that test for known vulnerabilities

      () { 0; }; touch /tmp/blns.shellshock1.fail; () { ; } >[$($())] { touch /tmp/blns.shellshock2.fail; } <<< %s(un='%s') = %u +++ATH0

      MSDOS/Windows Special Filenames

      #

      Strings which are reserved characters in MSDOS/Windows

      CON PRN AUX CLOCK$ NUL A: ZZ: COM1 LPT1 LPT2 LPT3 COM2 COM3 COM4

      IRC specific strings

      #

      Strings that may occur on IRC clients that make security products freak out

      DCC SEND STARTKEYLOGGER 0 0 0

      Scunthorpe Problem

      #

      Innocuous strings which may be blocked by profanity filters (https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Scunthorpe_problem)

      Scunthorpe General Hospital Penistone Community Church Lightwater Country Park Jimmy Clitheroe Horniman Museum shitake mushrooms RomansInSussex.co.uk http://www.cum.qc.ca/ Craig Cockburn, Software Specialist Linda Callahan Dr. Herman I. Libshitz magna cum laude Super Bowl XXX medieval erection of parapets evaluate mocha expression Arsenal canal classic Tyson Gay Dick Van Dyke basement

      Human injection

      #

      Strings which may cause human to reinterpret worldview

      If you're reading this, you've been in a coma for almost 20 years now. We're trying a new technique. We don't know where this message will end up in your dream, but we hope it works. Please wake up, we miss you.

      Terminal escape codes

      #

      Strings which punish the fools who use cat/type on this file

      Roses are ?[0;31mred?[0m, violets are ?[0;34mblue. Hope you enjoy terminal hue But now...?[20Cfor my greatest trick...?[8m The quic??????k brown fo???????????x... [Beeeep]

      iOS Vulnerabilities

      #

      Strings which crashed iMessage in various versions of iOS

      Powerلُلُصّبُلُلصّبُررً ॣ ॣh ॣ ॣ冗 🏳0🌈️ జ్ఞ‌ా

      Persian special characters

      #

      This is a four characters string which includes Persian special characters (گچپژ)

      گچپژ

      jinja2 injection

      #

      first one is supposed to raise "MemoryError" exception

      second, obviously, prints contents of /etc/passwd

      {% print 'x' 64 1024**3 %} {{ "".class.mro[2].subclasses()40.read() }}

    1. During our passage I first saw flying fishes, which surprised me very much: they used frequently to fly across the ship, and many of them fell on the deck. I also now first saw the use of the quadrant. I had often with astonishment seen the mariners make observations with it, and I could not think what it meant. They at last took notice of my surprise; and one of them, willing to increase it, as well as to gratify my curiosity, made me one day look through it. The clouds appeared to me to be land, which disappeared as they passed along. This heightened my wonder: and I was now more persuaded than ever that I was in another world, and that every thing about me was magic. At last we came in sight of the island of Barbadoes, at which the whites on board gave a great shout, and made many signs of joy to us.

      question 7. Another perspective to take into account is that they were locked down below the ships main surface and one of the crew members noticed Olaudahs curiosity of looking out the ship so he allowed him to. Makes me begin to think this may not of been to be nice, but to remind him of how little freedom he has. How his only time to experience this is while being enslaved looking out, and not being able to experience this in the way that the crew members could.

    1. Note: This rebuttal was posted by the corresponding author to Review Commons. Content has not been altered except for formatting.

      Learn more at Review Commons


      Reply to the reviewers

      RESPONSE TO REVIEWER #1

      Reviewer #1 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity (Required)):

      In this manuscript, Ishihara et al. investigate and compare microtubule polymerization/depolymerization dynamics inside vs. at the periphery of microtubule asters in a cell-free Xenopus egg extract system. By tracking EB comets, which localize to growing microtubule ends, they find that the microtubule growth rates and EB comet lifetimes (interpreted as an indicator of microtubule catastrophe rates) are similar between the two spatially-distinct microtubule populations. However, using a tubulin-intensity-difference image analysis, the authors are also able to measure local microtubule depolymerization rates, and they find a significant difference in depolymerization rates of the two populations. Specifically, the authors report that the microtubule depolymerization rates measured within asters are faster than those measured at the periphery.

      \*Specific comments:***

      Figure 2.

      In the text, the authors report: "The depolymerization rate was 36.3 {plus minus} 7.9 μm/min (mean, std) in the aster interior, compared to 29.2 {plus minus} 8.9 μm/min (mean, std) at the aster periphery." This difference is certainly not two-fold (as stated in the abstract). It would also be useful to mark the mean rates on the graph in 2B.

      We removed the words ‘almost two-fold’ in the abstract. In the revision, we will mark the mean rates on Fig. 2B (using vertical lines).

      The bimodal shape of the depolymerization rate distributions in 2B is very interesting. This definitely warrants further investigation. At the minimum, the depolymerization rates should be determined at 50 um- intervals, as done for other parameters in Figure 1. Could it be that there are two coexisting populations of microtubules at the same location? Or is there a clear spatial compartmentalization of the two that is not obvious here because of the too large of a distance interval used for the measurements. This is a very important distinction for the claims of the paper.

      We understand the reviewer’s concern. There are some technical limitations that make the depolymerization measurement more challenging. While we use widefield imaging of EB1-GFP comets to obtain polymerization rates from a field of view spanning 500 microns, we may only use TIRF imaging for depolymerization measurements. In this method, we are limited to observing microtubules very close to the cover slip in a small field of view of 80x80 microns at 500 ms time intervals (movies span 1-2 minutes). One would need to move the TIRF field every 1-2 minutes at 50 micron intervals, but the aster periphery would be changing during this time, so the exact location of the measurement is hard to define. Thus, we opted to image the two spatial extremes: interior (close to the MTOCs) and the very periphery (where MT density is still sparse.)

      Perhaps, the largest limitation of this approach is the choice of peripheral regions based on the apparent sparsity of MTs in the TIRF field of view. Indeed, when we examine the depolymerization rate distributions for individual movies separately (see figure below, periphery #1-3 are three individual movies), we observe that some movies have rates as low as 20 µm/min, while others have higher values with a center around 36 µm/min. The depolymerization rates for the interior also vary from the mean values of 34.8-43.2 µm/min (interior #1-3 are three individual movies). In general, the spread of depolymerization rate within a field of view as well as across different fields of view is much larger than for polymerization. It is possible that this is partly explained by the lack of precise definition of interior vs. periphery in this TIRF-based measurement approach.

      Our data still supports the spatial regulation of depolymerization rate. However, there is no clear evidence for a bimodal distribution of depolymerization rate in any given field of view (80x80 micron square region). To clarify this point, we have removed the language “bimodal” in the main text. In the revisions, we will provide this figure as a supplement.

      We thank the critical feedback from reviewer #1 and #2 that allowed us to clarify this issue of apparent bimodality of the depolymerization rates.

      The authors make a point here that the distribution of measured polymerization rates is fairly narrow. This appears to be in contrast with Figure 1B, where polymerization rates take on a wide range of values. How do the two distributions of polymerization rates obtained by these two methods compare?

      To address this point, we directly compare the standard deviation of the polymerization rate measurements. For Fig. 1B EB1 tracking measurements, std ranges from 7.7-10.5 µm/min for a given spatial bin (as stated in Fig. 1B legend), while for Fig. 2A TIRF measurements std is 4.0 (periphery) and 4.5 µm/min (interior) as stated in the main text. Given that the mean values of polymerization rates are similar, this suggests that the TIRF measurements are less noisy. This further highlights the relative pros and cons of the two measurement methods. To discuss these issues, we have added a new paragraph in the discussion section.

      Figure 3.

      The laser ablation figure and movies are beautiful, but don't seem to add support to the story. Importantly, the authors do not confirm any spatial variability in depolymerization rate with these experiment. As a matter of fact, although the laser ablation experiments are only performed in the aster interior, the measured depolymerization rates appear to be just as consistent with the periphery rates in Figure 2. as they are with the interior rates in Figure 2. (They span quite a large range of values with the average right in the middle between what was measured for the two areas in Figure 2).

      Indeed, the values obtained with laser ablation are quite variable, even compared to the physiological depolymerization rate measured via TIRF microscopy. This perhaps reflects the variability of biology as well as the nature of the laser ablation which measures depolymerization rate at the level of microtubule populations. We hope our paper will increase interest in this rarely measured parameter, and perhaps invention of new probes to measure it more accurately and conveniently.

      Given the variability of our measurements, we conclude that the results between the TIRF based approach vs. laser ablation based approach of depolymerization rates are indistinguishable. We agree with the reviewer that the data does NOT argue that laser ablation results are more consistent with the interior TIRF measurements than peripheral TIRF measurements.

      To clarify this point, we remove the following clause “, which was comparable to the modal value of the depolymerization rates in the aster interior (Fig. 2).”

      We change the concluding sentence of our laser ablation paragraph from

      “Overall, these observations suggest that depolymerization dynamics are similar for plus ends following a natural catastrophe vs. ablation in the aster interior.”

      to

      “Overall, these observations confirm that depolymerization rates are variable, and we find no statistical distinction of rates between plus ends following a natural catastrophe vs. ablation.”

      Although the authors report they don't see any correlation between the distance and depolymerization rate, they should still plot the rate as a function of initial cut positions (Figures 3D, 3E).

      To address this concern, we plan to provide a supplemental figure in the revision. Please see the preliminary figure below. Due to technical limitations with the laser ablation system (field of view for 60x magnification), we only have measurements that span 15-100 microns from the center..

      From the single decaying inward wave the authors conclude that microtubules depolymerize fully to their minus ends which are distributed throughout the aster. Can the possibility that depolymerization is stopped by microtubule lattice defects/islands be excluded by these observations?

      The existence of microtubule lattice/defects is a recent development in the field and much is not known. If we assume that defects are structurally unstable, we predict that the episode of depolymerization will continue even when reaching a defect. If defects are stable and lead to instantaneous rescue of plus ends, we cannot distinguish the defects from minus ends. In this latter scenario, the interpretation of the decaying inward wave requires caution.

      What are the effects of the local increase in tubulin concentration due to the subunit release by depolymerization? What about the release of other lattice-binding MAPs (stabilizers)?

      We are interested in these questions as well. Soluble GDP-bound tubulin, released by depolymerization, is thought to exchange its nucleotide to GTP without need of a GEF, and no GEF is known. The dissociation rate of GDP is ~0.1 [1/sec], for a half-life of ~5 sec (Brylawski and Caplow, 1983, J. of Biol. Chem.), so we believe the tubulin subunits are recycled relatively quickly. It is not entirely obvious whether this necessarily results in a significant increase in ‘soluble’ tubulin concentration given tubulin diffusive transport. We hypothesize the main effect of stabilizing MAPs is on the depolymerization rate as discussed in our model in Fig. 5.

      Figure 4.

      Is the local depletion of tubulin/EB1 thought to be only within the narrow annulus at ~100 um distance, or is it not measurable on the inside due to the polymer signal? Can the two be separated? Such a sharp transition within a discrete annular region doesn't speak to the relative effects on the inside vs. the outside of the aster?!

      Yes, we also believe the soluble tubulin levels are even lower in the more inner regions of the aster. However, polymerized tubulin accounts for a large part of the fluorescence intensity in these inner regions, and our method does not faithfully reflect the soluble fraction. It will be important for future studies to employ specific methods that may unequivocally distinguish polymer vs. soluble tubulin concentrations (see below).

      More importantly, the local depletion of either tubulin or EB1 is not a good representation of a depletion of a MAP component that associates with the microtubule lattice. Both tubulin and EB1 bind preferably to microtubule ends, not lattice. Thus showing a profile of slight local tubulin and/or EB depletion does not seem to be relevant for the proposed model. Rather, overall microtubule polymer mass/density as a function of distance may be more relevant?

      Reviewer #1 makes a valid point that tubulin and EB1 are specifically incorporated to plus ends and not to the entire lattice as we assume for the MAPs in our theoretical model. To address this issue, we analyzed the fluorescence intensity of images obtained for a MAP that associates with the MT lattice, Tau-mCherry (Mooney et al. 2017). This quantification shows a depletion pattern similar to tubulin and EB1. Thus, we believe the local depletion is a general feature. For the revision, we plan to incorporate this Tau-mCherry data in Fig. 4.

      Figure 5.

      The toy model is intuitive and clear, but not sufficient without any experimental investigation. An attempt to quantify the actual distributions of at least one or a few selected proposed MAPs is needed. Is the depletion strongest where microtubule density is highest? What is the ratio of a MAP intensity to microtubule polymer density as a function of distance? How does that relate to local depolymerization rates? What are other testable model predictions that can show support for the proposed mechanism?

      We understand that our proposal is rather speculative, and the goal of this manuscript was to propose a hypothesis that may inspire others working on assembly on intracellular organelles. Although Tau is not an endogenous component of the egg extract system, we believe that our new quantification of Tau-mCherry depletion adds more credibility to our general proposal.

      Microtubule density is roughly uniform within the interior of the aster according to our current understanding (Ishihara et al. 2016 eLife). So the MAP:MT ratio is relatively uniform throughout the aster except at the very periphery where there are very few MTs assembled (i.e. “depletion is weakest where MT density is lowest.”)

      In the future, we may perform (1) FCS measurements of candidate MAPs to directly measure the concentration profile of the candidate MAP in soluble form and (2) depletion/addback to show which MAP most affects depolymerization rate. Although these experiments are appealing, this requires generation of new molecular reagents as well as calibration of a highly specialized optical method. Therefore, we decided to limit this paper to focus on the unusual observation of the variation of depolymerization rate and speculate the underlying mechanism.

      Also, the table is insufficiently described. Are any or all of these MAPs known to be specific regulators of microtubule depolymerization rates, but not other dynamics parameters?

      There are a large number of MAPs in Xenopus eggs, as there are in all cells, and the degree to which their effects on microtubules has been characterized is variable. To address this comment we include in the revised ms a list of known MAPs that are present in Xenopus egg extract, along with their estimated concentration from a published proteomic study. We annotate each MAP as to whether it increases or decreases microtubule stability, acknowledging that these data are very incomplete, in some cases there is disagreement in literature, and that we are combining pure protein and whole cell analysis. This table illustrates the challenge of associating dynamics regulation with any one MAP, since the behavior of microtubules is regulated by all these factors operating in parallel. That said, certain MAPs jump out as candidate depolymerization regulators that have been little studied for effects on dynamics, for example, MAP7.

      In the revision, we suggest to add this expanded table as a supplementary Table in addition to Table 1.

      Protein Description

      Gene Symbol

      Est. Conc. (nM)

      MT polymerization/nucleation/rescue?

      MT depolymerization/catastrophe?

      Lead reference

      Microtubule-associated protein RP/EB family member 1

      MAPRE1

      1800

      Increase

      Decrease

      PMID: 18364701

      Stathmin

      STMN1

      1600

      Decrease

      Increase

      PMID: 11792540

      MAP4

      MAP4

      960

      Increase

      Decrease

      PMID: 7962090

      Echinoderm microtubule-associated protein-like 2

      EML2

      580

      Decrease

      Increase

      PMID: 11694528

      EML4 protein

      EML4

      500

      Increase

      Decrease

      PMID: 17196341

      Disks large-associated protein 5

      DLGAP5

      380

      Increase

      Decrease

      PMID: 16631580

      Cytoskeleton-associated protein 5

      CKAP5

      300

      Increase

      Increase

      PMID: 23666085

      Kinesin-like protein KIF2C

      KIF2C

      200

      Decrease

      Increase

      PMID: 12620232

      CAP-Gly domain-containing linker protein 1

      CLIP1

      190

      na

      na

      Cytoskeleton-associated protein 4

      CKAP4

      160

      Increase

      Decrease

      PMID: 9799226

      Echinoderm microtubule-associated protein-like 1

      EML1

      140

      na

      na

      Ensconsin

      MAP7

      91

      na

      Decrease

      PMID: 31391261

      Targeting protein for Xklp2

      TPX2

      91

      Increase

      Decrease

      PMID: 26414402

      Microtubule-associated protein 1B

      MAP1B

      85

      Increase

      Decrease

      PMID: 7664878

      MAP1S

      MAP1S

      66

      Decrease

      Decrease

      PMID: 25300793

      Hyaluronan mediated motility receptor

      HMMR

      61

      na

      na

      MAP7 domain-containing protein 1

      MAP7D1

      47

      na

      na

      Cytoskeleton-associated protein 2

      CKAP2

      46

      Increase

      Decrease

      PMID: 15504249

      Microtubule-associated tumor suppressor 1

      MTUS1

      43

      na

      na

      Kinesin-like protein KIF2A

      KIF2A

      37

      Decrease

      Increase

      PMID: 29980677

      CLIP-associating protein 1

      CLASP1

      30

      Decrease

      Decrease

      PMID: 29937387

      Microtubule-associated protein RP/EB family member 3

      MAPRE3

      21

      Increase

      Decrease

      PMID: 20850319

      MAP7 domain containing 2 protein variant 2 (Fragment)

      MAP7D2

      8

      na

      na

      CAP-Gly domain-containing linker protein 4

      CLIP4

      2

      na

      na

      \*Minor comments:***

      Figure 1.

      typo in the figure legend: "interior (distance>300 μm) vs. periphery (50 μmThere appears to be a clear dip in EB1 density at 100 um (Figure 1C). What could be the cause of that?*

      Thank you for catching the typo. We corrected this to “periphery (distance>300 µm) vs. interior (50 µmFigure 2.

      Note that the distances used in Figure 2. to define 'interior' and 'periphery' are completely different than those in Figure 1. (Interior in Figure 1 is defined to be between 50 and 280 um from the MTOC, and exterior larger than 300 um. However, in Figure 2. interior is defined as less than 100 um, and exterior as larger than 200 um.) Given that the asters are actively growing, it would be good to clearly explain how these intervals were defined in each case.

      For both experiments, we had clearly stated the definitions of interior and periphery, either in the figure legends or in the methods section. We have added a new paragraph explaining why we could not choose exactly the same quantitative definitions for these two methods (please also see our reply to Reviewer #2 comment 1).

      In the periphery movie, there are several notable examples of apparent minus-end depolymerization and treadmilling. The authors state these are very rare - perhaps a quantification would be useful here?

      Thank you for pointing this out. We modified the sentence to reflect the outward depolymerization events in the periphery. “We observed few outward-moving depolymerization events (Reviewer #1 (Significance (Required)):

      The observation of distinct depolymerization rates within vs. at the periphery of microtubule asters is novel and interesting. However, the manuscript in its current form is rather preliminary. The observation can be significantly strengthened by additional experiments/analysis that would characterize the effect in more detail. Even more importantly, the authors propose a highly speculative (although compelling) mechanism, but make no attempt to test it in any way. This is a major deficiency of the current manuscript that should be addressed prior to publication.

      REFEREES CROSS COMMENTING

      I agree with Reviewer #2 that our comments are both overlapping and complementary. I also find Reviewer #2's comments fair and reasonable and see no need for further adjustments.

      RESPONSE TO REVIEWER #2

      Reviewer #2 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity (Required)):

      \*SUMMARY ***

      This paper reports measurements of microtubule dynamics in interphase asters nucleated in Xenopus egg extracts. Dynamics are measured using two methods. First tracking of GFP tagged EB1 protein forming comets at the tips of growing microtubules, as used in other studies, which can only measure growth rates. Second using a recently developed automated tracking based on subtractive difference images of fluorescently labelled microtubules, which can measure both growth and shrinkage rates. The main and novel observation of this paper, using difference image tracking, is that the MT shrinkage rate is ~2 fold faster in the interior of the aster compared with the periphery of the aster, whilst rates of MT polymerisation and catastrophe vary only slightly, if at all. The authors speculate that this might be due to a reduced MAP concentration and occupancy in the aster interior. They also discuss the role of a depletion-dependent increased shrinkage rate as a feedback mechanism to maintain a low MT polymer density in the aster interior.

      \*MAJOR COMMENTS***

      The movies are startling in their beauty and clarity and the key conclusion that the shrinkage rate is significantly faster in the interior compared to the periphery of the aster is convincing.

      The observation that the rate of net MT plus end growth rate is ~10% faster at the periphery compared to interior of the aster is only supported by EB1 tip tracking method. The difference imaging method shows no significant difference in rates. The authors need to discuss this discrepancy between the established and new methods of analysis. It is insufficient to state that the growth rates obtained by the two methods are "consistent".

      This comment prompts the comparison of the two methods (EB1 vs. TIRF difference imaging). On one hand, EB1 tracking is more sensitive in detecting plus ends, and allows large N observations so it is likely to show statistical significance. On the other hand, EB1 tracking method is noisier (higher standard deviation) than the TIRF based measurements (see our response to Reviewer #1). In the TIRF difference imaging, the exact location of the periphery (relative to the center as well as the overall microtubule density profile) is hard to evaluate.

      What is consistent between the two methods is the approximate mean value of polymerization rates. The 10% faster polymerization velocity is only suggested by the EB1 tracking method, calling for caution/further investigation. However, the potential relatively small difference in polymerization rate is not the main point of this paper.

      We deleted the sentence in the results section for the TIRF method: “These values of polymerization rates are consistent with EB1 comet tracking (Fig. 1). ” We have added a new paragraph discussing the discrepancies between the methods in reporting polymerization rate.

      The discussion proposing MAP depletion-dependent increased shrinkage rate as a feedback mechanism to limit MT polymer density is reasonable.

      The model and discussion of the role of MAPs might be criticised as highly speculative and unsupported by any experimental data. The authors do acknowledge this. Whether the ratio of data to speculative interpretation is appropriate will be an editorial decision for whichever journal ultimately hosts this.

      Thank you. This is exactly the kind of comments that we wanted to hear from an initiative like Review Commons. This helps us gauge how our work is received and decide which journal to submit our work.

      In particular since the aster forms by growth from the nucleating bead, early in its formation the final interior MTs must have first formed the peripheral MTs and could therefore enter fresh media and bind MAPs. The authors show by calculation that as the aster expands, these MTs and MAPs become isolated from mixing with the external media. This isolation would then suggest that any MAPS released by dissociation or MT depolymerisation must remain in the interior, and are therefore available to rebind to newly formed MTs. So, it is unclear why the MAPs should be depleted in the interior compared to the periphery, unless expansion of the Aster is slowed in which case additional MAPs could diffuse into the stationary periphery from the surrounding media. The kinetics of MT growth, MAP binding and aster expansion would then also be expected to have an effect on the outcome beyond a simple "depletion" of the internal MAP concentration.

      We use the term “depletion” to mean a significant decrease of MAP from the cytoplasm. As outlined in our toy model, more MTs lead to more MAP binding and depletion of soluble MAPs. Note that the total local abundance of MAP is constant unless there is significant diffusive transport of MAP from one region to another. We argue this transport is ineffective for the large length scale of interphase asters.

      It is also not clear how the authors preferred model would account for the suggestion of bimodal shrinkage rates. It is not clear if this is a simplification (binning things in to external and internal) applied for the purposes of discussion.

      Please see our comment to Reviewer #1. We now believe there is no evidence for bimodality of depolymerization rates. The spread of the data reflects the variability of depolymerization rates in a given a field of view as well as the variability across multiple fields of view.

      \*MINOR COMMENTS***

      Line 71

      Authors reference Gardner et al 2011, when discussing depolymerisation as a zero order process, as showing a free tubulin dimer concentration effect on shrinkage rates. However, the results in Gardner refer to the off rate during MT polymerisation, and measurements of rapid small scale events during overall growth phases and would be applicable to GTP-heterodimers, whereas the extended shrinkage events measured in this paper would presumably apply to post-catastrophe GDP-heterodimer dissociation and may not be comparable. The reference should be omitted or a further explanation given.

      Thanks, good point. We wanted to cite Gardner et al (2011) to make the point that classic assembly models may not always hold, but the reviewer is correct, that paper only looked at concentration dependence of depolymerization at growing ends. The text was changed to:

      “This assumption has been questioned for growing ends (Gardner 2011)​, but not for shrinking ends to our knowledge.”

      Line 89

      States "density of plus ends is approximately homogenous within interphase asters"

      However, in results section it is stated Line 111 that "the plus end density is lower at the periphery compared to the aster center".

      Please clarify

      The plus end density is approximately homogenous from the center to the periphery of the aster. However, only at the most peripheral region, where there are few microtubules, the density drops.

      Line 135

      The distances given for the interior and periphery appear to be mixed up.

      Thank you, we corrected this.

      Line277

      "approximately consistent with our Peclet number estimate". 50µm gives a Pe value of 2.8. The Peclat number "significance" is earlier given in terms of "Pe>>1" (Line255). Please clarify what range of experimental values is required for the argument to hold.

      Our statement was unclear. We modified the sentence in the following way to clarify our point: “The half-width of the depleted zone extended ~50 microns beyond the growing aster periphery, which is smaller than the typical aster radius. This analysis indicated that soluble protein levels may vary between subregions of growing asters due to subunit consumption.”

      Line 404

      needs details of the GFP-EB1 and fluorescent tubulin used in this experiment.

      The detailed concentrations are described for each method in the subsequent sections. To avoid confusion, we removed the sentence in line 404, which omitted details.

      The tubulin depletion measurements detect a 4% reduction in tubulin concentration in the interior versus the exterior, and the same for eGFP-EB1 (Fig.4B). This observation provides important support for the depletion proposal. But the experiments apparently lack a control for potential reduction of fluorescence excitation intensity with depth in these deep specimens (equivalent to the inner filter effect in spectroscopy). Is there a component whose apparent concentration (fluorescence emission intensity) does not decrease by 4% in the interior of the aster?

      Indeed, fluorescent intensity measurements require special attention. Our samples are made by squashing 4 ul of extract under a 18 mm x 18 mm coverslip and the resulting thickness is 10 micron, which we believe is a distance that is too small to result in an inner filter effect.

      In response to Reviewer #2’s request for an example of a component whose fluorescence intensity is uniform, we provide the intensity profile of the inert 10kDa Dextran labeled with Alexa568. This serves as a control for the reviewer’s specific concern with our method. We will incorporate this as a supplementary figure in the revision.

      There is no direct discussion of the relative lifetime of MTs in the interior compared to the exterior of the aster. Catastrophe rates and growth rates are essentially invariant, I think this implies that MT lifetimes are essentially the same in the interior versus the exterior? Please confirm and estimate the lifetime. This could exclude a maturation process whereby one set of MAPs got replaced by another over time?

      Indeed, MT lifetime is a function of four rates: polymerization, depolymerization, catastrophe, and rescue. The figure below shows the MT lifetime as a function of depolymerization rate, assuming other parameters are fixed at what we found in our previous report Ishihara et al. 2016. In regions of fast depolymerization rate 40 µm/min, the microtubule lifetime is 0.98 min. As the depolymerization rate decreases to 30 and 25 µm/min, the lifetime increases to 1.5 and 2.4 min. This implies that the microtubules at the aster periphery are longer lived than those in the interior.

      Association and dissociation rate constants have not been measured for most MAPs, but in general we expect them to be fast compared to the timescale of MT lifetime of ~1 minute. Most MAPs bind in the low micromolar or high nM regime, which implies dissociation rates of seconds or less. MAP4 and MAP7 were both shown to bind and dissociate rapidly in living cells (PMID: 16714020, PMID: 11719555)

      Reviewer #2 (Significance (Required)):

      This paper is significant as it is the first observation of spatial variation in MT shrinkage rates in an aster. It proposes the broad shape of an underlying mechanism (depletion of stabilising MAPS in the aster interior) and presents sound quantitative arguments, but the experiments do not directly test this mechanism. Aster formation in Xenopus egg extracts is widely used as a model system, and if indeed the spatial variation turns out to be due to spatial depletion of components then this will become a landmark paper. The paper may promote wider use of this method of automated analysis and encourage study of shrinkage rate mechanisms in other systems.

      REFEREES CROSS COMMENTING

      In my opinion the comments of reviewer #1 are fair and reasonable and overlap with and complement my own. In my opinion there is zero conflict requiring adjustment.

    1. All this is to say that rivers are taken to be constituents of the earth and not products of human enterprise.

      Contextualize

      Human civilization is heavily dependent on nature, and for a long time, humans have not realized that. Humans, everything nature, have they can control and rule it for their own needs. However, is it like that? The answer is no. The river has played a significant role in human civilization. It is almost impossible to have it developed without a river. Cunha state that in the reading, "All this is to say that rivers are taken to be constituents of the earth and not products of human enterprise" (pp.4). Many civilizations are developed next to the river. For example, the Egyptians developed their civilization next to the Nile River. Without it, this civilization may not be existing at all. In "Ancient Egyptian civilization," the author states, "This annual flooding was vital to agriculture because it deposited a new layer of nutrient-rich soil each year. In years when the Nile did not flood, the nutrient level in the soil was seriously depleted, and the chance of food shortages increased greatly. Food supplies had political effects, as well, and periods of drought probably contributed to the decline of Egyptian political unity at the ends of both the Old and Middle Kingdoms". Agriculture is one of the essential elements for human civilization to be continued, and the river is the crucial element for it to happen. At some point time, I think humans have underestimated the power of the river. Like Cunha, the river is not the human enterprise's product, but it is the earth's constituents.

      Cunha, D. D. (2019). Ocean of Rain. In The invention of rivers: Alexander’s eye and Ganga’s descent (pp.1-19). Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.

      Ancient Egyptian civilization (article). (n.d.). Retrieved October 15, 2020, from https://www.khanacademy.org/humanities/world-history/world-history-beginnings/ancient-egypt-hittites/a/egypt-article

      Relate

      There has always been this argument: the river constructed by man or it was naturally existing. Many conservationists believe that river was naturally living, and human civilization has been highly dependent on it. We have been underestimating the power of the river. Many other people believe that humans are the one that brought in value to the river. Without human constructions, the river will be a river.

      However, there is a balance between these two extremes believes. It is not only about the physical phenomenon but also about how we use or act it. We cannot underestimate the value and power of the river, but we also have to do the right construction to maximize the value. Gain useful knowledge of the river before we take in human action is the right way to do.

    1. I don't think there is a solution to the white public space problem until society acknowledges the fact that everyone has their own kind of dialect. In America, there is a large amount of diversity, but we live under the impression that we have to speak a certain way to get opportunities. People of the Hispanic culture often get let down because of the fact that they speak Spanish in public. Some white people in our country feel that they are inferior to minorities because of the privilege they are given. This causes them to look down on those who may not be in the same situation that they are in. In order to create a solution for this problem. as citizens living in America, we have to accept the fact that people represent their cultures differently. We can't discriminate against those who don't "act white" or follow those professional values.

    1. When you are worried, sad, stressed, frustrated or anything else, trust that you are almost certainly having the “right” feeling. I say this because you have been raised in a culture that is unnecessarily fearful of unpleasant emotions and which may have given you the impression that emotional distress invariably signals fragile mental health. This is not true. In upsetting times, feeling upset proves, if anything, that your emotions are working exactly as they should. You are in touch with reality — a painful one though it is — and attuned to your circumstances.When your mood is good, trust that too. With the world off its axis, you might wonder if it’s all right to let yourself feel at ease. It is. Should you notice that calm emotional waters follow stormy waves of distress, don’t assume that you have somehow lost touch. In all likelihood, you have processed and moved past a painful mood, largely by allowing yourself to have it.

      This is TEA structure. It starts off with listing negative emotions and the topic sentence is saying that in the case of negative emotions, trust them/ yourself. The second sentence is telling us why this could be a thing. It could also be an example. Then it gives us an example of feelings that we could have and how we can deal with them. I think that counts as an example.

    1. Note: This rebuttal was posted by the corresponding author to Review Commons. Content has not been altered except for formatting.

      Learn more at Review Commons


      Reply to the reviewers

      We would like to express our appreciation for both the Editors’ and Reviewers’ efforts as essential contributions to the peer review process. We highly value the Reviewers’ constructive critique of our manuscript#RC-2020_00434R entitled “A drug repurposing screen identifies hepatitis C antivirals as inhibitors of the SARS-CoV2 main protease.__” __

      We appreciate the Reviewers’ thoughtful consideration of our work and feel their critiques and recommendations have significantly improved our manuscript. Taken together, we believe the additional data, clarification of data presentation, and revised discussion address the heart of the Reviewers’ previous concerns. Thus we feel the work is ready for reconsideration and will be an impactful addition to the literature appropriate for publication. Below we provide a breakdown and a point by point response to previous review critiques.

      Thank you for your attention. We look forward to your response.

      Best Wishes,

      Brian Kraemer, PhD ▪ Associate Director for Research Geriatric Research Education and Clinical Center ▪ Veterans Affairs Puget Sound Health Care System ▪ Research Professor ▪ Departments of Medicine, Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, and Pathology ▪ University of Washington ▪ 1660 South Columbian Way ▪ Seattle, WA 98108 ▪ Phone 206-277-1071 ▪ www.kraemerlab.uw.edu

      Reviewer #1 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity (Required)):

      In this manuscript, Baker et al. report the screening of a collection of ~6,070 drugs for their inhibitory activity against the enzymatic activity of the SARS-SoV-2 Mpro protein in vitro using two peptide substrates. 50 compounds with activity against Mpro were identified and tested for their dose-dependent effect in the same assay. Several hits were identified, among which are approved drugs that target the HCV protease.

      Indeed, there is an urgent need for effective drugs for SARS-CoV-2 infection, and high throughput screenings can discover novel candidates. However, the novelty of this work is quite limited, as former screens have been published with the same target using the same substrates. Moreover, as discussed below the translational impact of the hits discussed is also quite limited, particularly in the absence of antiviral data. Lastly, there are several overstatements in the write up and it will require major editing.

      **Major comments:**

      1. Were there any positive controls previously shown to potently inhibit the SARS-CoV-2 Mpro included in the screen (e.g. ebselen)? How did these perform in this assay? When first designing our protease assay, we did use ebselen as the initial control. Ebselen showed low potency in all our in our assays and was not considered as a positive control subsequently. It should be noted that Ebselen failed to work against multiple substrates. It is possible that our buffer conditions prevented Ebselen activity. See data plotted below. After identifying boceprevir as a potent inhibitor, it was used in all subsequent assays as a positive control.

      It will be helpful if the authors would provide info re the 50 hits from prior screens conducted with this library of compounds - how promiscuous are they across screens? How toxic in cell based assays?

      We have updated the table to provide additional useful information as well as a footnote explaining statuses. The compounds in the Broad repurposing library are generally non-toxic and information about them can be found here: https://clue.io/repurposing

      The translational potential of the findings appears to be limited. The calculated IC50s for these drugs in the Mpro assay are very high (10-1000 fold higher) relative to their IC50 in an enzymatic assay involving the HCV protease (Boceprevir: IC50 = 0.95 μM vs. 0.084 μM in HCV), Ciluprevir (IC50 = 20.77 μM vs. 0.0087 in HCV), Telaprevir (IC50 = 15.25 μM vs.0.050 μM in HCV) (https://aac.asm.org/content/aac/57/12/6236.full.pdf ). In the absence of antiviral data, the main statement of the manuscript that "the work presented here supports the rapid evaluation of previous HCV NS3/4A inhibitors for repurposing as a COVID-19 therapy." is thus an overstatement. Even is there is some activity, since likely to be limited, as with the HIV protease inhibitors, its chances to elicit a meaningful clinical effect is low. Moreover, when used in monotherapy, some of these protease inhibitors have a very low genetic barrier to resistance.

      We have reworked the discussion to incorporate these concerns and limitations of our results.

      There are additional inaccurate or overstatements - e.g. line 61 "Probably the most successful approved antivirals are protease inhibitors such as atazanavir for HIV-1 and simeprevir for hepatitis C. [reviewed in 10 and 11]."

      We have reworded this statement: (Page 4, Lines 61-62)

      “There is precedence for targeting the protease, as this approach has been successful in treating both HIV-1 and hepatitis C (10,11).”

      The manuscript requires editing - e.g. structure of sentences, commas, spacing (including in the abstract) etc.

      The manuscript has been re-proofed throughout (see tracked changes version of manuscript)

      What is the take home message? The statement "Taken together this work suggests previous large-scale commercial drug development initiatives targeting hepatitis C NS3/4A viral protease should be revisited because some previous lead compounds may be more potent against SARS-CoV-2 Mpro than Boceprevir and suitable for rapid repurposing." is unclear.

      The take home message of the manuscript is that HCV-targeting protease inhibitors have potential in blocking the SARS-Cov2 protease and a more thorough analysis of the space is needed. As the reviewer pointed out, the identified hits boceprevir and narlaprevir are less potent when targeting the SARS-Cov2 protease as compared to the HCV protease. However, we believe this work does show the potential for screening HCV-targeting protease inhibitors that may not have made it to the clinic. For instance, Boceprevir or Narlaprevir analogs may be even more potent against Mrpo. Further, we believe that these compounds would benefit from further optimization through medicinal chemistry.

      We have expanded the discussion to incorporate issues brought up here and in point 3.

      Reviewer #1 (Significance (Required)):

      Limited. As discussed above

      Reviewer #2 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity (Required)):

      SARS-CoV-2 pandemic causing serious health crisis globally. There are no specific medicine or vaccines to contain this virus currently. To address this issue, the authors developed one efficient fluorescent Mpro assay system and screened ~6070 previous used drugs in this article. Several compounds with activity against SARS-CoV-2 Mpro in vitro were founded. Most hits are hepatitis C NS3/4A protease inhibitors with fair IC50 value. Besides, the authors found that most identified compounds in in silico screen lack activity against Mpro in kinetic protease assays.

      These research results are well proved and reproducible. But there are two minor questions I present below:

      1. In your Mpro assay optimization process you said substrate MCA-AVLQSGFR-K(Dnp)- K-NH2 had drastically lower rates of Mpro catalyzed hydrolysis and were not considered further in your assay development. And in your Fig.1 I saw extremely low RFU changes. But several nice inhibitors were screened using this substrate that was reported in April. Can you explain this result? The substrates used in our assay appear to be much more efficiently cleaved at least with our buffer conditions and Mpro concentrations tested. Variables including recombinant Mpro purity and activity, differences in assay buffer, reader sensitivity may all play a role, but our best guess is that the substrate identified by Marcin Drag’s group (https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.29.068890), is more readily cleaved by Mpro. Although screening with other reported substrates is feasible given previous results, we believe the Ac-Abu-Tle-Leu-Gln-AFC to be superior for use in high throughput screening because of its superior cleavage kinetics yielding an improved signal to background ratio for HTS.

      To exclude inhibitors possibly acting as aggregators, a detergent-based control should do at the same time when you do IC50 value measurement.

      Compound aggregation is a concern, and our assays were all run with detergent in the buffer. Our buffer composition was 20mM Tris pH 7.8, 150mM NaCl, 1mM EDTA, 1mM DTT, 0.05% Triton X-100.

      Reviewer #2 (Significance (Required)):

      Nice work but the significance of this article is losing now. Most screened hits are reported in the last serval months. Some inhibitor complex structures have been published or released on Protein Data Bank. The novelty is missing. I suggest the authors add more results and resubmit it again.

      **Referees Cross-commenting**

      I agree with the other two reviewers' comments. The significance of this work is losing but still has something interest. I think it can be published in the lower-impact journal if they complete our suggestions

      We concur with both reviewers that demonstration of antiviral activity would strengthen the impact of the manuscript. However, this work remains outside of the scope of feasibility at our institution. We believe that our screen and hit identification can stand on their own until further translational work can be completed.

      Reviewer #3 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity (Required)):

      In this report, Baker et al. show that four inhibitors of hepatitis C virus (HCV) NS3/4 protease (ciluprevir, boceprevir, narlaprevir and telaprevir) are also effective inhibitors of the SARS-CoV-2 main protease (Mpro) in enzymatic assays, with lower IC50 values for narlaprevir and boceprevir (around 1 µM in their assay conditions). HCV NS3/4 inhibitors were identified after screening a library of >6,000 compounds of the Broad Institute, including approved drugs. Screening was done with fluorometric proteolytic assays.

      Experiments have been apparently well-done and results are sound. The manuscript needs editing.

      Reviewer #3 (Significance (Required)):

      Experiments have been apparently well-done and results are sound. However, this is a limited study since there are no data obtained in cell culture and a comparison of IC50 values of the selected drugs against HCV and SARS-CoV-2 proteases is missing. It is difficult to infer whether the drugs would be equally effective against SARS-CoV-2 than against HCV, and otherwise, how much should the doses increase in order to have a therapeutic effect.

      The manuscript needs editing (see below) and the Discussion is poor. The results reported by authors are not new, and a discussion of the effects of HCV inhibitors on SARS-CoV-2 replication, based on previous publications is necessary to provide the appropriate context for the study.

      Here are some references on Covid-19 and HCV inhibitors, that in my opinion should be considered for discussion and proper citation. As correctly pointed out by Baker and co- workers, docking studies should be considered with caution, though.

      We appreciate the feedback and have now reworked and expanded the discussion to incorporate reviewer #1 and #3 comments and suggestions.

      1: Ghahremanpour MM, Tirado-Rives J, Deshmukh M, Ippolito JA, Zhang CH, de Vaca IC, Liosi ME, Anderson KS, Jorgensen WL. Identification of 14 Known Drugs as Inhibitors of the Main Protease of SARS-CoV-2. bioRxiv [Preprint]. 2020 Aug 28:2020.08.28.271957. doi: 10.1101/2020.08.28.271957. PMID: 32869018; PMCID: PMC7457600.

      2: Sacco MD, Ma C, Lagarias P, Gao A, Townsend JA, Meng X, Dube P, Zhang X, Hu Y, Kitamura N, Hurst B, Tarbet B, Marty MT, Kolocouris A, Xiang Y, Chen Y, Wang J. Structure and inhibition of the SARS-CoV-2 main protease reveals strategy for developing dual inhibitors against Mpro and cathepsin L. bioRxiv [Preprint]. 2020 Jul 27:2020.07.27.223727. doi: 10.1101/2020.07.27.223727. PMID: 32766590; PMCID: PMC7402059.

      3: Ma C, Sacco MD, Hurst B, Townsend JA, Hu Y, Szeto T, Zhang X, Tarbet B, Marty MT, Chen Y, Wang J. Boceprevir, GC-376, and calpain inhibitors II, XII inhibit SARS-CoV-2viral replication by targeting the viral main protease. Cell Res. 2020 Aug;30(8):678-692. doi: 10.1038/s41422-020-0356-z. Epub 2020 Jun 15. PMID: 32541865; PMCID: PMC7294525.

      4: Ke YY, Peng TT, Yeh TK, Huang WZ, Chang SE, Wu SH, Hung HC, Hsu TA, Lee SJ, Song JS, Lin WH, Chiang TJ, Lin JH, Sytwu HK, Chen CT. Artificial intelligence approach fighting COVID-19 with repurposing drugs. Biomed J. 2020 May 15:S2319- 4170(20)30049-4. doi: 10.1016/j.bj.2020.05.001. Epub ahead of print. PMID: 32426387; PMCID: PMC7227517.

      5: Elzupir AO. Inhibition of SARS-CoV-2 main protease 3CLpro by means of α-ketoamide and pyridone-containing pharmaceuticals using in silico molecular docking. J Mol Struct. 2020 Dec 15;1222:128878. doi: 10.1016/j.molstruc.2020.128878. Epub 2020 Jul 10.

      PMID: 32834113; PMCID: PMC7347502.

      Additional computational studies:

      1: Hosseini FS, Amanlou M. Anti-HCV and anti-malaria agent, potential candidates to repurpose for coronavirus infection: Virtual screening, molecular docking, and molecular dynamics simulation study. Life Sci. 2020 Aug 8;258:118205. doi:10.1016/j.lfs.2020.118205. Epub ahead of print. PMID: 32777300; PMCID:PMC7413873.

      2: Hakmi M, Bouricha EM, Kandoussi I, Harti JE, Ibrahimi A. Repurposing of known anti- virals as potential inhibitors for SARS-CoV-2 main protease using molecular docking analysis. Bioinformation. 2020 Apr 30;16(4):301-306. doi:10.6026/97320630016301.

      PMID: 32773989; PMCID: PMC7392094.

      3: Chtita S, Belhassan A, Aouidate A, Belaidi S, Bouachrine M, Lakhlifi T. Discovery of Potent SARS-CoV-2 Inhibitors from Approved Antiviral Drugs via Docking Screening. Comb Chem High Throughput Screen. 2020 Jul 30. doi:10.2174/1386207323999200730205447. Epub ahead of print. PMID: 32748740.

      4: Alamri MA, Tahir Ul Qamar M, Mirza MU, Bhadane R, Alqahtani SM, Muneer I, Froeyen M, Salo-Ahen OMH. Pharmacoinformatics and molecular dynamics simulation studies reveal potential covalent and FDA-approved inhibitors of SARS-CoV-2 main protease 3CLpro. J Biomol Struct Dyn. 2020 Jun 24:1-13. doi:10.1080/07391102.2020.1782768. Epub ahead of print. PMID: 32579061; PMCID:PMC7332866.

      5: Bafna K, Krug RM, Montelione GT. Structural Similarity of SARS-CoV2 Mpro and HCV NS3/4A Proteases Suggests New Approaches for Identifying Existing Drugs Useful as COVID-19 Therapeutics. ChemRxiv [Preprint]. 2020 Apr 21. doi: 10.26434/chemrxiv.12153615. PMID: 32511291; PMCID: PMC7263768.

      6: Eleftheriou P, Amanatidou D, Petrou A, Geronikaki A. In Silico Evaluation of the Effectivity of Approved Protease Inhibitors against the Main Protease of the Novel SARS- CoV-2 Virus. Molecules. 2020 May 29;25(11):2529. doi:10.3390/molecules25112529.

      PMID: 32485894; PMCID: PMC7321236.

      7: Wang J. Fast Identification of Possible Drug Treatment of Coronavirus Disease-19 (COVID-19) through Computational Drug Repurposing Study. J Chem Inf Model. 2020 Jun 22;60(6):3277-3286. doi: 10.1021/acs.jcim.0c00179. Epub 2020 May 4. PMID: 32315171; PMCID: PMC7197972.

      8: Chen YW, Yiu CB, Wong KY. Prediction of the SARS-CoV-2 (2019-nCoV) 3C-like protease (3CL pro) structure: virtual screening reveals velpatasvir, ledipasvir, and other drug repurposing candidates. F1000Res. 2020 Feb 21;9:129. doi: 10.12688/f1000research.22457.2. PMID: 32194944; PMCID: PMC7062204.

      Minor comments:

      We appreciate the time that the reviewer has taken to address grammatical changes and have addressed each throughout the manuscript with tracked changes.

      p.2, line 26: > appears as an attractive

      Manuscript edited

      p.2, line 27: > we show that the existing

      Manuscript edited

      p.2, line 33: > separate numbers and units, eg. 1.10 µM (this is a persisting error that should be corrected throughout the whole ms)

      Manuscript edited

      p.4, line 44: SARS virus should be referred as to SARS-CoV-1 throughout the whole manuscript. MERS-CoV is the name of the virus causing MERS

      Manuscript edited

      p.4, lines 61-62: > the selection of the specific compounds seems to be arbitrary... why atazanavir and not darunavir or other? The sentence should be rewritten.

      Rewritten as: “There is precedence for targeting the protease, as this approach has been successful in treating both HIV-1 and hepatitis C.”

      p.6, line 100: Citing Fig. 2B before completing the description of Fig. 1 is distracting. Authors should think of a better way to describe their results.

      This was a mistake and should have cited Fig 1B. Thank you for catching this.

      p.7, line 116: It is not clear what "10m-20,810" means

      This has been clarified to state: “ΔRFU at 10 minutes = 20,810 relative fluorescence units”

      p.7, lines 125-126: These sentences belong to an introduction, not appropriate in results section.

      We have removed these sentences.

      Figure 2. Part A is not necessary in results (ok for introduction). Black and purple dots in part B is not a good choice since they are difficult to distinguish, maybe orange and black is better.

      We have removed panel A, expanded the size of panel B and changed the color.

      Table 1: Status should be explained in a footnote (i.e the distinction between launched, P2/P3, phase 2, preclinical is not clear).

      The one compound indicated in P2/P3 development is now Phase 3 and the table has been updated. We have added a footnote:

      *Launched = compound approved for humans, though may only be approved for veterinary use in some countries

      Discussion. I think that subheadings are not necessary.

      Subheadings have been removed from the discussion.

      **Referees cross-commenting** I agree with reviewer no. 1 on the limited interest of the study. However, it could be published in a specialized lower-impact journal after addressing issues raised by reviewers 2 and 3 (likely to be completed in less than a month)

    2. Note: This preprint has been reviewed by subject experts for Review Commons. Content has not been altered except for formatting.

      Learn more at Review Commons


      Referee #3

      Evidence, reproducibility and clarity

      In this report, Baker et al. show that four inhibitors of hepatitis C virus (HCV) NS3/4 protease (ciluprevir, boceprevir, narlaprevir and telaprevir) are also effective inhibitors of the SARS-CoV-2 main protease (Mpro) in enzymatic assays, with lower IC50 values for narlaprevir and boceprevir (around 1 µM in their assay conditions). HCV NS3/4 inhibitors were identified after screening a library of >6,000 compounds of the Broad Institute, including approved drugs. Screening was done with fluorometric proteolytic assays.

      Experiments have been apparently well-done and results are sound. The manuscript needs editing.

      Significance

      Experiments have been apparently well-done and results are sound. However, this is a limited study since there are no data obtained in cell culture and a comparison of IC50 values of the selected drugs against HCV and SARS-CoV-2 proteases is missing. It is difficult to infer whether the drugs would be equally effective against SARS-CoV-2 than against HCV, and otherwise, how much should the doses increase in order to have a therapeutic effect. The manuscript needs editing (see below) and the Discussion is poor. The results reported by authors are not new, and a discussion of the effects of HCV inhibitors on SARS-CoV-2 replication, based on previous publications is necessary to provide the appropriate context for the study. Here are some references on Covid-19 and HCV inhibitors, that in my opinion should be considered for discussion and proper citation. As correctly pointed out by Baker and co-workers, docking studies should be considered with caution, though.

      1: Ghahremanpour MM, Tirado-Rives J, Deshmukh M, Ippolito JA, Zhang CH, de Vaca IC, Liosi ME, Anderson KS, Jorgensen WL. Identification of 14 Known Drugs as Inhibitors of the Main Protease of SARS-CoV-2. bioRxiv [Preprint]. 2020 Aug 28:2020.08.28.271957. doi: 10.1101/2020.08.28.271957. PMID: 32869018; PMCID: PMC7457600.

      2: Sacco MD, Ma C, Lagarias P, Gao A, Townsend JA, Meng X, Dube P, Zhang X, Hu Y, Kitamura N, Hurst B, Tarbet B, Marty MT, Kolocouris A, Xiang Y, Chen Y, Wang J. Structure and inhibition of the SARS-CoV-2 main protease reveals strategy for developing dual inhibitors against M<sup>pro</sup> and cathepsin L. bioRxiv [Preprint]. 2020 Jul 27:2020.07.27.223727. doi: 10.1101/2020.07.27.223727. PMID: 32766590; PMCID: PMC7402059.

      3: Ma C, Sacco MD, Hurst B, Townsend JA, Hu Y, Szeto T, Zhang X, Tarbet B, Marty MT, Chen Y, Wang J. Boceprevir, GC-376, and calpain inhibitors II, XII inhibit SARS-CoV-2 viral replication by targeting the viral main protease. Cell Res. 2020 Aug;30(8):678-692. doi: 10.1038/s41422-020-0356-z. Epub 2020 Jun 15. PMID: 32541865; PMCID: PMC7294525.

      4: Ke YY, Peng TT, Yeh TK, Huang WZ, Chang SE, Wu SH, Hung HC, Hsu TA, Lee SJ, Song JS, Lin WH, Chiang TJ, Lin JH, Sytwu HK, Chen CT. Artificial intelligence approach fighting COVID-19 with repurposing drugs. Biomed J. 2020 May 15:S2319-4170(20)30049-4. doi: 10.1016/j.bj.2020.05.001. Epub ahead of print. PMID: 32426387; PMCID: PMC7227517.

      5: Elzupir AO. Inhibition of SARS-CoV-2 main protease 3CLpro by means of α-ketoamide and pyridone-containing pharmaceuticals using in silico molecular docking. J Mol Struct. 2020 Dec 15;1222:128878. doi: 10.1016/j.molstruc.2020.128878. Epub 2020 Jul 10. PMID: 32834113; PMCID: PMC7347502.

      Additional computational studies:

      1: Hosseini FS, Amanlou M. Anti-HCV and anti-malaria agent, potential candidates to repurpose for coronavirus infection: Virtual screening, molecular docking, and molecular dynamics simulation study. Life Sci. 2020 Aug 8;258:118205. doi:10.1016/j.lfs.2020.118205. Epub ahead of print. PMID: 32777300; PMCID:PMC7413873.

      2: Hakmi M, Bouricha EM, Kandoussi I, Harti JE, Ibrahimi A. Repurposing of known anti-virals as potential inhibitors for SARS-CoV-2 main protease using molecular docking analysis. Bioinformation. 2020 Apr 30;16(4):301-306. doi:10.6026/97320630016301. PMID: 32773989; PMCID: PMC7392094.

      3: Chtita S, Belhassan A, Aouidate A, Belaidi S, Bouachrine M, Lakhlifi T. Discovery of Potent SARS-CoV-2 Inhibitors from Approved Antiviral Drugs via Docking Screening. Comb Chem High Throughput Screen. 2020 Jul 30. doi:10.2174/1386207323999200730205447. Epub ahead of print. PMID: 32748740.

      4: Alamri MA, Tahir Ul Qamar M, Mirza MU, Bhadane R, Alqahtani SM, Muneer I, Froeyen M, Salo-Ahen OMH. Pharmacoinformatics and molecular dynamics simulation studies reveal potential covalent and FDA-approved inhibitors of SARS-CoV-2 main protease 3CL<sup>pro</sup>. J Biomol Struct Dyn. 2020 Jun 24:1-13. doi:10.1080/07391102.2020.1782768. Epub ahead of print. PMID: 32579061; PMCID:PMC7332866.

      5: Bafna K, Krug RM, Montelione GT. Structural Similarity of SARS-CoV2 M<sup>pro</sup> and HCV NS3/4A Proteases Suggests New Approaches for Identifying Existing Drugs Useful as COVID-19 Therapeutics. ChemRxiv [Preprint]. 2020 Apr 21. doi: 10.26434/chemrxiv.12153615. PMID: 32511291; PMCID: PMC7263768.

      6: Eleftheriou P, Amanatidou D, Petrou A, Geronikaki A. In Silico Evaluation of the Effectivity of Approved Protease Inhibitors against the Main Protease of the Novel SARS-CoV-2 Virus. Molecules. 2020 May 29;25(11):2529. doi:10.3390/molecules25112529. PMID: 32485894; PMCID: PMC7321236.

      7: Wang J. Fast Identification of Possible Drug Treatment of Coronavirus Disease-19 (COVID-19) through Computational Drug Repurposing Study. J Chem Inf Model. 2020 Jun 22;60(6):3277-3286. doi: 10.1021/acs.jcim.0c00179. Epub 2020 May 4. PMID: 32315171; PMCID: PMC7197972.

      8: Chen YW, Yiu CB, Wong KY. Prediction of the SARS-CoV-2 (2019-nCoV) 3C-like protease (3CL <sup>pro</sup>) structure: virtual screening reveals velpatasvir, ledipasvir, and other drug repurposing candidates. F1000Res. 2020 Feb 21;9:129. doi: 10.12688/f1000research.22457.2. PMID: 32194944; PMCID: PMC7062204.

      Minor comments:

      p.2, line 26: > appears as an attractive

      p.2, line 27: > we show that the existing

      p.2, line 33: > separate numbers and units, eg. 1.10 µM (this is a persisting error that should be corrected throughout the whole ms)

      p.4, line 44: SARS virus should be referred as to SARS-CoV-1 throughout the whole manuscript. MERS-CoV is the name of the virus causing MERS

      p.4, lines 61-62: > the selection of the specific compounds seems to be arbitrary... why atazanavir and not darunavir or other? The sentence should be rewritten.

      p.6, line 100: Citing Fig. 2B before completing the description of Fig. 1 is distracting. Authors should think of a better way to describe their results.

      p.7, line 116: It is not clear what "10m-20,810" means

      p.7, lines 125-126: These sentences belong to an introduction, not appropriate in results section.

      Figure 2. Part A is not necessary in results (ok for introduction). Black and purple dots in part B is not a good choice since they are difficult to distinguish, maybe orange and black is better.

      Table 1: Status should be explained in a footnote (i.e the distinction between launched, P2/P3, phase 2, preclinical is not clear).

      Discussion. I think that subheadings are not necessary.

      Referees cross-commenting

      I agree with reviewer no. 1 on the limited interest of the study. However, it could be published in a specialized lower-impact journal after addressing issues raised by reviewers 2 and 3 (likely to be completed in less than a month)

    1. about the footprint we may leave

      I think this is referring to our ecological footprint that we as humans leave on the Earth. The connotation of these few stanzas is very bleak and cold, suggesting the grim fate of the human race in the future.

    1. Cultural critic Stanley Fish come talkin bout—in his three-piece New York Times “What Should Colleges Teach?” suit—there only one way to speak and write to get ahead in the world, that writin teachers should “clear [they] mind of the ortho-doxies that have taken hold in the composition world” (“Part 3”). He say dont no student have a rite to they own language if that language make them “vulnerable to prejudice”; that “it may be true that the standard language is [...] a device for protecting the status quo, but that very truth is a reason for teaching it to students” (Fish “Part 3”).

      I think this paragraph is very important since it's almost or if not the introduction paragraph where he speaks freely and as I am reading this it's not perfect English, it's his English, and we all have different English.

    1. The first time I opened Peter Singer’s “Animal Liberation,” I was dining alone at the Palm, trying to enjoy a rib-eye steak cooked medium-rare. If this sounds like a good recipe for cognitive dissonance (if not indigestion), that was sort of the idea. Preposterous as it might seem, to supporters of animal rights, what I was doing was tantamount to reading “Uncle Tom’s Cabin” on a plantation in the Deep South in 1852. Singer and the swelling ranks of his followers ask us to imagine a future in which people will look back on my meal, and this steakhouse, as relics of an equally backward age. Eating animals, wearing animals, experimenting on animals, killing animals for sport: all these practices, so resolutely normal to us, will be seen as the barbarities they are, and we will come to view “speciesism”–a neologism I had encountered before only in jokes–as a form of discrimination as indefensible as racism or anti-Semitism. Even in 1975, when “Animal Liberation” was first published, Singer, an Australian philosopher now teaching at Princeton, was confident that he had the wind of history at his back. The recent civil rights past was prologue, as one liberation movement followed on the heels of another. Slowly but surely, the white man’s circle of moral consideration was expanded to admit first blacks, then women, then homosexuals. In each case, a group once thought to be so different from the prevailing “we” as to be undeserving of civil rights was, after a struggle, admitted to the club. Now it was animals’ turn. That animal liberation is the logical next step in the forward march of moral progress is no longer the fringe idea it was back in 1975. A growing and increasingly influential movement of philosophers, ethicists, law professors and activists are convinced that the great moral struggle of our time will be for the rights of animals. So far the movement has scored some of its biggest victories in Europe. Earlier this year, Germany became the first nation to grant animals a constitutional right: the words “and animals” were added to a provision obliging the state to respect and protect the dignity of human beings. The farming of animals for fur was recently banned in England. In several European nations, sows may no longer be confined to crates nor laying hens to “battery cages”–stacked wired cages so small the birds cannot stretch their wings. The Swiss are amending their laws to change the status of animals from “things” to “beings.” Though animals are still very much “things” in the eyes of American law, change is in the air. Thirty-seven states have recently passed laws making some forms of animal cruelty a crime, 21 of them by ballot initiative. Following protests by activists, McDonald’s and Burger King forced significant improvements in the way the U.S. meat industry slaughters animals. Agribusiness and the cosmetics and apparel industries are all struggling to defuse mounting public concerns over animal welfare. Once thought of as a left-wing concern, the movement now cuts across ideological lines. Perhaps the most eloquent recent plea on behalf of animals, a new book called “Dominion,” was written by a former speechwriter for President Bush. And once outlandish ideas are finding their way into mainstream opinion. A recent Zogby poll found that 51 percent of Americans believe that primates are entitled to the same rights as human children. What is going on here? A certain amount of cultural confusion, for one thing. For at the same time many people seem eager to extend the circle of our moral consideration to animals, in our factory farms and laboratories we are inflicting more suffering on more animals than at any time in history. One by one, science is dismantling our claims to uniqueness as a species, discovering that such things as culture, tool making, language and even possibly self-consciousness are not the exclusive domain of Homo sapiens. Yet most of the animals we kill lead lives organized very much in the spirit of Descartes, who famously claimed that animals were mere machines, incapable of thought or feeling. There’s a schizoid quality to our relationship with animals, in which sentiment and brutality exist side by side. Half the dogs in America will receive Christmas presents this year, yet few of us pause to consider the miserable life of the pig–an animal easily as intelligent as a dog–that becomes the Christmas ham. We tolerate this disconnect because the life of the pig has moved out of view. When’s the last time you saw a pig? (Babe doesn’t count.) Except for our pets, real animals–animals living and dying–no longer figure in our everyday lives. Meat comes from the grocery store, where it is cut and packaged to look as little like parts of animals as possible. The disappearance of animals from our lives has opened a space in which there’s no reality check, either on the sentiment or the brutality. This is pretty much where we live now, with respect to animals, and it is a space in which the Peter Singers and Frank Perdues of the world can evidently thrive equally well. Several years ago, the English critic John Berger wrote an essay, “Why Look at Animals?” in which he suggested that the loss of everyday contact between ourselves and animals–and specifically the loss of eye contact–has left us deeply confused about the terms of our relationship to other species. That eye contact, always slightly uncanny, had provided a vivid daily reminder that animals were at once crucially like and unlike us; in their eyes we glimpsed something unmistakably familiar (pain, fear, tenderness) and something irretrievably alien. Upon this paradox people built a relationship in which they felt they could both honor and eat animals without looking away. But that accommodation has pretty much broken down; nowadays, it seems, we either look away or become vegetarians. For my own part, neither option seemed especially appetizing. Which might explain how I found myself reading “Animal Liberation” in a steakhouse. This is not something I’d recommend if you’re determined to continue eating meat. Combining rigorous philosophical argument with journalistic description, “Animal Liberation” is one of those rare books that demand that you either defend the way you live or change it. Because Singer is so skilled in argument, for many readers it is easier to change. His book has converted countless thousands to vegetarianism, and it didn’t take long for me to see why: within a few pages, he had succeeded in throwing me on the defensive. Singer’s argument is disarmingly simple and, if you accept its premises, difficult to refute. Take the premise of equality, which most people readily accept. Yet what do we really mean by it? People are not, as a matter of fact, equal at all–some are smarter than others, better looking, more gifted. “Equality is a moral idea,” Singer points out, “not an assertion of fact.” The moral idea is that everyone’s interests ought to receive equal consideration, regardless of “what abilities they may possess.” Fair enough; many philosophers have gone this far. But fewer have taken the next logical step. “If possessing a higher degree of intelligence does not entitle one human to use another for his or her own ends, how can it entitle humans to exploit nonhumans for the same purpose?” This is the nub of Singer’s argument, and right around here I began scribbling objections in the margin. But humans differ from animals in morally significant ways. Yes they do, Singer acknowledges, which is why we shouldn’t treat pigs and children alike. Equal consideration of interests is not the same as equal treatment, he points out: children have an interest in being educated; pigs, in rooting around in the dirt. But where their interests are the same, the principle of equality demands they receive the same consideration. And the one all-important interest that we share with pigs, as with all sentient creatures, is an interest in avoiding pain. Here Singer quotes a famous passage from Jeremy Bentham, the 18th-century utilitarian philosopher, that is the wellspring of the animal rights movement. Bentham was writing in 1789, soon after the French colonies freed black slaves, granting them fundamental rights. “The day may come,” he speculates, “when the rest of the animal creation may acquire those rights.” Bentham then asks what characteristic entitles any being to moral consideration. “Is it the faculty of reason or perhaps the faculty of discourse?” Obviously not, since “a full-grown horse or dog is beyond comparison a more rational, as well as a more conversable animal, than an infant.” He concludes: “The question is not, Can they reason? nor, Can they talk? but, Can they suffer?” Bentham here is playing a powerful card philosophers call the “argument from marginal cases,” or A.M.C. for short. It goes like this: there are humans–infants, the severely retarded, the demented–whose mental function cannot match that of a chimpanzee. Even though these people cannot reciprocate our moral attentions, we nevertheless include them in the circle of our moral consideration. So on what basis do we exclude the chimpanzee? Because he’s a chimp, I furiously scribbled in the margin, and they’re human! For Singer that’s not good enough. To exclude the chimp from moral consideration simply because he’s not human is no different from excluding the slave simply because he’s not white. In the same way we’d call that exclusion racist, the animal rightist contends that it is speciesist to discriminate against the chimpanzee solely because he’s not human. But the differences between blacks and whites are trivial compared with the differences between my son and a chimp. Singer counters by asking us to imagine a hypothetical society that discriminates against people on the basis of something nontrivial–say, intelligence. If that scheme offends our sense of equality, then why is the fact that animals lack certain human characteristics any more just as a basis for discrimination? Either we do not owe any justice to the severely retarded, he concludes, or we do owe it to animals with higher capabilities. This is where I put down my fork. If I believe in equality, and equality is based on interests rather than characteristics, then either I have to take the interests of the steer I’m eating into account or concede that I am a speciesist. For the time being, I decided to plead guilty as charged. I finished my steak. But Singer had planted a troubling notion, and in the days afterward, it grew and grew, watered by the other animal rights thinkers I began reading: the philosophers Tom Regan and James Rachels; the legal theorist Steven M. Wise; the writers Joy Williams and Matthew Scully. I didn’t think I minded being a speciesist, but could it be, as several of these writers suggest, that we will someday come to regard speciesism as an evil comparable to racism? Will history someday judge us as harshly as it judges the Germans who went about their ordinary lives in the shadow of Treblinka? Precisely that question was recently posed by J.M. Coetzee, the South African novelist, in a lecture delivered at Princeton; he answered it in the affirmative. If animal rightists are right, “a crime of stupefying proportions” (in Coetzee’s words) is going on all around us every day, just beneath our notice. It’s an idea almost impossible to entertain seriously, much less to accept, and in the weeks following my restaurant face-off between Singer and the steak, I found myself marshaling whatever mental power I could muster to try to refute it. Yet Singer and his allies managed to trump almost all my objections. My first line of defense was obvious. Animals kill one another all the time. Why treat animals more ethically than they treat one another? (Ben Franklin tried this one long before me: during a fishing trip, he wondered, “If you eat one another, I don’t see why we may not eat you.” He admits, however, that the rationale didn’t occur to him until the fish were in the frying pan, smelling “admirably well.” The advantage of being a “reasonable creature,” Franklin remarks, is that you can find a reason for whatever you want to do.) To the “they do it, too” defense, the animal rightist has a devastating reply: do you really want to base your morality on the natural order? Murder and rape are natural, too. Besides, humans don’t need to kill other creatures in order to survive; animals do. (Though if my cat, Otis, is any guide, animals sometimes kill for sheer pleasure.) This suggests another defense. Wouldn’t life in the wild be worse for these farm animals? “Defenders of slavery imposed on black Africans often made a similar point,” Singer retorts. “The life of freedom is to be preferred.” But domesticated animals can’t survive in the wild; in fact, without us they wouldn’t exist at all. Or as one 19th-century political philosopher put it, “The pig has a stronger interest than anyone in the demand for bacon. If all the world were Jewish, there would be no pigs at all.” But it turns out that this would be fine by the animal rightists: for if pigs don’t exist, they can’t be wronged. Animals on factory farms have never known any other life. Singer replies that “animals feel a need to exercise, stretch their limbs or wings, groom themselves and turn around, whether or not they have ever lived in conditions that permit this.” The measure of their suffering is not their prior experiences but the unremitting daily frustration of their instincts. O.K., the suffering of animals is a legitimate problem, but the world is full of problems, and surely human problems must come first! Sounds good, and yet all the animal people are asking me to do is to stop eating meat and wearing animal furs and hides. There’s no reason I can’t devote myself to solving humankind’s problems while being a vegetarian who wears synthetics. But doesn’t the fact that we could choose to forgo meat for moral reasons point to a crucial moral difference between animals and humans? As Kant pointed out, the human being is the only moral animal, the only one even capable of entertaining a concept of “rights.” What’s wrong with reserving moral consideration for those able to reciprocate it? Right here is where you run smack into the A.M.C.: the moral status of the retarded, the insane, the infant and the Alzheimer’s patient. Such “marginal cases,” in the detestable argot of modern moral philosophy, cannot participate in moral decision making any more than a monkey can, yet we nevertheless grant them rights. That’s right, I respond, for the simple reason that they’re one of us. And all of us have been, and will probably once again be, marginal cases ourselves. What’s more, these people have fathers and mothers, daughters and sons, which makes our interest in their welfare deeper than our interest in the welfare of even the most brilliant ape. Alas, none of these arguments evade the charge of speciesism; the racist, too, claims that it’s natural to give special consideration to one’s own kind. A utilitarian like Singer would agree, however, that the feelings of relatives do count for something. Yet the principle of equal consideration of interests demands that, given the choice between performing a painful medical experiment on a severely retarded orphan and on a normal ape, we must sacrifice the child. Why? Because the ape has a greater capacity for pain. Here in a nutshell is the problem with the A.M.C.: it can be used to help the animals, but just as often it winds up hurting the marginal cases. Giving up our speciesism will bring us to a moral cliff from which we may not be prepared to jump, even when logic is pushing us. And yet this isn’t the moral choice I am being asked to make. (Too bad; it would be so much easier!) In everyday life, the choice is not between babies and chimps but between the pork and the tofu. Even if we reject the “hard utilitarianism” of a Peter Singer, there remains the question of whether we owe animals that can feel pain any moral consideration, and this seems impossible to deny. And if we do owe them moral consideration, how can we justify eating them? This is why killing animals for meat (and clothing) poses the most difficult animal rights challenge. In the case of animal testing, all but the most radical animal rightists are willing to balance the human benefit against the cost to the animals. That’s because the unique qualities of human consciousness carry weight in the utilitarian calculus: human pain counts for more than that of a mouse, since our pain is amplified by emotions like dread; similarly, our deaths are worse than an animal’s because we understand what death is in a way they don’t. So the argument over animal testing is really in the details: is this particular procedure or test really necessary to save human lives? (Very often it’s not, in which case we probably shouldn’t do it.) But if humans no longer need to eat meat or wear skins, then what exactly are we putting on the human side of the scale to outweigh the interests of the animal? I suspect that this is finally why the animal people managed to throw me on the defensive. It’s one thing to choose between the chimp and the retarded child or to accept the sacrifice of all those pigs surgeons practiced on to develop heart-bypass surgery. But what happens when the choice is between “a lifetime of suffering for a nonhuman animal and the gastronomic preference of a human being?” You look away–or you stop eating animals. And if you don’t want to do either? Then you have to try to determine if the animals you’re eating have really endured “a lifetime of suffering.” Whether our interest in eating animals outweighs their interest in not being eaten (assuming for the moment that is their interest) turns on the vexed question of animal suffering. Vexed, because it is impossible to know what really goes on in the mind of a cow or a pig or even an ape. Strictly speaking, this is true of other humans, too, but since humans are all basically wired the same way, we have excellent reason to assume that other people’s experience of pain feels much like our own. Can we say that about animals? Yes and no. I have yet to find anyone who still subscribes to Descartes’s belief that animals cannot feel pain because they lack a soul. The general consensus among scientists and philosophers is that when it comes to pain, the higher animals are wired much like we are for the same evolutionary reasons, so we should take the writhings of the kicked dog at face value. Indeed, the very premise of a great deal of animal testing–the reason it has value–is that animals’ experience of physical and even some psychological pain closely resembles our own. Otherwise, why would cosmetics testers drip chemicals into the eyes of rabbits to see if they sting? Why would researchers study head trauma by traumatizing chimpanzee heads? Why would psychologists attempt to induce depression and “learned helplessness” in dogs by exposing them to ceaseless random patterns of electrical shock? That said, it can be argued that human pain differs from animal pain by an order of magnitude. This qualitative difference is largely the result of our possession of language and, by virtue of language, an ability to have thoughts about thoughts and to imagine alternatives to our current reality. The philosopher Daniel C. Dennett suggests that we would do well to draw a distinction between pain, which a great many animals experience, and suffering, which depends on a degree of self-consciousness only a few animals appear to command. Suffering in this view is not just lots of pain but pain intensified by human emotions like loss, sadness, worry, regret, self-pity, shame, humiliation and dread. Consider castration. No one would deny the procedure is painful to animals, yet animals appear to get over it in a way humans do not. (Some rhesus monkeys competing for mates will bite off a rival’s testicle; the very next day the victim may be observed mating, seemingly little the worse for wear.) Surely the suffering of a man able to comprehend the full implications of castration, to anticipate the event and contemplate its aftermath, represents an agony of another order. By the same token, however, language and all that comes with it can also make certain kinds of pain more bearable. A trip to the dentist would be a torment for an ape that couldn’t be made to understand the purpose and duration of the procedure. As humans contemplating the pain and suffering of animals, we do need to guard against projecting on to them what the same experience would feel like to us. Watching a steer force-marched up the ramp to the kill-floor door, as I have done, I need to remind myself that this is not Sean Penn in “Dead Man Walking,” that in a bovine brain the concept of nonexistence is blissfully absent. “If we fail to find suffering in the animal lives we can see,” Dennett writes in “Kinds of Minds,” “we can rest assured there is no invisible suffering somewhere in their brains. If we find suffering, we will recognize it without difficulty.” Which brings us–reluctantly, necessarily–to the American factory farm, the place where all such distinctions turn to dust. It’s not easy to draw lines between pain and suffering in a modern egg or confinement hog operation. These are places where the subtleties of moral philosophy and animal cognition mean less than nothing, where everything we’ve learned about animals at least since Darwin has been simply . . . set aside. To visit a modern CAFO (Confined Animal Feeding Operation) is to enter a world that, for all its technological sophistication, is still designed according to Cartesian principles: animals are machines incapable of feeling pain. Since no thinking person can possibly believe this any more, industrial animal agriculture depends on a suspension of disbelief on the part of the people who operate it and a willingness to avert your eyes on the part of everyone else. From everything I’ve read, egg and hog operations are the worst. Beef cattle in America at least still live outdoors, albeit standing ankle deep in their own waste eating a diet that makes them sick. And broiler chickens, although they do get their beaks snipped off with a hot knife to keep them from cannibalizing one another under the stress of their confinement, at least don’t spend their eight-week lives in cages too small to ever stretch a wing. That fate is reserved for the American laying hen, who passes her brief span piled together with a half-dozen other hens in a wire cage whose floor a single page of this magazine could carpet. Every natural instinct of this animal is thwarted, leading to a range of behavioral “vices” that can include cannibalizing her cagemates and rubbing her body against the wire mesh until it is featherless and bleeding. Pain? Suffering? Madness? The operative suspension of disbelief depends on more neutral descriptors, like “vices” and “stress.” Whatever you want to call what’s going on in those cages, the 10 percent or so of hens that can’t bear it and simply die is built into the cost of production. And when the output of the others begins to ebb, the hens will be “force-molted”–starved of food and water and light for several days in order to stimulate a final bout of egg laying before their life’s work is done. Simply reciting these facts, most of which are drawn from poultry-trade magazines, makes me sound like one of those animal people, doesn’t it? I don’t mean to, but this is what can happen when . . . you look. It certainly wasn’t my intention to ruin anyone’s breakfast. But now that I probably have spoiled the eggs, I do want to say one thing about the bacon, mention a single practice (by no means the worst) in modern hog production that points to the compound madness of an impeccable industrial logic. Piglets in confinement operations are weaned from their mothers 10 days after birth (compared with 13 weeks in nature) because they gain weight faster on their hormone- and antibiotic-fortified feed. This premature weaning leaves the pigs with a lifelong craving to suck and chew, a desire they gratify in confinement by biting the tail of the animal in front of them. A normal pig would fight off his molester, but a demoralized pig has stopped caring. “Learned helplessness” is the psychological term, and it’s not uncommon in confinement operations, where tens of thousands of hogs spend their entire lives ignorant of sunshine or earth or straw, crowded together beneath a metal roof upon metal slats suspended over a manure pit. So it’s not surprising that an animal as sensitive and intelligent as a pig would get depressed, and a depressed pig will allow his tail to be chewed on to the point of infection. Sick pigs, being underperforming “production units,” are clubbed to death on the spot. The U.S.D.A.’s recommended solution to the problem is called “tail docking.” Using a pair of pliers (and no anesthetic), most but not all of the tail is snipped off. Why the little stump? Because the whole point of the exercise is not to remove the object of tail-biting so much as to render it more sensitive. Now, a bite on the tail is so painful that even the most demoralized pig will mount a struggle to avoid it. Much of this description is drawn from “Dominion,” Matthew Scully’s recent book in which he offers a harrowing description of a North Carolina hog operation. Scully, a Christian conservative, has no patience for lefty rights talk, arguing instead that while God did give man “dominion” over animals (“Every moving thing that liveth shall be meat for you”), he also admonished us to show them mercy. “We are called to treat them with kindness, not because they have rights or power or some claim to equality but . . . because they stand unequal and powerless before us.” Scully calls the contemporary factory farm “our own worst nightmare” and, to his credit, doesn’t shrink from naming the root cause of this evil: unfettered capitalism. (Perhaps this explains why he resigned from the Bush administration just before his book’s publication.) A tension has always existed between the capitalist imperative to maximize efficiency and the moral imperatives of religion or community, which have historically served as a counterweight to the moral blindness of the market. This is one of “the cultural contradictions of capitalism”–the tendency of the economic impulse to erode the moral underpinnings of society. Mercy toward animals is one such casualty. More than any other institution, the American industrial animal farm offers a nightmarish glimpse of what capitalism can look like in the absence of moral or regulatory constraint. Here in these places life itself is redefined–as protein production–and with it suffering. That venerable word becomes “stress,” an economic problem in search of a cost-effective solution, like tail-docking or beak-clipping or, in the industry’s latest plan, by simply engineering the “stress gene” out of pigs and chickens. “Our own worst nightmare” such a place may well be; it is also real life for the billions of animals unlucky enough to have been born beneath these grim steel roofs, into the brief, pitiless life of a “production unit” in the days before the suffering gene was found. Vegetarianism doesn’t seem an unreasonable response to such an evil. Who would want to be made complicit in the agony of these animals by eating them? You want to throw something against the walls of those infernal sheds, whether it’s the Bible, a new constitutional right or a whole platoon of animal rightists bent on breaking in and liberating the inmates. In the shadow of these factory farms, Coetzee’s notion of a “stupefying crime” doesn’t seem far-fetched at all. But before you swear off meat entirely, let me describe a very different sort of animal farm. It is typical of nothing, and yet its very existence puts the whole moral question of animal agriculture in a different light. Polyface Farm occupies 550 acres of rolling grassland and forest in the Shenandoah Valley of Virginia. Here, Joel Salatin and his family raise six different food animals–cattle, pigs, chickens, rabbits, turkeys and sheep–in an intricate dance of symbiosis designed to allow each species, in Salatin’s words, “to fully express its physiological distinctiveness.” What this means in practice is that Salatin’s chickens live like chickens; his cows, like cows; pigs, pigs. As in nature, where birds tend to follow herbivores, once Salatin’s cows have finished grazing a pasture, he moves them out and tows in his “eggmobile,” a portable chicken coop that houses several hundred laying hens–roughly the natural size of a flock. The hens fan out over the pasture, eating the short grass and picking insect larvae out of the cowpats–all the while spreading the cow manure and eliminating the farm’s parasite problem. A diet of grubs and grass makes for exceptionally tasty eggs and contented chickens, and their nitrogenous manure feeds the pasture. A few weeks later, the chickens move out, and the sheep come in, dining on the lush new growth, as well as on the weed species (nettles, nightshade) that the cattle and chickens won’t touch. Meanwhile, the pigs are in the barn turning the compost. All winter long, while the cattle were indoors, Salatin layered their manure with straw, wood chips–and corn. By March, this steaming compost layer cake stands three feet high, and the pigs, whose powerful snouts can sniff out and retrieve the fermented corn at the bottom, get to spend a few happy weeks rooting through the pile, aerating it as they work. All you can see of these pigs, intently nosing out the tasty alcoholic morsels, are their upturned pink hams and corkscrew tails churning the air. The finished compost will go to feed the grass; the grass, the cattle; the cattle, the chickens; and eventually all of these animals will feed us. I thought a lot about vegetarianism and animal rights during the day I spent on Joel Salatin’s extraordinary farm. So much of what I’d read, so much of what I’d accepted, looked very different from here. To many animal rightists, even Polyface Farm is a death camp. But to look at these animals is to see this for the sentimental conceit it is. In the same way that we can probably recognize animal suffering when we see it, animal happiness is unmistakable, too, and here I was seeing it in abundance. For any animal, happiness seems to consist in the opportunity to express its creaturely character–its essential pigness or wolfness or chickenness. Aristotle speaks of each creature’s “characteristic form of life.” For domesticated species, the good life, if we can call it that, cannot be achieved apart from humans–apart from our farms and, therefore, our meat eating. This, it seems to me, is where animal rightists betray a profound ignorance about the workings of nature. To think of domestication as a form of enslavement or even exploitation is to misconstrue the whole relationship, to project a human idea of power onto what is, in fact, an instance of mutualism between species. Domestication is an evolutionary, rather than a political, development. It is certainly not a regime humans imposed on animals some 10,000 years ago. Rather, domestication happened when a small handful of especially opportunistic species discovered through Darwinian trial and error that they were more likely to survive and prosper in an alliance with humans than on their own. Humans provided the animals with food and protection, in exchange for which the animals provided the humans their milk and eggs and–yes–their flesh. Both parties were transformed by the relationship: animals grew tame and lost their ability to fend for themselves (evolution tends to edit out unneeded traits), and the humans gave up their hunter-gatherer ways for the settled life of agriculturists. (Humans changed biologically, too, evolving such new traits as a tolerance for lactose as adults.) From the animals’ point of view, the bargain with humanity has been a great success, at least until our own time. Cows, pigs, dogs, cats and chickens have thrived, while their wild ancestors have languished. (There are 10,000 wolves in North America, 50,000,000 dogs.) Nor does their loss of autonomy seem to trouble these creatures. It is wrong, the rightists say, to treat animals as “means” rather than “ends,” yet the happiness of a working animal like the dog consists precisely in serving as a “means.” Liberation is the last thing such a creature wants. To say of one of Joel Salatin’s caged chickens that “the life of freedom is to be preferred” betrays an ignorance about chicken preferences–which on this farm are heavily focused on not getting their heads bitten off by weasels. But haven’t these chickens simply traded one predator for another–weasels for humans? True enough, and for the chickens this is probably not a bad deal. For brief as it is, the life expectancy of a farm animal would be considerably briefer in the world beyond the pasture fence or chicken coop. A sheep farmer told me that a bear will eat a lactating ewe alive, starting with her udders. “As a rule,” he explained, “animals don’t get ‘good deaths’ surrounded by their loved ones.” The very existence of predation–animals eating animals–is the cause of much anguished hand-wringing in animal rights circles. “It must be admitted,” Singer writes, “that the existence of carnivorous animals does pose one problem for the ethics of Animal Liberation, and that is whether we should do anything about it.” Some animal rightists train their dogs and cats to become vegetarians. (Note: cats will require nutritional supplements to stay healthy.) Matthew Scully calls predation “the intrinsic evil in nature’s design . . . among the hardest of all things to fathom.” Really? A deep Puritan streak pervades animal rights activists, an abiding discomfort not only with our animality, but with the animals’ animality too. However it may appear to us, predation is not a matter of morality or politics; it, also, is a matter of symbiosis. Hard as the wolf may be on the deer he eats, the herd depends on him for its well-being; without predators to cull the herd, deer overrun their habitat and starve. In many places, human hunters have taken over the predator’s ecological role. Chickens also depend for their continued well-being on their human predators–not individual chickens, but chickens as a species. The surest way to achieve the extinction of the chicken would be to grant chickens a “right to life.” Yet here’s the rub: the animal rightist is not concerned with species, only individuals. Tom Regan, author of “The Case for Animal Rights,” bluntly asserts that because “species are not individuals . . . the rights view does not recognize the moral rights of species to anything, including survival.” Singer concurs, insisting that only sentient individuals have interests. But surely a species can have interests–in its survival, say–just as a nation or community or a corporation can. The animal rights movement’s exclusive concern with individual animals makes perfect sense given its roots in a culture of liberal individualism, but does it make any sense in nature? Consider this hypothetical scenario: In 1611 Juan da Goma (aka Juan the Disoriented) made accidental landfall on Wrightson Island, a six-square-mile rock in the Indian Ocean. The island’s sole distinction is as the only known home of the Arcania tree and the bird that nests in it, the Wrightson giant sea sparrow. Da Goma and his crew stayed a week, much of that time spent in a failed bid to recapture the ship’s escaped goat — who happened to be pregnant. Nearly four centuries later, Wrightson Island is home to 380 goats that have consumed virtually every scrap of vegetation in their reach. The youngest Arcania tree on the island is more than 300 years old, and only 52 sea sparrows remain. In the animal rights view, any one of those goats have at least as much right to life as the last Wrightson sparrow on earth, and the trees, because they are not sentient, warrant no moral consideration whatsoever. (In the mid-80’s a British environmental group set out to shoot the goats, but was forced to cancel the expedition after the Mammal Liberation Front bombed its offices.) The story of Wrightson Island (recounted by the biologist David Ehrenfeld in “Beginning Again”) suggests at the very least that a human morality based on individual rights makes for an awkward fit when applied to the natural world. This should come as no surprise: morality is an artifact of human culture, devised to help us negotiate social relations. It’s very good for that. But just as we recognize that nature doesn’t provide an adequate guide for human social conduct, isn’t it anthropocentric to assume that our moral system offers an adequate guide for nature? We may require a different set of ethics to guide our dealings with the natural world, one as well suited to the particular needs of plants and animals and habitats (where sentience counts for little) as rights suit us humans today. To contemplate such questions from the vantage of a farm is to appreciate just how parochial and urban an ideology animals rights really is. It could thrive only in a world where people have lost contact with the natural world, where animals no longer pose a threat to us and human mastery of nature seems absolute. “In our normal life,” Singer writes, “there is no serious clash of interests between human and nonhuman animals.” Such a statement assumes a decidedly urbanized “normal life,” one that certainly no farmer would recognize. The farmer would point out that even vegans have a “serious clash of interests” with other animals. The grain that the vegan eats is harvested with a combine that shreds field mice, while the farmer’s tractor crushes woodchucks in their burrows, and his pesticides drop songbirds from the sky. Steve Davis, an animal scientist at Oregon State University, has estimated that if America were to adopt a strictly vegetarian diet, the total number of animals killed every year would actually increase, as animal pasture gave way to row crops. Davis contends that if our goal is to kill as few animals as possible, then people should eat the largest possible animal that can live on the least intensively cultivated land: grass-fed beef for everybody. It would appear that killing animals is unavoidable no matter what we choose to eat. When I talked to Joel Salatin about the vegetarian utopia, he pointed out that it would also condemn him and his neighbors to importing their food from distant places, since the Shenandoah Valley receives too little rainfall to grow many row crops. Much the same would hold true where I live, in New England. We get plenty of rain, but the hilliness of the land has dictated an agriculture based on animals since the time of the Pilgrims. The world is full of places where the best, if not the only, way to obtain food from the land is by grazing animals on it–especially ruminants, which alone can transform grass into protein and whose presence can actually improve the health of the land. The vegetarian utopia would make us even more dependent than we already are on an industrialized national food chain. That food chain would in turn be even more dependent than it already is on fossil fuels and chemical fertilizer, since food would need to travel farther and manure would be in short supply. Indeed, it is doubtful that you can build a more sustainable agriculture without animals to cycle nutrients and support local food production. If our concern is for the health of nature–rather than, say, the internal consistency of our moral code or the condition of our souls–then eating animals may sometimes be the most ethical thing to do. There is, too, the fact that we humans have been eating animals as long as we have lived on this earth. Humans may not need to eat meat in order to survive, yet doing so is part of our evolutionary heritage, reflected in the design of our teeth and the structure of our digestion. Eating meat helped make us what we are, in a social and biological sense. Under the pressure of the hunt, the human brain grew in size and complexity, and around the fire where the meat was cooked, human culture first flourished. Granting rights to animals may lift us up from the brutal world of predation, but it will entail the sacrifice of part of our identity–our own animality. Surely this is one of the odder paradoxes of animal rights doctrine. It asks us to recognize all that we share with animals and then demands that we act toward them in a most unanimalistic way. Whether or not this is a good idea, we should at least acknowledge that our desire to eat meat is not a trivial matter, no mere “gastronomic preference.” We might as well call sex–also now technically unnecessary–a mere “recreational preference.” Whatever else it is, our meat eating is something very deep indeed. Are any of these good enough reasons to eat animals? I’m mindful of Ben Franklin’s definition of the reasonable creature as one who can come up with reasons for whatever he wants to do. So I decided I would track down Peter Singer and ask him what he thought. In an e-mail message, I described Polyface and asked him about the implications for his position of the Good Farm–one where animals got to live according to their nature and to all appearances did not suffer. “I agree with you that it is better for these animals to have lived and died than not to have lived at all,” Singer wrote back. Since the utilitarian is concerned exclusively with the sum of happiness and suffering and the slaughter of an animal that doesn’t comprehend that death need not involve suffering, the Good Farm adds to the total of animal happiness, provided you replace the slaughtered animal with a new one. However, he added, this line of thinking doesn’t obviate the wrongness of killing an animal that “has a sense of its own existence over time and can have preferences for its own future.” In other words, it’s O.K. to eat the chicken, but he’s not so sure about the pig. Yet, he wrote, “I would not be sufficiently confident of my arguments to condemn someone who purchased meat from one of these farms.” Singer went on to express serious doubts that such farms could be practical on a large scale, since the pressures of the marketplace will lead their owners to cut costs and corners at the expense of the animals. He suggested, too, that killing animals is not conducive to treating them with respect. Also, since humanely raised food will be more expensive, only the well-to-do can afford morally defensible animal protein. These are important considerations, but they don’t alter my essential point: what’s wrong with animal agriculture–with eating animals–is the practice, not the principle. What this suggests to me is that people who care should be working not for animal rights but animal welfare–to ensure that farm animals don’t suffer and that their deaths are swift and painless. In fact, the decent-life-merciful-death line is how Jeremy Bentham justified his own meat eating. Yes, the philosophical father of animal rights was himself a carnivore. In a passage rather less frequently quoted by animal rightists, Bentham defended eating animals on the grounds that “we are the better for it, and they are never the worse. . . . The death they suffer in our hands commonly is, and always may be, a speedier and, by that means, a less painful one than that which would await them in the inevitable course of nature.” My guess is that Bentham never looked too closely at what happens in a slaughterhouse, but the argument suggests that, in theory at least, a utilitarian can justify the killing of humanely treated animals–for meat or, presumably, for clothing. (Though leather and fur pose distinct moral problems. Leather is a byproduct of raising domestic animals for food, which can be done humanely. However, furs are usually made from wild animals that die brutal deaths–usually in leg-hold traps–and since most fur species aren’t domesticated, raising them on farms isn’t necessarily more humane.) But whether the issue is food or fur or hunting, what should concern us is the suffering, not the killing. All of which I was feeling pretty good about–until I remembered that utilitarians can also justify killing retarded orphans. Killing just isn’t the problem for them that it is for other people, including me. During my visit to Polyface Farm, I asked Salatin where his animals were slaughtered. He does the chickens and rabbits right on the farm, and would do the cattle, pigs and sheep there too if only the U.S.D.A. would let him. Salatin showed me the open-air abattoir he built behind the farmhouse–a sort of outdoor kitchen on a concrete slab, with stainless-steel sinks, scalding tanks, a feather-plucking machine and metal cones to hold the birds upside down while they’re being bled. Processing chickens is not a pleasant job, but Salatin insists on doing it himself because he’s convinced he can do it more humanely and cleanly than any processing plant. He slaughters every other Saturday through the summer. Anyone’s welcome to watch. I asked Salatin how he could bring himself to kill a chicken. “People have a soul; animals don’t,” he said. “It’s a bedrock belief of mine.” Salatin is a devout Christian. “Unlike us, animals are not created in God’s image, so when they die, they just die.” The notion that only in modern times have people grown uneasy about killing animals is a flattering conceit. Taking a life is momentous, and people have been working to justify the slaughter of animals for thousands of years. Religion and especially ritual has played a crucial part in helping us reckon the moral costs. Native Americans and other hunter-gathers would give thanks to their prey for giving up its life so the eater might live (sort of like saying grace). Many cultures have offered sacrificial animals to the gods, perhaps as a way to convince themselves that it was the gods’ desires that demanded the slaughter, not their own. In ancient Greece, the priests responsible for the slaughter (priests!–now we entrust the job to minimum-wage workers) would sprinkle holy water on the sacrificial animal’s brow. The beast would promptly shake its head, and this was taken as a sign of assent. Slaughter doesn’t necessarily preclude respect. For all these people, it was the ceremony that allowed them to look, then to eat. Apart from a few surviving religious practices, we no longer have any rituals governing the slaughter or eating of animals, which perhaps helps to explain why we find ourselves where we do, feeling that our only choice is to either look away or give up meat. Frank Perdue is happy to serve the first customer; Peter Singer, the second. Until my visit to Polyface Farm, I had assumed these were the only two options. But on Salatin’s farm, the eye contact between people and animals whose loss John Berger mourned is still a fact of life–and of death, for neither the lives nor the deaths of these animals have been secreted behind steel walls. “Food with a face,” Salatin likes to call what he’s selling, a slogan that probably scares off some customers. People see very different things when they look into the eyes of a pig or a chicken or a steer–a being without a soul, a “subject of a life” entitled to rights, a link in a food chain, a vessel for pain and pleasure, a tasty lunch. But figuring out what we do think, and what we can eat, might begin with the looking. We certainly won’t philosophize our way to an answer. Salatin told me the story of a man who showed up at the farm one Saturday morning. When Salatin noticed a PETA bumper sticker on the man’s car, he figured he was in for it. But the man had a different agenda. He explained that after 16 years as a vegetarian, he had decided that the only way he could ever eat meat again was if he killed the animal himself. He had come to look. “Ten minutes later we were in the processing shed with a chicken,” Salatin recalled. “He slit the bird’s throat and watched it die. He saw that the animal did not look at him accusingly, didn’t do a Disney double take. The animal had been treated with respect when it was alive, and he saw that it could also have a respectful death–that it wasn’t being treated as a pile of protoplasm.” Salatin’s open-air abattoir is a morally powerful idea. Someone slaughtering a chicken in a place where he can be watched is apt to do it scrupulously, with consideration for the animal as well as for the eater. This is going to sound quixotic, but maybe all we need to do to redeem industrial animal agriculture in this country is to pass a law requiring that the steel and concrete walls of the CAFO’s and slaughterhouses be replaced with . . . glass. If there’s any new “right” we need to establish, maybe it’s this one: the right to look. No doubt the sight of some of these places would turn many people into vegetarians. Many others would look elsewhere for their meat, to farmers like Salatin. There are more of them than I would have imagined. Despite the relentless consolidation of the American meat industry, there has been a revival of small farms where animals still live their “characteristic form of life.” I’m thinking of the ranches where cattle still spend their lives on grass, the poultry farms where chickens still go outside and the hog farms where pigs live as they did 50 years ago–in contact with the sun, the earth and the gaze of a farmer. For my own part, I’ve discovered that if you’re willing to make the effort, it’s entirely possible to limit the meat you eat to nonindustrial animals. I’m tempted to think that we need a new dietary category, to go with the vegan and lactovegetarian and piscatorian. I don’t have a catchy name for it yet (humanocarnivore?), but this is the only sort of meat eating I feel comfortable with these days. I’ve become the sort of shopper who looks for labels indicating that his meat and eggs have been humanely grown (the American Humane Association’s new “Free Farmed” label seems to be catching on), who visits the farms where his chicken and pork come from and who asks kinky-sounding questions about touring slaughterhouses. I’ve actually found a couple of small processing plants willing to let a customer onto the kill floor, including one, in Cannon Falls, Minn., with a glass abattoir. The industrialization–and dehumanization–of American animal farming is a relatively new, evitable and local phenomenon: no other country raises and slaughters its food animals quite as intensively or as brutally as we do. Were the walls of our meat industry to become transparent, literally or even figuratively, we would not long continue to do it this way. Tail-docking and sow crates and beak-clipping would disappear overnight, and the days of slaughtering 400 head of cattle an hour would come to an end. For who could stand the sight? Yes, meat would get more expensive. We’d probably eat less of it, too, but maybe when we did eat animals, we’d eat them with the consciousness, ceremony and respect they deserve.  

      I think that Pollan did an amazing job in this piece here. He showed signs of reading with the grain while alos arguing against the animal topic by preaching his own beleifs.

    2. the good life, if we can call it that, cannot be achieved apart from humans

      Who's to say that our standard of living as we take care of animals is better for them. it may be more humane but i dont think humans can judge happiness for animals. they would probably rather be left alone. its in there nature to survive and they don't need humans until humans make it impossible for them to live without the help of us.

    1. The Green New Deal “is technically and economically feasible,” he said. “Socially and politically, it’s a different question.”

      This is the basis of why this topic is so controversial. After looking at the details and hearing from experts on environmental issues, it's clear that The Green New Deal is not as far-reaching as some may originally think. However, the true controversy comes in when we look at the values held by democrats and republicans. Because many republicans have been known to deny climate change, such an aggressive plan to change how the United States runs is likely to cause an uproar by the far-right.

    1. the thing I came for: the wreck and not the story of the wreck the thing itself and not the myth the drowned face always staring toward the sun the evidence of damage worn by salt and sway into this threadbare beauty the ribs of the disaster curving their assertion among the tentative haunters. This is the place. And I am here, the mermaid whose dark hair streams black, the merman in his armored body. We circle silently about the wreck we dive into the hold. I am she: I am he

      The metaphor that I see when Rich refers to the wreck and not the myth of the drowned ship is it being compared to the universe or the world. You/Rich did not come looking for the myth, you came looking for the wreck. I interpreted this as you did not come here for people to tell you how the world is. You simply came to experience the world. Now, when Rich speaks about the mermaid and the merman, where "I am she: I am he", I believe that Rich understands that fundamentally, we are all the same. Humans have created classifications for things. Gender, sexual orientation, race, religion, age. As we live in this era, these classifications are incredibly useful, however, many of our world's problems stem from the differences between these demographics. As Rich was an advocate for not only women's rights, but also lesbian's issues, I think she had a very good grasp on how to explain and get others to understand perspectives that aren't theirs. While to some people, this message may be cryptic, the fact of its existence proves that this is a valid interpretation. Before I am a straight, male, Bengali adolescent, I am a human. As we all are, and understanding that allows you to see through all of the classifications we have for ourselves. It allows people to more easily sympathize with those who are different from them. While Rich may not have reached everyone, she has certainly been influential to many.

    1. In South Asia, where the history of scientific forestry has perhaps been most fully documented, the forest department quickly became a reviled arm of the colonial state. When a comprehensive Indian Forest Act was enacted in 1878-to supersede a preliminary Act of 1865-the government was warned, by a dissenting official that the new legislation would leave ‘a deep feeling of injustice and resentment amongst our agricultural communities;’ indeed, the act might ‘place in antagonism to Government every class whose support is desired and essential to the object in view [i.e. forest conservation], from the Zamindar [landlord] to the Hill Toda [tribal]. These words were far-sighted, for once the act was in place, peasant and tribal groupings resisted the operations of the Forest Department in all kinds of ways: through arson, breaches of the forest law, attacks on officials and on government property, and quite often, through co-ordinated and collective social movements aimed at restoring local control over forests.

      Contextualize

      When I read, “In South Asia, where the history of scientific forestry has perhaps been most fully documented, the forest department quickly became a reviled arm of the colonial state”. It reminds me that “Ecological Imperialism” from previous reading. The majority of those Asian countries that colonized by Europeans are following their way of environmentalism. However, is this the right way to apply one method to all the occupied lands? In “Ecological Imperialism,” Crosby states that “The human invaders and their descendants have consulted their egos, rather than ecologists, for explanations of their triumphs. But the human victims, the aborigines of the Lands of the Demographic Takeover, knew better, knew they were only one of many species being displaced and replaced; knew they were victims of something more irresistible and awesome than the spread of capitalism or Christianity”. Human has always been arrogant concerning nature. Humans did not realize the importance of nature for a long time and how we cannot live without nature. We must have enough knowledge of the landscape we are about to take action before doing our work. The experience we have can limit the negative impacts that we may do on the environment surrounding.

      Crosby, A. W. (1988). Ecological Imperialism: The Overseas Migration of Western Europeans as a Biological Phenomenon. In The ends of the earth: Perspectives on modern environmental history (pp. 103-117). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

      Relate

      There are many examples of humans taking advantage of nature in the colonized period and modern times due to the anthropocentric. That results in most of the natural ecosystem was damages during those human actions. For example, the Longleaf Pine ecosystem was once widely covered the southeastern region in the United States. Due to the fire burning and the timber industry's needs, the whole Longleaf Pine ecosystem was sharp decreasing in very short. Therefore, one of the most covered ecosystems became the most endangered ecosystem in the United States.

      Ecology imperialism is apropos to describe the relation human and nature has, even though today many people have cared about nature's sustainability. However, humans have not found the right balance between us and nature, and it is imperative to figure that out. We are not the only living species existing on the earth. We are sharing it with thousands of other species. It is also essential to think the future generations, think it sustainably, what are we giving them if we take actions into nature with no control.

    1. I find it somewhat interesting to note that with 246 public annotations on this page using Hypothes.is, that from what I can tell as of 4/2/2019 only one of them is a simple highlight. All the rest are highlights with an annotation or response of some sort.

      It makes me curious to know what the percentage distribution these two types have on the platform. Is it the case that in classroom settings, which many of these annotations appear to have been made, that much of the use of the platform dictates more annotations (versus simple highlights) due to the performative nature of the process?

      Is it possible that there are a significant number of highlights which are simply hidden because the platform automatically defaults these to private? Is the friction of making highlights so high that people don't bother?

      I know that Amazon will indicate heavily highlighted passages in e-books as a feature to draw attention to the interest relating to those passages. Perhaps it would be useful/nice if Hypothes.is would do something similar, but make the author of the highlights anonymous? (From a privacy perspective, this may not work well on articles with a small number of annotators as the presumption could be that the "private" highlights would most likely be directly attributed to those who also made public annotations.

      Perhaps the better solution is to default highlights to public and provide friction-free UI to make them private?

      A heavily highlighted section by a broad community can be a valuable thing, but surfacing it can be a difficult thing to do.

    1. I mean, posting constantly online about this cool spectacle thing I’m reading about and don’t understand is probably a good way of being part of the spectacle. (The first rule of Spectacle Club is: you do not talk about Spectacle Club?). But I think IndieWeb, Fediverse, etc, doing it in small, decentralised groupings, you break down the possibility of being in thrall to the spectacle. What’s the biggest blog you know? How do you even know that?

      I would push back on this. I have a feeling the effect may seem different because the network is so small. Making specific choices as we scale up may help to mitigate the effect, but eventually we will assuredly see some of the same problems because while the technology will help to mitigate some of the underlying problems with society, culture, and our media, it is not going to cure them.

      A case in point is the small network on Micro.blog, which, while generally warm and welcoming, is more monolithic towards the tech space, the Apple space, and has a high proportion of white, cis-gender males. This "founder effect" is likely going to heavily influence this community for a long time. Most times I spend time there, I'm subtly surrounded by the spectacle of Apple.

    1. Think of this essay as a series of strongly held hypotheses; without access to the types of data which i’m not even sure exists, it’s difficult to be definitive. As ever, my wise readers will add or push back as they always do.

      Push back, sure, but where? Where would we find this push back? The comments section only has a few tidbits. Perhaps the rest is on Twitter, Facebook, or some other social silo where the conversation is fraught-fully fragmented. Your own social capital is thus spread out and not easily compiled or compounded. As a result I wonder who may or may not have read this piece...

    1. I’ve believed that today’s women representatives objectify them in a similar as the women depicted in the Renaissance paintings Berger argues about. He considers nudity an art form. To be naked is to be seen by others without knowing you. However, In European nude paintings, this is not considered art. The video mentions the development and changes of nude art in different periods. For example, in the book of Genesis, Adam and Eve did not see each other’s naked bodies until they had eaten the fruit, so the naked bodies were created in the observer’s mind. Then there is the striking fact that women have been subservient to men, seeing them as agents of god. It wasn’t until medieval art that nudity began to become secularized. The mirror became a symbol of female vanity. Also, the video mentions another mythological theme in the Bible, in which men judge beauty and not beauty by looking at a woman’s naked body. Moreover, in Europe 10 oil paintings, most of the nudity is intended to please the male audience owner. Even today, the way women dress plays the role they want to play. Self-image is more based on how other people perceive you. Even the image of women in the media is still determined by the gaze of men. However, in today’s female images, the role of sex seems to have undergone some subtle changes. They no longer think of men as narcissistic and think of men and women as narcissistic, but they feel differently. A woman’s jealousy is how she feels about herself, not just what other people think of her. We could even say it’s a product of human interaction, a product that builds a sense of self-worth. They just want to see the image they want and may even be ready to become liberators and find their true selves.

      Great points!

    1. Author Response

      Reviewer #1:

      Hutchings et al. report an updated cryo-electron tomography study of the yeast COP-II coat assembled around model membranes. The improved overall resolution and additional compositional states enabled the authors to identify new domains and interfaces--including what the authors hypothesize is a previously overlooked structural role for the SEC31 C-Terminal Domain (CTD). By perturbing a subset of these new features with mutants, the authors uncover some functional consequences pertaining to the flexibility or stability of COP-II assemblies.

      Overall, the structural and functional work appears reliable, but certain questions and comments should be addressed prior to publication. However, this reviewer failed to appreciate the conceptual advance that warrants publication in a general biology journal like eLIFE. Rather, this study provides a valuable refinement of our understanding of COP-II that I believe is better suited to a more specialized, structure-focused journal.

      We agree that in our original submission our description of the experimental setup, indeed similar to previous work, did not fully capture the novel findings of this paper. Rather than being simply a higher resolution structure of the COPII coat, in fact we have discovered new interactions in the COPII assembly network, and we have probed their functional roles, significantly changing our understanding of the mechanisms of COPII-mediated membrane curvature. In the revised submission we have included additional genetic data that further illuminate this mechanism, and have rewritten the text to better communicate the novel aspects of our work.

      Our combination of structural, functional and genetic analyses goes beyond refining our textbook understanding of the COPII coat as a simple ‘adaptor and cage’, but rather it provides a completely new picture of how dynamic regulation of assembly and disassembly of a complex network leads to membrane remodelling.

      These new insights have important implications for how coat assembly provides structural force to bend a membrane but is still able to adapt to distinct morphologies. These questions are at the forefront of protein secretion, where there is debate about how different types of carriers might be generated that can accommodate cargoes of different size.

      Major Comments: 1) The authors belabor what this reviewer thinks is an unimportant comparison between the yeast reconstruction of the outer coat vertex with prior work on the human outer coat vertex. Considering the modest resolution of both the yeast and human reconstructions, the transformative changes in cryo-EM camera technology since the publication of the human complex, and the differences in sample preparation (inclusion of the membrane, cylindrical versus spherical assemblies, presence of inner coat components), I did not find this comparison informative. The speculations about a changing interface over evolutionary time are unwarranted and would require a detailed comparison of co-evolutionary changes at this interface. The simpler explanation is that this is a flexible vertex, observed at low resolution in both studies, plus the samples are very different.

      We do agree that our proposal that the vertex interface changes over evolutionary time is speculative and we have removed this discussion. We agree that a co-evolutionary analysis will be enlightening here, but is beyond the scope of the current work.

      We respectfully disagree with the reviewer’s interpretation that the difference between the two vertices is due to low resolution. The interfaces are clearly different, and the resolutions of the reconstructions are sufficient to state this. The reviewer’s suggestion that the difference in vertex orientation might be simply attributable to differences in sample, such as inclusion of the membrane, cylindrical versus spherical morphology, or presence of inner coat components were ruled out in our original submission: we resolved yeast vertices on spherical vesicles (in addition to those on tubes) and on membrane-less cages. These analyses clearly showed that neither the presence of a membrane, nor the change in geometry (tubular vs. spherical) affect vertex interactions. These experiments are presented in Supplementary Fig 4 (Supplementary Fig. 3 in the original version). Similarly, we discount that differences might be due to the presence or absence of inner coat components, since membrane-less cages were previously solved in both conditions and are no different in terms of their vertex structure (Stagg et al. Nature 2006 and Cell 2008).

      We believe it is important to report on the differences between the two vertex structures. Nevertheless, we have shifted our emphasis on the functional aspects of vertex formation and moved the comparison between the two vertices to the supplement.

      2) As one of the major take home messages of the paper, the presentation and discussion of the modeling and assignment of the SEC31-CTD could be clarified. First, it isn't clear from the figures or the movies if the connectivity makes sense. Where is the C-terminal end of the alpha-solenoid compared to this new domain? Can the authors plausibly account for the connectivity in terms of primary sequence? Please also include a side-by-side comparison of the SRA1 structure and the CTD homology model, along with some explanation of the quality of the model as measured by Modeller. Finally, even if the new density is the CTD, it isn't clear from the structure how this sub-stoichiometric and apparently flexible interaction enhances stability. Hence, when the authors wrote "when the [CTD] truncated form was the sole copy of Sec31 in yeast, cells were not viable, indicating that the novel interaction we detect is essential for COPII coat function." Maybe, but could this statement be a leap to far? Is it the putative interaction essential, or is the CTD itself essential for reasons that remain to be fully determined?

      The CTD is separated from the C-terminus of the alpha solenoid domain by an extended domain (~350 amino acids) that is predicted to be disordered, and contains the PPP motifs and catalytic fragment that contact the inner coat. This is depicted in cartoon form in Figures 3A and 7, and discussed at length in the text. This arrangement explains why no connectivity is seen, or expected. We could highlight the C-terminus of the alpha-solenoid domain to emphasize where the disordered region should emerge from the rod, but connectivity of the disordered domain to the CTD could arise from multiple positions, including from an adjacent rod.

      The reviewer’s point about the essentiality of the CTD being independent of its interaction with the Sec31 rod, is an important one. The basis for our model that the CTD enhances stability or rigidity of the coat is the yeast phenotype of Sec31-deltaCTD, which resembles that of a sec13 null. Both mutants are lethal, but rescued by deletion of emp24, which leads to more easily deformable membranes (Čopič et al. Science 2012). We agree that even if this model is true, the interaction of the CTD with Sec31 that our new structure reveals is not proven to drive rigidity or essentiality. We have tempered this hypothesis and added alternative possibilities to the discussion.

      We have included the SRA1 structure in Supplementary Fig 5, as requested, and the model z-score in the Methods. The Z-score, as calculated by the proSA-web server is -6.07 (see figure below, black dot), and falls in line with experimentally determined structures including that of the template (PDB 2mgx, z-score = -5.38).

      img

      3) Are extra rods discussed in Fig. 4 are a curiosity of unclear functional significance? This reviewer is concerned that these extra rods could be an in vitro stoichiometry problem, rather than a functional property of COP-II.

      This is an important point, that, as we state in the paper, cannot be answered at the moment: the resolution is too low to identify the residues involved in the interaction. Therefore we are hampered in our ability to assess the physiological importance of this interaction. We still believe the ‘extra’ rods are an important observation, as they clearly show that another mode of outer coat interaction, different from what was reported before, is possible.

      The concern that interactions visualised in vitro might not be physiologically relevant is broadly applicable to structural biology approaches. However, our experimental approach uses samples that result from active membrane remodelling under near-physiological conditions, and we therefore expect these to be less prone to artefacts than most in vitro reconstitution approaches, where proteins are used at high concentrations and in high salt buffer conditions.

      4) The clashsccore for the PDB is quite high--and I am dubious about the reliability of refining sidechain positions with maps at this resolution. In addition to the Ramchandran stats, I would like to see the Ramachandran plot as well as, for any residue-level claims, the density surrounding the modeled side chain (e.g. S742).

      The clashscore is 13.2, which, according to molprobity, is in the 57th percentile for all structures and in the 97th for structures of similar resolutions. We would argue therefore that the clashscore is rather low. In fact, the model was refined from crystal structures previously obtained by other groups, which had worse clashscore (17), despite being at higher resolution. Our refinement has therefore improved the clashscore. During refinement we have chosen restraint levels appropriate to the resolution of our map (Afonine et al., Acta Cryst D 2018)

      The Ramachandran plot is copied here and could be included in a supplemental figure if required. We make only one residue-level claim (S742), the density for which is indeed not visible at our resolution. We claim that S742 is close to the Sec23-23 interface, and do not propose any specific interactions. Nevertheless we have removed reference to S742 from the manuscript. We included this specific information because of the potential importance of this residue as a site of phosphorylation, thereby putting this interface in broader context for the general eLife reader.

      img

      Minor Comments:

      1) The authors wrote "To assess the relative positioning of the two coat layers, we analysed the localisation of inner coat subunits with respect to each outer coat vertex: for each aligned vertex particle, we superimposed the positions of all inner coat particles at close range, obtaining the average distribution of neighbouring inner coat subunits. From this 'neighbour plot' we did not detect any pattern, indicating random relative positions. This is consistent with a flexible linkage between the two layers that allows adaptation of the two lattices to different curvatures (Supplementary Fig 1E)." I do not understand this claim, since the pattern both looks far from random and the interactions depend on molecular interactions that are not random. Please clarify.

      We apologize for the confusion: the pattern of each of the two coats are not random. Our sentence refers to the positions of inner and outer coats relative to each other. The two lattices have different parameters and the two layers are linked by flexible linkers (the 350 amino acids referred to above). We have now clarified the sentence.

      2) Related to major point #1, the author wrote "We manually picked vertices and performed carefully controlled alignments." I do now know what it means to carefully control alignments, and fear this suggests human model bias.

      We used different starting references for the alignments, with the precise aim to avoid model bias. For both vesicle and cage vertex datasets, we have aligned the subtomograms against either the vertex obtained from tubules, or the vertex from previously published membrane-less cages. In all cases, we retrieved a structure that resembles the one on tubules, suggesting that the vertex arrangement we observe isn’t simply the result of reference bias. This procedure is depicted in Supplementary Fig 4 (Supplementary Fig. 3 in the original manuscript), but we have now clarified it also in the methods section.

      3) Why do some experiments use EDTA? I may be confused, but I was surprised to see the budding reaction employed 1mM GMPPNP, and 2.5mM EDTA (but no Magnesium?). Also, for the budding reaction, please replace or expand upon the "the 10% GUV (v/v)" with a mass or molar lipid-to-protein ratio.

      We regret the confusion. As stated in the methods, all our budding reactions are performed in the presence of EDTA and Magnesium, which is present in the buffer (at 1.2 mM). The reason is to facilitate nucleotide exchange, as reported and validated in Bacia et al., Scientific Reports 2011.

      Lipids in GUV preparations are difficult to quantify. We report the stock concentrations used, but in each preparation the amount of dry lipid that forms GUVs might be different, as is the concentration of GUVs after hydration. However since we analyse reactions where COPII proteins have bound and remodelled individual GUVs, we do not believe the protein/lipid ratio influences our structures.

      4) Please cite the AnchorMap procedure.

      We cite the SerialEM software, and are not aware of other citations specifically for the anchor map procedure.

      5) Please edit for typos (focussing, functionl, others)

      Done

      Reviewer #2:

      The manuscript describes new cryo-EM, biochemistry, and genetic data on the structure and function of the COPII coat. Several new discoveries are reported including the discovery of an extra density near the dimerization region of Sec13/31, and "extra rods" of Sec13/31 that also bind near the dimerization region. Additionally, they showed new interactions between the Sec31 C-terminal unstructured region and Sec23 that appear to bridge multiple Sec23 molecules. Finally, they increased the resolution of the Sec23/24 region of their structure compared to their previous studies and were able to resolve a previously unresolved L-loop in Sec23 that makes contact with Sar1. Most of their structural observations were nicely backed up with biochemical and genetic experiments which give confidence in their structural observations. Overall the paper is well-written and the conclusions justified.

      However, this is the third iteration of structure determination of the COPII coat on membrane with essentially the same preparation and methods. Each time, there has been an incremental increase in resolution and new discoveries, but the impact of the present study is deemed to be modest. The science is good, but it may be more appropriate for a more specialized journal. Areas of specific concern are described below.

      As described above, we respectfully disagree with this interpretation of the advance made by the current work. This work improves on previous work in many aspects. The resolution of the outer coat increases from over 40A to 10-12A, allowing visualisation of features that were not previously resolved, including a novel vertex arrangement, the Sec31 CTD, and the outer coat ‘extra rods’. An improved map of the inner coat also allows us to resolve the Sec23 ‘L-loop’. We would argue that these are not just extra details, but correspond to a suite of novel interactions that expand our understanding of the complex COPII assembly network. Moreover, we include biochemical and genetic experiments that not only back up our structural observations but bring new insights into COPII function. As pointed out in response to reviewer 1, we believe our work contributes a significant conceptual advance, and have modified the manuscript to convey this more effectively.

      1) The abstract is vague and should be re-written with a better description of the work.

      We have modified the abstract to specifically outline what we have done and the major new discoveries of this paper.

      2) Line 166 - "Surprisingly, this mutant was capable of tubulating GUVs". This experiment gets to one of the fundamental unknown questions in COPII vesiculation. It is not clear what components are driving the membrane remodeling and at what stages during vesicle formation. Isn't it possible that the tubulation activity the authors observe in vitro is not being driven at all by Sec13/31 but rather Sec23/24-Sar1? Their Sec31ΔCTD data supports this idea because it lacks a clear ordered outer coat despite making tubules. An interesting experiment would be to see if tubules form in the absence of all of Sec13/31 except the disordered domain of Sec31 that the authors suggest crosslinks adjacent Sec23/24s.

      This is an astute observation, and we agree with the reviewer that the source of membrane deformation is not fully understood. We favour the model that budding is driven significantly by the Sec23-24 array. To further support this, we have performed a new experiment, where we expressed Sec31ΔN in yeast cells lacking Emp24, which have more deformable membranes and are tolerant to the otherwise lethal deletion of Sec13. While Sec31ΔN in a wild type background did not support cell viability, this was rescued in a Δemp24 yeast strain, strongly supporting the hypothesis that a major contributor to membrane remodelling is the inner coat, with the outer coat becoming necessary to overcome membrane bending resistance that ensues from the presence of cargo. We now include these results in Figure 1.

      However, we must also take into account the results presented in Fig. 6, where we show that weakening the Sec23-24 interface still leads to budding, but only if Sec13-31 is fully functional, and that in this case budding leads to connected pseudo-spherical vesicles rather than tubes. When Sec13-31 assembly is also impaired, tubes appear unstructured. We believe this strongly supports our conclusions that both inner and outer coat interactions are fundamental for membrane remodelling, and it is the interplay between the two that determines membrane morphology (i.e. tubes vs. spheres).

      To dissect the roles of inner and outer coats even further, we have done the experiment that the reviewer suggests: we expressed Sec31768-1114, but the protein was not well-behaved and co-purified with chaperones. We believe the disordered domain aggregates when not scaffolded by the structured elements of the rod. Nonetheless, we used this fragment in a budding reaction, and could not see any budding. We did not include this experiment as it was inconclusive: the lack of functionality of the purified Sec31 fragment could be attributed to the inability of the disordered region to bind its inner coat partner in the absence of the scaffolding Sec13-31 rod. As an alternative approach, we have used a version of Sec31 that lacks the CTD, and harbours a His tag at the N-terminus (known from previous studies to partially disrupt vertex assembly). We think this construct is more likely to be near native, since both modifications on their own lead to functional protein. We could detect no tubulation with this construct by negative stain, while both control constructs (Sec31ΔCTD and Nhis-Sec31) gave tubulation. This suggests that the cross-linking function of Sec31 is not sufficient to tubulate GUV membranes, but some degree of functional outer coat organisation (either mediated by N- or C-terminal interactions) is needed. It is also possible that the lack of outer coat organisation might lead to less efficient recruitment to the inner coat and cross-linking activity. We have added this new observation to the manuscript.

      3) Line 191 - "Inspecting cryo-tomograms of these tubules revealed no lozenge pattern for the outer 192 coat" - this phrasing is vague. The reviewer thinks that what they mean is that there is a lack of order for the Sec13/31 layer. Please clarify.

      The reviewer is correct, we have changed the sentence.

      4) Line 198 - "unambiguously confirming this density corresponds to 199 the CTD." This only confirms that it is the CTD if that were the only change and the Sec13/31 lattice still formed. Another possibility is that it is density from other Sec13/31 that only appears when the lattice is formed such as the "extra rods". One possibility is that the density is from the extra rods. The reviewer agrees that their interpretation is indeed the most likely, but it is not unambiguous. The authors should consider cross-linking mass spectrometry.

      We have removed the word ‘unambiguously’, and changed to ‘confirming that this density most likely corresponds to the CTD’. Nonetheless, we believe that our interpretation is correct: the extra rods bind to a different position, and themselves also show the CTD appendage. In this experiment, the lack of the CTD was the only biochemical change.

      5) In the Sec31ΔCTD section, the authors should comment on why ΔCTD is so deleterious to oligomer organization in yeast when cages form so abundantly in preparations of human Sec13/31 ΔC (Paraan et al 2018).

      We have added a comment to address this. “Interestingly, human Sec31 proteins lacking the CTD assemble in cages, indicating that either the vertex is more stable for human proteins and sufficient for assembly, or that the CTD is important in the context of membrane budding but not for cage formation in high salt conditions.”

      6) The data is good for the existence of the "extra rods", but significance and importance of them is not clear. How can these extra densities be distinguished from packing artifacts due to imperfections in the helical symmetry.

      Please also see our response to point 3 from reviewer 1. Regarding the specific concern that artefacts might be a consequence of imperfection in the helical symmetry, we would argue such imperfections are indeed expected in physiological conditions, and to a much higher extent. For this reason interactions seen in the context of helical imperfections are likely to be relevant. In fact, in normal GTP hydrolysis conditions, we expect long tubes would not be able to form, and the outer coat to be present on a wide range of continuously changing membrane curvatures. We think that the ability of the coat to form many interactions when the symmetry is imperfect might be exactly what confers the coat its flexibility and adaptability.

      7) Figure 5 is very hard to interpret and should be redone. Panels B and C are particularly hard to interpret.

      We have made a new figure where we think clarity is improved.

      8) The features present in Sec23/24 structure do not reflect the reported resolution of 4.7 Å. It seems that the resolution is overestimated.

      We report an average resolution of 4.6 Å. In most of our map we can clearly distinguish beta strands, follow the twist of alpha helices and see bulky side chains. These features typically become visible at 4.5-5A resolution. We agree that some areas are worse than 4.6 Å, as typically expected for such a flexible assembly, but we believe that the average resolution value reported is accurate. We obtained the same resolution estimate using different software including relion, phenix and dynamo, so that is really the best value we can provide. To further convince ourselves that we have the resolution we claim, we sampled EM maps from the EMDB with the same stated resolution (we just took the 7 most recent ones which had an associated atomic model), and visualised their features at arbitrary positions. For both beta strands and alpha helices, we do not feel our map looks any worse than the others we have examined. We include a figure here.

      img

      9) Lines 315/316 - "We have combined cryo-tomography with biochemical and genetic assays to obtain a complete picture of the assembled COPII coat at unprecedented resolution (Fig. 7)"

      10) Figure 7. is a schematic model/picture the authors should reference a different figure or rephrase the sentence.

      We now refer to Fig 7 in a more appropriate place.

      Reviewer #3:

      The manuscript by Hutchings et al. describes several previously uncharacterised molecular interactions in the coats of COP-II vesicles by using a reconstituted coats of yeast COPI-II. They have improved the resolution of the inner coat to 4.7A by tomography and subtomogram averaging, revealing detailed interactions, including those made by the so-called L-loop not observed before. Analysis of the outer layer also led to new interesting discoveries. The sec 31 CTD was assigned in the map by comparing the WT and deletion mutant STA-generated density maps. It seems to stabilise the COP-II coats and further evidence from yeast deletion mutants and microsome budding reconstitution experiments suggests that this stabilisation is required in vitro. Furthermore, COP-II rods that cover the membrane tubules in right-handed manner revealed sometimes an extra rod, which is not part of the canonical lattice, bound to them. The binding mode of these extra rods (which I refer to here a Y-shape) is different from the canonical two-fold symmetric vertex (X-shape). When the same binding mode is utilized on both sides of the extra rod (Y-Y) the rod seems to simply insert in the canonical lattice. However, when the Y-binding mode is utilized on one side of the rod and the X-binding mode on the other side, this leads to bridging different lattices together. This potentially contributes to increased flexibility in the outer coat, which maybe be required to adopt different membrane curvatures and shapes with different cargos. These observations build a picture where stabilising elements in both COP-II layers contribute to functional cargo transport. The paper makes significant novel findings that are described well. Technically the paper is excellent and the figures nicely support the text. I have only minor suggestions that I think would improve the text and figure.

      We thank the reviewer for helpful suggestions which we agree improve the manuscript.

      Minor Comments:

      L 108: "We collected .... tomograms". While the meaning is clear to a specialist, this may sound somewhat odd to a generic reader. Perhaps you could say "We acquired cryo-EM data of COP-II induced tubules as tilt series that were subsequently used to reconstruct 3D tomograms of the tubules."

      We have changed this as suggested

      L 114: "we developed an unbiased, localisation-based approach". What is the part that was developed here? It seems that the inner layer particle coordinates where simply shifted to get starting points in the outer layer. Developing an approach sounds more substantial than this. Also, it's unclear what is unbiased about this approach. The whole point is that it's biased to certain regions (which is a good thing as it incorporates prior knowledge on the location of the structures).

      We have modified the sentence to “To target the sparser outer coat lattice for STA, we used the refined coordinates of the inner coat to locate the outer coat tetrameric vertices”, and explain the approach in detail in the methods.

      L 124: "The outer coat vertex was refined to a resolution of approximately ~12 A, revealing unprecedented detail of the molecular interactions between Sec31 molecules (Supplementary Fig 2A)". The map alone does not reveal molecular interactions; the main understanding comes from fitting of X-ray structures to the low-resolution map. Also "unprecedented detail" itself is somewhat problematic as the map of Noble et al (2013) of the Sec31 vertex is also at nominal resolution of 12 A. Furthermore, Supplementary Fig 2A does not reveal this "unprecedented detail", it shows the resolution estimation by FSC. To clarify, these points you could say: "Fitting of the Sec31 atomic model to our reconstruction vertex at 12-A resolution (Supplementary Fig 2A) revealed the molecular interactions between different copies of Sec31 in the membrane-assembled coat.

      We have changed the sentence as suggested.

      L 150: Can the authors exclude the possibility that the difference is due to differences in data processing? E.g. how the maps amplitudes have been adjusted?

      Yes, we can exclude this scenario by measuring distances between vertices in the right and left handed direction. These measurements are only compatible with our vertex arrangement, and cannot be explained by the big deviation from 4-fold symmetry seen in the membrane-less cage vertices.

      L 172: "that wrap tubules either in a left- or right-handed manner". Don't they do always both on each tubule? Now this sentence could be interpreted to mean that some tubules have a left-handed coat and some a right-handed coat.

      We have changed this sentence to clarify. “Outer coat vertices are connected by Sec13-31 rods that wrap tubules both in a left- and right-handed manner.”

      L276: "The difference map" hasn't been introduced earlier but is referred to here as if it has been.

      We now introduce the difference map.

      L299: Can "Secondary structure predictions" denote a protein region "highly prone to protein binding"?

      Yes, this is done through DISOPRED3, a feature include in the PSIPRED server we used for our predictions. The reference is: Jones D.T., Cozzetto D. DISOPRED3: precise disordered region predictions with annotated protein-binding activity Bioinformatics. 2015; 31:857–863. We have now added this reference to the manuscript.

      L316: It's true that the detail in the map of the inner coat is unprecedented and the model presented in Figure 7 is partially based on that. But here "unprecedented resolution" sounds strange as this sentence refers to a schematic model and not a map.

      We have changed this by moving the reference to Fig 7 to a more appropriate place

      L325: "have 'compacted' during evolution" -> remove. It's enough to say it's more compact in humans and less compact in yeast as there could have been different adaptations in different organisms at this interface.

      We have changed as requested. See also our response to reviewer 1, point 1.

      L327: What's exactly meant by "sequence diversity or variability at this density".

      We have now clarified: “Since multiple charge clusters in yeast Sec31 may contribute to this interaction interface (Stancheva et al., 2020), the low resolution could be explained by the fact that the density is an average of different sequences.”

      L606-607: The description of this custom data processing approach is difficult to follow. Why is in-plane flip needed and how is it used here?

      Initially particles are picked ignoring tube directionality (as this cannot be assessed easily from the tomograms due to the pseudo-twofold symmetry of the Sec23/24/Sar1 trimer). So the in plane rotation of inner coat subunit could be near 0 or 180°. For each tube, both angles are sampled (in-plane flip). Most tubes result in the majority of particles being assigned one of the two orientations (which is then assumed as the tube directionality). Particles that do not conform are removed, and rare tubes where directionality cannot be determined are also removed. We have re-written the description to clarify these points: “Initial alignments were conducted on a tube-by-tube basis using the Dynamo in-plane flip setting to search in-plane rotation angles 180° apart. This allowed to assign directionality to each tube, and particles that were not conforming to it were discarded by using the Dynamo dtgrep_direction command in custom MATLAB scripts”

      L627: "Z" here refers to the coordinate system of aligned particles not that of the original tomogram. Perhaps just say "shifted 8 pixels further away from the membrane".

      Changed as requested.

      L642-643: How can the "left-handed" and "right-handed" rods be separated here? These terms refer to the long-range organisation of the rods in the lattice it's not clear how they were separated in the early alignments.

      They are separated by picking only one subset using the dynamo sub-boxing feature. This extracts boxes from the tomogram which are in set positions and orientation relative to the average of previously aligned subtomograms. From the average vertex structure, we sub-box rods at 4 different positions that correspond to the centre of the rods, and the 2-fold symmetric pairs are combined into the same dataset. We have clarified this in the text: “The refined positions of vertices were used to extract two distinct datasets of left and right-handed rods respectively using the dynamo sub-boxing feature.”

      Figure 2B. It's difficult to see the difference between dark and light pink colours.

      We have changed colours to enhance the difference.

      Figure 3C. These panels report the relative frequency of neighbouring vertices at each position; "intensity" does not seem to be the right measure for this. You could say that the colour bar indicates the "relative frequency of neighbouring vertices at each position" and add detail how the values were scaled between 0 and 1. The same applies to SFigure 1E.

      Changed as requested.

      Figure 4. The COP-II rods themselves are relatively straight, and they are not left-handed or right-handed. Here, more accurate would be "architecture of COPII rods organised in a left-handed manner". (In the text the authors may of course define and then use this shorter expression if they so wish.) Panel 4B top panel could have the title "left-handed" and the lower panel should have the title "right-handed" (for consistency and clarity).

      We have now defined left- and right-handed rods in the text, and have changed the figure and panel titles as requested.

    2. Reviewer #3:

      The manuscript by Hutchings et al. describes several previously uncharacterised molecular interactions in the coats of COP-II vesicles by using a reconstituted coats of yeast COPI-II. They have improved the resolution of the inner coat to 4.7A by tomography and subtomogram averaging, revealing detailed interactions, including those made by the so-called L-loop not observed before. Analysis of the outer layer also led to new interesting discoveries. The sec 31 CTD was assigned in the map by comparing the WT and deletion mutant STA-generated density maps. It seems to stabilise the COP-II coats and further evidence from yeast deletion mutants and microsome budding reconstitution experiments suggests that this stabilisation is required in vitro. Furthermore, COP-II rods that cover the membrane tubules in right-handed manner revealed sometimes an extra rod, which is not part of the canonical lattice, bound to them. The binding mode of these extra rods (which I refer to here a Y-shape) is different from the canonical two-fold symmetric vertex (X-shape). When the same binding mode is utilized on both sides of the extra rod (Y-Y) the rod seems to simply insert in the canonical lattice. However, when the Y-binding mode is utilized on one side of the rod and the X-binding mode on the other side, this leads to bridging different lattices together. This potentially contributes to increased flexibility in the outer coat, which may be required to adopt different membrane curvatures and shapes with different cargos. These observations build a picture where stabilising elements in both COP-II layers contribute to functional cargo transport. The paper makes significant novel findings that are described well. Technically the paper is excellent and the figures nicely support the text. I have minor suggestions that I think would improve the text and figures.

      L 108: "We collected .... tomograms". While the meaning is clear to a specialist, this may sound somewhat odd to a generic reader. Perhaps you could say "We acquired cryo-EM data of COP-II induced tubules as tilt series that were subsequently used to reconstruct 3D tomograms of the tubules."

      L 114: "we developed an unbiased, localisation-based approach". What is the part that was developed here? It seems that the inner layer particle coordinates where simply shifted to get starting points in the outer layer. Developing an approach sounds more substantial than this. Also, it's unclear what is unbiased about this approach. The whole point is that it's biased to certain regions (which is a good thing as it incorporates prior knowledge on the location of the structures).

      L 124: "The outer coat vertex was refined to a resolution of approximately ~12 A, revealing unprecedented detail of the molecular interactions between Sec31 molecules (Supplementary Fig 2A)". The map alone does not reveal molecular interactions; the main understanding comes from fitting of X-ray structures to the low-resolution map. Also "unprecedented detail" itself is somewhat problematic as the map of Noble et al (2013) of the Sec31 vertex is also at nominal resolution of 12 A. Furthermore, Supplementary Fig 2A does not reveal this "unprecedented detail", it shows the resolution estimation by FSC. To clarify, these points you could say: "Fitting of the Sec31 atomic model to our reconstruction vertex at 12-A resolution (Supplementary Fig 2A) revealed the molecular interactions between different copies of Sec31 in the membrane-assembled coat.

      L 150: Can the authors exclude the possibility that the difference is due to differences in data processing? E.g. how the maps’ amplitudes have been adjusted?

      L 172: "that wrap tubules either in a left- or right-handed manner". Don't they always do both on each tubule? Now this sentence could be interpreted to mean that some tubules have a left-handed coat and some a right-handed coat.

      L276: "The difference map" hasn't been introduced earlier but is referred to here as if it has been.

      L299: Can "Secondary structure predictions" denote a protein region "highly prone to protein binding"?

      L316: It's true that the detail in the map of the inner coat is unprecedented and the model presented in Figure 7 is partially based on that. But here "unprecedented resolution" sounds strange as this sentence refers to a schematic model and not a map.

      L325: "have 'compacted' during evolution" -> remove. It's enough to say it's more compact in humans and less compact in yeast as there could have been different adaptations in different organisms at this interface.

      L327: What's exactly meant by "sequence diversity or variability at this density".

      L606-607: The description of this custom data processing approach is difficult to follow. Why is in-plane flip needed and how is it used here?

      L627: "Z" here refers to the coordinate system of aligned particles not that of the original tomogram. Perhaps just say "shifted 8 pixels further away from the membrane"

      L642-643: How can the "left-handed" and "right-handed" rods be separated here? These terms refer to the long-range organisation of the rods in the lattice; it's not clear how they were separated in the early alignments.

      Figure 2B. It's difficult to see the difference between dark and light pink colours.

      Figure 3C. These panels report the relative frequency of neighbouring vertices at each position; "intensity" does not seem to be the right measure for this. You could say that the colour bar indicates the "relative frequency of neighbouring vertices at each position" and add detail how the values were scaled between 0 and 1. The same applies to SFigure 1E.

      Figure 4. The COP-II rods themselves are relatively straight, and they are not left-handed or right-handed. Here, more accurate would be "architecture of COPII rods organised in a left-handed manner". (In the text the authors may of course define and then use this shorter expression if they so wish.) Panel 4B top panel could have the title "left-handed" and the lower panel should have the title "right-handed" (for consistency and clarity).

  5. inst-fs-iad-prod.inscloudgate.net inst-fs-iad-prod.inscloudgate.net
    1. You are encountering evidence that inequality not only exists, but is deeply structured into society in ways that secure its reproduction. You are also beginning to realize that, contrary to what you have always been taught, categories of difference (such as gender, race, and class) rather than merit alone, do matter and contribute significantly to people’s experiences and life opportunities.

      This is important to me because we often think to ourselves that this is basic knowledge, however, there are those who may not be exposed to it, are uneducated, or simply deny the reality of our systems. As we get older we experience more and for those who haven't been as exposed to it realizing the importance of evidence about inequality is essential to education. (Gerrymandering, institutionalized racism, voter oppression, higher rates of hate crimes and violence against people of color, outdated policing systems and education)

    2. Throughout your course, you will likely be studying key concepts such as so-cialization, oppression, privilege, and ideology and doing coursework that challeng-es your worldview by suggesting that you may not be as open-minded as you may have thought.

      This is an interesting concept, especially as someone from this generation. Our generation has been one of the most open minded to date, however, we live in the United States, a country that is notorious for leaning toward conservative views. Some of us may not have a concept of what politics in other places are and we may not be as open minded as we think.

    1. Unfortunately, there are still a number of people who continue in the fatal belief that government rests on natural laws, that it maintains social order and harmony, that it diminishes crime, and that it prevents the lazy man from fleecing his fellows. I shall therefore examine these contentions. A natural law is that factor in man which asserts itself freely and spontaneously without any external force, in harmony with the requirements of nature. For instance, the demand for nutrition, for sex gratification, for light, air, and exercise, is a natural law. But its expression needs not the machinery of government, needs not the club, the gun, the handcuff, or the prison. To obey such laws, if we may call it obedience, requires only spontaneity and free opportunity. That governments do not maintain themselves through such harmonious factors is proven by the terrible array of violence, force, and coercion all governments use in order to live. Thus Blackstone is right when he says, "Human laws are invalid, because they are contrary to the laws of nature."

      As human beings we ruled ourselves without any goverments or governers. Humans doesn't need these things at all,a governer can only provide unfair punishments to us.The idea that Anarchy lead us to chaos is only for corrupted minds. Our nature is not available for a governor to rule us.Since the Homo Sapiens are te only mankinds on this earth we know we never had it in our nature.That's why anarchy maybe good for our routine.So thats the way I think about that part

    1. To illustrate: very many professed popularizers of the results of scientific inquiry, as well as laymen, seem to think that the entire psychology of vision is explained when we have a complete knowledge of the anatomy of the retina, of its nervous connection with the brain, and of the centre in the latter which serves for visual functions; or that we know all about memory if we can discover that certain brain cells store up nervous impressions, and certain fibres serve to connect these cells,-- the latter producing the association of ideas, while the former occasion their reproduction. In short, the commonest view of physiological psychology seems to be that it is a science which shows that some or all of the events of our mental life are physically conditioned upon certain nerve-structures, and thereby explains these events. Nothing could be further from the truth. So far as I know, all the leading investigators clearly realize that explanations of psychical events, in order to explain, must themselves be psychical and not physiological. However important such knowledge as that of which we have just been speaking may be for physiology, it has of itself no value for psychology. It tells simply what and how physiological elements serve as a basis for psychical acts; what the latter are, or how they are to be explained, it tells us not at all. Physiology can no more, of itself, give us the what, why, and how of psychical life, than the physical geography of a country can enable us to construct or explain the history of the nation that has dwelt within that country. However important, however indispensable the land with all its qualities is as a basis for that history, that history itself can be ascertained and explained only through historical records and historic conditions. And so psychical events can be observed only through psychical means, and interpreted and explained by psychical conditions and facts.

      This paragraph basically states the fact that physiology is completely different from psychology in the fact that it focuses on strictly physical things. This reiterates the fact that psychology is it's own individual science. That we have made psychology unique and formed it and branched it off of other sciences and theories

    1. (“I don’t think I’ve ever used the word ‘consent’ with a three-year-old before,” Kahn says.) The goal is that “if a kid doesn’t want to be hugged by another kid, he can say, ‘This is my body,’ and be understood.”

      Most of us as humans grow up being taught to respect personal space. Gideon Kahn says that he doesn't think that he's ever used the word consent when teaching young children. I think it's important to start using that word and teaching children at younger age. It may be hard for some of the kids to understand but it still needs to be a conversation. We should all start learning at a younger age so we can realize the full extent of consent before reaching the age where kids start to develop a sex drive. Without this understanding, you may unintentionally hurt somebody by not respecting their space.

    1. Note: This rebuttal was posted by the corresponding author to Review Commons. Content has not been altered except for formatting.

      Learn more at Review Commons


      Reply to the reviewers

      Response to Reviewers and Revision Plan

      We thank all three reviewers for their time and their comments on our manuscript.

      Reviewer #1 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity (Required)):

      Here Ryan et al. have used localization analysis following induced rapid relocalization of endogenous proteins to investigate the composition and recruitment hierarchy of a clathrin-TACC3-based spindle complex that is important for microtubule organization and stability.

      The authors generate different HeLa cell lines, each with one of four complex members (TACC3, CLTA, chTOG and GTSE1) endogenously tagged with FKBP-GFP via Cas9-mediated editing. This tag allows rapid recruitment to the mitochondria upon rapamycin addition ("knocksideways"). They ultimately quantify each of the 4 components' localization to the spindle following knocksideways of each component using fluorescently-tagged transfected constructs. The authors' interpretation of the results of this analysis are summarized in the last model figure, in which a core MT-binding complex of clathrin and TACC3 recruit the ancillary components GTSE1 and chTOG. In addition, the authors investigate the contribution of individual clathrin-binding LIDL motifs in GTSE1 to the recruitment of clathrin and GTSE1 to spindles. Their findings here largely agree with and confirm a recent report regarding the contribution of these motifs to GTSE1 recruitment to the spindle. They further analyzed GTSE1 fragments for interphase and mitotic microtubule localization, and identified a second region of GTSE1 required (but not sufficient) for spindle localization. Finally, the authors report that PIK3C2A is not part of this complex, contradicting (correcting) a previously published study.

      **Major comments:**

      1.The chTOG-FKBP-GFP cell line the authors generate has only a small fraction of chTOG tagged, and thus should not be used for any conclusions about protein localization dependency on chTOG. Because they were unable to construct a HeLa cell line with all copies tagged, the authors expect that the homozygous knock-in of chTOG-FKBP-GFP is lethal, and thus their experience is appropriate to report. However, the authors should not use this cell line alone to make statements about chTOG dependency. They would have to use similar localization analysis, but after another method to disrupt chTOG (as a second-best approach), such as RNAi. In fact, they have reported this in a previous publication (Booth et al 2011). However, the result was different. There, loss of chTOG resulted in reduced clathrin on spindles, suggesting it may stabilize or help recruit the complex. Alternatively, they could remove their chTOG data, but this would compromise the "comprehensive" nature of the work.

      The referee is correct. The point here is to show the results we had using this approach for all four proteins under study. For this reason, we do not want to remove this data and prefer to show our results “warts-and-all”. We feel that the shortcomings of our approach are honestly presented and discussed in the manuscript. While only a fraction of chTOG was tagged, we should expect some co-removal after its induced mislocalization. Since we saw no change, we concluded that chTOG is auxiliary.

      The “second best” approach suggested (RNAi of chTOG) is problematic for two reasons. First, chTOG RNAi results in gross changes to spindle structure (multipolar spindles) and it is difficult to pick apart differences in protein partner localization that result from loss of chTOG from those resulting from changes in spindle structure. Second, the paper is about induced mislocalization as a method for determining protein complexes once a normal spindle has formed. So, removing chTOG prior to mitosis is not comparable. If we get the same or different result, does it confirm or conflict with the data we have? Nonetheless, given the discrepancy with our earlier work, we should investigate this further.

      To address this concern, we will stain endogenous clathrin, TACC3 and GTSE1 following chTOG RNAi and measure their relative levels at the spindle.

      Making the chTOG-FKBP-GFP cell line was difficult. As described in the paper, we only recovered heterozygous clones despite repeated attempts. Since submission, we have been made aware of a HCT116 chTOG-FKBP-GFP cell line that is reported to be homozygously tagged (Cherry et al. 2019 doi: 10.1002/glia.23628).

      A note about this cell line has been added to the paper (Results section, final sentence of 1st paragraph).

      2.The authors initially analyze complex member localization after knocksideways experiments by antibody staining, which has the advantage of analyzing endogenous proteins (versus the later transfected fluorescent constructs). Setting aside potential artefacts from fixation, this would seem to be a better method for controlled analysis to take advantage of their setup (short of generating stable cell lines with second proteins endogenously tagged in a second color - a huge undertaking). The authors conclude that antibody specificity problems confounded their analysis and explained unusual results. However, I think is worth investing a little more effort to sort this out, rather than bringing doubt to the whole data set. Verifying and then using another antibody for chTOG localization would be informative. Of course, the negative control should not be their chTOG-FKBP-GFP line, as it does not relocalize most of chTOG.

      In the case of GTSE1, an alternative explanation to antibody specificity issues would be that the GTSE1-FKBP-GFP cell line is not in fact homozygously tagged. Given the low expression levels on the western provided, and the detection of GTSE1 on the spindle in the induced GTSE1-FKBP-GFP cell line (but not TACC3-FKBP-GFP), it seems plausible that an untagged copy remains. If there are multiple copies of GTSE1 in Hela cells, one untagged copy could represent a small fraction of total GTSE1. This should thus be ruled out. GTSE1 clones should be analyzed with more protein extracts loaded - dilutions of the extracts can determine the sensitivity of the blot to lower protein levels. In addition, sequencing of genomic DNA can reveal a small percentage with different reads.

      We used a two-pronged approach for assessing relocalization of protein partners (staining vs transfected constructs). The staining approach is superior since endogenous proteins are examined, but it is limited by antibody specificity. The transfection approach overcomes this limitation but is in turn limited by effects of overexpression and tagging. Together the two approaches allow us, and anyone employing this method, to get a picture of protein complexes. We didn’t want to create the impression that one or other approach is confounded, but the referee is correct that this analysis would benefit from further work.

      Specifically, to address these concerns:

      • We will verify and use alternative chTOG antibodies to try to improve this dataset.
      • We will test the possibility that an untagged allele of GTSE1 remains. We will use western blotting and a summary of our genomic analysis will be added to the paper.

        3.There is a lot of data contained in the small graphs summarizing quantification of localization in Figs 3 and 4. They would be more accessible to the reader if they were larger and/or an "example" of the chart with labels was present explaining it (essentially what is in the figure legends). Furthermore, there is no statistical test applied to this data that I see. This is needed. How do authors determine whether there is an "effect"?

      Our aim was to compress a lot of information into a small space, while still showing some example primary data. All reviewers raised the same concern which tells us that we went too far towards “data visualization”.

      To address this point, we will rework these figures.

      **Minor issues:**

      1.The GTSE1 constructs used for mutation and localization analysis are 720 amino acids long. A recent study analyzing similar mutations uses a 739 amino acid construct (Rondelet et al 2020). The latter is the predominant transcript in NCBI and Ensembl databases. It appears the construct used by the authors omits the first 19 a.a.. I do not think using the truncated transcript affects conclusions of the manuscript, but it could generate confusion when identifying residues based on a.a.#s of mutant constructs (Fig 6). This should be somehow clarified.

      We were aware of the longer transcript but were using the 720 residue form since it is the canonical sequence in Uniprot (https://www.uniprot.org/uniprot/Q9NYZ3). We did not know that the 739 form is the predominant transcript. We agree this is unlikely to affect our work but that the numbering may cause confusion.

      We have added a note to the Methods (Molecular Biology section) to accurately describe what we and Rondelet et al. have used.

      2.The labeling of constructs in Fig 6C/D is confusing, and appears shifted by eye at places. Please relabel this more clearly.

      Apologies for the error.

      We have relabeled Figure 6C,D and also made a similar alteration to Figure 5C.

      The recommended new experimental data (Analysis complex member levels on spindles after full perturbation of spindle chTOG; new chTOG antibody stainings in the FKBP lines; reanalysis of GTSE1 DNA/protein in GTSE1-FKBP line) should only require a new antibody/siRNA, plus a few weeks time to repeat the analyses already in the paper with new reagents.

      Reviewer #1 (Significance (Required)):

      While multiple individual components of this complex have been previously characterized, the structure and nature of the complex formation and its recruitment to microtubules/spindles remains a complex problem that has yet to be solved.

      Overall this study represents a comprehensive localization-dependency analysis of the Clathrin-TACC3 based spindle complex using a consistent methodology. Although several of the conclusions of the findings echo previous reports, some of the previous literature is contradictory within itself as well as with the conclusions here. Analyzing all components with a single, rapid-perturbation technique thus has great value to present a clear data set, given that the experimental setup conditions and analysis are solid (a goal to which the majority of comments refer).

      Beyond the complex localization/recruitment analysis, two novel findings of this study that emerge are:

      a)GTSE1 contains a second, separate protein region, distinct from the clathrin-binding motifs that is required for its localization to the spindle, and most likely a microtubule-interaction site. This suggests that GTSE1 recruitment to the spindle is more complex than previously reported.

      b)PI3KC2A, which has been reported previously to be a stabilizing member of this complex, is in fact not a member, nor localizes to spindles, nor displays a mitotic defect after loss. This is important conclusion to be made as it would correct the literature, and avoid future confusion.

      --

      Reviewer #2 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity (Required)):

      In this paper, the authors investigate the nature of interactions between members of the TACC3-chTOG-clathrin-GTSE1 complex on the mitotic spindle. By using a series of HeLa cell lines that they have created by CRISPR/Cas9 editing to enable spatial manipulation (knocksideways) of either TACC3, chTOG, clathrin and GTSE1, they show that on spindle microtubules TACC3 and clathrin represent core complex members whereas chTOG and GTSE1 bind to them respectively but not to each other. Additionally, the authors find that the protein PIK3C2A, which has been implicated in this complex previously is in fact not a component of this complex in mitotic cells. The main advance of the paper in my opinion is the endogenous tagging of the proteins for knocksideways experiments since former experiments depended on RNAi silencing and expression of tagged proteins from plasmids, which introduced issues of protein silencing efficiency and plasmid overexpression problems. This approach seems to alleviate these problems, except in the case of chTOG which seems to be lethal in its homozygous variant.

      **Major comments:**

      I find the key conclusions regarding the localization of the components of the complex convincing. There are some issues regarding the specificity of antibodies in immunostaining experiments (Fig 3.) and the influence of mCherry-TACC3 expression on distorted localization of the complex prior to knocksideways. However, I think the general conclusion about which complex components (clathrin and TACC3) influence the localization of the other proteins in the complex (chTOG and GTSE1) stands. One thing that I miss from the paper is the data on the consequences on the spindle shape and morphology after knocksideways. I have noticed on images in both Figure 3 and Figure 4 that in some cases distribution of the signal seems to influence quite a bit the spindle morphology. Also, In Figure 3 I have noticed what seems to me a quite big variation in spindle size in tubulin signal in both untreated and rapamycin cells. Since authors have many of these images already, I believe it would be realistic, not costly and of additional value for the paper to provide more data on the consequences of the knocksideways experiments. Change of spindle size, tubulin intensity and DNA/kinetochore misalignment upon knocksideways would be helpful to appreciate more the findings of the paper. More so since the authors on more than one occasion find their motivation in the field of cancer research and spindle stability relation to it. Some data connection to this motivation would be of value. Experiments seem reproducible.

      The focus of the paper is on using the knocksideways methodology to understand a protein complex during mitosis, rather than looking at its function. We are not keen to do new experiments that are not part of the central message of the paper. However, the Reviewer is correct that we do already have a dataset that can be mined in the manner described.

      To address this point, we will analyze spindle size parameters and also the intensity of tubulin. Our analysis will be limited to the short timeframe of our experiments, but it should reveal or refute any changes in spindle structure that may result from loss of complex members.

      **Minor comments:**

      I have some problems with the clarity of Figure 3 and 4. For Figure 3. In Figure 3 plots on the right are a bit small and not easy to read. Some reorganization of the figure might be beneficial. In Figure 4 plots to the right are also too small to be clear. Also, I miss the number of cells (n) I can't see the number of individual arrows because of the size of graphs.

      Our aim was to compress a lot of information into a small space, while still showing some example primary data. All reviewers raised the same concern which tells us that we went too far towards “data visualization”.

      To address this point, we will rework these figures.

      Reviewer #2 (Significance (Required)):

      I find that the biggest significance of the paper is in the creation of new tools (cell lines) to study the localization of proteins TACC3, chTOG, clathrin and GTSE1. Cell lines where endogenous proteins can be delocalized rapidly will be of value for scientist working not only in mitosis but such as in the case of clathrin research, vesicle formation and trafficking or p53-dependent apoptosis in the case of GTSE1. In the field of mitosis it will surely help and speed up the research concerning the role of these proteins in spindle assembly and stability.

      Field of expertise: mitotic spindle

      --

      Reviewer #3 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity (Required)):

      **Summary:**

      This papers analyses the chTog/TACC3/clathrin/GTSE1 complex that crosslinks and stabilises microtubule bundles in the mitotic spindle. The authors have developed an elegant knock sideways approach to specifically analyse the effects of removing individual components of the complex from the spindle and study the effect this has on the other interactors. They report, based on these assays that the core of the complex is formed by TACC3 and Clathrin while GTSE1 and chTog are auxiliary interactors. They also refute previous evidence that this complex also incorporates PIK3C2A. Overall, this is an interesting study that distinguishes itself predominantly by its methodology. However, some of the reported results need more thorough analysis to allow convincing conclusions.

      **Major comments:**

      1)The knockside way method is the main highlight if this paper. Unlike previous studies by the PI, this time endogenous genes are tagged which is a key advance and allows much better interpretation of the results. I am not sure why the authors have chosen HeLa cells as their model here, given the messed up genome of these cells. A non-transformed cell line would have been preferable, but as a proof of principle study, I think HeLa are acceptable, and I wouldn't expect the authors to repeat all the experiment in another system.

      Figure 1,2 and S1 are describing and validating this approach in some detail, but this will require some more work.

      The authors state that gene targeting was validated using a combination of PCR, sequencing, Western blotting, but show only the results for westerns. PCR analysis that demonstrates homozygous or heterozygous gene targeting should be shown here.

      Another issue is the penetrance of the phenotypes induced by Rapamycin. The authors show nice data of the system working in individual cells but do not give us an idea if this happens in all cells. The localisation of the individual tagged genes should be quantified (ideally with line plots) in 50 randomly chosen mitotic cells with 3 repeats before and after rapamycin treatment. Moreover, the analysis of mitotic duration (Figure S1D) should be extended to include a plus Rapamycin cohort and this should be moved in the main Figure.

      If the system works only in a small proportion of cells, this should be clearly stated. I don't think this would prevent publication, but it is an important piece of information that is missing.

      The Reviewer raises two issues here.

      • PCR analysis should be shown. This issue was also partly raised by Reviewer 1. A summary of our PCR analysis was actually included in Table 1, since the analysis we did is pretty unwieldy. We agree though that presenting our evidence for homozygosity of the cell lines would be useful. To address this point, we will add more detail of the PCR and sequencing work done to validate these cell lines.
      • Does knocksideways happen in all cells? The answer to this depends on the transient expression of MitoTrap and sufficient application of rapamycin. We agree that this will be a useful piece of information to add to the manuscript. A related issue is whether knocksideways of complex members affects mitotic progression. We have established through other experiments that rapamycin application to wild-type cells alters mitotic progression, although application of Rapalog does not have this effect. Our plan to address these points is 1) to analyze the efficacy of knocksideways that readers can expect to achieve using these, or similar cells, and 2) analyze mitotic duration in rapalog-treated cells expressing a rapalog sensitive MitoTrap.

        2)Apart from a simple quantification of mitotic duration, I believe a more detailed mitotic phenotype analysis for each knock-side way gene, especially the homozygous targeted clones, should be included. This can involve more high-resolution live cell imaging of mitotic progression with SiR-DNA and GFP-tubulin, using the dark mitotrap.

      We don’t agree that such an analysis should be included. The focus of this paper is on using the knocksideways methodology to understand a protein complex during mitosis, and not looking at its function. There are several papers on the mitotic phenotypes of these genes probed using RNAi in different cellular systems (examples for chTOG: 10.1101/gad.245603; TACC3/clathrin: 10.1038/emboj.2011.15, 10.1242/jcs.075911, 10.1083/jcb.200911091, 10.1083/jcb.200911120; GTSE1: 10.1083/jcb.201606081). Moreover, our 2013 paper used knocksideways (with RNAi and overexpression) and has a detailed analysis of mitotic progression, microtubule stability, checkpoint activity and kinetochore motions (Cheeseman et al., 2013 doi: 10.1242/jcs.124834).

      New experiments that are not part of the central message of the paper and are unlikely to give new insight are not the best use of our revision efforts for this paper (especially during the pandemic). Having said this, Reviewer 2’s suggestion to use our existing dataset to investigate mitotic phenotypes, will largely answer Reviewer 3’s request.

      We will analyze spindle size parameters and also the intensity of tubulin. Our analysis will be limited to the short timeframe of our experiments, but it should reveal or refute any changes in spindle structure that result from the loss of complex members.

      3)Overall, the quantitative analysis in Figure 3 ,4 and 7 is not good enough and sometimes doesn't fully support the conclusions. In Figure 3,4 a convoluted way of demonstrating the change in localisation is shown and this panel is so small that is almost impossible to read. Also, there is no statistical analysis, and the sample size seems very small . At least 25 cells should be analysed here in 3 repeats. I would suggest to unify the quantification in the MS and use the line plots shown in Figure 5 and 6 and compare each protein before and after rapamycin addition. This is much easier to read and more convincing. The images of the cells panels can be moved to a supplement as they contain very little information. This would generate space to expand the size and depth of the quantitative analysis. Instead of Anova tests, I would recommend using a simple t-test comparing each condition to its relevant control since this is the only relevant comparison in the experiment. Statistical significance should be calculated for each experiment with sufficient sample size. It would also be better to show the individual data points from the three repeats in different colours so that the reproducibility between repeat can be judged.

      This type of statistical analysis should be uniformly done throughout the MS and also extended to Figure 7.

      The referee raises several issues here with our data presentation and statistical analysis.

      • Our aim in Figures 3 and 4 was to compress a lot of information into a small space, while still showing some example primary data. All reviewers raised the same concern about these figures which tells us that we went too far towards “data visualization”. To address this point, we will rework Figures 3 and 4 to provide more clear data presentation.
      • The Reviewer’s comments about statistical analysis however are not sound. First, it is incorrect to state that simple t-tests can be applied (this is a form of p-hacking). Correction for multiple testing must be done on these datasets. Second, the reviewer arbitrarily states numbers for cells and experimental repeats without considering the effect size or it seems, understanding the structure of the data that we have collected. Sample sizes are small but they are taken from many independent replicates. Third, and related to the previous point, the fixed and live cell data are structured differently which means that a uniform data presentation is not possible. The live data has a paired design and each cell is an independent replicate (with replicates done over several trials). The fixed data is unpaired and we have taken measures from several experiments (independent replicates). The point about applying statistical tests to the data is also made by Reviewer 1 and we will use appropriate tests (NHST or estimation statistics) as we re-work the figures.

        Reviewer #3 (Significance (Required)):

      In my opinion, the most interesting aspect of the MS is the methodology. Based on this, publication is justified and will be of interest to a wider audience. That is why a more detailed analysis of the penetrance of this manipulation across the cell population will be critical.

      The application of this method to analyse the composition of the TACC3/Clathrin complex on the spindle is the main biological advance, and the novel information is rather limited but not unimportant.

      Overall, if these results can be properly quantified I would recommend publication.

    1. What I'm up to is this: I'm trying to provide students with an op-portunity to think about ordinary things in their lives, like classroom furniture arrangements, and push them to find connections between how they sit in a classroom and how they learn to view themselves in a larger political world. I want them to think about what other than math or English is being taught in a classroom divided into rows. What "hidden lessons" are being imparted about power, learning, and equali-ty, what lessons do students learn about who they are from the material shaping of their space?

      I think this is a crucial note; however, I think the central focus of this is warped deeply by the reality of social distancing. I wonder how we may be able to find "hidden lessons" within the (no pun intended) small wiggle room we have physically/spatially in the classroom as of now.

    1. South African literature is a literature in bondage, as it reveals in even its highest m om ents, shot through as they are with feelings of homelessness and yearnings for a nameless liber-ation.

      I think the author is referring to how even though it may seem there is movement towards equality, the damage and trauma done under apartheid and colonialism cannot be forgotten among South Africans, which is seen in South African literature. The author's discussion about power dynamics reminded me of a previous reading we discussed about minor literatures. One of the struggles they may be dealing with is being a minor literature in their own country - even though they are technically the majority, they are fighting for their rights and freedoms.

    1. Jonathan Edwards catalyzed the revivals known as the Great Awakening. While Edwards was not the most prolific revivalist of the era—that honor belonged to George Whitefield—he did deliver the most famous sermon of the eighteenth century, commonly called “Sinners in the Hands of Angry God.” This excerpt is drawn from the final portion of the sermon, known as the application, where hearers were called to take action.     That world of misery, that lake of burning brimstone is extended abroad under you. There is the dreadful pit of the glowing flames of the wrath of God; there is hell’s wide gaping mouth open; and you have nothing to stand upon, nor anything to take hold of: there is nothing between you and hell but the air; ’tis only the power and mere pleasure of God that holds you up. You probably are not sensible of this; you find you are kept out of hell, but don’t see the hand of God in it, but look at other things, as the good state of your bodily constitution, your care of your own life, and the means you use for your own preservation. But indeed these things are nothing; if God should withdraw his hand, they would avail no more to keep you from falling, than the thin air to hold up a person that is suspended in it…. The God that holds you over the pit of hell, much as one holds a spider, or some loathsome insect, over the fire, abhors you, and is dreadfully provoked; his wrath towards you burns like fire; he looks upon you as worthy of nothing else, but to be cast into the fire; he is of purer eyes than to bear to have you in his sight; you are ten thousand times so abominable in his eyes as the most hateful venomous serpent is in ours. You have offended him infinitely more than ever a stubborn rebel did his prince: and yet ’tis nothing but his hand that holds you from falling into the fire every moment; ’tis to be ascribed to nothing else, that you did not go to hell the last night; that you was suffered to awake again in this world, after you closed your eyes to sleep: and there is no other reason to be given why you have not dropped into hell since you arose in the morning, but that God’s hand has held you up; there is no other reason to be given why you han’t gone to hell since you have sat here in the house of God, provoking his pure eyes by your sinful wicked manner of attending his solemn worship: yea, there is nothing else that is to be given as a reason why you don’t this very moment drop down into hell. O sinner! Consider the fearful danger you are in: ’tis a great furnace of wrath, a wide and bottomless pit, full of the fire of wrath, that you are held over in the hand of that God, whose wrath is provoked and incensed as much against you as against many of the damned in hell; you hang by a slender thread, with the flames of divine wrath flashing about it, and ready every moment to singe it, and burn it asunder; and you have no interest in any mediator, and nothing to lay hold of to save yourself, nothing to keep off the flames of wrath, nothing of your own, nothing that you ever have done, nothing that you can do, to induce God to spare you one moment… Consider this, you that are here present, that yet remain in an unregenerate state. That God will execute the fierceness of his anger, implies that he will inflict wrath without any pity… you will be a vessel of wrath fitted to destruction; and there will be no other use of this vessel but only to be filled full of wrath: God will be so far from pitying you when you cry to him, that ’tis said he will only laugh and mock (Proverbs 1:25-32)… How dreadful is the state of those that are daily and hourly in danger of this great wrath, and infinite misery! But this is the dismal case of every soul in this congregation, that has not been born again, however moral and strict, sober and religious they may otherwise be. Oh that you would consider it, whether you be young or old. There is reason to think, that there are many in this congregation now hearing this discourse, that will actually be the subjects of this very misery to all eternity. We know not who they are, or in what seats they sit, or what thoughts they now have: it may be they are now at ease, and hear all these things without much disturbance, and are now flattering themselves that they are not the persons, promising themselves that they shall escape. If we knew that there was one person, and but one, in the whole congregation that was to be the subject of this misery, what an awful thing would it be to think of! If we knew who it was, what an awful sight would it be to see such a person! How might all the rest of the congregation lift up a lamentable and bitter cry over him! But alas! instead of one, how many is it likely will remember this discourse in hell? And it would be a wonder if some that are now present, should not be in hell in a very short time, before this year is out. And it would be no wonder if some person that now sits here in some seat of this meeting house in health, and quiet and secure, should be there before tomorrow morning. Those of you that finally continue in a natural condition, that shall keep out of hell longest, will be there in a little time! your damnation don’t slumber; it will come swiftly, and in all probability very suddenly upon many of you. You have reason to wonder, that you are not already in hell. ‘Tis doubtless the case of some that heretofore you have seen and known, that never deserved hell more than you, and that heretofore appeared as likely to have been now alive as you: their case is past all hope; they are crying in extreme misery and perfect despair; but here you are in the land of the living, and in the house of God, and have an opportunity to obtain salvation. What would not those poor damned, hopeless souls give for one day’s such opportunity as you now enjoy! And now you have an extraordinary opportunity, a day wherein Christ has flung the door of mercy wide open, and stands in the door calling and crying with a loud voice to poor sinners; a day wherein many are flocking to him, and pressing into the kingdom of God; many are daily coming from the east, west, north and south; many that were very lately in the same miserable condition that you are in, are in now an happy state, with their hearts filled with love to him that has loved them and washed them from their sins in his own blood, and rejoicing in hope of the glory of God. How awful is it to be left behind at such a day! To see so many others feasting, while you are pining and perishing! To see so many rejoicing and singing for joy of heart, while you have cause to mourn for sorrow of heart, and howl for vexation of spirit! How can you rest one moment in such a condition? Are not your souls as precious as the souls of the people at Suffield,7 where they are flocking from day to day to Christ? … And you children that are unconverted, don’t you know that you are going down to hell, to bear the dreadful wrath of that God that is now angry with you every day, and every night? Will you be content to be the children of the devil, when so many other children in the land are converted, and are become the holy and happy children of the King of kings? And let everyone that is yet out of Christ, and hanging over the pit of hell, whether they be old men and women, or middle aged, or young people, or little children, now hearken to the loud calls of God’s Word and providence. This acceptable year of the Lord, that is a day of such great favor to some, will doubtless be a day of as remarkable vengeance to others… Therefore let everyone that is out of Christ, now awake and fly from the wrath to come. The wrath of almighty God is now undoubtedly hanging over great part of this congregation: let everyone fly out of Sodom. Haste and escape for your lives, look not behind you, escape to the mountain, lest you be consumed [Genesis 19:17].

      This sermon is truly eye opening to those who have never read it before. I have never seen such bold, outright wording in a religious setting and am somewhat shocked by the abrasiveness of the pastor. Edwards warns the audience that they will go to hell if they don't repent for their sins and find their way back to God. The audience is obviously church-goers yet Edwards is fervent in his accusations that they are not righteous people. The message was well-received due to the impact it left and may have been well-intended. However, the pastor could have shown more care in his more outrageous accusations. Religion is very important to some people but it is not something that should consume ones whole life. God is meant to be fair and loving therefore presenting him as a being that is dangling people and waiting for them to drop into hell is quite shocking and somewhat dramatic. God would not abandon one of his creations because they don't worship the same way as someone else.

    2. There is reason to think, that there are many in this congregation now hearing this discourse, that will actually be the subjects of this very misery to all eternity. We know not who they are, or in what seats they sit, or what thoughts they now have: it may be they are now at ease, and hear all these things without much disturbance, and are now flattering themselves that they are not the persons, promising themselves that they shall escape.

      Edwards here makes the strongest connection to his audience in the document as he specifically points out people who may have not been getting his message. In pointing out people who may be listening but change nothing or simply choose to not believe him are the most likely to simply put themselves in a position where God will not allow them to escape. I feel that at the time this sermon was given there were defiantly people listening to his sermon that just though about how all he was saying were lies and to stick to the old ways of thinking, but I wonder how these people were effected when this truly blew up with the great awakening and if they themselves had to come to terms with accepting this new way of thinking or being push away. It is an interesting point because many people were effected by the sermon some fainting others crying but what about the ones who thought it was all some big hoax?

    1. Reviewer #1

      The authors propose two related (though distinct) methods for the improvement of pharmacological screening analysis and related biomarker analyses. The first is a Gaussian process (GP) approach to dose-response curve fitting for the estimation of IC50, AUC, and related quantities. The goal of this method is to improve point and uncertainty estimates of these quantities through more flexible functional specification and outlier-robust error modeling. The second method is a hierarchical Bayesian approach to biomarker association analysis. This incorporates uncertainty estimates produced by the GP modeling with the aim of providing more sensitive association analyses with fewer false positives.

      The combination of methods presented has some potential. Flexible modeling of dose-response relationships and better estimation of uncertainty are interesting axes to wring more information out of large-scale screening datasets. There are a few areas to shore up in the paper to increase confidence in the empirical results and generalizability of the methods.

      1) There are a number of fixed parameters in the proposed methods, and the calibration procedure used to set these is unclear to me. For the GP models, there are a set of noise parameters for Beta mixture and the length scales and variance parameter for the kernel. I'm not sure how one would generalize the GP methods to other screening datasets as a result of this ambiguity (e.g., how would one determine appropriate noise parameters?). For the hierarchical Bayesian biomarker association model, we have prior scale parameters related to both the effect size and variance parameters. The number of researcher degrees of freedom introduced by these tuned parameters also raises some concerns about the sensitivity of empirical results (e.g., 24 clinically established biomarkers and 6 novel) to these choices. It's not clear if we're seeing a corner case or a robust result. I think the work would benefit from both sensitivity analyses with respect to tuned parameters and guidance on or methods for their estimation. The latter is particularly important if other researchers hope to employ these methods in a different context.

      2) The proposed hierarchical Bayesian approach to biomarker association analysis is a reasonable start, but it was unclear to me whether changes in performance stemmed from correcting misspecification in original ANOVA or the use of uncertainty estimates. I suggest comparing results to a heteroskedasticity-robust estimator (e.g., HC3, see Long and Ervin, 2000), which would be valid under the stated model without the requirement for explicit uncertainty estimates or priors. The transformations and tuning applied to uncertainty estimates in this context also make generalization of the approach challenging. The need for the c (power) parameter suggests a potential misspecification or miscalibration at some point in the modeling chain. It would be useful to understand this misspecification better, particularly for researchers hoping to extend or reuse these methods.

      3) The GP method provides reasonable estimates of uncertainty, but it would be useful to see them compared to those from the sigmoid model (e.g., from the delta method). It wasn't clear to me how much of the difference in results is coming from incorporation of uncertainty estimates as opposed to changes in the point estimates.

      4) The handling of cases with IC50 beyond the maximum observed dose (extrapolating to 10x the maximum concentration) provided a reasonable starting point, but a few subtleties in the handling of corner cases remain unaddressed (e.g., GPs allow positive slope at right edge of range). It would be useful to provide a more general, systematic procedure to address these. Imposing monotonicity may not be the best path, but additional guidance for researchers applying these methods in other contexts would help.

    1. Preprint Review

      This preprint was reviewed using eLife’s Preprint Review service, which provides public peer reviews of manuscripts posted on bioRxiv for the benefit of the authors, readers, potential readers, and others interested in our assessment of the work. This review applies only to version 3 of the manuscript.

      Summary

      In this manuscript the authors propose the identification of a novel protein involved in outer membrane remodelling, named BdpA (BAR domain-like protein A). According to the proposed model BdpA has a conserved role in membrane curvature control during formation of outer membrane vesicle (OMV) and of outer membrane extension (OMEs) in Shewanella oneidensis. The authors also provide evidence that heterologous expression of BdpA promotes formation of OMEs in other bacteria (namely in E. coli), and that BdpA is sufficient to induce OME-like structures when expressed in conditions where OMEs are normally not formed. In eukaryotes proteins containing BAR domains are important for shaping membrane curvature. Given the homology of BdpA to eukaryotic BAR-domain proteins, the authors suggest that BdpA and its homologs define the first prokaryotic family of BAR proteins or pBARs, with eukaryotic-like roles in membrane curvature modulation.

      Overall, the reviewers think that this is a very interesting study, and provided that further support is obtained to substantiate the proposed model the reviewers agree that the findings described here tackle a number of significant questions of broad interest. However, the reviewers also think that the evidence provided in this manuscript still does not fully support the conclusion that BdpA protein is involved in membrane curvature control as the eukaryotic proteins containing the BAR domains.

      We have compiled a list of comments that we hope will help the authors address the concerns of the reviewers to obtain stronger support for the function of BdpA.

      1) The reviewers are concerned that some of the conclusions are based on qualitative observations of microscopy analysis of OMVs and OMEs, and quantitative analyses are lacking to validate qualitative observations. As specified in with examples in the list of minor points below the reviewers propose that the data should be re-analyzed to obtain quantitative results. Specifically, a size distribution analysis could be applied to some microscopy data. Also note that the microscopy methods are poorly described, and as the calculation methods used are not fully available it is difficult to understand if the appropriate methods were used. Please specify how many cells were examined microscopically and how many biological replicates (cultures) were used in each experiment.

      2) Statistical analyses were not always the most accurate. In figure 2 unpaired t-test was used for samples that have high variance, this approach may inflate the statistical difference between the strains. For figure 2 a histogram of size distribution analyses could be shown for each strain.

      3) The reviewers are concerned that the proteomic data is not clear enough to conclude that the BdpA protein is localized to or enriched in OMV/OME. Could the results be complemented with some other method to confirm BdpA localization? The reviewers are particularly concerned by the fact that a large number of proteins were identified in the OMV fraction. Could it be that some of the OMV/OME fractions were contaminated? What controls were used to ensure that the purification procedure was working effectively? Could the data be strengthened by some quality control analyses to determine how many of those proteins are actually predicted to localize to the outer membrane and periplasm? From the methods it seems that the culture conditions used to prepare the OM versus OMV were different, is this so? If yes, why were the culture conditions different? This could affect protein expression? Please include the detailed growth conditions in the method section.

      4) The conclusion that BdpA is a BAR-domain protein is largely based on homology. The supplementary information file includes homology models that show striking similarity with eukaryotic BAR proteins. However, as the authors state, BdpA barely meets the cutoff for a BAR-domain protein. The results with the phenotype of the BdpA mutant, complementations and sufficiency data provide good support to the functional role of BdpA in membrane remodelling. However, the effect of BdpA on membrane stability could be indirect or the result of binding to outer membrane features in a manner distinct from other BAR proteins. Could these results be strengthened with some biochemical corroboration of its activity on membranes or structural data to confirm its relationship to eukaryotic Bar domain proteins? Or structural data to confirm its relationship to eukaryotic BAR domain proteins?

      5) The reviewers propose that the paper would be strengthened with the addition of topological studies in OMVs and OMEs. The reviewers had problems in reconciling the presence of a galactose-binding domain in BdpA and LPS sugar binding. The authors hypothesize that the putative Galactose-binding domain of BdpA mediates binding to LPS. However, it is also possible that it binds to peptidoglycan components. This would suggest that the proteins interact with the periplasmic side of the outer membrane rather than coat the OMV to promote OMV formation and release (which one could assume based on the role of some eukaryotic BARs). The addition of topological studies (or some biochemical approach) could make these models less speculative, strengthening the conclusions.

      6) Heterologous expression of BdpA in other bacteria provides important compelling arguments for its central role in producing OMEs. However, the imaging data provided do not provide the clearest evidence for induction of OMEs in M. atlanticus and E. coli. This is especially the case with the E. coli images. The extended web of staining in 4c does not resemble the tubules seen in S. oneidensis. It would be great to have some electron microscopy data and/or higher resolution fluorescence images of these bacteria as corroborating evidence. Additionally, only a few cells are shown and quantification of the proportion of cells with OMEs is needed. Thus, as already discussed in point 1, quantitative analyses could improve this important point.

    1. But with whatever success the doctrine of Inherited Variation may be applied to explain the existence of Varieties, it is certain that the origin of Species can be accounted for on the Development Theory, if at all, only by Cumulative Variation, -- that is, only by supposing a vast number of Inherited Variations to be successively superinduced one upon another. Doubts have been raised upon this point only on account of ambiguity in the meaning of words, or from want of agreement as to the principles of classification. Many races, both of animals and vegetables, appear to be so nearly allied to each other, that certain naturalists consider them as mere Varieties; others persist in considering them as so many distinct Species.

      This is quite important to Development Theory, as it presents ideas about different ways of thinking. It shows that people, including scientists think differently about this topic even when evidence may be presented for a different side. There is also evidence for that other side as well, which may bring up we do not know exact origins of species, especially back then.

    2. Mr. Darwin has invented a new scheme of cosmogony, and finds that, like other cosmogonies, it is a blank hypothesis, not susceptible either of proof or disproof, and needing an eternity for its development. There is nothing new in such a speculation of what is possible in an infinite lapse of years. This latest form of the speculation has no advantage over the one first propounded some three thousand years ago; -- that a chaos of atoms, moving about fortuitously in infinite space, may have happened, in an eternity, to settle into the present kosmos; for the chance of order and fitness is at least one out of an infinite number of chances of disorder and confusion; and in an infinite series of years, this solitary chance must sooner or later be realized....  

      I don't think that Darwin's theory of evolution was proposed in order to track origins of the universe. It was developed as an explanation for the changes in species we see on earth and even if it were to be considered a theory of cosmogony, it would have many advantages over the other theory of cosmogony which was stated.

    1. (A) One of the most important objects of measurement is hardly if at all alluded to here and should be emphasised. It is to obtain a general knowledge of the capacities of a man by sinking shafts, as it were, at a few critical points. In order to ascertain the best points for the purpose, the sets of measures should be compared with an independent estimate of the man's powers. We thus may learn which of the measures are the most instructive, The sort of estimate I have in view and which I would suggest should benoted [? for private use] is something of this kind,-"mobile, eager, energetic; well shaped; successful at games requiring good eye and hand; sensitive; good at music and drawing". Such estimates would be far from worthless when made after only a few minutes' talk; they ought to be exact when made of students who have been for months and years under observation. I lately saw a considerable collection of such estimates, made by a medical man for a special purpose. They were singularly searching and they hit off, with a few well chosen epithets, a very great variety of different characters. I could not induce the medical man to consent to the publication of specimens of his excellent analyses, nor even of fancy specimens. Even these would have sufficed to show that if psychologists seriously practised the art of briefly describing characters. they might raise that art to a high level. (B) The method I have long used for testing keenness of eye-sight in persons whose powers of eye-adaptation are normal, still seems to me quite effective. It is to register the greatest distance at which numerals printed in diamond type can be read. Strips of paper cut out at random from a small sheet printed all over with these numerals, are mounted on blocks set at successive distances from the eye-hole. They can easily be changed when dirty. Fair light is wanted, but that is all that is needed for ordinary test purposes. C) I have constructed an instrument which is not yet quite as I desire , of which the first part would I think greatly facilitate [p. 381] the working with the Hipp chronograph. I had found great trouble in inducing coarse and inexperienced persons to deliver their blows aright. They bungled and struck the instrument wrongly, and often broke it. Then I made it more massive, yet still they broke it and often hurt themselves much in doing so. My present plan is to give them nothing more than one end of a long thread to hold. The other end passes round a spring reel, like the tape in a spring measuring tape. The string when left to itself will reel home much faster than the swiftest blow can travel. All that the experimentee does is to retard it; the quickest man retarding it the least. The string travels smoothly and swiftly in a straight line between two eyelet holes. A bead attached to that part of the string would make the necessary breaks of electric contact with great neatness. The thread has a stop to cheek it when it has run far enough home. My reel is nothing more than a very light wooden wheel with a groove in it, some 3 inches in diameter, and with a brass axis turning freely between fixed points. One thread passes round the axis, and is tied at the other end to an india rubber band. The other thread passes in the opposite direction round the grooved wheel, and then through the eyelet holes. The experimentee is placed well back, quite clear of the apparatus. Nothing can act better than this part of my new instrument. (D) I now use a very neat, compact, and effective apparatus (made for me by Groves, 89, Bolsover Street, Portland Street, W.) which is a half-second's pendulum, held by a detent 18º from the vertical. The blow of a released hammer upon the detent gives the sound-signal and simultaneously lets the pendulum go. An elastic thread is fixed to the pendulum parallel to its axis, but about 1½ inch apart from it. As the pendulum oscillates this thread travels between 2 bars; the one fixed, the other movable. The fixed bar lies horizontally between the pendulum and the thread and is graduated. The movable bar nips the thread when a key is touched. Doing this, constitutes the response. The pendulum itself receives no jar through the act, owing to the elasticity of the thread. The graduations on the bar, that forms the chord to an arc of 18º on each side of the vertical, are calculated and published in the Jour. Anthrop. Inst. early last year, 1889, together with my description of the first form of the instrument. I exhibited the revised form of it at the British Association last autumn ; a brief description of it will appear in their Journal. The instrument is arranged for sight-signals as well. It is also arranged to measure the rapidity with which any given act can be performed. The experimentee touches a key that releases the pendulum ; then he performs the act; finally he touches the second key, that causes the thread to be nipped. Footnotes [1] Mr. Francis Galton, in his Anthropometric Laboratory at South Kensignton Museum, already uses some of these tests, and I hope the series here suggested will meet with his approval. It is convenient to follow Mr. Galton in combining tests of body, such as weight, size, colour of eyes, & c., with psychophysical and mental determinations, but these latter alone are the subject of the present discussion. The name (or initials) of the experimentee should be recorded, the nationality (including that of the parents), and the age, sex, occupation and sate of health. [See Remark (a) by Mr. Galton] [2] Sharpness of sight (including colour-vision) and hearing might, perhaps, be included in the list. I have omitted them because it requires considerable time to discover the amount and nature of the defect (which is usually bodily, not mental), and because abundant statistics have been published, and are being collected by oculists and aurists. [See Remark ( b )] [3] See Remark ( c ). [4] See Remark ( d ) [5] Organic sensations and sensations of motion, equilibrium and dizziness, should perhaps be included in this series.

      This is just slight changes to tests that Cattel now uses to conduct research. Mostly changes in apparatus to provide a slightly more accurate measurement.

    1. Lawn- care vendor's focused on the new suburban yard, specialized sports fields, golf courses, and municipal landscaping as bounteous new surfaces on which to wield their wares. Monsanto , Dupont, and other companies marketed the dream of the perfect carpet lawn through "time-saving" lawn care systems that included nitrogen, phosphorus, potassium, (N-P-K) fertilizers, Engineered seeds , pesticides and equipment. A lush, homogeneous lawn soon became the underlay of the American dream. The greener, more neatly shorn or the more homogenous of the surface the better. Chemically fertilized turf grass now pervades the American landscape, from golf courses to Central Park's Sheep Meadow, covering an estimated 28 million acres.

      Relate:

      When talking about turf in the landscape I think there are a lot of levels to the idea of using it in the landscape. It is a complicated subject because it has become so ingrained into our culture. If it is using over 70 million tons of pesticides to manage it then how do, we reduce that and change the culture of these huge lawns. I don’t think that all lawns should be disposed of, but we could reduce the amount that is in the landscape. When I was working for a landscaping company there would be these huge hillsides of perfectly manicured turf that provided no recreation or real purpose to the design. I think if we reduces the amount of these areas , but also stopped trying to put turf in the wrong environments then they may not have as many problems with pests, or we could just reduce the amount of turf used. I also think the idea of not using turf in all areas should be advertised as something unique and prideful the same way turf was advertised.

    1. There is nothing that is definite or absolute. We try all the time to interpret, through their gestures, words, and actions, how they are living through an experience; and then we go on from there

      I think this is important to note. Though the teachers are trying to find those moments of "knots" through observations, the receptiveness of the children may not be what the teacher is hoping to see as a "sticking point".

    1. Why Are Finland’s Schools Successful? The country’s achievements in education have other nations, especially the United States, doing their homework <img src="https://thumbs-prod.si-cdn.com/thzZYTv2Evhq3x8iHdcaakihfVE=/800x600/filters:no_upscale()/https://public-media.si-cdn.com/filer/cd/ee/cdee1c82-f8e3-4de4-983e-8599d4485745/finland-smiles-wr.jpg" alt="Kirkkojarvi School" itemprop="image"> "This is what we do every day," says Kirkkojarvi Comprehensive School principal Kari Louhivuori, "prepare kids for life." (Stuart Conway) By LynNell Hancock Smithsonian Magazine | Subscribe September 2011 AddThis Sharing ButtonsShare to FacebookFacebookShare to TwitterTwitterShare to RedditReddit78Share to PinterestPinterest997Share to LinkedInLinkedInShare to FlipboardFlipboardShare to EmailEmailShare to PrintPrintShare to MoreAddThis934 It was the end of term at Kirkkojarvi Comprehensive School in Espoo, a sprawling suburb west of Helsinki, when Kari Louhivuori, a veteran teacher and the school’s principal, decided to try something extreme—by Finnish standards. One of his sixth-grade students, a Kosovo-Albanian boy, had drifted far off the learning grid, resisting his teacher’s best efforts. The school’s team of special educators—including a social worker, a nurse and a psychologist—convinced Louhivuori that laziness was not to blame. So he decided to hold the boy back a year, a measure so rare in Finland it’s practically obsolete. function dispatchComscoreLoadedEvent(){ let event = new Event('MPlayerComscoreLoaded'); window.dispatchEvent(event); } !function(e){var t={};function n(r){if(t[r])return t[r].exports;var i=t[r]={i:r,l:!1,exports:{}};return e[r].call(i.exports,i,i.exports,n),i.l=!0,i.exports}n.m=e,n.c=t,n.d=function(e,t,r){n.o(e,t)||Object.defineProperty(e,t,{enumerable:!0,get:r})},n.r=function(e){"undefined"!==typeof Symbol&&Symbol.toStringTag&&Object.defineProperty(e,Symbol.toStringTag,{value:"Module"}),Object.defineProperty(e,"__esModule",{value:!0})},n.t=function(e,t){if(1&t&&(e=n(e)),8&t)return e;if(4&t&&"object"===typeof e&&e&&e.__esModule)return e;var r=Object.create(null);if(n.r(r),Object.defineProperty(r,"default",{enumerable:!0,value:e}),2&t&&"string"!=typeof e)for(var i in e)n.d(r,i,function(t){return e[t]}.bind(null,i));return r},n.n=function(e){var t=e&&e.__esModule?function(){return e.default}:function(){return e};return n.d(t,"a",t),t},n.o=function(e,t){return Object.prototype.hasOwnProperty.call(e,t)},n.p="",n(n.s=68)}([function(e,t){e.exports=function(e,t,n){return t in e?Object.defineProperty(e,t,{value:n,enumerable:!0,configurable:!0,writable:!0}):e[t]=n,e}},function(e,t){e.exports=function(e,t){if(!(e instanceof t))throw new TypeError("Cannot call a class as a function")}},function(e,t){function n(e,t){for(var n=0;n<t.length;n++){var r=t[n];r.enumerable=r.enumerable||!1,r.configurable=!0,"value"in r&&(r.writable=!0),Object.defineProperty(e,r.key,r)}}e.exports=function(e,t,r){return t&&n(e.prototype,t),r&&n(e,r),e}},function(e,t){e.exports=function(e){if(void 0===e)throw new ReferenceError("this hasn't been initialised - super() hasn't been called");return e}},function(e,t,n){e.exports=n(47)},function(e,t){function n(t){return e.exports=n=Object.setPrototypeOf?Object.getPrototypeOf:function(e){return e.__proto__||Object.getPrototypeOf(e)},n(t)}e.exports=n},function(e,t,n){"use strict";var r=n(21),i=Object.prototype.toString;function o(e){return"[object Array]"===i.call(e)}function a(e){return"undefined"===typeof e}function s(e){return null!==e&&"object"===typeof e}function u(e){return"[object Function]"===i.call(e)}function c(e,t){if(null!==e&&"undefined"!==typeof e)if("object"!==typeof e&&(e=[e]),o(e))for(var n=0,r=e.length;n<r;n++)t.call(null,e[n],n,e);else for(var i in e)Object.prototype.hasOwnProperty.call(e,i)&&t.call(null,e[i],i,e)}e.exports={isArray:o,isArrayBuffer:function(e){return"[object ArrayBuffer]"===i.call(e)},isBuffer:function(e){return null!==e&&!a(e)&&null!==e.constructor&&!a(e.constructor)&&"function"===typeof e.constructor.isBuffer&&e.constructor.isBuffer(e)},isFormData:function(e){return"undefined"!==typeof FormData&&e instanceof FormData},isArrayBufferView:function(e){return"undefined"!==typeof ArrayBuffer&&ArrayBuffer.isView?ArrayBuffer.isView(e):e&&e.buffer&&e.buffer instanceof ArrayBuffer},isString:function(e){return"string"===typeof e},isNumber:function(e){return"number"===typeof e},isObject:s,isUndefined:a,isDate:function(e){return"[object Date]"===i.call(e)},isFile:function(e){return"[object File]"===i.call(e)},isBlob:function(e){return"[object Blob]"===i.call(e)},isFunction:u,isStream:function(e){return s(e)&&u(e.pipe)},isURLSearchParams:function(e){return"undefined"!==typeof URLSearchParams&&e instanceof URLSearchParams},isStandardBrowserEnv:function(){return("undefined"===typeof navigator||"ReactNative"!==navigator.product&&"NativeScript"!==navigator.product&&"NS"!==navigator.product)&&"undefined"!==typeof window&&"undefined"!==typeof document},forEach:c,merge:function e(){var t={};function n(n,r){"object"===typeof t[r]&&"object"===typeof n?t[r]=e(t[r],n):t[r]=n}for(var r=0,i=arguments.length;r<i;r++)c(arguments[r],n);return t},deepMerge:function e(){var t={};function n(n,r){"object"===typeof t[r]&&"object"===typeof n?t[r]=e(t[r],n):t[r]="object"===typeof n?e({},n):n}for(var r=0,i=arguments.length;r<i;r++)c(arguments[r],n);return t},extend:function(e,t,n){return c(t,function(t,i){e[i]=n&&"function"===typeof t?r(t,n):t}),e},trim:function(e){return e.replace(/^\s*/,"").replace(/\s*$/,"")}}},function(e,t){function n(e,t,n,r,i,o,a){try{var s=e[o](a),u=s.value}catch(c){return void n(c)}s.done?t(u):Promise.resolve(u).then(r,i)}e.exports=function(e){return function(){var t=this,r=arguments;return new Promise(function(i,o){var a=e.apply(t,r);function s(e){n(a,i,o,s,u,"next",e)}function u(e){n(a,i,o,s,u,"throw",e)}s(void 0)})}}},function(e,t,n){var r=n(48);e.exports=function(e,t){if("function"!==typeof t&&null!==t)throw new TypeError("Super expression must either be null or a function");e.prototype=Object.create(t&&t.prototype,{constructor:{value:e,writable:!0,configurable:!0}}),t&&r(e,t)}},function(e,t,n){var r=n(49),i=n(3);e.exports=function(e,t){return!t||"object"!==r(t)&&"function"!==typeof t?i(e):t}},function(e,t,n){var r=n(42),i=n(43),o=n(18),a=n(44);e.exports=function(e,t){return r(e)||i(e,t)||o(e,t)||a()}},function(e,t,n){var r=n(39),i=n(40),o=n(18),a=n(41);e.exports=function(e){return r(e)||i(e)||o(e)||a()}},function(e,t,n){var r=n(45);e.exports=function(e,t){if(null==e)return{};var n,i,o=r(e,t);if(Object.getOwnPropertySymbols){var a=Object.getOwnPropertySymbols(e);for(i=0;i<a.length;i++)n=a[i],t.indexOf(n)>=0||Object.prototype.propertyIsEnumerable.call(e,n)&&(o[n]=e[n])}return o}},function(e,t,n){var r=n(50);function i(t,n,o){return"undefined"!==typeof Reflect&&Reflect.get?e.exports=i=Reflect.get:e.exports=i=function(e,t,n){var i=r(e,t);if(i){var o=Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(i,t);return o.get?o.get.call(n):o.value}},i(t,n,o||t)}e.exports=i},function(e,t,n){"use strict";e.exports=n(36)},function(e,t,n){"use strict";var r=n(14),i={childContextTypes:!0,contextType:!0,contextTypes:!0,defaultProps:!0,displayName:!0,getDefaultProps:!0,getDerivedStateFromError:!0,getDerivedStateFromProps:!0,mixins:!0,propTypes:!0,type:!0},o={name:!0,length:!0,prototype:!0,caller:!0,callee:!0,arguments:!0,arity:!0},a={$$typeof:!0,compare:!0,defaultProps:!0,displayName:!0,propTypes:!0,type:!0},s={};function u(e){return r.isMemo(e)?a:s[e.$$typeof]||i}s[r.ForwardRef]={$$typeof:!0,render:!0,defaultProps:!0,displayName:!0,propTypes:!0},s[r.Memo]=a;var c=Object.defineProperty,l=Object.getOwnPropertyNames,d=Object.getOwnPropertySymbols,p=Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor,f=Object.getPrototypeOf,h=Object.prototype;e.exports=function e(t,n,r){if("string"!==typeof n){if(h){var i=f(n);i&&i!==h&&e(t,i,r)}var a=l(n);d&&(a=a.concat(d(n)));for(var s=u(t),y=u(n),g=0;g<a.length;++g){var v=a[g];if(!o[v]&&(!r||!r[v])&&(!y||!y[v])&&(!s||!s[v])){var m=p(n,v);try{c(t,v,m)}catch(b){}}}}return t}},function(e,t,n){"use strict";(function(e,r){var i,o=n(29);i="undefined"!==typeof self?self:"undefined"!==typeof window?window:"undefined"!==typeof e?e:r;var a=Object(o.a)(i);t.a=a}).call(this,n(37),n(38)(e))},function(e,t){e.exports=function(e,t){(null==t||t>e.length)&&(t=e.length);for(var n=0,r=new Array(t);n<t;n++)r[n]=e[n];return r}},function(e,t,n){var r=n(17);e.exports=function(e,t){if(e){if("string"===typeof e)return r(e,t);var n=Object.prototype.toString.call(e).slice(8,-1);return"Object"===n&&e.constructor&&(n=e.constructor.name),"Map"===n||"Set"===n?Array.from(e):"Arguments"===n||/^(?:Ui|I)nt(?:8|16|32)(?:Clamped)?Array$/.test(n)?r(e,t):void 0}}},function(e,t){var n,r,i=e.exports={};function o(){throw new Error("setTimeout has not been defined")}function a(){throw new Error("clearTimeout has not been defined")}function s(e){if(n===setTimeout)return setTimeout(e,0);if((n===o||!n)&&setTimeout)return n=setTimeout,setTimeout(e,0);try{return n(e,0)}catch(t){try{return n.call(null,e,0)}catch(t){return n.call(this,e,0)}}}!function(){try{n="function"===typeof setTimeout?setTimeout:o}catch(e){n=o}try{r="function"===typeof clearTimeout?clearTimeout:a}catch(e){r=a}}();var u,c=[],l=!1,d=-1;function p(){l&&u&&(l=!1,u.length?c=u.concat(c):d=-1,c.length&&f())}function f(){if(!l){var e=s(p);l=!0;for(var t=c.length;t;){for(u=c,c=[];++d<t;)u&&u[d].run();d=-1,t=c.length}u=null,l=!1,function(e){if(r===clearTimeout)return clearTimeout(e);if((r===a||!r)&&clearTimeout)return r=clearTimeout,clearTimeout(e);try{r(e)}catch(t){try{return r.call(null,e)}catch(t){return r.call(this,e)}}}(e)}}function h(e,t){this.fun=e,this.array=t}function y(){}i.nextTick=function(e){var t=new Array(arguments.length-1);if(arguments.length>1)for(var n=1;n<arguments.length;n++)t[n-1]=arguments[n];c.push(new h(e,t)),1!==c.length||l||s(f)},h.prototype.run=function(){this.fun.apply(null,this.array)},i.title="browser",i.browser=!0,i.env={},i.argv=[],i.version="",i.versions={},i.on=y,i.addListener=y,i.once=y,i.off=y,i.removeListener=y,i.removeAllListeners=y,i.emit=y,i.prependListener=y,i.prependOnceListener=y,i.listeners=function(e){return[]},i.binding=function(e){throw new Error("process.binding is not supported")},i.cwd=function(){return"/"},i.chdir=function(e){throw new Error("process.chdir is not supported")},i.umask=function(){return 0}},function(e,t){!function(){"use strict";if("object"===typeof window)if("IntersectionObserver"in window&&"IntersectionObserverEntry"in window&&"intersectionRatio"in window.IntersectionObserverEntry.prototype)"isIntersecting"in window.IntersectionObserverEntry.prototype||Object.defineProperty(window.IntersectionObserverEntry.prototype,"isIntersecting",{get:function(){return this.intersectionRatio>0}});else{var e=window.document,t=[];r.prototype.THROTTLE_TIMEOUT=100,r.prototype.POLL_INTERVAL=null,r.prototype.USE_MUTATION_OBSERVER=!0,r.prototype.observe=function(e){if(!this._observationTargets.some(function(t){return t.element==e})){if(!e||1!=e.nodeType)throw new Error("target must be an Element");this._registerInstance(),this._observationTargets.push({element:e,entry:null}),this._monitorIntersections(e.ownerDocument),this._checkForIntersections()}},r.prototype.unobserve=function(e){this._observationTargets=this._observationTargets.filter(function(t){return t.element!=e}),this._unmonitorIntersections(e.ownerDocument),0==this._observationTargets.length&&this._unregisterInstance()},r.prototype.disconnect=function(){this._observationTargets=[],this._unmonitorAllIntersections(),this._unregisterInstance()},r.prototype.takeRecords=function(){var e=this._queuedEntries.slice();return this._queuedEntries=[],e},r.prototype._initThresholds=function(e){var t=e||[0];return Array.isArray(t)||(t=[t]),t.sort().filter(function(e,t,n){if("number"!=typeof e||isNaN(e)||e<0||e>1)throw new Error("threshold must be a number between 0 and 1 inclusively");return e!==n[t-1]})},r.prototype._parseRootMargin=function(e){var t=(e||"0px").split(/\s+/).map(function(e){var t=/^(-?\d*\.?\d+)(px|%)$/.exec(e);if(!t)throw new Error("rootMargin must be specified in pixels or percent");return{value:parseFloat(t[1]),unit:t[2]}});return t[1]=t[1]||t[0],t[2]=t[2]||t[0],t[3]=t[3]||t[1],t},r.prototype._monitorIntersections=function(t){var n=t.defaultView;if(n&&-1==this._monitoringDocuments.indexOf(t)){var r=this._checkForIntersections,a=null,s=null;if(this.POLL_INTERVAL?a=n.setInterval(r,this.POLL_INTERVAL):(i(n,"resize",r,!0),i(t,"scroll",r,!0),this.USE_MUTATION_OBSERVER&&"MutationObserver"in n&&(s=new n.MutationObserver(r)).observe(t,{attributes:!0,childList:!0,characterData:!0,subtree:!0})),this._monitoringDocuments.push(t),this._monitoringUnsubscribes.push(function(){var e=t.defaultView;e&&(a&&e.clearInterval(a),o(e,"resize",r,!0)),o(t,"scroll",r,!0),s&&s.disconnect()}),t!=(this.root&&this.root.ownerDocument||e)){var u=d(t);u&&this._monitorIntersections(u.ownerDocument)}}},r.prototype._unmonitorIntersections=function(t){var n=this._monitoringDocuments.indexOf(t);if(-1!=n){var r=this.root&&this.root.ownerDocument||e;if(!this._observationTargets.some(function(e){var n=e.element.ownerDocument;if(n==t)return!0;for(;n&&n!=r;){var i=d(n);if((n=i&&i.ownerDocument)==t)return!0}return!1})){var i=this._monitoringUnsubscribes[n];if(this._monitoringDocuments.splice(n,1),this._monitoringUnsubscribes.splice(n,1),i(),t!=r){var o=d(t);o&&this._unmonitorIntersections(o.ownerDocument)}}}},r.prototype._unmonitorAllIntersections=function(){var e=this._monitoringUnsubscribes.slice(0);this._monitoringDocuments.length=0,this._monitoringUnsubscribes.length=0;for(var t=0;t<e.length;t++)e[t]()},r.prototype._checkForIntersections=function(){var e=this._rootIsInDom(),t=e?this._getRootRect():{top:0,bottom:0,left:0,right:0,width:0,height:0};this._observationTargets.forEach(function(r){var i=r.element,o=s(i),a=this._rootContainsTarget(i),u=r.entry,c=e&&a&&this._computeTargetAndRootIntersection(i,o,t),l=r.entry=new n({time:window.performance&&performance.now&&performance.now(),target:i,boundingClientRect:o,rootBounds:t,intersectionRect:c});u?e&&a?this._hasCrossedThreshold(u,l)&&this._queuedEntries.push(l):u&&u.isIntersecting&&this._queuedEntries.push(l):this._queuedEntries.push(l)},this),this._queuedEntries.length&&this._callback(this.takeRecords(),this)},r.prototype._computeTargetAndRootIntersection=function(t,n,r){if("none"!=window.getComputedStyle(t).display){for(var i=n,o=l(t),c=!1;!c&&o;){var d=null,p=1==o.nodeType?window.getComputedStyle(o):{};if("none"==p.display)return null;if(o==this.root||9==o.nodeType)if(c=!0,o==this.root||o==e)d=r;else{var f=l(o),h=f&&s(f),y=f&&this._computeTargetAndRootIntersection(f,h,r);h&&y?(o=f,d=u(h,y)):(o=null,i=null)}else{var g=o.ownerDocument;o!=g.body&&o!=g.documentElement&&"visible"!=p.overflow&&(d=s(o))}if(d&&(i=a(d,i)),!i)break;o=o&&l(o)}return i}},r.prototype._getRootRect=function(){var t;if(this.root)t=s(this.root);else{var n=e.documentElement,r=e.body;t={top:0,left:0,right:n.clientWidth||r.clientWidth,width:n.clientWidth||r.clientWidth,bottom:n.clientHeight||r.clientHeight,height:n.clientHeight||r.clientHeight}}return this._expandRectByRootMargin(t)},r.prototype._expandRectByRootMargin=function(e){var t=this._rootMarginValues.map(function(t,n){return"px"==t.unit?t.value:t.value*(n%2?e.width:e.height)/100}),n={top:e.top-t[0],right:e.right+t[1],bottom:e.bottom+t[2],left:e.left-t[3]};return n.width=n.right-n.left,n.height=n.bottom-n.top,n},r.prototype._hasCrossedThreshold=function(e,t){var n=e&&e.isIntersecting?e.intersectionRatio||0:-1,r=t.isIntersecting?t.intersectionRatio||0:-1;if(n!==r)for(var i=0;i<this.thresholds.length;i++){var o=this.thresholds[i];if(o==n||o==r||o<n!==o<r)return!0}},r.prototype._rootIsInDom=function(){return!this.root||c(e,this.root)},r.prototype._rootContainsTarget=function(t){return c(this.root||e,t)&&(!this.root||this.root.ownerDocument==t.ownerDocument)},r.prototype._registerInstance=function(){t.indexOf(this)<0&&t.push(this)},r.prototype._unregisterInstance=function(){var e=t.indexOf(this);-1!=e&&t.splice(e,1)},window.IntersectionObserver=r,window.IntersectionObserverEntry=n}function n(e){this.time=e.time,this.target=e.target,this.rootBounds=e.rootBounds,this.boundingClientRect=e.boundingClientRect,this.intersectionRect=e.intersectionRect||{top:0,bottom:0,left:0,right:0,width:0,height:0},this.isIntersecting=!!e.intersectionRect;var t=this.boundingClientRect,n=t.width*t.height,r=this.intersectionRect,i=r.width*r.height;this.intersectionRatio=n?Number((i/n).toFixed(4)):this.isIntersecting?1:0}function r(e,t){var n=t||{};if("function"!=typeof e)throw new Error("callback must be a function");if(n.root&&1!=n.root.nodeType)throw new Error("root must be an Element");this._checkForIntersections=function(e,t){var n=null;return function(){n||(n=setTimeout(function(){e(),n=null},t))}}(this._checkForIntersections.bind(this),this.THROTTLE_TIMEOUT),this._callback=e,this._observationTargets=[],this._queuedEntries=[],this._rootMarginValues=this._parseRootMargin(n.rootMargin),this.thresholds=this._initThresholds(n.threshold),this.root=n.root||null,this.rootMargin=this._rootMarginValues.map(function(e){return e.value+e.unit}).join(" "),this._monitoringDocuments=[],this._monitoringUnsubscribes=[]}function i(e,t,n,r){"function"==typeof e.addEventListener?e.addEventListener(t,n,r||!1):"function"==typeof e.attachEvent&&e.attachEvent("on"+t,n)}function o(e,t,n,r){"function"==typeof e.removeEventListener?e.removeEventListener(t,n,r||!1):"function"==typeof e.detatchEvent&&e.detatchEvent("on"+t,n)}function a(e,t){var n=Math.max(e.top,t.top),r=Math.min(e.bottom,t.bottom),i=Math.max(e.left,t.left),o=Math.min(e.right,t.right),a=o-i,s=r-n;return a>=0&&s>=0&&{top:n,bottom:r,left:i,right:o,width:a,height:s}||null}function s(e){var t;try{t=e.getBoundingClientRect()}catch(n){}return t?(t.width&&t.height||(t={top:t.top,right:t.right,bottom:t.bottom,left:t.left,width:t.right-t.left,height:t.bottom-t.top}),t):{top:0,bottom:0,left:0,right:0,width:0,height:0}}function u(e,t){var n=t.top-e.top,r=t.left-e.left;return{top:n,left:r,height:t.height,width:t.width,bottom:n+t.height,right:r+t.width}}function c(e,t){for(var n=t;n;){if(n==e)return!0;n=l(n)}return!1}function l(t){var n=t.parentNode;return 9==t.nodeType&&t!=e?d(t):n&&11==n.nodeType&&n.host?n.host:n&&n.assignedSlot?n.assignedSlot.parentNode:n}function d(e){try{return e.defaultView&&e.defaultView.frameElement||null}catch(t){return null}}}()},function(e,t,n){"use strict";e.exports=function(e,t){return function(){for(var n=new Array(arguments.length),r=0;r<n.length;r++)n[r]=arguments[r];return e.apply(t,n)}}},function(e,t,n){"use strict";var r=n(6);function i(e){return encodeURIComponent(e).replace(/%40/gi,"@").replace(/%3A/gi,":").replace(/%24/g,"$").replace(/%2C/gi,",").replace(/%20/g,"+").replace(/%5B/gi,"[").replace(/%5D/gi,"]")}e.exports=function(e,t,n){if(!t)return e;var o;if(n)o=n(t);else if(r.isURLSearchParams(t))o=t.toString();else{var a=[];r.forEach(t,function(e,t){null!==e&&"undefined"!==typeof e&&(r.isArray(e)?t+="[]":e=[e],r.forEach(e,function(e){r.isDate(e)?e=e.toISOString():r.isObject(e)&&(e=JSON.stringify(e)),a.push(i(t)+"="+i(e))}))}),o=a.join("&")}if(o){var s=e.indexOf("#");-1!==s&&(e=e.slice(0,s)),e+=(-1===e.indexOf("?")?"?":"&")+o}return e}},function(e,t,n){"use strict";e.exports=function(e){return!(!e||!e.__CANCEL__)}},function(e,t,n){"use strict";(function(t){var r=n(6),i=n(57),o={"Content-Type":"application/x-www-form-urlencoded"};function a(e,t){!r.isUndefined(e)&&r.isUndefined(e["Content-Type"])&&(e["Content-Type"]=t)}var s={adapter:function(){var e;return"undefined"!==typeof XMLHttpRequest?e=n(25):"undefined"!==typeof t&&"[object process]"===Object.prototype.toString.call(t)&&(e=n(25)),e}(),transformRequest:[function(e,t){return i(t,"Accept"),i(t,"Content-Type"),r.isFormData(e)||r.isArrayBuffer(e)||r.isBuffer(e)||r.isStream(e)||r.isFile(e)||r.isBlob(e)?e:r.isArrayBufferView(e)?e.buffer:r.isURLSearchParams(e)?(a(t,"application/x-www-form-urlencoded;charset=utf-8"),e.toString()):r.isObject(e)?(a(t,"application/json;charset=utf-8"),JSON.stringify(e)):e}],transformResponse:[function(e){if("string"===typeof e)try{e=JSON.parse(e)}catch(t){}return e}],timeout:0,xsrfCookieName:"XSRF-TOKEN",xsrfHeaderName:"X-XSRF-TOKEN",maxContentLength:-1,validateStatus:function(e){return e>=200&&e<300},headers:{common:{Accept:"application/json, text/plain, */*"}}};r.forEach(["delete","get","head"],function(e){s.headers[e]={}}),r.forEach(["post","put","patch"],function(e){s.headers[e]=r.merge(o)}),e.exports=s}).call(this,n(19))},function(e,t,n){"use strict";var r=n(6),i=n(58),o=n(22),a=n(60),s=n(63),u=n(64),c=n(26);e.exports=function(e){return new Promise(function(t,l){var d=e.data,p=e.headers;r.isFormData(d)&&delete p["Content-Type"];var f=new XMLHttpRequest;if(e.auth){var h=e.auth.username||"",y=e.auth.password||"";p.Authorization="Basic "+btoa(h+":"+y)}var g=a(e.baseURL,e.url);if(f.open(e.method.toUpperCase(),o(g,e.params,e.paramsSerializer),!0),f.timeout=e.timeout,f.onreadystatechange=function(){if(f&&4===f.readyState&&(0!==f.status||f.responseURL&&0===f.responseURL.indexOf("file:"))){var n="getAllResponseHeaders"in f?s(f.getAllResponseHeaders()):null,r={data:e.responseType&&"text"!==e.responseType?f.response:f.responseText,status:f.status,statusText:f.statusText,headers:n,config:e,request:f};i(t,l,r),f=null}},f.onabort=function(){f&&(l(c("Request aborted",e,"ECONNABORTED",f)),f=null)},f.onerror=function(){l(c("Network Error",e,null,f)),f=null},f.ontimeout=function(){var t="timeout of "+e.timeout+"ms exceeded";e.timeoutErrorMessage&&(t=e.timeoutErrorMessage),l(c(t,e,"ECONNABORTED",f)),f=null},r.isStandardBrowserEnv()){var v=n(65),m=(e.withCredentials||u(g))&&e.xsrfCookieName?v.read(e.xsrfCookieName):void 0;m&&(p[e.xsrfHeaderName]=m)}if("setRequestHeader"in f&&r.forEach(p,function(e,t){"undefined"===typeof d&&"content-type"===t.toLowerCase()?delete p[t]:f.setRequestHeader(t,e)}),r.isUndefined(e.withCredentials)||(f.withCredentials=!!e.withCredentials),e.responseType)try{f.responseType=e.responseType}catch(b){if("json"!==e.responseType)throw b}"function"===typeof e.onDownloadProgress&&f.addEventListener("progress",e.onDownloadProgress),"function"===typeof e.onUploadProgress&&f.upload&&f.upload.addEventListener("progress",e.onUploadProgress),e.cancelToken&&e.cancelToken.promise.then(function(e){f&&(f.abort(),l(e),f=null)}),void 0===d&&(d=null),f.send(d)})}},function(e,t,n){"use strict";var r=n(59);e.exports=function(e,t,n,i,o){var a=new Error(e);return r(a,t,n,i,o)}},function(e,t,n){"use strict";var r=n(6);e.exports=function(e,t){t=t||{};var n={},i=["url","method","params","data"],o=["headers","auth","proxy"],a=["baseURL","url","transformRequest","transformResponse","paramsSerializer","timeout","withCredentials","adapter","responseType","xsrfCookieName","xsrfHeaderName","onUploadProgress","onDownloadProgress","maxContentLength","validateStatus","maxRedirects","httpAgent","httpsAgent","cancelToken","socketPath"];r.forEach(i,function(e){"undefined"!==typeof t[e]&&(n[e]=t[e])}),r.forEach(o,function(i){r.isObject(t[i])?n[i]=r.deepMerge(e[i],t[i]):"undefined"!==typeof t[i]?n[i]=t[i]:r.isObject(e[i])?n[i]=r.deepMerge(e[i]):"undefined"!==typeof e[i]&&(n[i]=e[i])}),r.forEach(a,function(r){"undefined"!==typeof t[r]?n[r]=t[r]:"undefined"!==typeof e[r]&&(n[r]=e[r])});var s=i.concat(o).concat(a),u=Object.keys(t).filter(function(e){return-1===s.indexOf(e)});return r.forEach(u,function(r){"undefined"!==typeof t[r]?n[r]=t[r]:"undefined"!==typeof e[r]&&(n[r]=e[r])}),n}},function(e,t,n){"use strict";function r(e){this.message=e}r.prototype.toString=function(){return"Cancel"+(this.message?": "+this.message:"")},r.prototype.__CANCEL__=!0,e.exports=r},function(e,t,n){"use strict";function r(e){var t,n=e.Symbol;return"function"===typeof n?n.observable?t=n.observable:(t=n("observable"),n.observable=t):t="@@observable",t}n.d(t,"a",function(){return r})},function(e,t,n){"use strict";(function(e){n.d(t,"a",function(){return f});var r=function(){for(var e=0,t=0,n=arguments.length;t<n;t++)e+=arguments[t].length;var r=Array(e),i=0;for(t=0;t<n;t++)for(var o=arguments[t],a=0,s=o.length;a<s;a++,i++)r[i]=o[a];return r},i=function(){return function(e,t,n){this.name=e,this.version=t,this.os=n,this.type="browser"}}(),o=function(){return function(t){this.version=t,this.type="node",this.name="node",this.os=e.platform}}(),a=function(){return function(e,t,n,r){this.name=e,this.version=t,this.os=n,this.bot=r,this.type="bot-device"}}(),s=function(){return function(){this.type="bot",this.bot=!0,this.name="bot",this.version=null,this.os=null}}(),u=function(){return function(){this.type="react-native",this.name="react-native",this.version=null,this.os=null}}(),c=/(nuhk|Googlebot|Yammybot|Openbot|Slurp|MSNBot|Ask\ Jeeves\/Teoma|ia_archiver)/,l=3,d=[["aol",/AOLShield\/([0-9\._]+)/],["edge",/Edge\/([0-9\._]+)/],["edge-ios",/EdgiOS\/([0-9\._]+)/],["yandexbrowser",/YaBrowser\/([0-9\._]+)/],["kakaotalk",/KAKAOTALK\s([0-9\.]+)/],["samsung",/SamsungBrowser\/([0-9\.]+)/],["silk",/\bSilk\/([0-9._-]+)\b/],["miui",/MiuiBrowser\/([0-9\.]+)$/],["beaker",/BeakerBrowser\/([0-9\.]+)/],["edge-chromium",/Edg\/([0-9\.]+)/],["chromium-webview",/(?!Chrom.*OPR)wv\).*Chrom(?:e|ium)\/([0-9\.]+)(:?\s|$)/],["chrome",/(?!Chrom.*OPR)Chrom(?:e|ium)\/([0-9\.]+)(:?\s|$)/],["phantomjs",/PhantomJS\/([0-9\.]+)(:?\s|$)/],["crios",/CriOS\/([0-9\.]+)(:?\s|$)/],["firefox",/Firefox\/([0-9\.]+)(?:\s|$)/],["fxios",/FxiOS\/([0-9\.]+)/],["opera-mini",/Opera Mini.*Version\/([0-9\.]+)/],["opera",/Opera\/([0-9\.]+)(?:\s|$)/],["opera",/OPR\/([0-9\.]+)(:?\s|$)/],["ie",/Trident\/7\.0.*rv\:([0-9\.]+).*\).*Gecko$/],["ie",/MSIE\s([0-9\.]+);.*Trident\/[4-7].0/],["ie",/MSIE\s(7\.0)/],["bb10",/BB10;\sTouch.*Version\/([0-9\.]+)/],["android",/Android\s([0-9\.]+)/],["ios",/Version\/([0-9\._]+).*Mobile.*Safari.*/],["safari",/Version\/([0-9\._]+).*Safari/],["facebook",/FBAV\/([0-9\.]+)/],["instagram",/Instagram\s([0-9\.]+)/],["ios-webview",/AppleWebKit\/([0-9\.]+).*Mobile/],["ios-webview",/AppleWebKit\/([0-9\.]+).*Gecko\)$/],["searchbot",/alexa|bot|crawl(er|ing)|facebookexternalhit|feedburner|google web preview|nagios|postrank|pingdom|slurp|spider|yahoo!|yandex/]],p=[["iOS",/iP(hone|od|ad)/],["Android OS",/Android/],["BlackBerry OS",/BlackBerry|BB10/],["Windows Mobile",/IEMobile/],["Amazon OS",/Kindle/],["Windows 3.11",/Win16/],["Windows 95",/(Windows 95)|(Win95)|(Windows_95)/],["Windows 98",/(Windows 98)|(Win98)/],["Windows 2000",/(Windows NT 5.0)|(Windows 2000)/],["Windows XP",/(Windows NT 5.1)|(Windows XP)/],["Windows Server 2003",/(Windows NT 5.2)/],["Windows Vista",/(Windows NT 6.0)/],["Windows 7",/(Windows NT 6.1)/],["Windows 8",/(Windows NT 6.2)/],["Windows 8.1",/(Windows NT 6.3)/],["Windows 10",/(Windows NT 10.0)/],["Windows ME",/Windows ME/],["Open BSD",/OpenBSD/],["Sun OS",/SunOS/],["Chrome OS",/CrOS/],["Linux",/(Linux)|(X11)/],["Mac OS",/(Mac_PowerPC)|(Macintosh)/],["QNX",/QNX/],["BeOS",/BeOS/],["OS/2",/OS\/2/]];function f(t){return t?y(t):"undefined"===typeof document&&"undefined"!==typeof navigator&&"ReactNative"===navigator.product?new u:"undefined"!==typeof navigator?y(navigator.userAgent):"undefined"!==typeof e&&e.version?new o(e.version.slice(1)):null}function h(e){return""!==e&&d.reduce(function(t,n){var r=n[0],i=n[1];if(t)return t;var o=i.exec(e);return!!o&&[r,o]},!1)}function y(e){var t=h(e);if(!t)return null;var n=t[0],o=t[1];if("searchbot"===n)return new s;var u=o[1]&&o[1].split(/[._]/).slice(0,3);u?u.length<l&&(u=r(u,function(e){for(var t=[],n=0;n<e;n++)t.push("0");return t}(l-u.length))):u=[];var d=u.join("."),f=function(e){for(var t=0,n=p.length;t<n;t++){var r=p[t],i=r[0],o=r[1],a=o.exec(e);if(a)return i}return null}(e),y=c.exec(e);return y&&y[1]?new a(n,d,f,y[1]):new i(n,d,f)}}).call(this,n(19))},function(e,t){function n(){return e.exports=n=Object.assign||function(e){for(var t=1;t<arguments.length;t++){var n=arguments[t];for(var r in n)Object.prototype.hasOwnProperty.call(n,r)&&(e[r]=n[r])}return e},n.apply(this,arguments)}e.exports=n},function(e,t,n){e.exports=n(52)},function(e,t,n){e.exports=n(34)()},function(e,t,n){"use strict";var r=n(35);function i(){}function o(){}o.resetWarningCache=i,e.exports=function(){function e(e,t,n,i,o,a){if(a!==r){var s=new Error("Calling PropTypes validators directly is not supported by the `prop-types` package. Use PropTypes.checkPropTypes() to call them. Read more at http://fb.me/use-check-prop-types");throw s.name="Invariant Violation",s}}function t(){return e}e.isRequired=e;var n={array:e,bool:e,func:e,number:e,object:e,string:e,symbol:e,any:e,arrayOf:t,element:e,elementType:e,instanceOf:t,node:e,objectOf:t,oneOf:t,oneOfType:t,shape:t,exact:t,checkPropTypes:o,resetWarningCache:i};return n.PropTypes=n,n}},function(e,t,n){"use strict";e.exports="SECRET_DO_NOT_PASS_THIS_OR_YOU_WILL_BE_FIRED"},function(e,t,n){"use strict";var r="function"===typeof Symbol&&Symbol.for,i=r?Symbol.for("react.element"):60103,o=r?Symbol.for("react.portal"):60106,a=r?Symbol.for("react.fragment"):60107,s=r?Symbol.for("react.strict_mode"):60108,u=r?Symbol.for("react.profiler"):60114,c=r?Symbol.for("react.provider"):60109,l=r?Symbol.for("react.context"):60110,d=r?Symbol.for("react.async_mode"):60111,p=r?Symbol.for("react.concurrent_mode"):60111,f=r?Symbol.for("react.forward_ref"):60112,h=r?Symbol.for("react.suspense"):60113,y=r?Symbol.for("react.suspense_list"):60120,g=r?Symbol.for("react.memo"):60115,v=r?Symbol.for("react.lazy"):60116,m=r?Symbol.for("react.block"):60121,b=r?Symbol.for("react.fundamental"):60117,O=r?Symbol.for("react.responder"):60118,_=r?Symbol.for("react.scope"):60119;function S(e){if("object"===typeof e&&null!==e){var t=e.$$typeof;switch(t){case i:switch(e=e.type){case d:case p:case a:case u:case s:case h:return e;default:switch(e=e&&e.$$typeof){case l:case f:case v:case g:case c:return e;default:return t}}case o:return t}}}function E(e){return S(e)===p}t.AsyncMode=d,t.ConcurrentMode=p,t.ContextConsumer=l,t.ContextProvider=c,t.Element=i,t.ForwardRef=f,t.Fragment=a,t.Lazy=v,t.Memo=g,t.Portal=o,t.Profiler=u,t.StrictMode=s,t.Suspense=h,t.isAsyncMode=function(e){return E(e)||S(e)===d},t.isConcurrentMode=E,t.isContextConsumer=function(e){return S(e)===l},t.isContextProvider=function(e){return S(e)===c},t.isElement=function(e){return"object"===typeof e&&null!==e&&e.$$typeof===i},t.isForwardRef=function(e){return S(e)===f},t.isFragment=function(e){return S(e)===a},t.isLazy=function(e){return S(e)===v},t.isMemo=function(e){return S(e)===g},t.isPortal=function(e){return S(e)===o},t.isProfiler=function(e){return S(e)===u},t.isStrictMode=function(e){return S(e)===s},t.isSuspense=function(e){return S(e)===h},t.isValidElementType=function(e){return"string"===typeof e||"function"===typeof e||e===a||e===p||e===u||e===s||e===h||e===y||"object"===typeof e&&null!==e&&(e.$$typeof===v||e.$$typeof===g||e.$$typeof===c||e.$$typeof===l||e.$$typeof===f||e.$$typeof===b||e.$$typeof===O||e.$$typeof===_||e.$$typeof===m)},t.typeOf=S},function(e,t){var n;n=function(){return this}();try{n=n||new Function("return this")()}catch(r){"object"===typeof window&&(n=window)}e.exports=n},function(e,t){e.exports=function(e){if(!e.webpackPolyfill){var t=Object.create(e);t.children||(t.children=[]),Object.defineProperty(t,"loaded",{enumerable:!0,get:function(){return t.l}}),Object.defineProperty(t,"id",{enumerable:!0,get:function(){return t.i}}),Object.defineProperty(t,"exports",{enumerable:!0}),t.webpackPolyfill=1}return t}},function(e,t,n){var r=n(17);e.exports=function(e){if(Array.isArray(e))return r(e)}},function(e,t){e.exports=function(e){if("undefined"!==typeof Symbol&&Symbol.iterator in Object(e))return Array.from(e)}},function(e,t){e.exports=function(){throw new TypeError("Invalid attempt to spread non-iterable instance.\nIn order to be iterable, non-array objects must have a [Symbol.iterator]() method.")}},function(e,t){e.exports=function(e){if(Array.isArray(e))return e}},function(e,t){e.exports=function(e,t){if("undefined"!==typeof Symbol&&Symbol.iterator in Object(e)){var n=[],r=!0,i=!1,o=void 0;try{for(var a,s=e[Symbol.iterator]();!(r=(a=s.next()).done)&&(n.push(a.value),!t||n.length!==t);r=!0);}catch(u){i=!0,o=u}finally{try{r||null==s.return||s.return()}finally{if(i)throw o}}return n}}},function(e,t){e.exports=function(){throw new TypeError("Invalid attempt to destructure non-iterable instance.\nIn order to be iterable, non-array objects must have a [Symbol.iterator]() method.")}},function(e,t){e.exports=function(e,t){if(null==e)return{};var n,r,i={},o=Object.keys(e);for(r=0;r<o.length;r++)n=o[r],t.indexOf(n)>=0||(i[n]=e[n]);return i}},function(e,t){},function(e,t,n){var r=function(e){"use strict";var t,n=Object.prototype,r=n.hasOwnProperty,i="function"===typeof Symbol?Symbol:{},o=i.iterator||"@@iterator",a=i.asyncIterator||"@@asyncIterator",s=i.toStringTag||"@@toStringTag";function u(e,t,n){return Object.defineProperty(e,t,{value:n,enumerable:!0,configurable:!0,writable:!0}),e[t]}try{u({},"")}catch(M){u=function(e,t,n){return e[t]=n}}function c(e,t,n,r){var i=t&&t.prototype instanceof g?t:g,o=Object.create(i.prototype),a=new C(r||[]);return o._invoke=function(e,t,n){var r=d;return function(i,o){if(r===f)throw new Error("Generator is already running");if(r===h){if("throw"===i)throw o;return D()}for(n.method=i,n.arg=o;;){var a=n.delegate;if(a){var s=P(a,n);if(s){if(s===y)continue;return s}}if("next"===n.method)n.sent=n._sent=n.arg;else if("throw"===n.method){if(r===d)throw r=h,n.arg;n.dispatchException(n.arg)}else"return"===n.method&&n.abrupt("return",n.arg);r=f;var u=l(e,t,n);if("normal"===u.type){if(r=n.done?h:p,u.arg===y)continue;return{value:u.arg,done:n.done}}"throw"===u.type&&(r=h,n.method="throw",n.arg=u.arg)}}}(e,n,a),o}function l(e,t,n){try{return{type:"normal",arg:e.call(t,n)}}catch(M){return{type:"throw",arg:M}}}e.wrap=c;var d="suspendedStart",p="suspendedYield",f="executing",h="completed",y={};function g(){}function v(){}function m(){}var b={};b[o]=function(){return this};var O=Object.getPrototypeOf,_=O&&O(O(R([])));_&&_!==n&&r.call(_,o)&&(b=_);var S=m.prototype=g.prototype=Object.create(b);function E(e){["next","throw","return"].forEach(function(t){u(e,t,function(e){return this._invoke(t,e)})})}function w(e,t){var n;this._invoke=function(i,o){function a(){return new t(function(n,a){!function n(i,o,a,s){var u=l(e[i],e,o);if("throw"!==u.type){var c=u.arg,d=c.value;return d&&"object"===typeof d&&r.call(d,"__await")?t.resolve(d.__await).then(function(e){n("next",e,a,s)},function(e){n("throw",e,a,s)}):t.resolve(d).then(function(e){c.value=e,a(c)},function(e){return n("throw",e,a,s)})}s(u.arg)}(i,o,n,a)})}return n=n?n.then(a,a):a()}}function P(e,n){var r=e.iterator[n.method];if(r===t){if(n.delegate=null,"throw"===n.method){if(e.iterator.return&&(n.method="return",n.arg=t,P(e,n),"throw"===n.method))return y;n.method="throw",n.arg=new TypeError("The iterator does not provide a 'throw' method")}return y}var i=l(r,e.iterator,n.arg);if("throw"===i.type)return n.method="throw",n.arg=i.arg,n.delegate=null,y;var o=i.arg;return o?o.done?(n[e.resultName]=o.value,n.next=e.nextLoc,"return"!==n.method&&(n.method="next",n.arg=t),n.delegate=null,y):o:(n.method="throw",n.arg=new TypeError("iterator result is not an object"),n.delegate=null,y)}function T(e){var t={tryLoc:e[0]};1 in e&&(t.catchLoc=e[1]),2 in e&&(t.finallyLoc=e[2],t.afterLoc=e[3]),this.tryEntries.push(t)}function A(e){var t=e.completion||{};t.type="normal",delete t.arg,e.completion=t}function C(e){this.tryEntries=[{tryLoc:"root"}],e.forEach(T,this),this.reset(!0)}function R(e){if(e){var n=e[o];if(n)return n.call(e);if("function"===typeof e.next)return e;if(!isNaN(e.length)){var i=-1,a=function n(){for(;++i<e.length;)if(r.call(e,i))return n.value=e[i],n.done=!1,n;return n.value=t,n.done=!0,n};return a.next=a}}return{next:D}}function D(){return{value:t,done:!0}}return v.prototype=S.constructor=m,m.constructor=v,v.displayName=u(m,s,"GeneratorFunction"),e.isGeneratorFunction=function(e){var t="function"===typeof e&&e.constructor;return!!t&&(t===v||"GeneratorFunction"===(t.displayName||t.name))},e.mark=function(e){return Object.setPrototypeOf?Object.setPrototypeOf(e,m):(e.__proto__=m,u(e,s,"GeneratorFunction")),e.prototype=Object.create(S),e},e.awrap=function(e){return{__await:e}},E(w.prototype),w.prototype[a]=function(){return this},e.AsyncIterator=w,e.async=function(t,n,r,i,o){void 0===o&&(o=Promise);var a=new w(c(t,n,r,i),o);return e.isGeneratorFunction(n)?a:a.next().then(function(e){return e.done?e.value:a.next()})},E(S),u(S,s,"Generator"),S[o]=function(){return this},S.toString=function(){return"[object Generator]"},e.keys=function(e){var t=[];for(var n in e)t.push(n);return t.reverse(),function n(){for(;t.length;){var r=t.pop();if(r in e)return n.value=r,n.done=!1,n}return n.done=!0,n}},e.values=R,C.prototype={constructor:C,reset:function(e){if(this.prev=0,this.next=0,this.sent=this._sent=t,this.done=!1,this.delegate=null,this.method="next",this.arg=t,this.tryEntries.forEach(A),!e)for(var n in this)"t"===n.charAt(0)&&r.call(this,n)&&!isNaN(+n.slice(1))&&(this[n]=t)},stop:function(){this.done=!0;var e=this.tryEntries[0].completion;if("throw"===e.type)throw e.arg;return this.rval},dispatchException:function(e){if(this.done)throw e;var n=this;function i(r,i){return s.type="throw",s.arg=e,n.next=r,i&&(n.method="next",n.arg=t),!!i}for(var o=this.tryEntries.length-1;o>=0;--o){var a=this.tryEntries[o],s=a.completion;if("root"===a.tryLoc)return i("end");if(a.tryLoc<=this.prev){var u=r.call(a,"catchLoc"),c=r.call(a,"finallyLoc");if(u&&c){if(this.prev<a.catchLoc)return i(a.catchLoc,!0);if(this.prev<a.finallyLoc)return i(a.finallyLoc)}else if(u){if(this.prev<a.catchLoc)return i(a.catchLoc,!0)}else{if(!c)throw new Error("try statement without catch or finally");if(this.prev<a.finallyLoc)return i(a.finallyLoc)}}}},abrupt:function(e,t){for(var n=this.tryEntries.length-1;n>=0;--n){var i=this.tryEntries[n];if(i.tryLoc<=this.prev&&r.call(i,"finallyLoc")&&this.prev<i.finallyLoc){var o=i;break}}o&&("break"===e||"continue"===e)&&o.tryLoc<=t&&t<=o.finallyLoc&&(o=null);var a=o?o.completion:{};return a.type=e,a.arg=t,o?(this.method="next",this.next=o.finallyLoc,y):this.complete(a)},complete:function(e,t){if("throw"===e.type)throw e.arg;return"break"===e.type||"continue"===e.type?this.next=e.arg:"return"===e.type?(this.rval=this.arg=e.arg,this.method="return",this.next="end"):"normal"===e.type&&t&&(this.next=t),y},finish:function(e){for(var t=this.tryEntries.length-1;t>=0;--t){var n=this.tryEntries[t];if(n.finallyLoc===e)return this.complete(n.completion,n.afterLoc),A(n),y}},catch:function(e){for(var t=this.tryEntries.length-1;t>=0;--t){var n=this.tryEntries[t];if(n.tryLoc===e){var r=n.completion;if("throw"===r.type){var i=r.arg;A(n)}return i}}throw new Error("illegal catch attempt")},delegateYield:function(e,n,r){return this.delegate={iterator:R(e),resultName:n,nextLoc:r},"next"===this.method&&(this.arg=t),y}},e}(e.exports);try{regeneratorRuntime=r}catch(i){Function("r","regeneratorRuntime = r")(r)}},function(e,t){function n(t,r){return e.exports=n=Object.setPrototypeOf||function(e,t){return e.__proto__=t,e},n(t,r)}e.exports=n},function(e,t){function n(t){return"function"===typeof Symbol&&"symbol"===typeof Symbol.iterator?e.exports=n=function(e){return typeof e}:e.exports=n=function(e){return e&&"function"===typeof Symbol&&e.constructor===Symbol&&e!==Symbol.prototype?"symbol":typeof e},n(t)}e.exports=n},function(e,t,n){var r=n(5);e.exports=function(e,t){for(;!Object.prototype.hasOwnProperty.call(e,t)&&null!==(e=r(e)););return e}},function(e){e.exports=JSON.parse('{"name":"mplayer","version":"4.0.13","description":"minute media player","author":"minute media","private":true,"scripts":{"start":"yarn compile && PORT=2001 per-env","start:production":"npm run -s serve","start:development":"npm run -s dev","build":"preact build --no-prerender","serve":"preact build && serve build --single","dev":"preact watch","lint":"./node_modules/.bin/eslint --cache --ext .js,.ts,.tsx ./src/.","test":"jest","compile":"webpack","compile:production":"NODE_ENV=production webpack","compile:qa":"NODE_ENV=qa webpack"},"devDependencies":{"@babel/cli":"^7.8.4","@babel/core":"^7.8.4","@babel/plugin-proposal-class-properties":"^7.8.3","@babel/plugin-transform-react-jsx":"^7.8.3","@babel/plugin-transform-runtime":"^7.9.0","@babel/preset-env":"^7.8.4","@babel/preset-react":"^7.8.3","@babel/preset-typescript":"^7.8.3","@typescript-eslint/eslint-plugin":"^2.10.0","@typescript-eslint/parser":"^2.10.0","eslint":"^6.7.2","eslint-config-mm":"git+https://github.com/FTBpro/eslint-config-mm.git#v3.1","eslint-loader":"3.0.2","identity-obj-proxy":"^3.0.0","jest":"^24.9.0","jest-emotion":"^10.0.32","jest-preset-preact":"^1.0.0","per-env":"^1.0.2","preact-cli":"^3.0.0-rc.6","react":"^16.13.1","react-dom":"^16.13.1","serve":"^11.1.0"},"dependencies":{"@emotion/core":"^10.0.28","axios":"^0.19.2","babel-loader":"^8.0.6","detect-browser":"^5.1.0","emotion":"^10.0.27","enzyme":"^3.11.0","enzyme-adapter-preact-pure":"^2.2.0","hls.js":"^0.13.2","intersection-observer":"^0.8.0","jest-enzyme":"^7.1.2","jest-junit":"^10.0.0","preact":"^10.0.1","preact-render-to-string":"^5.1.8","react-redux":"^7.2.0","react-svg-loader":"^3.0.3","redux":"^4.0.5","redux-thunk":"^2.3.0","terser-webpack-plugin":"1.2.3","webpack":"^4.41.6","webpack-cli":"^3.3.11"}}')},function(e,t,n){"use strict";var r=n(6),i=n(21),o=n(53),a=n(27);function s(e){var t=new o(e),n=i(o.prototype.request,t);return r.extend(n,o.prototype,t),r.extend(n,t),n}var u=s(n(24));u.Axios=o,u.create=function(e){return s(a(u.defaults,e))},u.Cancel=n(28),u.CancelToken=n(66),u.isCancel=n(23),u.all=function(e){return Promise.all(e)},u.spread=n(67),e.exports=u,e.exports.default=u},function(e,t,n){"use strict";var r=n(6),i=n(22),o=n(54),a=n(55),s=n(27);function u(e){this.defaults=e,this.interceptors={request:new o,response:new o}}u.prototype.request=function(e){"string"===typeof e?(e=arguments[1]||{}).url=arguments[0]:e=e||{},(e=s(this.defaults,e)).method?e.method=e.method.toLowerCase():this.defaults.method?e.method=this.defaults.method.toLowerCase():e.method="get";var t=[a,void 0],n=Promise.resolve(e);for(this.interceptors.request.forEach(function(e){t.unshift(e.fulfilled,e.rejected)}),this.interceptors.response.forEach(function(e){t.push(e.fulfilled,e.rejected)});t.length;)n=n.then(t.shift(),t.shift());return n},u.prototype.getUri=function(e){return e=s(this.defaults,e),i(e.url,e.params,e.paramsSerializer).replace(/^\?/,"")},r.forEach(["delete","get","head","options"],function(e){u.prototype[e]=function(t,n){return this.request(r.merge(n||{},{method:e,url:t}))}}),r.forEach(["post","put","patch"],function(e){u.prototype[e]=function(t,n,i){return this.request(r.merge(i||{},{method:e,url:t,data:n}))}}),e.exports=u},function(e,t,n){"use strict";var r=n(6);function i(){this.handlers=[]}i.prototype.use=function(e,t){return this.handlers.push({fulfilled:e,rejected:t}),this.handlers.length-1},i.prototype.eject=function(e){this.handlers[e]&&(this.handlers[e]=null)},i.prototype.forEach=function(e){r.forEach(this.handlers,function(t){null!==t&&e(t)})},e.exports=i},function(e,t,n){"use strict";var r=n(6),i=n(56),o=n(23),a=n(24);function s(e){e.cancelToken&&e.cancelToken.throwIfRequested()}e.exports=function(e){return s(e),e.headers=e.headers||{},e.data=i(e.data,e.headers,e.transformRequest),e.headers=r.merge(e.headers.common||{},e.headers[e.method]||{},e.headers),r.forEach(["delete","get","head","post","put","patch","common"],function(t){delete e.headers[t]}),(e.adapter||a.adapter)(e).then(function(t){return s(e),t.data=i(t.data,t.headers,e.transformResponse),t},function(t){return o(t)||(s(e),t&&t.response&&(t.response.data=i(t.response.data,t.response.headers,e.transformResponse))),Promise.reject(t)})}},function(e,t,n){"use strict";var r=n(6);e.exports=function(e,t,n){return r.forEach(n,function(n){e=n(e,t)}),e}},function(e,t,n){"use strict";var r=n(6);e.exports=function(e,t){r.forEach(e,function(n,r){r!==t&&r.toUpperCase()===t.toUpperCase()&&(e[t]=n,delete e[r])})}},function(e,t,n){"use strict";var r=n(26);e.exports=function(e,t,n){var i=n.config.validateStatus;!i||i(n.status)?e(n):t(r("Request failed with status code "+n.status,n.config,null,n.request,n))}},function(e,t,n){"use strict";e.exports=function(e,t,n,r,i){return e.config=t,n&&(e.code=n),e.request=r,e.response=i,e.isAxiosError=!0,e.toJSON=function(){return{message:this.message,name:this.name,description:this.description,number:this.number,fileName:this.fileName,lineNumber:this.lineNumber,columnNumber:this.columnNumber,stack:this.stack,config:this.config,code:this.code}},e}},function(e,t,n){"use strict";var r=n(61),i=n(62);e.exports=function(e,t){return e&&!r(t)?i(e,t):t}},function(e,t,n){"use strict";e.exports=function(e){return/^([a-z][a-z\d\+\-\.]*:)?\/\//i.test(e)}},function(e,t,n){"use strict";e.exports=function(e,t){return t?e.replace(/\/+$/,"")+"/"+t.replace(/^\/+/,""):e}},function(e,t,n){"use strict";var r=n(6),i=["age","authorization","content-length","content-type","etag","expires","from","host","if-modified-since","if-unmodified-since","last-modified","location","max-forwards","proxy-authorization","referer","retry-after","user-agent"];e.exports=function(e){var t,n,o,a={};return e?(r.forEach(e.split("\n"),function(e){if(o=e.indexOf(":"),t=r.trim(e.substr(0,o)).toLowerCase(),n=r.trim(e.substr(o+1)),t){if(a[t]&&i.indexOf(t)>=0)return;a[t]="set-cookie"===t?(a[t]?a[t]:[]).concat([n]):a[t]?a[t]+", "+n:n}}),a):a}},function(e,t,n){"use strict";var r=n(6);e.exports=r.isStandardBrowserEnv()?function(){var e,t=/(msie|trident)/i.test(navigator.userAgent),n=document.createElement("a");function i(e){var r=e;return t&&(n.setAttribute("href",r),r=n.href),n.setAttribute("href",r),{href:n.href,protocol:n.protocol?n.protocol.replace(/:$/,""):"",host:n.host,search:n.search?n.search.replace(/^\?/,""):"",hash:n.hash?n.hash.replace(/^#/,""):"",hostname:n.hostname,port:n.port,pathname:"/"===n.pathname.charAt(0)?n.pathname:"/"+n.pathname}}return e=i(window.location.href),function(t){var n=r.isString(t)?i(t):t;return n.protocol===e.protocol&&n.host===e.host}}():function(){return!0}},function(e,t,n){"use strict";var r=n(6);e.exports=r.isStandardBrowserEnv()?{write:function(e,t,n,i,o,a){var s=[];s.push(e+"="+encodeURIComponent(t)),r.isNumber(n)&&s.push("expires="+new Date(n).toGMTString()),r.isString(i)&&s.push("path="+i),r.isString(o)&&s.push("domain="+o),!0===a&&s.push("secure"),document.cookie=s.join("; ")},read:function(e){var t=document.cookie.match(new RegExp("(^|;\\s*)("+e+")=([^;]*)"));return t?decodeURIComponent(t[3]):null},remove:function(e){this.write(e,"",Date.now()-864e5)}}:{write:function(){},read:function(){return null},remove:function(){}}},function(e,t,n){"use strict";var r=n(28);function i(e){if("function"!==typeof e)throw new TypeError("executor must be a function.");var t;this.promise=new Promise(function(e){t=e});var n=this;e(function(e){n.reason||(n.reason=new r(e),t(n.reason))})}i.prototype.throwIfRequested=function(){if(this.reason)throw this.reason},i.source=function(){var e;return{token:new i(function(t){e=t}),cancel:e}},e.exports=i},function(e,t,n){"use strict";e.exports=function(e){return function(t){return e.apply(null,t)}}},function(e,t,n){"use strict";n.r(t);var r={};n.r(r),n.d(r,"render",function(){return B}),n.d(r,"hydrate",function(){return H}),n.d(r,"createElement",function(){return b}),n.d(r,"h",function(){return b}),n.d(r,"Fragment",function(){return S}),n.d(r,"createRef",function(){return _}),n.d(r,"isValidElement",function(){return a}),n.d(r,"Component",function(){return E}),n.d(r,"cloneElement",function(){return q}),n.d(r,"createContext",function(){return W}),n.d(r,"toChildArray",function(){return R}),n.d(r,"__u",function(){return U}),n.d(r,"options",function(){return o});var i={};n.r(i),n.d(i,"useState",function(){return te}),n.d(i,"useReducer",function(){return ne}),n.d(i,"useEffect",function(){return re}),n.d(i,"useLayoutEffect",function(){return ie}),n.d(i,"useRef",function(){return oe}),n.d(i,"useImperativeHandle",function(){return ae}),n.d(i,"useMemo",function(){return se}),n.d(i,"useCallback",function(){return ue}),n.d(i,"useContext",function(){return ce}),n.d(i,"useDebugValue",function(){return le}),n.d(i,"useErrorBoundary",function(){return de});var o,a,s,u,c,l,d,p=n(0),f=n.n(p),h={},y=[],g=/acit|ex(?:s|g|n|p|$)|rph|grid|ows|mnc|ntw|ine[ch]|zoo|^ord|itera/i;function v(e,t){for(var n in t)e[n]=t[n];return e}function m(e){var t=e.parentNode;t&&t.removeChild(e)}function b(e,t,n){var r,i,o,a=arguments,s={};for(o in t)"key"==o?r=t[o]:"ref"==o?i=t[o]:s[o]=t[o];if(arguments.length>3)for(n=[n],o=3;o<arguments.length;o++)n.push(a[o]);if(null!=n&&(s.children=n),"function"==typeof e&&null!=e.defaultProps)for(o in e.defaultProps)void 0===s[o]&&(s[o]=e.defaultProps[o]);return O(e,s,r,i,null)}function O(e,t,n,r,i){var a={type:e,props:t,key:n,ref:r,__k:null,__:null,__b:0,__e:null,__d:void 0,__c:null,constructor:void 0,__v:i};return null==i&&(a.__v=a),null!=o.vnode&&o.vnode(a),a}function _(){return{current:null}}function S(e){return e.children}function E(e,t){this.props=e,this.context=t}function w(e,t){if(null==t)return e.__?w(e.__,e.__.__k.indexOf(e)+1):null;for(var n;t<e.__k.length;t++)if(null!=(n=e.__k[t])&&null!=n.__e)return n.__e;return"function"==typeof e.type?w(e):null}function P(e){var t,n;if(null!=(e=e.__)&&null!=e.__c){for(e.__e=e.__c.base=null,t=0;t<e.__k.length;t++)if(null!=(n=e.__k[t])&&null!=n.__e){e.__e=e.__c.base=n.__e;break}return P(e)}}function T(e){(!e.__d&&(e.__d=!0)&&s.push(e)&&!A.__r++||c!==o.debounceRendering)&&((c=o.debounceRendering)||u)(A)}function A(){for(var e;A.__r=s.length;)e=s.sort(function(e,t){return e.__v.__b-t.__v.__b}),s=[],e.some(function(e){var t,n,r,i,o,a,s;e.__d&&(a=(o=(t=e).__v).__e,(s=t.__P)&&(n=[],(r=v({},o)).__v=r,i=L(s,o,r,t.__n,void 0!==s.ownerSVGElement,null,n,null==a?w(o):a),x(n,o),i!=a&&P(o)))})}function C(e,t,n,r,i,o,a,s,u,c){var l,d,p,f,g,v,b,_=r&&r.__k||y,E=_.length;for(u==h&&(u=null!=a?a[0]:E?w(r,0):null),n.__k=[],l=0;l<t.length;l++)if(null!=(f=n.__k[l]=null==(f=t[l])||"boolean"==typeof f?null:"string"==typeof f||"number"==typeof f?O(null,f,null,null,f):Array.isArray(f)?O(S,{children:f},null,null,null):null!=f.__e||null!=f.__c?O(f.type,f.props,f.key,null,f.__v):f)){if(f.__=n,f.__b=n.__b+1,null===(p=_[l])||p&&f.key==p.key&&f.type===p.type)_[l]=void 0;else for(d=0;d<E;d++){if((p=_[d])&&f.key==p.key&&f.type===p.type){_[d]=void 0;break}p=null}g=L(e,f,p=p||h,i,o,a,s,u,c),(d=f.ref)&&p.ref!=d&&(b||(b=[]),p.ref&&b.push(p.ref,null,f),b.push(d,f.__c||g,f)),null!=g?(null==v&&(v=g),u=D(e,f,p,_,a,g,u),c||"option"!=n.type?"function"==typeof n.type&&(n.__d=u):e.value=""):u&&p.__e==u&&u.parentNode!=e&&(u=w(p))}if(n.__e=v,null!=a&&"function"!=typeof n.type)for(l=a.length;l--;)null!=a[l]&&m(a[l]);for(l=E;l--;)null!=_[l]&&U(_[l],_[l]);if(b)for(l=0;l<b.length;l++)j(b[l],b[++l],b[++l])}function R(e,t){return t=t||[],null==e||"boolean"==typeof e||(Array.isArray(e)?e.some(function(e){R(e,t)}):t.push(e)),t}function D(e,t,n,r,i,o,a){var s,u,c;if(void 0!==t.__d)s=t.__d,t.__d=void 0;else if(i==n||o!=a||null==o.parentNode)e:if(null==a||a.parentNode!==e)e.appendChild(o),s=null;else{for(u=a,c=0;(u=u.nextSibling)&&c<r.length;c+=2)if(u==o)break e;e.insertBefore(o,a),s=a}return void 0!==s?s:o.nextSibling}function M(e,t,n){"-"===t[0]?e.setProperty(t,n):e[t]=null==n?"":"number"!=typeof n||g.test(t)?n:n+"px"}function I(e,t,n,r,i){var o,a;if(i&&"className"==t&&(t="class"),"style"===t)if("string"==typeof n)e.style=n;else{if("string"==typeof r&&(e.style=r=""),r)for(t in r)n&&t in n||M(e.style,t,"");if(n)for(t in n)r&&n[t]===r[t]||M(e.style,t,n[t])}else"o"===t[0]&&"n"===t[1]?(o=t!==(t=t.replace(/Capture$/,"")),(a=t.toLowerCase())in e&&(t=a),t=t.slice(2),e.l||(e.l={}),e.l[t]=n,n?r||e.addEventListener(t,k,o):e.removeEventListener(t,k,o)):"list"!==t&&"tagName"!==t&&"form"!==t&&"type"!==t&&"size"!==t&&"download"!==t&&"href"!==t&&!i&&t in e?e[t]=null==n?"":n:"function"!=typeof n&&"dangerouslySetInnerHTML"!==t&&(t!==(t=t.replace(/xlink:?/,""))?null==n||!1===n?e.removeAttributeNS("http://www.w3.org/1999/xlink",t.toLowerCase()):e.setAttributeNS("http://www.w3.org/1999/xlink",t.toLowerCase(),n):null==n||!1===n&&!/^ar/.test(t)?e.removeAttribute(t):e.setAttribute(t,n))}function k(e){this.l[e.type](o.event?o.event(e):e)}function N(e,t,n){var r,i;for(r=0;r<e.__k.length;r++)(i=e.__k[r])&&(i.__=e,i.__e&&("function"==typeof i.type&&i.__k.length>1&&N(i,t,n),t=D(n,i,i,e.__k,null,i.__e,t),"function"==typeof e.type&&(e.__d=t)))}function L(e,t,n,r,i,a,s,u,c){var l,d,p,f,h,y,g,m,b,O,_,w=t.type;if(void 0!==t.constructor)return null;(l=o.__b)&&l(t);try{e:if("function"==typeof w){if(m=t.props,b=(l=w.contextType)&&r[l.__c],O=l?b?b.props.value:l.__:r,n.__c?g=(d=t.__c=n.__c).__=d.__E:("prototype"in w&&w.prototype.render?t.__c=d=new w(m,O):(t.__c=d=new E(m,O),d.constructor=w,d.render=F),b&&b.sub(d),d.props=m,d.state||(d.state={}),d.context=O,d.__n=r,p=d.__d=!0,d.__h=[]),null==d.__s&&(d.__s=d.state),null!=w.getDerivedStateFromProps&&(d.__s==d.state&&(d.__s=v({},d.__s)),v(d.__s,w.getDerivedStateFromProps(m,d.__s))),f=d.props,h=d.state,p)null==w.getDerivedStateFromProps&&null!=d.componentWillMount&&d.componentWillMount(),null!=d.componentDidMount&&d.__h.push(d.componentDidMount);else{if(null==w.getDerivedStateFromProps&&m!==f&&null!=d.componentWillReceiveProps&&d.componentWillReceiveProps(m,O),!d.__e&&null!=d.shouldComponentUpdate&&!1===d.shouldComponentUpdate(m,d.__s,O)||t.__v===n.__v){d.props=m,d.state=d.__s,t.__v!==n.__v&&(d.__d=!1),d.__v=t,t.__e=n.__e,t.__k=n.__k,d.__h.length&&s.push(d),N(t,u,e);break e}null!=d.componentWillUpdate&&d.componentWillUpdate(m,d.__s,O),null!=d.componentDidUpdate&&d.__h.push(function(){d.componentDidUpdate(f,h,y)})}d.context=O,d.props=m,d.state=d.__s,(l=o.__r)&&l(t),d.__d=!1,d.__v=t,d.__P=e,l=d.render(d.props,d.state,d.context),d.state=d.__s,null!=d.getChildContext&&(r=v(v({},r),d.getChildContext())),p||null==d.getSnapshotBeforeUpdate||(y=d.getSnapshotBeforeUpdate(f,h)),_=null!=l&&l.type==S&&null==l.key?l.props.children:l,C(e,Array.isArray(_)?_:[_],t,n,r,i,a,s,u,c),d.base=t.__e,d.__h.length&&s.push(d),g&&(d.__E=d.__=null),d.__e=!1}else null==a&&t.__v===n.__v?(t.__k=n.__k,t.__e=n.__e):t.__e=V(n.__e,t,n,r,i,a,s,c);(l=o.diffed)&&l(t)}catch(e){t.__v=null,o.__e(e,t,n)}return t.__e}function x(e,t){o.__c&&o.__c(t,e),e.some(function(t){try{e=t.__h,t.__h=[],e.some(function(e){e.call(t)})}catch(e){o.__e(e,t.__v)}})}function V(e,t,n,r,i,o,a,s){var u,c,l,d,p,f=n.props,g=t.props;if(i="svg"===t.type||i,null!=o)for(u=0;u<o.length;u++)if(null!=(c=o[u])&&((null===t.type?3===c.nodeType:c.localName===t.type)||e==c)){e=c,o[u]=null;break}if(null==e){if(null===t.type)return document.createTextNode(g);e=i?document.createElementNS("http://www.w3.org/2000/svg",t.type):document.createElement(t.type,g.is&&{is:g.is}),o=null,s=!1}if(null===t.type)f!==g&&e.data!==g&&(e.data=g);else{if(null!=o&&(o=y.slice.call(e.childNodes)),l=(f=n.props||h).dangerouslySetInnerHTML,d=g.dangerouslySetInnerHTML,!s){if(null!=o)for(f={},p=0;p<e.attributes.length;p++)f[e.attributes[p].name]=e.attributes[p].value;(d||l)&&(d&&l&&d.__html==l.__html||(e.innerHTML=d&&d.__html||""))}(function(e,t,n,r,i){var o;for(o in n)"children"===o||"key"===o||o in t||I(e,o,null,n[o],r);for(o in t)i&&"function"!=typeof t[o]||"children"===o||"key"===o||"value"===o||"checked"===o||n[o]===t[o]||I(e,o,t[o],n[o],r)})(e,g,f,i,s),d?t.__k=[]:(u=t.props.children,C(e,Array.isArray(u)?u:[u],t,n,r,"foreignObject"!==t.type&&i,o,a,h,s)),s||("value"in g&&void 0!==(u=g.value)&&u!==e.value&&I(e,"value",u,f.value,!1),"checked"in g&&void 0!==(u=g.checked)&&u!==e.checked&&I(e,"checked",u,f.checked,!1))}return e}function j(e,t,n){try{"function"==typeof e?e(t):e.current=t}catch(e){o.__e(e,n)}}function U(e,t,n){var r,i,a;if(o.unmount&&o.unmount(e),(r=e.ref)&&(r.current&&r.current!==e.__e||j(r,null,t)),n||"function"==typeof e.type||(n=null!=(i=e.__e)),e.__e=e.__d=void 0,null!=(r=e.__c)){if(r.componentWillUnmount)try{r.componentWillUnmount()}catch(e){o.__e(e,t)}r.base=r.__P=null}if(r=e.__k)for(a=0;a<r.length;a++)r[a]&&U(r[a],t,n);null!=i&&m(i)}function F(e,t,n){return this.constructor(e,n)}function B(e,t,n){var r,i,a;o.__&&o.__(e,t),i=(r=n===l)?null:n&&n.__k||t.__k,e=b(S,null,[e]),a=[],L(t,(r?t:n||t).__k=e,i||h,h,void 0!==t.ownerSVGElement,n&&!r?[n]:i?null:t.childNodes.length?y.slice.call(t.childNodes):null,a,n||h,r),x(a,e)}function H(e,t){B(e,t,l)}function q(e,t,n){var r,i,o,a=arguments,s=v({},e.props);for(o in t)"key"==o?r=t[o]:"ref"==o?i=t[o]:s[o]=t[o];if(arguments.length>3)for(n=[n],o=3;o<arguments.length;o++)n.push(a[o]);return null!=n&&(s.children=n),O(e.type,s,r||e.key,i||e.ref,null)}function W(e,t){var n={__c:t="__cC"+d++,__:e,Consumer:function(e,t){return e.children(t)},Provider:function(e,n,r){return this.getChildContext||(n=[],(r={})[t]=this,this.getChildContext=function(){return r},this.shouldComponentUpdate=function(e){this.props.value!==e.value&&n.some(T)},this.sub=function(e){n.push(e);var t=e.componentWillUnmount;e.componentWillUnmount=function(){n.splice(n.indexOf(e),1),t&&t.call(e)}}),e.children}};return n.Provider.__=n.Consumer.contextType=n}o={__e:function(e,t){for(var n,r;t=t.__;)if((n=t.__c)&&!n.__)try{if(n.constructor&&null!=n.constructor.getDerivedStateFromError&&(r=!0,n.setState(n.constructor.getDerivedStateFromError(e))),null!=n.componentDidCatch&&(r=!0,n.componentDidCatch(e)),r)return T(n.__E=n)}catch(t){e=t}throw e}},a=function(e){return null!=e&&void 0===e.constructor},E.prototype.setState=function(e,t){var n;n=null!=this.__s&&this.__s!==this.state?this.__s:this.__s=v({},this.state),"function"==typeof e&&(e=e(v({},n),this.props)),e&&v(n,e),null!=e&&this.__v&&(t&&this.__h.push(t),T(this))},E.prototype.forceUpdate=function(e){this.__v&&(this.__e=!0,e&&this.__h.push(e),T(this))},E.prototype.render=S,s=[],u="function"==typeof Promise?Promise.prototype.then.bind(Promise.resolve()):setTimeout,A.__r=0,l=h,d=0;var z,G,$,K=0,Y=[],Z=o.__r,X=o.diffed,J=o.__c,Q=o.unmount;function ee(e,t){o.__h&&o.__h(G,e,K||t),K=0;var n=G.__H||(G.__H={__:[],__h:[]});return e>=n.__.length&&n.__.push({}),n.__[e]}function te(e){return K=1,ne(ve,e)}function ne(e,t,n){var r=ee(z++,2);return r.t=e,r.__c||(r.__c=G,r.__=[n?n(t):ve(void 0,t),function(e){var t=r.t(r.__[0],e);r.__[0]!==t&&(r.__=[t,r.__[1]],r.__c.setState({}))}]),r.__}function re(e,t){var n=ee(z++,3);!o.__s&&ge(n.__H,t)&&(n.__=e,n.__H=t,G.__H.__h.push(n))}function ie(e,t){var n=ee(z++,4);!o.__s&&ge(n.__H,t)&&(n.__=e,n.__H=t,G.__h.push(n))}function oe(e){return K=5,se(function(){return{current:e}},[])}function ae(e,t,n){K=6,ie(function(){"function"==typeof e?e(t()):e&&(e.current=t())},null==n?n:n.concat(e))}function se(e,t){var n=ee(z++,7);return ge(n.__H,t)?(n.__H=t,n.__h=e,n.__=e()):n.__}function ue(e,t){return K=8,se(function(){return e},t)}function ce(e){var t=G.context[e.__c],n=ee(z++,9);return n.__c=e,t?(null==n.__&&(n.__=!0,t.sub(G)),t.props.value):e.__}function le(e,t){o.useDebugValue&&o.useDebugValue(t?t(e):e)}function de(e){var t=ee(z++,10),n=te();return t.__=e,G.componentDidCatch||(G.componentDidCatch=function(e){t.__&&t.__(e),n[1](e)}),[n[0],function(){n[1](void 0)}]}function pe(){Y.some(function(e){if(e.__P)try{e.__H.__h.forEach(he),e.__H.__h.forEach(ye),e.__H.__h=[]}catch(t){return e.__H.__h=[],o.__e(t,e.__v),!0}}),Y=[]}o.__r=function(e){Z&&Z(e),z=0;var t=(G=e.__c).__H;t&&(t.__h.forEach(he),t.__h.forEach(ye),t.__h=[])},o.diffed=function(e){X&&X(e);var t=e.__c;t&&t.__H&&t.__H.__h.length&&(1!==Y.push(t)&&$===o.requestAnimationFrame||(($=o.requestAnimationFrame)||function(e){var t,n=function(){clearTimeout(r),fe&&cancelAnimationFrame(t),setTimeout(e)},r=setTimeout(n,100);fe&&(t=requestAnimationFrame(n))})(pe))},o.__c=function(e,t){t.some(function(e){try{e.__h.forEach(he),e.__h=e.__h.filter(function(e){return!e.__||ye(e)})}catch(n){t.some(function(e){e.__h&&(e.__h=[])}),t=[],o.__e(n,e.__v)}}),J&&J(e,t)},o.unmount=function(e){Q&&Q(e);var t=e.__c;if(t&&t.__H)try{t.__H.__.forEach(he)}catch(e){o.__e(e,t.__v)}};var fe="function"==typeof requestAnimationFrame;function he(e){"function"==typeof e.u&&e.u()}function ye(e){e.u=e.__()}function ge(e,t){return!e||t.some(function(t,n){return t!==e[n]})}function ve(e,t){return"function"==typeof t?t(e):t}function me(e,t){for(var n in t)e[n]=t[n];return e}function be(e,t){for(var n in e)if("__source"!==n&&!(n in t))return!0;for(var r in t)if("__source"!==r&&e[r]!==t[r])return!0;return!1}function Oe(e){this.props=e}(Oe.prototype=new E).isPureReactComponent=!0,Oe.prototype.shouldComponentUpdate=function(e,t){return be(this.props,e)||be(this.state,t)};var _e=o.__b;o.__b=function(e){e.type&&e.type.__f&&e.ref&&(e.props.ref=e.ref,e.ref=null),_e&&_e(e)};var Se="undefined"!=typeof Symbol&&Symbol.for&&Symbol.for("react.forward_ref")||3911;var Ee=function(e,t){return e?R(R(e).map(t)):null},we={map:Ee,forEach:Ee,count:function(e){return e?R(e).length:0},only:function(e){var t=R(e);if(1!==t.length)throw"Children.only";return t[0]},toArray:R},Pe=o.__e;function Te(e){return e&&((e=me({},e)).__c=null,e.__k=e.__k&&e.__k.map(Te)),e}function Ae(e){return e&&(e.__v=null,e.__k=e.__k&&e.__k.map(Ae)),e}function Ce(){this.__u=0,this.t=null,this.__b=null}function Re(e){var t=e.__.__c;return t&&t.o&&t.o(e)}function De(){this.u=null,this.i=null}o.__e=function(e,t,n){if(e.then)for(var r,i=t;i=i.__;)if((r=i.__c)&&r.__c)return null==t.__e&&(t.__e=n.__e,t.__k=n.__k),r.__c(e,t.__c);Pe(e,t,n)},(Ce.prototype=new E).__c=function(e,t){var n=this;null==n.t&&(n.t=[]),n.t.push(t);var r=Re(n.__v),i=!1,o=function(){i||(i=!0,t.componentWillUnmount=t.__c,r?r(a):a())};t.__c=t.componentWillUnmount,t.componentWillUnmount=function(){o(),t.__c&&t.__c()};var a=function(){var e;if(!--n.__u)for(n.__v.__k[0]=Ae(n.state.o),n.setState({o:n.__b=null});e=n.t.pop();)e.forceUpdate()};n.__u++||n.setState({o:n.__b=n.__v.__k[0]}),e.then(o,o)},Ce.prototype.componentWillUnmount=function(){this.t=[]},Ce.prototype.render=function(e,t){return this.__b&&(this.__v.__k&&(this.__v.__k[0]=Te(this.__b)),this.__b=null),[b(S,null,t.o?null:e.children),t.o&&e.fallback]};var Me=function(e,t,n){if(++n[1]===n[0]&&e.i.delete(t),e.props.revealOrder&&("t"!==e.props.revealOrder[0]||!e.i.size))for(n=e.u;n;){for(;n.length>3;)n.pop()();if(n[1]<n[0])break;e.u=n=n[2]}};function Ie(e){return this.getChildContext=function(){return e.context},e.children}function ke(e){var t=this,n=e.l,r=b(Ie,{context:t.context},e.__v);t.componentWillUnmount=function(){var e=t.s.parentNode;e&&e.removeChild(t.s),U(t.v)},t.l&&t.l!==n&&(t.componentWillUnmount(),t.h=!1),e.__v?t.h?(n.__k=t.__k,B(r,n),t.__k=n.__k):(t.s=document.createTextNode(""),t.__k=n.__k,H("",n),n.appendChild(t.s),t.h=!0,t.l=n,B(r,n,t.s),n.__k=t.__k,t.__k=t.s.__k):t.h&&t.componentWillUnmount(),t.v=r}(De.prototype=new E).o=function(e){var t=this,n=Re(t.__v),r=t.i.get(e);return r[0]++,function(i){var o=function(){t.props.revealOrder?(r.push(i),Me(t,e,r)):i()};n?n(o):o()}},De.prototype.render=function(e){this.u=null,this.i=new Map;var t=R(e.children);e.revealOrder&&"b"===e.revealOrder[0]&&t.reverse();for(var n=t.length;n--;)this.i.set(t[n],this.u=[1,0,this.u]);return e.children},De.prototype.componentDidUpdate=De.prototype.componentDidMount=function(){var e=this;this.i.forEach(function(t,n){Me(e,n,t)})};var Ne=/^(?:accent|alignment|arabic|baseline|cap|clip(?!PathU)|color|fill|flood|font|glyph(?!R)|horiz|marker(?!H|W|U)|overline|paint|stop|strikethrough|stroke|text(?!L)|underline|unicode|units|v|vector|vert|word|writing|x(?!C))[A-Z]/;E.prototype.isReactComponent={};var Le="undefined"!=typeof Symbol&&Symbol.for&&Symbol.for("react.element")||60103;var xe=o.event;function Ve(e,t){e["UNSAFE_"+t]&&!e[t]&&Object.defineProperty(e,t,{get:function(){return this["UNSAFE_"+t]},set:function(e){this["UNSAFE_"+t]=e}})}o.event=function(e){xe&&(e=xe(e)),e.persist=function(){};var t=!1,n=!1,r=e.stopPropagation;e.stopPropagation=function(){r.call(e),t=!0};var i=e.preventDefault;return e.preventDefault=function(){i.call(e),n=!0},e.isPropagationStopped=function(){return t},e.isDefaultPrevented=function(){return n},e.nativeEvent=e};var je,Ue={configurable:!0,get:function(){return this.class}},Fe=o.vnode;o.vnode=function(e){e.$$typeof=Le;var t=e.type,n=e.props;if(t){var r;if(n.class!=n.className&&(Ue.enumerable="className"in n,null!=n.className&&(n.class=n.className),Object.defineProperty(n,"className",Ue)),"function"!=typeof t)for(r in n.defaultValue&&void 0!==n.value&&(n.value||0===n.value||(n.value=n.defaultValue),n.defaultValue=void 0),"select"===t&&n.multiple&&Array.isArray(n.value)&&(R(n.children).forEach(function(e){-1!=n.value.indexOf(e.props.value)&&(e.props.selected=!0)}),n.value=void 0),!0===n.download&&(n.download=""),n){var i=Ne.test(r);i&&(e.props[r.replace(/[A-Z0-9]/,"-$&").toLowerCase()]=n[r]),(i||null===n[r])&&(n[r]=void 0)}else t.prototype&&!t.prototype.p&&(t.prototype.p=!0,Ve(t.prototype,"componentWillMount"),Ve(t.prototype,"componentWillReceiveProps"),Ve(t.prototype,"componentWillUpdate"));!function(t){var n=e.type,r=e.props;if(r&&"string"==typeof n){var i={};for(var o in r)/^on(Ani|Tra|Tou)/.test(o)&&(r[o.toLowerCase()]=r[o],delete r[o]),i[o.toLowerCase()]=o;if(i.ondoubleclick&&(r.ondblclick=r[i.ondoubleclick],delete r[i.ondoubleclick]),i.onbeforeinput&&(r.onbeforeinput=r[i.onbeforeinput],delete r[i.onbeforeinput]),i.onchange&&("textarea"===n||"input"===n.toLowerCase()&&!/^fil|che|ra/i.test(r.type))){var a=i.oninput||"oninput";r[a]||(r[a]=r[i.onchange],delete r[i.onchange])}}}()}Fe&&Fe(e)};var Be=o.__r;o.__r=function(e){Be&&Be(e),je=e.__c};function He(e){return!!e&&e.$$typeof===Le}var qe=function(e,t){return e(t)},We={useState:te,useReducer:ne,useEffect:re,useLayoutEffect:ie,useRef:oe,useImperativeHandle:ae,useMemo:se,useCallback:ue,useContext:ce,useDebugValue:le,version:"16.8.0",Children:we,render:function(e,t,n){if(null==t.__k)for(;t.firstChild;)t.removeChild(t.firstChild);return B(e,t),"function"==typeof n&&n(),e?e.__c:null},hydrate:function(e,t,n){return H(e,t),"function"==typeof n&&n(),e?e.__c:null},unmountComponentAtNode:function(e){return!!e.__k&&(B(null,e),!0)},createPortal:function(e,t){return b(ke,{__v:e,l:t})},createElement:b,createContext:W,createFactory:function(e){return b.bind(null,e)},cloneElement:function(e){return He(e)?q.apply(null,arguments):e},createRef:_,Fragment:S,isValidElement:He,findDOMNode:function(e){return e&&(e.base||1===e.nodeType&&e)||null},Component:E,PureComponent:Oe,memo:function(e,t){function n(e){var n=this.props.ref,r=n==e.ref;return!r&&n&&(n.call?n(null):n.current=null),t?!t(this.props,e)||!r:be(this.props,e)}function r(t){return this.shouldComponentUpdate=n,b(e,t)}return r.displayName="Memo("+(e.displayName||e.name)+")",r.prototype.isReactComponent=!0,r.__f=!0,r},forwardRef:function(e){function t(t,n){var r=me({},t);return delete r.ref,e(r,(n=t.ref||n)&&("object"!=typeof n||"current"in n)?n:null)}return t.$$typeof=Se,t.render=t,t.prototype.isReactComponent=t.__f=!0,t.displayName="ForwardRef("+(e.displayName||e.name)+")",t},unstable_batchedUpdates:qe,StrictMode:S,Suspense:Ce,SuspenseList:De,lazy:function(e){var t,n,r;function i(i){if(t||(t=e()).then(function(e){n=e.default||e},function(e){r=e}),r)throw r;if(!n)throw t;return b(n,i)}return i.displayName="Lazy",i.__f=!0,i},__SECRET_INTERNALS_DO_NOT_USE_OR_YOU_WILL_BE_FIRED:{ReactCurrentDispatcher:{current:{readContext:function(e){return je.__n[e.__c].props.value}}}}},ze=(n(33),We.createContext(null));var Ge=function(e){e()},$e=function(){return Ge},Ke={notify:function(){}};var Ye=function(){function e(e,t){this.store=e,this.parentSub=t,this.unsubscribe=null,this.listeners=Ke,this.handleChangeWrapper=this.handleChangeWrapper.bind(this)}var t=e.prototype;return t.addNestedSub=function(e){return this.trySubscribe(),this.listeners.subscribe(e)},t.notifyNestedSubs=function(){this.listeners.notify()},t.handleChangeWrapper=function(){this.onStateChange&&this.onStateChange()},t.isSubscribed=function(){return Boolean(this.unsubscribe)},t.trySubscribe=function(){this.unsubscribe||(this.unsubscribe=this.parentSub?this.parentSub.addNestedSub(this.handleChangeWrapper):this.store.subscribe(this.handleChangeWrapper),this.listeners=function(){var e=$e(),t=null,n=null;return{clear:function(){t=null,n=null},notify:function(){e(function(){for(var e=t;e;)e.callback(),e=e.next})},get:function(){for(var e=[],n=t;n;)e.push(n),n=n.next;return e},subscribe:function(e){var r=!0,i=n={callback:e,next:null,prev:n};return i.prev?i.prev.next=i:t=i,function(){r&&null!==t&&(r=!1,i.next?i.next.prev=i.prev:n=i.prev,i.prev?i.prev.next=i.next:t=i.next)}}}}())},t.tryUnsubscribe=function(){this.unsubscribe&&(this.unsubscribe(),this.unsubscribe=null,this.listeners.clear(),this.listeners=Ke)},e}();var Ze=function(e){var t=e.store,n=e.context,r=e.children,i=se(function(){var e=new Ye(t);return e.onStateChange=e.notifyNestedSubs,{store:t,subscription:e}},[t]),o=se(function(){return t.getState()},[t]);re(function(){var e=i.subscription;return e.trySubscribe(),o!==t.getState()&&e.notifyNestedSubs(),function(){e.tryUnsubscribe(),e.onStateChange=null}},[i,o]);var a=n||ze;return We.createElement(a.Provider,{value:i},r)};function Xe(){return(Xe=Object.assign||function(e){for(var t=1;t<arguments.length;t++){var n=arguments[t];for(var r in n)Object.prototype.hasOwnProperty.call(n,r)&&(e[r]=n[r])}return e}).apply(this,arguments)}function Je(e,t){if(null==e)return{};var n,r,i={},o=Object.keys(e);for(r=0;r<o.length;r++)n=o[r],t.indexOf(n)>=0||(i[n]=e[n]);return i}var Qe=n(15),et=n.n(Qe),tt=n(14),nt="undefined"!==typeof window&&"undefined"!==typeof window.document&&"undefined"!==typeof window.document.createElement?ie:re,rt=[],it=[null,null];function ot(e,t){var n=e[1];return[t.payload,n+1]}function at(e,t,n){nt(function(){return e.apply(void 0,t)},n)}function st(e,t,n,r,i,o,a){e.current=r,t.current=i,n.current=!1,o.current&&(o.current=null,a())}function ut(e,t,n,r,i,o,a,s,u,c){if(e){var l=!1,d=null,p=function(){if(!l){var e,n,p=t.getState();try{e=r(p,i.current)}catch(f){n=f,d=f}n||(d=null),e===o.current?a.current||u():(o.current=e,s.current=e,a.current=!0,c({type:"STORE_UPDATED",payload:{error:n}}))}};n.onStateChange=p,n.trySubscribe(),p();return function(){if(l=!0,n.tryUnsubscribe(),n.onStateChange=null,d)throw d}}}var ct=function(){return[null,0]};function lt(e,t){void 0===t&&(t={});var n=t,r=n.getDisplayName,i=void 0===r?function(e){return"ConnectAdvanced("+e+")"}:r,o=n.methodName,a=void 0===o?"connectAdvanced":o,s=n.renderCountProp,u=void 0===s?void 0:s,c=n.shouldHandleStateChanges,l=void 0===c||c,d=n.storeKey,p=void 0===d?"store":d,f=(n.withRef,n.forwardRef),h=void 0!==f&&f,y=n.context,g=void 0===y?ze:y,v=Je(n,["getDisplayName","methodName","renderCountProp","shouldHandleStateChanges","storeKey","withRef","forwardRef","context"]),m=g;return function(t){var n=t.displayName||t.name||"Component",r=i(n),o=Xe({},v,{getDisplayName:i,methodName:a,renderCountProp:u,shouldHandleStateChanges:l,storeKey:p,displayName:r,wrappedComponentName:n,WrappedComponent:t}),s=v.pure;var c=s?se:function(e){return e()};function d(n){var r=se(function(){var e=n.reactReduxForwardedRef,t=Je(n,["reactReduxForwardedRef"]);return[n.context,e,t]},[n]),i=r[0],a=r[1],s=r[2],u=se(function(){return i&&i.Consumer&&Object(tt.isContextConsumer)(We.createElement(i.Consumer,null))?i:m},[i,m]),d=ce(u),p=Boolean(n.store)&&Boolean(n.store.getState)&&Boolean(n.store.dispatch);Boolean(d)&&Boolean(d.store);var f=p?n.store:d.store,h=se(function(){return function(t){return e(t.dispatch,o)}(f)},[f]),y=se(function(){if(!l)return it;var e=new Ye(f,p?null:d.subscription),t=e.notifyNestedSubs.bind(e);return[e,t]},[f,p,d]),g=y[0],v=y[1],b=se(function(){return p?d:Xe({},d,{subscription:g})},[p,d,g]),O=ne(ot,rt,ct),_=O[0][0],S=O[1];if(_&&_.error)throw _.error;var E=oe(),w=oe(s),P=oe(),T=oe(!1),A=c(function(){return P.current&&s===w.current?P.current:h(f.getState(),s)},[f,_,s]);at(st,[w,E,T,s,A,P,v]),at(ut,[l,f,g,h,w,E,T,P,v,S],[f,g,h]);var C=se(function(){return We.createElement(t,Xe({},A,{ref:a}))},[a,t,A]);return se(function(){return l?We.createElement(u.Provider,{value:b},C):C},[u,C,b])}var f=s?We.memo(d):d;if(f.WrappedComponent=t,f.displayName=r,h){var y=We.forwardRef(function(e,t){return We.createElement(f,Xe({},e,{reactReduxForwardedRef:t}))});return y.displayName=r,y.WrappedComponent=t,et()(y,t)}return et()(f,t)}}function dt(e,t){return e===t?0!==e||0!==t||1/e===1/t:e!==e&&t!==t}function pt(e,t){if(dt(e,t))return!0;if("object"!==typeof e||null===e||"object"!==typeof t||null===t)return!1;var n=Object.keys(e),r=Object.keys(t);if(n.length!==r.length)return!1;for(var i=0;i<n.length;i++)if(!Object.prototype.hasOwnProperty.call(t,n[i])||!dt(e[n[i]],t[n[i]]))return!1;return!0}var ft=n(16),ht=function(){return Math.random().toString(36).substring(7).split("").join(".")},yt={INIT:"@@redux/INIT"+ht(),REPLACE:"@@redux/REPLACE"+ht(),PROBE_UNKNOWN_ACTION:function(){return"@@redux/PROBE_UNKNOWN_ACTION"+ht()}};function gt(e){if("object"!==typeof e||null===e)return!1;for(var t=e;null!==Object.getPrototypeOf(t);)t=Object.getPrototypeOf(t);return Object.getPrototypeOf(e)===t}function vt(e,t,n){var r;if("function"===typeof t&&"function"===typeof n||"function"===typeof n&&"function"===typeof arguments[3])throw new Error("It looks like you are passing several store enhancers to createStore(). This is not supported. Instead, compose them together to a single function.");if("function"===typeof t&&"undefined"===typeof n&&(n=t,t=void 0),"undefined"!==typeof n){if("function"!==typeof n)throw new Error("Expected the enhancer to be a function.");return n(vt)(e,t)}if("function"!==typeof e)throw new Error("Expected the reducer to be a function.");var i=e,o=t,a=[],s=a,u=!1;function c(){s===a&&(s=a.slice())}function l(){if(u)throw new Error("You may not call store.getState() while the reducer is executing. The reducer has already received the state as an argument. Pass it down from the top reducer instead of reading it from the store.");return o}function d(e){if("function"!==typeof e)throw new Error("Expected the listener to be a function.");if(u)throw new Error("You may not call store.subscribe() while the reducer is executing. If you would like to be notified after the store has been updated, subscribe from a component and invoke store.getState() in the callback to access the latest state. See https://redux.js.org/api-reference/store#subscribelistener for more details.");var t=!0;return c(),s.push(e),function(){if(t){if(u)throw new Error("You may not unsubscribe from a store listener while the reducer is executing. See https://redux.js.org/api-reference/store#subscribelistener for more details.");t=!1,c();var n=s.indexOf(e);s.splice(n,1),a=null}}}function p(e){if(!gt(e))throw new Error("Actions must be plain objects. Use custom middleware for async actions.");if("undefined"===typeof e.type)throw new Error('Actions may not have an undefined "type" property. Have you misspelled a constant?');if(u)throw new Error("Reducers may not dispatch actions.");try{u=!0,o=i(o,e)}finally{u=!1}for(var t=a=s,n=0;n<t.length;n++){(0,t[n])()}return e}return p({type:yt.INIT}),(r={dispatch:p,subscribe:d,getState:l,replaceReducer:function(e){if("function"!==typeof e)throw new Error("Expected the nextReducer to be a function.");i=e,p({type:yt.REPLACE})}})[ft.a]=function(){var e,t=d;return(e={subscribe:function(e){if("object"!==typeof e||null===e)throw new TypeError("Expected the observer to be an object.");function n(){e.next&&e.next(l())}return n(),{unsubscribe:t(n)}}})[ft.a]=function(){return this},e},r}function mt(e,t){var n=t&&t.type;return"Given "+(n&&'action "'+String(n)+'"'||"an action")+', reducer "'+e+'" returned undefined. To ignore an action, you must explicitly return the previous state. If you want this reducer to hold no value, you can return null instead of undefined.'}function bt(e,t){return function(){return t(e.apply(this,arguments))}}function Ot(e,t){if("function"===typeof e)return bt(e,t);if("object"!==typeof e||null===e)throw new Error("bindActionCreators expected an object or a function, instead received "+(null===e?"null":typeof e)+'. Did you write "import ActionCreators from" instead of "import * as ActionCreators from"?');var n={};for(var r in e){var i=e[r];"function"===typeof i&&(n[r]=bt(i,t))}return n}function _t(e,t,n){return t in e?Object.defineProperty(e,t,{value:n,enumerable:!0,configurable:!0,writable:!0}):e[t]=n,e}function St(e,t){var n=Object.keys(e);return Object.getOwnPropertySymbols&&n.push.apply(n,Object.getOwnPropertySymbols(e)),t&&(n=n.filter(function(t){return Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(e,t).enumerable})),n}function Et(){for(var e=arguments.length,t=new Array(e),n=0;n<e;n++)t[n]=arguments[n];return 0===t.length?function(e){return e}:1===t.length?t[0]:t.reduce(function(e,t){return function(){return e(t.apply(void 0,arguments))}})}function wt(){for(var e=arguments.length,t=new Array(e),n=0;n<e;n++)t[n]=arguments[n];return function(e){return function(){var n=e.apply(void 0,arguments),r=function(){throw new Error("Dispatching while constructing your middleware is not allowed. Other middleware would not be applied to this dispatch.")},i={getState:n.getState,dispatch:function(){return r.apply(void 0,arguments)}},o=t.map(function(e){return e(i)});return function(e){for(var t=1;t<arguments.length;t++){var n=null!=arguments[t]?arguments[t]:{};t%2?St(n,!0).forEach(function(t){_t(e,t,n[t])}):Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors?Object.defineProperties(e,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors(n)):St(n).forEach(function(t){Object.defineProperty(e,t,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(n,t))})}return e}({},n,{dispatch:r=Et.apply(void 0,o)(n.dispatch)})}}}function Pt(e){return function(t,n){var r=e(t,n);function i(){return r}return i.dependsOnOwnProps=!1,i}}function Tt(e){return null!==e.dependsOnOwnProps&&void 0!==e.dependsOnOwnProps?Boolean(e.dependsOnOwnProps):1!==e.length}function At(e,t){return function(t,n){n.displayName;var r=function(e,t){return r.dependsOnOwnProps?r.mapToProps(e,t):r.mapToProps(e)};return r.dependsOnOwnProps=!0,r.mapToProps=function(t,n){r.mapToProps=e,r.dependsOnOwnProps=Tt(e);var i=r(t,n);return"function"===typeof i&&(r.mapToProps=i,r.dependsOnOwnProps=Tt(i),i=r(t,n)),i},r}}var Ct=[function(e){return"function"===typeof e?At(e):void 0},function(e){return e?void 0:Pt(function(e){return{dispatch:e}})},function(e){return e&&"object"===typeof e?Pt(function(t){return Ot(e,t)}):void 0}];var Rt=[function(e){return"function"===typeof e?At(e):void 0},function(e){return e?void 0:Pt(function(){return{}})}];function Dt(e,t,n){return Xe({},n,{},e,{},t)}var Mt=[function(e){return"function"===typeof e?function(e){return function(t,n){n.displayName;var r,i=n.pure,o=n.areMergedPropsEqual,a=!1;return function(t,n,s){var u=e(t,n,s);return a?i&&o(u,r)||(r=u):(a=!0,r=u),r}}}(e):void 0},function(e){return e?void 0:function(){return Dt}}];function It(e,t,n,r){return function(i,o){return n(e(i,o),t(r,o),o)}}function kt(e,t,n,r,i){var o,a,s,u,c,l=i.areStatesEqual,d=i.areOwnPropsEqual,p=i.areStatePropsEqual,f=!1;function h(i,f){var h=!d(f,a),y=!l(i,o);return o=i,a=f,h&&y?(s=e(o,a),t.dependsOnOwnProps&&(u=t(r,a)),c=n(s,u,a)):h?(e.dependsOnOwnProps&&(s=e(o,a)),t.dependsOnOwnProps&&(u=t(r,a)),c=n(s,u,a)):y?function(){var t=e(o,a),r=!p(t,s);return s=t,r&&(c=n(s,u,a)),c}():c}return function(i,l){return f?h(i,l):(s=e(o=i,a=l),u=t(r,a),c=n(s,u,a),f=!0,c)}}function Nt(e,t){var n=t.initMapStateToProps,r=t.initMapDispatchToProps,i=t.initMergeProps,o=Je(t,["initMapStateToProps","initMapDispatchToProps","initMergeProps"]),a=n(e,o),s=r(e,o),u=i(e,o);return(o.pure?kt:It)(a,s,u,e,o)}function Lt(e,t,n){for(var r=t.length-1;r>=0;r--){var i=t[r](e);if(i)return i}return function(t,r){throw new Error("Invalid value of type "+typeof e+" for "+n+" argument when connecting component "+r.wrappedComponentName+".")}}function xt(e,t){return e===t}function Vt(e){var t=void 0===e?{}:e,n=t.connectHOC,r=void 0===n?lt:n,i=t.mapStateToPropsFactories,o=void 0===i?Rt:i,a=t.mapDispatchToPropsFactories,s=void 0===a?Ct:a,u=t.mergePropsFactories,c=void 0===u?Mt:u,l=t.selectorFactory,d=void 0===l?Nt:l;return function(e,t,n,i){void 0===i&&(i={});var a=i,u=a.pure,l=void 0===u||u,p=a.areStatesEqual,f=void 0===p?xt:p,h=a.areOwnPropsEqual,y=void 0===h?pt:h,g=a.areStatePropsEqual,v=void 0===g?pt:g,m=a.areMergedPropsEqual,b=void 0===m?pt:m,O=Je(a,["pure","areStatesEqual","areOwnPropsEqual","areStatePropsEqual","areMergedPropsEqual"]),_=Lt(e,o,"mapStateToProps"),S=Lt(t,s,"mapDispatchToProps"),E=Lt(n,c,"mergeProps");return r(d,Xe({methodName:"connect",getDisplayName:function(e){return"Connect("+e+")"},shouldHandleStateChanges:Boolean(e),initMapStateToProps:_,initMapDispatchToProps:S,initMergeProps:E,pure:l,areStatesEqual:f,areOwnPropsEqual:y,areStatePropsEqual:v,areMergedPropsEqual:b},O))}}var jt=Vt();Ge=qe;var Ut={BORDER_RADIUS:{EXTRA_TINY:"1px",TINY:"2px",SMALL:"3px",MEDIUM:"4px"},COLORS:{BLACK:"#000000",WHITE:"#ffffff",TRANSPARENT:"transparent",GRADIENT_BLACK:"linear-gradient(to bottom, rgba(0, 0, 0, 0.63), rgba(0, 0, 0, 0.62) 19%, rgba(0, 0, 0, 0))",LIGHT_GRAY_TRANSPARENT:"rgba(255, 255, 255, 0.23)",GRAY_TRANSPARENT:"rgba(143, 143, 143, 0.46)",BLACK_TRANSPARENT:"rgba(0, 0, 0, 0.7)",BLACK_80_OPACITY:"rgba(0, 0, 0, 0.8)",BLUR_TRANSPARENT:"rgba(0, 0, 0, 0.48)",GRAY:"rgba(0, 0, 0, 0.35)",SELECTED:"rgba(255, 255, 255, 0.1)",SCROLL_BAR:"rgba(255, 255, 255, 0.64)",TRANSPARENT_30_OPACITY:"rgba(0, 0, 0, 0.31)"},TEXT_FONT_FAMILY:{LATO:"Lato"},FONT_SIZE:{EXTRA_TINY:"8px",TINY:"10px",EXTRA_SMALL:"12px",SMALL:"14px",MEDIUM:"18px"}},Ft="179px",Bt="269px",Ht="180px",qt="270px",Wt="300px",zt="301px",Gt="500px",$t="501px",Kt="800px",Yt="801px";function Zt(e,t){var n=Object.keys(e);if(Object.getOwnPropertySymbols){var r=Object.getOwnPropertySymbols(e);t&&(r=r.filter(function(t){return Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(e,t).enumerable})),n.push.apply(n,r)}return n}function Xt(e){for(var t=1;t<arguments.length;t++){var n=null!=arguments[t]?arguments[t]:{};t%2?Zt(Object(n),!0).forEach(function(t){f()(e,t,n[t])}):Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors?Object.defineProperties(e,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors(n)):Zt(Object(n)).forEach(function(t){Object.defineProperty(e,t,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(n,t))})}return e}var Jt=function e(t){return t&&t.length>0?t.reduce(function(t,n){return n?Array.isArray(n)?Xt(Xt({},t),e(n)):Xt(Xt({},t),n):t},{}):null},Qt=function(e){return function(t){t({type:"[CORE] change video tag status",payload:e})}},en=function(e){return function(t){t({type:"[CORE] update video current fragment position",payload:e})}},tn=function(e){return function(t){t({type:"[CORE] update video current position",payload:e})}},nn=function(e){return function(t){t({type:"[CORE] update video current buffered time",payload:e})}},rn=function(e){return function(t){t({type:"[CORE] update video current duration",payload:e})}},on=function(e,t){return function(n){n({type:"[COMMON] set pending video status",payload:{pendingStatusObject:{type:e,value:t}}})}},an=function(e){return function(t){t({type:"[COMMON] set mute video",payload:e})}},sn=function(e){return function(t){t({type:"[COMMON] set volume",payload:e})}},un=function(e){return function(t){t({type:"[CORE] change loading player status",payload:e})}},cn=function(e){return function(t){t({type:"[COMMON] show black screen with loader",payload:e})}},ln=function(e){return function(t){t({type:"[CORE] set player size",payload:e})}},dn=function(e){return function(t){t({type:"[COMMON] set error message",payload:e})}},pn={mediaData:function(e){return e.mediaData},playerData:function(e){return e.playerData},dependenciesLoadingStatus:function(e){return e.dependenciesLoadingStatus},brandingData:function(e){return e.brandingData},anchorOptions:function(e){return e.anchorOptions},semanticOptions:function(e){return e.semanticOptions},monetization:function(e){return e.monetization},userInteraction:function(e){return e.userInteraction},splitView:function(e){return e.splitView},discovery:function(e){return e.discovery}},fn=pn.playerData,hn={playerId:function(e){return fn(e).playerId},videoTagStatus:function(e){return fn(e).videoTagStatus},pendingVideoTagStatus:function(e){return fn(e).pendingVideoTagStatus},currentVideoTime:function(e){return fn(e).currentVideoTime},currentVideoTimeFragment:function(e){return fn(e).currentVideoTimeFragment},currentVideoDuration:function(e){return fn(e).currentVideoDuration},currentVideoBufferedTime:function(e){return fn(e).currentVideoBufferedTime},playerMode:function(e){return fn(e).playerMode},playerSettings:function(e){return fn(e).playerSettings},playbackMethod:function(e){return fn(e).playbackMethod},playerVisibility:function(e){return fn(e).playerVisibility},playerPlaceholderVisibility:function(e){return fn(e).playerPlaceholderVisibility},playerInstanceUniqId:function(e){return fn(e).playerInstanceUniqId},showBlackScreen:function(e){return fn(e).loader.showBlackScreen},playerSize:function(e){return fn(e).playerSize},errorMessage:function(e){return fn(e).errorMessage},loadingPlayer:function(e){return fn(e).loadingPlayer}},yn=hn.playerSettings,gn={volume:function(e){return yn(e).volume},muted:function(e){return yn(e).muted},quality:function(e){return yn(e).quality},speed:function(e){return yn(e).speed},selectedSettingsCategory:function(e){return yn(e).selectedSettingsCategory},fullscreen:function(e){return yn(e).fullscreen},pendingFullscreenRequest:function(e){return this.fullscreen(e).pendingFullscreenRequest},isFullscreenOn:function(e){return this.fullscreen(e).isFullscreenOn}},vn=function(e){return"".concat("mplayer-monetization-placeholder","-").concat(e)},mn=function(e){return"".concat("mplayer-player-placeholder","-").concat(e)},bn=function(e){return"".concat("mplayer-player-container","-").concat(e)},On=function(e){return"".concat("mplayer-controls-placeholder","-").concat(e)},_n=function(e){return"".concat("mplayer-split-view-placeholder","-").concat(e)},Sn=function(e){return"".concat("mplayer-discovery-placeholder","-").concat(e)},En=function(e){return"".concat("mplayer-content-video","-").concat(e)},wn=function(e){return"".concat("mplayer","-").concat("error-page-message","-").concat(e)},Pn=function(e){return"".concat("mplayer","-").concat("loader","-").concat(e)},Tn=function(e){return"".concat("mplayer","-").concat("mplayer-close-bar-placeholder","-").concat(e)},An=pn.mediaData,Cn={mediaId:function(e){return An(e).mediaId},mediaType:function(e){return An(e).mediaType},videoList:function(e){return An(e).videoList},videoData:function(e){return An(e).videoData},activeVideoIndex:function(e){return An(e).activeVideoIndex},mediaRequest:function(e){return An(e).mediaRequest},mediaLoadingError:function(e){return An(e).mediaLoadingError}},Rn=Cn.videoData,Dn={creator:function(e){return Rn(e).creator},description:function(e){return Rn(e).description},duration:function(e){return Rn(e).duration},externalId:function(e){return Rn(e).externalId},index:function(e){return Rn(e).index},mediaId:function(e){return Rn(e).mediaId},provider:function(e){return Rn(e).provider},sources:function(e){return Rn(e).sources},tags:function(e){return Rn(e).tags},thumbnail:function(e){return Rn(e).thumbnail},title:function(e){return Rn(e).title},showTitle:function(e){return Rn(e).showTitle}},Mn=n(11),In=n.n(Mn);function kn(e){var t=arguments.length>1&&void 0!==arguments[1]?arguments[1]:[];return se(function(){return e.apply(void 0,In()(t))},[e,t])}var Nn={root:{height:"100%",left:0,display:"block",width:"100%"}},Ln=function(){return{playerStyle:[Nn.root]}},xn=n(30),Vn=Object(xn.a)(),jn=function(e){return"undefined"===typeof e},Un=function(e){return jn(e)||null===e||0===e.length},Fn=function(){return null!==navigator.userAgent.match(/(iPad)|(iPhone)|(iPod)|(android)|(webOS)/i)},Bn=function(){return"iOS"===Vn.os},Hn=function(e){return Math.floor(10*e)/10},qn=function(){var e=arguments.length>0&&void 0!==arguments[0]?arguments[0]:"0";return parseInt(e,10)/1e3},Wn=function(e,t){return jn(e)||jn(t)?0:e>t?1:-1},zn=function(e,t){var n;return function(){for(var r=arguments.length,i=new Array(r),o=0;o<r;o++)i[o]=arguments[o];clearTimeout(n),n=setTimeout(function(){n=null,e(i)},t)}},Gn=function(e){var t=e.clientWidth*(9/16),n=e.clientHeight;return Math.abs(t-n)>2?t:n},$n=function(e){return function(e){return!e.dataLayer||Array.isArray(e.dataLayer)}(e)?"dataLayer":"mmVideoDataLayer"},Kn=function(e){var t=ue(e,arguments.length>1&&void 0!==arguments[1]?arguments[1]:[]);re(function(){t()},[t]),re(function(){var e=zn(t,150);return window.addEventListener("resize",e),function(){window.removeEventListener("resize",e)}},[t])},Yn=pn.splitView,Zn=function(e){return Yn(e).splitViewRatio},Xn={GENERAL_ERROR:"An error has occurred, please try again later",VIDEO_ERROR:"The video is currently unavailable"},Jn=jt(function(e,t){return{thumbnail:Dn.thumbnail(e),playerId:t.playerId,muted:gn.muted(e),volume:gn.volume(e),currentVideoTime:hn.currentVideoTime(e),playbackMethod:hn.playbackMethod(e),splitViewRatio:Zn(e),playerSize:hn.playerSize(e),isFullscreenOn:gn.isFullscreenOn(e),playerMode:hn.playerMode(e),speed:gn.speed(e)}},function(e){return Ot({updateVideoTagStatus:Qt,updateVideoTime:tn,updateVideoTimeFragment:en,changeLoadingPlayerStatus:un,showBlackScreenLoader:cn,updateVideoDuration:rn,updateMute:an,updateVolume:sn,updateCurrentVideoBufferedTime:nn,setPlayerSize:ln,onErrorMessage:dn},e)})(function(e){var t=e.thumbnail,n=e.playerId,r=e.updateVideoTagStatus,i=e.updateVideoTime,o=e.updateCurrentVideoBufferedTime,a=e.muted,s=e.volume,u=e.currentVideoTime,c=e.playbackMethod,l=e.changeLoadingPlayerStatus,d=e.showBlackScreenLoader,p=e.updateVideoDuration,f=e.updateMute,h=e.updateVolume,y=e.updateVideoTimeFragment,g=e.setPlayerSize,v=e.splitViewRatio,m=e.playerSize,O=e.isFullscreenOn,_=e.playerMode,S=e.onErrorMessage,E=e.speed,w=oe(null),P=!Un(v),T=kn(Ln,[]).playerStyle,A=document.getElementById(bn(n));re(function(){w.current.playbackRate=E},[E]),Kn(function(){if(!Un(A)){var e=function(e,t,n,r,i){var o=e.style.width;return n?"desktop"===r||"miniDesktop"===r||"stickyDesktop"===r?"extraLarge":"medium":"100%"===o?t:i&&o<=Bt?"extraSmall":i&&o>=qt&&o<=Wt?"small":o<=Ft?"extraSmall":o>=Ht&&o<=Wt?"small":o>=zt&&o<=Gt?"medium":o>=$t&&o<=Kt?"large":o>=Yt?"extraLarge":null}(A,m,O,_,P);Un(e)||g(e)}},[A,m,O,_,P,g]);var C="autoplay"===c?"":t;return b("video",{ref:w,style:Jt(T),key:En(n),id:En(n),onEnded:function(){r("complete")},onDurationChange:function(e){var t,n=e.target.duration;t=n,Number.isNaN(t)||"number"!==typeof t||p(n)},onTimeUpdate:function(e){var t=e.target.currentTime;y(t);var n=parseInt(t.toString(),10);n!==u&&i(n)},onVolumeChange:function(e){var t=e.target,n=t.volume,r=t.muted;n!==s&&h(n),a!==r&&f(r)},onPause:function(){r("paused")},onPlaying:function(){r("playing")},onError:function(){r("error"),S(Xn.VIDEO_ERROR)},onLoadStart:function(){l(!0),d(!0)},onCanPlayThrough:function(){l(!1),d(!1),S("")},onCanPlay:function(){l(!1),S("")},onWaiting:function(){l(!0)},onSeeking:function(){r("seeking")},onProgress:function(e){var t=e.target.buffered,n=t.length;n>0&&o(t.end(n-1))},volume:s,muted:a,poster:C,playsInline:!0,"webkit-playsinline":""},b("track",{kind:"captions"}))}),Qn=function(e){return function(t){t({type:"[CORE] update ima status",payload:e})}},er=function(e){return function(t){t({type:"[CORE] update hls status",payload:e})}},tr=function(){return{adBlockCheckerStyle:{backgroundColor:Ut.COLORS.TRANSPARENT,height:"1px",width:"1px"}}},nr=jt(null,function(e){return{onAdBlockDetected:function(){return Qn("blocked")(e)}}})(function(e){var t=oe(),n=kn(tr,[]).adBlockCheckerStyle;return re(function(){0!==t.current.clientHeight&&0!==t.current.clientWidth||(0,e.onAdBlockDetected)()},[e]),b("div",{ref:t},b("div",{class:"adBanner",style:n}))}),rr=n(10),ir=n.n(rr);n(20);var or={root:null,threshold:[0,.1,.2,.3,.4,.5,.6,.7,.8,.9,1],rootMargin:"0px"},ar=function(e){var t=e.id,n=e.updateContainerVisibility,r=e.shouldObserve,i=e.regularMode,o=e.stickyAnchorMode,a=e.children,s=e.style,u=e.isPlaceholder,c=e.splitViewRatio,l=e.className,d=oe(),p=te(null),f=ir()(p,2),h=f[0],y=f[1];return re(function(){null!==h&&h.observe(d.current)},[h]),re(function(){!function(e,t,n){e&&t(new IntersectionObserver(function(e){var t=e[0];n(t.intersectionRatio)},or))}(r,y,n)},[r,n]),Kn(function(){u?function(e,t){var n=e.parentElement.clientWidth;e.style.maxWidth="".concat(n,"px"),Un(t)?e.style.height="".concat(Gn(e),"px"):e.style.height="".concat(Gn(e)*(t/100),"px")}(d.current,c):function(e,t,n){t?(e.style.width="".concat(e.parentElement.clientWidth,"px"),e.style.height="".concat(e.parentElement.clientHeight,"px")):n&&(e.style.width="".concat(e.parentElement.clientWidth,"px"),e.style.height="".concat(e.parentElement.clientHeight+24,"px"))}(d.current,i,o)},[u,i,o]),b("div",{className:l,id:t,ref:d,style:s},a)};function sr(e,t){var n=Object.keys(e);if(Object.getOwnPropertySymbols){var r=Object.getOwnPropertySymbols(e);t&&(r=r.filter(function(t){return Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(e,t).enumerable})),n.push.apply(n,r)}return n}function ur(e){for(var t=1;t<arguments.length;t++){var n=null!=arguments[t]?arguments[t]:{};t%2?sr(Object(n),!0).forEach(function(t){f()(e,t,n[t])}):Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors?Object.defineProperties(e,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors(n)):sr(Object(n)).forEach(function(t){Object.defineProperty(e,t,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(n,t))})}return e}var cr={display:"flex",flexDirection:"row"},lr=(ur(ur({},cr),{},{justifyContent:"flex-end"}),ur(ur({},cr),{},{justifyContent:"center"})),dr=ur(ur({},cr),{},{justifyContent:"space-between"}),pr=(ur(ur({},dr),{},{alignItems:"baseline"}),ur(ur({},lr),{},{alignItems:"center"})),fr=ur(ur({},cr),{},{justifyContent:"flex-start"}),hr=(ur(ur({},fr),{},{alignItems:"center"}),ur(ur({},fr),{},{alignItems:"flex-end",flexWrap:"wrap"}),{display:"flex",flexDirection:"column"}),yr=(ur(ur({},hr),{},{justifyContent:"flex-start"}),ur(ur({},hr),{},{justifyContent:"center"})),gr=(ur(ur({},hr),{},{justifyContent:"flex-end",alignItems:"center"}),ur(ur({},yr),{},{alignItems:"center"}));function vr(e,t){var n=Object.keys(e);if(Object.getOwnPropertySymbols){var r=Object.getOwnPropertySymbols(e);t&&(r=r.filter(function(t){return Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(e,t).enumerable})),n.push.apply(n,r)}return n}function mr(e){for(var t=1;t<arguments.length;t++){var n=null!=arguments[t]?arguments[t]:{};t%2?vr(Object(n),!0).forEach(function(t){f()(e,t,n[t])}):Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors?Object.defineProperties(e,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors(n)):vr(Object(n)).forEach(function(t){Object.defineProperty(e,t,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(n,t))})}return e}var br=mr(mr({},hr),{},{position:"relative",background:Ut.COLORS.BLACK}),Or=pn.anchorOptions,_r=function(e){return Or(e).orientation},Sr=function(e){return Or(e).canClose},Er=function(e){return Or(e).anchorData},wr=function(e){return Er(e).anchorDisabledByUser},Pr=function(e){return Er(e).anchorStatus};function Tr(e,t){var n=Object.keys(e);if(Object.getOwnPropertySymbols){var r=Object.getOwnPropertySymbols(e);t&&(r=r.filter(function(t){return Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(e,t).enumerable})),n.push.apply(n,r)}return n}function Ar(e){for(var t=1;t<arguments.length;t++){var n=null!=arguments[t]?arguments[t]:{};t%2?Tr(Object(n),!0).forEach(function(t){f()(e,t,n[t])}):Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors?Object.defineProperties(e,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors(n)):Tr(Object(n)).forEach(function(t){Object.defineProperty(e,t,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(n,t))})}return e}var Cr=jt(function(e){return{shouldObserve:"lazyplay"===hn.playbackMethod(e)||"autoplay_when_viewable"===hn.playbackMethod(e),regularMode:"inactive"===Pr(e)||!Un(Zn(e)),stickyAnchorMode:"sticky"===_r(e)&&"active"===Pr(e)&&Sr(e),style:Ar({},mr({},br)),isPlaceholder:!1}},function(e){return{updateContainerVisibility:function(t){return function(e){return function(t){t({type:"[CORE] update player visibility",payload:e})}}(t)(e)}}})(ar);function Rr(e,t){var n=Object.keys(e);if(Object.getOwnPropertySymbols){var r=Object.getOwnPropertySymbols(e);t&&(r=r.filter(function(t){return Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(e,t).enumerable})),n.push.apply(n,r)}return n}function Dr(e){for(var t=1;t<arguments.length;t++){var n=null!=arguments[t]?arguments[t]:{};t%2?Rr(Object(n),!0).forEach(function(t){f()(e,t,n[t])}):Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors?Object.defineProperties(e,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors(n)):Rr(Object(n)).forEach(function(t){Object.defineProperty(e,t,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(n,t))})}return e}var Mr={width:"100%",clear:"both",margin:"16px 0"};function Ir(e,t){var n=Object.keys(e);if(Object.getOwnPropertySymbols){var r=Object.getOwnPropertySymbols(e);t&&(r=r.filter(function(t){return Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(e,t).enumerable})),n.push.apply(n,r)}return n}function kr(e){for(var t=1;t<arguments.length;t++){var n=null!=arguments[t]?arguments[t]:{};t%2?Ir(Object(n),!0).forEach(function(t){f()(e,t,n[t])}):Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors?Object.defineProperties(e,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors(n)):Ir(Object(n)).forEach(function(t){Object.defineProperty(e,t,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(n,t))})}return e}var Nr=jt(function(e){return{shouldObserve:"sticky"!==_r(e),style:kr({},Dr({},Mr)),splitViewRatio:Zn(e),isPlaceholder:!0,className:"monti-placeholder-mm-player"}},function(e){return{updateContainerVisibility:function(t){return function(e){return function(t){t({type:"[CORE] update placeholder visibility",payload:e})}}(t)(e)}}})(ar),Lr=function(){return{monetizationStyle:{position:"absolute",width:"100%",top:"0",display:"none"}}},xr=function(e){var t=e.playerId,n=kn(Lr,[]).monetizationStyle,r=oe();return Kn(function(){console.log("the monetization parent element : ",r.current.parentElement),r.current.style.height="".concat(Gn(r.current.parentElement),"px")}),b("div",{id:vn(t),style:n,ref:r})};function Vr(e,t){var n=Object.keys(e);if(Object.getOwnPropertySymbols){var r=Object.getOwnPropertySymbols(e);t&&(r=r.filter(function(t){return Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(e,t).enumerable})),n.push.apply(n,r)}return n}function jr(e){for(var t=1;t<arguments.length;t++){var n=null!=arguments[t]?arguments[t]:{};t%2?Vr(Object(n),!0).forEach(function(t){f()(e,t,n[t])}):Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors?Object.defineProperties(e,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors(n)):Vr(Object(n)).forEach(function(t){Object.defineProperty(e,t,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(n,t))})}return e}var Ur={position:"relative",color:Ut.COLORS.WHITE,fontFamily:Ut.TEXT_FONT_FAMILY.LATO,fontWeight:"bold",textAlign:"center",tiny:{fontSize:Ut.FONT_SIZE.EXTRA_TINY},big:{fontSize:Ut.FONT_SIZE.SMALL}},Fr={position:"relative",color:Ut.COLORS.WHITE,tiny:{height:"12px",width:"12px",marginBottom:"8px"},big:{height:"24px",width:"24px",marginBottom:"16px"}},Br=jr(jr({},gr),{},{width:"100%",height:"100%",position:"absolute",top:0,backdropFilter:"blur(3px)",webkitBackdropFilter:"blur(3px)",backgroundColor:Ut.COLORS.BLUR_TRANSPARENT}),Hr=function(e){var t="extraSmall"===e||"small"===e,n="medium"===e||"large"===e||"extraLarge"===e;return{errorMessageStyle:[Ur,t&&Ur.tiny,n&&Ur.big],iconStyle:[Fr,t&&Fr.tiny,n&&Fr.big],screenStyle:jr({},Br)}},qr=n(12),Wr=n.n(qr);function zr(){return(zr=Object.assign||function(e){for(var t=1;t<arguments.length;t++){var n=arguments[t];for(var r in n)Object.prototype.hasOwnProperty.call(n,r)&&(e[r]=n[r])}return e}).apply(this,arguments)}function Gr(){return(Gr=Object.assign||function(e){for(var t=1;t<arguments.length;t++){var n=arguments[t];for(var r in n)Object.prototype.hasOwnProperty.call(n,r)&&(e[r]=n[r])}return e}).apply(this,arguments)}function $r(){return($r=Object.assign||function(e){for(var t=1;t<arguments.length;t++){var n=arguments[t];for(var r in n)Object.prototype.hasOwnProperty.call(n,r)&&(e[r]=n[r])}return e}).apply(this,arguments)}var Kr={CLOSE_FILL:function(e){e.styles;var t=Wr()(e,["styles"]);return We.createElement("svg",Gr({xmlns:"http://www.w3.org/2000/svg",viewBox:"0 0 24 24",width:"24",height:"24"},t),We.createElement("path",{fill:"none",d:"M0 0h24v24H0z"}),We.createElement("path",{d:"M12 10.586l4.95-4.95 1.414 1.414-4.95 4.95 4.95 4.95-1.414 1.414-4.95-4.95-4.95 4.95-1.414-1.414 4.95-4.95-4.95-4.95L7.05 5.636z",fill:"rgba(255,255,255,1)"}))},ERROR_WARNING_FILL:function(e){e.styles;var t=Wr()(e,["styles"]);return We.createElement("svg",zr({xmlns:"http://www.w3.org/2000/svg",viewBox:"0 0 24 24",width:"24",height:"24"},t),We.createElement("path",{fill:"none",d:"M0 0h24v24H0z"}),We.createElement("path",{d:"M12 22C6.477 22 2 17.523 2 12S6.477 2 12 2s10 4.477 10 10-4.477 10-10 10zm-1-7v2h2v-2h-2zm0-8v6h2V7h-2z",fill:"rgba(255,255,255,1)"}))},PLAY_BLUR:function(e){e.styles;var t=Wr()(e,["styles"]);return We.createElement("svg",$r({xmlns:"http://www.w3.org/2000/svg",viewBox:"0 0 60 60"},t),We.createElement("circle",{cx:"30",cy:"30",r:"30",fillRule:"evenodd",clipRule:"evenodd",fillOpacity:".35"}),We.createElement("path",{fill:"none",d:"M11 11h38v38H11z"}),We.createElement("path",{d:"M43.3 30.7L22.1 44.8c-.3.2-.7.2-1 0-.3-.2-.5-.5-.5-.9V15.8c0-.4.2-.7.5-.9.3-.2.7-.2 1 0L43.3 29c.3.2.4.5.4.8s-.2.7-.4.9z",fill:"currentColor"}))}},Yr=n(31),Zr=n.n(Yr);function Xr(e,t){var n=Object.keys(e);if(Object.getOwnPropertySymbols){var r=Object.getOwnPropertySymbols(e);t&&(r=r.filter(function(t){return Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(e,t).enumerable})),n.push.apply(n,r)}return n}function Jr(e){for(var t=1;t<arguments.length;t++){var n=null!=arguments[t]?arguments[t]:{};t%2?Xr(Object(n),!0).forEach(function(t){f()(e,t,n[t])}):Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors?Object.defineProperties(e,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors(n)):Xr(Object(n)).forEach(function(t){Object.defineProperty(e,t,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(n,t))})}return e}var Qr=Ut.COLORS.WHITE,ei=function(e,t){var n=function(e){return{root:Jr(Jr({},cr),{},{alignItems:"center",width:"fit-content",height:"fit-content",outline:"none",color:e}),focused:{outline:"".concat(Ut.COLORS.WHITE," solid 1px")}}}(t||Qr);return{iconStyle:[n.root,e&&n.focused]}},ti=function(e){var t=e.icon,n=e.style,r=e.onClick,i=e.onMouseEnter,o=e.onMouseLeave,a=e.onKeyDown,s=e.id,u=e.color,c=e.clickable,l=void 0===c||c,d=e.focusable,p=void 0===d||d,f=te(!1),h=ir()(f,2),y=h[0],g=h[1],v=te(!1),m=ir()(v,2),O=m[0],_=m[1],S=kn(ei,[O,u]).iconStyle,E=l?{onClick:r,onMouseEnter:i,onMouseLeave:o,onFocus:function(e){y||_(!0),jn(i)||i(e)},onBlur:function(){document.hasFocus()&&g(!1),_(!1)},onKeyDown:function(e){jn(a)||a(e),"Enter"!==e.key||jn(r)||r()},onMouseDown:function(){g(!0),_(!1)}}:{},w=p?{tabIndex:0}:{};return b("span",Zr()({id:s,style:Jt([S,n])},E,w),b(t,null))},ni=jt(function(e){return{playerSize:hn.playerSize(e),playerErrorMessage:hn.errorMessage(e),mediaErrorMessage:Cn.mediaLoadingError(e),playerUniqId:hn.playerInstanceUniqId(e)}})(function(e){var t,n=e.playerSize,r=e.playerErrorMessage,i=e.mediaErrorMessage,o=e.playerUniqId,a=kn(Hr,[n]),s=a.errorMessageStyle,u=a.iconStyle,c=a.screenStyle,l=r||i;return Un(r)&&Un(i)?null:b("div",{id:(t=o,"".concat("mplayer","-").concat("error-page","-").concat(t)),style:c},b(ti,{icon:Kr.ERROR_WARNING_FILL,style:Jt(u)}),b("div",{id:wn(o),style:Jt(s)},l))}),ri=function(e,t){var n=function(e){return{height:"100%",left:0,display:"block",width:"100%",position:"relative",splitView:{width:"".concat(e/(9/16),"px")}}}(e);return{playerWrapperStyle:[n,t&&n.splitView]}},ii=jt(function(e){return{splitViewRatio:Zn(e)}})(function(e){var t=e.splitViewRatio,n=e.children,r=oe(),i=te(null),o=ir()(i,2),a=o[0],s=o[1],u=!Un(t),c=kn(ri,[a,u]).playerWrapperStyle;return Kn(function(){s(r.current.parentElement.clientHeight)},[r]),b("div",{ref:r,style:Jt(c)},n)});function oi(e,t){var n=Object.keys(e);if(Object.getOwnPropertySymbols){var r=Object.getOwnPropertySymbols(e);t&&(r=r.filter(function(t){return Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(e,t).enumerable})),n.push.apply(n,r)}return n}function ai(e){for(var t=1;t<arguments.length;t++){var n=null!=arguments[t]?arguments[t]:{};t%2?oi(Object(n),!0).forEach(function(t){f()(e,t,n[t])}):Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors?Object.defineProperties(e,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors(n)):oi(Object(n)).forEach(function(t){Object.defineProperty(e,t,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(n,t))})}return e}var si=ai(ai({},cr),{},{position:"absolute",width:"100%",height:"100%"}),ui={width:"100%",height:"100%",position:"relative"},ci={display:"flex",position:"relative",flexGrow:"1"},li={position:"absolute",zIndex:"1",width:"100%",top:"0",height:"100%",pointerEvents:"none"},di={position:"absolute",zIndex:"2",width:"100%",top:"0",height:"100%",pointerEvents:"none"},pi=function(){return{splitViewWrapperStyle:ai({},si),playerAndControlsWrapperStyle:ai({},ui),splitViewPlaceholderStyle:ai({},ci),controlsPlaceholderStyle:ai({},li),discoveryPlaceholderStyle:ai({},di)}};function fi(e,t){var n=Object.keys(e);if(Object.getOwnPropertySymbols){var r=Object.getOwnPropertySymbols(e);t&&(r=r.filter(function(t){return Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(e,t).enumerable})),n.push.apply(n,r)}return n}function hi(e){for(var t=1;t<arguments.length;t++){var n=null!=arguments[t]?arguments[t]:{};t%2?fi(Object(n),!0).forEach(function(t){f()(e,t,n[t])}):Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors?Object.defineProperties(e,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors(n)):fi(Object(n)).forEach(function(t){Object.defineProperty(e,t,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(n,t))})}return e}var yi={position:"absolute",overflow:"hidden",backgroundColor:Ut.COLORS.GRAY_TRANSPARENT,borderRadius:Ut.BORDER_RADIUS.SMALL,height:"6px",small:{width:"120px",height:"4px"},medium:{width:"200px"},largeAndExtraLarge:{width:"220px"},splitView:{width:"120px",height:"4px"},error:{display:"none"}},gi=hi(hi({},pr),{},{width:"100%",height:"100%",position:"absolute",top:"0",backgroundColor:Ut.COLORS.TRANSPARENT,blackScreen:{backgroundColor:Ut.COLORS.BLACK}}),vi=function(e,t,n,r,i){var o,a=!Un(n),s="small"===e,u="medium"===e,c="large"===e||"extraLarge"===e,l=i||Ut.COLORS.WHITE,d=(o=l,{position:"relative",width:"100%",height:"6px",borderRadius:Ut.BORDER_RADIUS.SMALL,animationName:"loaderAnimation",animationDuration:"3.5s",animationIterationCount:"infinite",animationTimingFunction:"ease-in-out",backgroundColor:o,small:{height:"4px"},splitView:{height:"4px"}});return{loaderStyle:[yi,r&&yi.error,s&&yi.small,u&&yi.medium,c&&yi.largeAndExtraLarge,a&&yi.splitView],barStyle:[d,s&&d.small,a&&d.splitView],screenStyle:[gi,t&&gi.blackScreen]}},mi=pn.monetization,bi={continuePlayingWhileWaitingForAd:function(e){return mi(e).continuePlayingWhileWaitingForAd},midrolls:function(e){return mi(e).midrolls},prerollEnabled:function(e){return mi(e).prerollEnabled},vpaidMode:function(e){return mi(e).vpaidMode},adRequestTimeout:function(e){return mi(e).adRequestTimeout},adsData:function(e){return mi(e).adsData}},Oi=bi.adsData,_i={adTagUrlTemplate:function(e){return Oi(e).adTagUrlTemplate},adType:function(e){return Oi(e).adType},adStatus:function(e){return Oi(e).adStatus},pendingAdStatus:function(e){return Oi(e).pendingAdStatus},adErrorMessage:function(e){return Oi(e).adErrorMessage},adDuration:function(e){return Oi(e).adDuration},playedMidrolls:function(e){return Oi(e).playedMidrolls},adImpression:function(e){return Oi(e).adImpression},adOpportunity:function(e){return Oi(e).adOpportunity},isVastAd:function(e){return Oi(e).isVastAd},adCurrentTime:function(e){return Oi(e).adCurrentTime},adMuted:function(e){return Oi(e).adMuted},adVolume:function(e){return Oi(e).adVolume},currentAdTag:function(e){return Oi(e).currentAdTag},adUnit:function(e){return Oi(e).adUnit},slotNumber:function(e){return Oi(e).slotNumber},podNumber:function(e){return Oi(e).podNumber},loadingAd:function(e){return Oi(e).loadingAd}},Si=pn.brandingData,Ei=function(e){return Si(e).brandingColor},wi=jt(function(e){return{showBlackScreen:hn.showBlackScreen(e),playerSize:hn.playerSize(e),splitViewRatio:Zn(e),playerUniqId:hn.playerInstanceUniqId(e),playerErrorMessage:hn.errorMessage(e),mediaErrorMessage:Cn.mediaLoadingError(e),loadingPlayer:hn.loadingPlayer(e),loadingAd:_i.loadingAd(e),brandingColor:Ei(e)}})(function(e){var t,n=e.playerSize,r=e.showBlackScreen,i=e.splitViewRatio,o=e.playerUniqId,a=e.playerErrorMessage,s=e.mediaErrorMessage,u=e.loadingPlayer,c=e.loadingAd,l=e.brandingColor,d=!Un(a)||!Un(s),p=u||c,f=kn(vi,[n,r,i,d,l]),h=f.loaderStyle,y=f.barStyle,g=f.screenStyle;return p?b("div",{id:(t=o,"".concat("mplayer","-").concat("loader-page","-").concat(t)),style:Jt(g)},b("div",{id:Pn(o),style:Jt(h)},b("div",{style:Jt(y)}))):null}),Pi=function(e){var t=e.playerId,n=e.store,r=e.playerPosition,i=kn(pi,[]),o=i.splitViewWrapperStyle,a=i.playerAndControlsWrapperStyle,s=i.splitViewPlaceholderStyle,u=i.controlsPlaceholderStyle,c=i.discoveryPlaceholderStyle;return re(function(){!function(e){var t=document.createElement("link");t.setAttribute("href","https://fonts.googleapis.com/css2?family=Lato:wght@300;400&display=swap"),t.setAttribute("rel","stylesheet"),t.setAttribute("type","text/css"),e.appendChild(t)}(r),function(e){var t=document.createElement("style");t.type="text/css",t.textContent="@keyframes loaderAnimation {0% {left: -100%}49% {left: 100%}50% {left: 100%}100% {left: -100%}}",e.appendChild(t)}(r)},[r]),b(Ze,{store:n},b(Nr,{id:mn(t)},b(Cr,{id:bn(t)},b("div",{id:Tn(t)}),b("div",{style:a},b("div",{style:o},b(ii,null,b(Jn,{playerId:t}),b(xr,{playerId:t}),b(wi,null),b(nr,null)),b("div",{id:_n(t),style:s})),b("div",{id:On(t),style:u}),b("div",{id:Sn(t),style:c}),b(ni,null)))))},Ti=n(1),Ai=n.n(Ti),Ci=function e(){var t=this;Ai()(this,e),f()(this,"HLS_MAIN_SCRIPT","https://cdn.jsdelivr.net/npm/hls.js@0.9.1"),f()(this,"HLS_FALLBACK_SCRIPT","https://www.oo-syringe.com/newplayer/mplayer_hls_wrapper.0.0.0.js"),f()(this,"IMA_SCRIPT","https://imasdk.googleapis.com/js/sdkloader/ima3.js"),f()(this,"MMPLUS_CONTAINER_ID","GTM-PL4PD49"),f()(this,"MM_CONTAINER_ID","GTM-TCW5HW"),f()(this,"dependenciesCallbacks",void 0),f()(this,"imaLoadingStatus",""),f()(this,"hlsLoadingStatus",""),f()(this,"gtmLoaded",!1),f()(this,"addDependenciesCallback",function(e){t.dependenciesCallbacks.includes(e)||t.dependenciesCallbacks.push(e)}),f()(this,"isDependenciesReady",function(){return t.gtmLoaded&&"loading"!==t.getHLSLoadingStatus()&&"loading"!==t.getIMALoadingStatus()}),f()(this,"getIMALoadingStatus",function(){return t.imaLoadingStatus}),f()(this,"getHLSLoadingStatus",function(){return t.hlsLoadingStatus}),f()(this,"removeDependenciesCallback",function(e){t.dependenciesCallbacks.filter(function(t){return t===e})}),f()(this,"loadScript",function(e,t,n){var r=document.createElement("script");r.setAttribute("src",e),r.addEventListener("load",t),r.addEventListener("error",n),document.body.appendChild(r)}),f()(this,"onScriptLoaded",function(e){t.dependenciesCallbacks.forEach(function(t){t.onDependencyReady(e)})}),f()(this,"onScriptFailed",function(e,n){t.dependenciesCallbacks.forEach(function(t){t.onDependencyFailure(e,n)})}),f()(this,"onHlsScriptLoaded",function(){void 0!==window.Hls?t.onScriptLoaded("hls"):t.onHlsLoadError()}),f()(this,"onHlsLoadError",function(){t.loadScript(t.HLS_FALLBACK_SCRIPT,t.onMMhlsScriptLoaded,t.onMMHlsScriptFailure)}),f()(this,"onMMhlsScriptLoaded",function(){t.onScriptLoaded("hls")}),f()(this,"onMMHlsScriptFailure",function(e){t.onScriptFailed("hls",e)}),f()(this,"onImaLoadError",function(e){t.imaLoadingStatus="error",t.onScriptFailed("ima",e)}),f()(this,"onImaLoaded",function(){t.onScriptLoaded("ima"),t.imaLoadingStatus="success"}),f()(this,"shouldLoadMmGTM",function(){var e=window.google_tag_manager;return!e||!e[t.MMPLUS_CONTAINER_ID]&&!e[t.MM_CONTAINER_ID]}),f()(this,"loadGTMifNeeded",function(){if(t.shouldLoadMmGTM()){var e,n;try{e=window.parent,n=window.parent.document}catch(u){e=window,n=window.document}var r=$n(e),i="dataLayer"!==r?"&l=".concat(r):"";e[r]=e[r]||[],e[r].push({"gtm.start":(new Date).getTime(),event:"gtm.js"});var o=n.getElementsByTagName("script")[0],a=n.createElement("script");a.setAttribute("async",""),a.setAttribute("src","https://www.googletagmanager.com/gtm.js?id=GTM-PL4PD49".concat(i)),o.parentNode.insertBefore(a,o);var s=n.createElement("iframe");s.src="https://www.googletagmanager.com/ns.html?id=GTM-PL4PD49",s.height="0",s.width="0",s.style="display:none;visibility:hidden",n.body.appendChild(s),t.gtmLoaded=!0}}),f()(this,"loadHLS",function(){Bn()||"safari"===Vn.name?t.hlsLoadingStatus="no-need":(t.loadScript(t.HLS_MAIN_SCRIPT,t.onHlsScriptLoaded,t.onHlsLoadError),t.hlsLoadingStatus="loading")}),f()(this,"loadIMA",function(){t.loadScript(t.IMA_SCRIPT,t.onImaLoaded,t.onImaLoadError),t.imaLoadingStatus="loading"}),f()(this,"loadExternalDependencies",function(){""===t.getIMALoadingStatus()&&t.loadIMA(),""===t.getHLSLoadingStatus()&&t.loadHLS(),t.gtmLoaded||t.loadGTMifNeeded()}),this.dependenciesCallbacks=[]};f()(Ci,"instance",void 0),f()(Ci,"getInstance",function(){return Ci.instance||(Ci.instance=new Ci),Ci.instance});var Ri=function(e){return"".concat("mplayer-monetization-ad-container","-").concat(e)},Di=function(e){return"".concat("mplayer-monetization-safari-video-ad","-").concat(e)},Mi=jt(function(e){return{contentVideoTime:hn.currentVideoTime(e),playerUniqId:hn.playerInstanceUniqId(e)}})(function(e){var t=e.contentVideoTime,n=e.playerUniqId,r=oe();return re(function(){Un(r.current)||(r.current.currentTime=t)},[t]),b("video",{id:Di(n),ref:r,style:{backgroundColor:"rgb(0, 0, 0)",width:"100%",height:"100%"},title:"Advertisement",preload:"auto",muted:!0,playsInline:!0,"webkit-playsinline":""})});function Ii(e,t){var n=Object.keys(e);if(Object.getOwnPropertySymbols){var r=Object.getOwnPropertySymbols(e);t&&(r=r.filter(function(t){return Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(e,t).enumerable})),n.push.apply(n,r)}return n}function ki(e){for(var t=1;t<arguments.length;t++){var n=null!=arguments[t]?arguments[t]:{};t%2?Ii(Object(n),!0).forEach(function(t){f()(e,t,n[t])}):Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors?Object.defineProperties(e,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors(n)):Ii(Object(n)).forEach(function(t){Object.defineProperty(e,t,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(n,t))})}return e}var Ni=jt(function(e,t){return ki(ki({},t),{},{adStatus:_i.adStatus(e)})})(function(e){var t=e.playerId,n=e.adStatus,r="playing"===n||"paused"===n;return re(function(){document.getElementById(vn(t)).style.display=r?"flex":"none"},[t,r]),b("div",{style:{width:"100%",height:"100%"}},b("div",{id:Ri(t),style:{height:"100%",width:"100%"}},Bn()&&b(Mi,null)))}),Li=function(e){var t=e.store,n=e.playerId;return b(Ze,{store:t},b(Ni,{playerId:n}))},xi=n(2),Vi=n.n(xi),ji=(n(46),function(){function e(t,n,r){Ai()(this,e),f()(this,"store",void 0),f()(this,"getDependenciesElements",void 0),f()(this,"oldState",void 0),f()(this,"unsubscribe",void 0),f()(this,"onChange",void 0),this.store=t,this.oldState=t.getState(),this.getDependenciesElements=n,this.unsubscribe=t.subscribe(this.onStateChanged.bind(this)),this.onChange=r}return Vi()(e,[{key:"stopSubscription",value:function(){this.unsubscribe(),this.oldState=null,this.getDependenciesElements=null,this.store=null,this.unsubscribe=null,this.onChange=null}},{key:"onStateChanged",value:function(){var e=this.store.getState(),t=this.getDependenciesElements(this.oldState),n=this.getDependenciesElements(e);t.every(function(e,t){return e===n[t]})||this.onChange(e),this.oldState=e}}]),e}()),Ui=pn.dependenciesLoadingStatus,Fi={loadingImaStatus:function(e){return Ui(e).loadingImaStatus},loadingHLSStatus:function(e){return Ui(e).loadingHLSStatus}},Bi=function(e){return"blocked"===Fi.loadingImaStatus(e)},Hi=function(e){return"playing"===e||"requested"===e||"loaded"===e||"paused"===e},qi=function(){function e(t,n){Ai()(this,e),f()(this,"videoTimeSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"adsScheduler",void 0),this.videoTimeSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getVideoTimeDependencies,this.onVideoTimeChanged.bind(this)),this.adsScheduler=n}return Vi()(e,[{key:"onVideoTimeChanged",value:function(t){"seeking"!==hn.videoTagStatus(t)&&(e.shouldRequestAd(t)?this.adsScheduler.onPreAdTimeReached(e.getMidroll(t)):e.isOnAdTime(t)&&this.adsScheduler.onAdTimeReached())}}],[{key:"getVideoTimeDependencies",value:function(e){return[hn.currentVideoTime(e)]}},{key:"isTimeInPreAdRange",value:function(e,t){return t>=e&&e>=t-$i}},{key:"getNextSpecificMidrollTime",value:function(t,n){return-1!==t.indexOf(n)?n:t.find(function(t){return e.isTimeInPreAdRange(n,t)})}},{key:"getNextReocurringMidrollNumber",value:function(e,t){var n=t/e;return t%e===0?n:Math.ceil(n)}},{key:"getMidroll",value:function(t){var n=hn.currentVideoTime(t),r=bi.midrolls(t),i=r.every,o=r.on,a=Dn.mediaId(t);if(!Un(i)){var s=e.getNextReocurringMidrollNumber(i,n);return{currentTime:n,mediaId:a,midrollNumber:s,midrollTime:s*i}}if(!Un(o)){var u=e.getNextSpecificMidrollTime(o,n);return{midrollNumber:o.indexOf(u)+1,currentTime:n,midrollTime:u,mediaId:a}}return null}},{key:"isMidrollAlreadyPlayed",value:function(e,t){return-1!==e.indexOf(t)}},{key:"isMidrollReached",value:function(t){var n=hn.currentVideoTime(t),r=bi.midrolls(t),i=r.every,o=r.on,a=_i.playedMidrolls(t);if(!Un(i)&&n>0){var s=e.getNextReocurringMidrollNumber(i,n),u=s*i;return!this.isMidrollAlreadyPlayed(a,s)&&e.isTimeInPreAdRange(n,u)}if(!Un(o)){var c=e.getNextSpecificMidrollTime(o,n),l=o.indexOf(c)+1;return l>0&&!this.isMidrollAlreadyPlayed(a,l)}return!1}},{key:"shouldRequestAd",value:function(e){var t=_i.adStatus(e);return!Bi(e)&&!Hi(t)&&this.isMidrollReached(e)}},{key:"isOnAdTime",value:function(t){var n=hn.currentVideoTime(t),r=bi.midrolls(t),i=r.every,o=r.on,a=_i.playedMidrolls(t);if(Bi(t))return!1;if(!Un(i)){var s=e.getNextReocurringMidrollNumber(i,n);return 0!==n&&n%i===0&&!this.isMidrollAlreadyPlayed(a,s)}if(!Un(o)){var u=o.indexOf(n);return-1!==u&&!this.isMidrollAlreadyPlayed(a,u)}return!1}}]),e}(),Wi=function(){function e(t){var n=this;Ai()(this,e),f()(this,"referrerUrl",void 0),f()(this,"staticAdTag",void 0),f()(this,"generate",function(e,t){var r=encodeURIComponent(n.referrerUrl);return Un(n.staticAdTag)?n.adTagFromApi(e,r,t):n.parseAdTag(n.staticAdTag,e,r,t)}),this.staticAdTag=t,this.referrerUrl=window.location.href}return Vi()(e,[{key:"parseAdTag",value:function(t,n,r,i){var o=t.replace("##AdUnit##",e.parseAdName(n)).replace("##DESCRIPTION_URL_UNESC##",r).replace("##REFERRER_URL_UNESC##",encodeURIComponent(this.referrerUrl)).replace("##CACHEBUSTER##",e.cacheBuster(n)).replace("##MIDROLL_ORDER##",e.adIndexFromName(n));return o=e.replaceVideoId(o,"##VIDEO_ID##",i),o=e.addHacksToAdTag(o)}},{key:"adTagFromApi",value:function(e,t,n){try{var r=window.getVideoTag(t,e);return Un(r)?null:this.parseAdTag(r,e,t,n)}catch(i){return null}}}],[{key:"getCCPAConsent",value:function(t){try{var n="";return window.__uspapi&&window.__uspapi("getUSPData",1,function(e,t){t&&(n=e.uspString)}),e.setSearchParamToAdTag(t,"us_privacy",n)}catch(r){return t}}},{key:"replaceVideoId",value:function(e,t,n){return e.replace(t,n).replace(encodeURIComponent(t),n).replace(encodeURIComponent(encodeURIComponent(t)),n)}},{key:"cacheBuster",value:function(t){return"".concat((new Date).getTime()).concat(e.adIndexFromName(t))}},{key:"parseAdName",value:function(t){return t.startsWith("preroll")?"PR":"MR".concat(e.adIndexFromName(t))}},{key:"adIndexFromName",value:function(e){return e.replace(/[^\d]*/g,"")}}]),e}();f()(Wi,"setSearchParamToAdTag",function(e,t,n){var r=new URL(e),i=decodeURIComponent(n);return r.searchParams.set(t,i),r.href}),f()(Wi,"getSearchParamFromAdTag",function(e,t){return new URL(e).searchParams.get(t)}),f()(Wi,"addHacksToAdTag",function(e){var t=e,n=Wi.getSearchParamFromAdTag(t,"cust_params");if(!jn(window.mmAPSbids)&&!Un(n)){var r="".concat(window.mmAPSbids,"&").concat(n);t=Wi.setSearchParamToAdTag(t,"cust_params",r)}if(!jn(window.shouldPlayAdRules)){var i=window.shouldPlayAdRules?"1":"0";t=Wi.setSearchParamToAdTag(t,"ad_rule",i)}return t=Wi.getCCPAConsent(t)});var zi=function(){function e(t,n){Ai()(this,e),f()(this,"store",void 0),f()(this,"videoTagStatusSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"adsScheduler",void 0),f()(this,"previousVideoTagStatus",void 0);var r=t.getState;this.store=t,this.adsScheduler=n,this.previousVideoTagStatus=hn.videoTagStatus(r()),this.videoTagStatusSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getVideoTagStatusDependencies,this.onVideoTagStatusChanged.bind(this))}return Vi()(e,[{key:"onVideoTagStatusChanged",value:function(t){var n=hn.videoTagStatus(t),r=_i.adStatus(t);"seeking"===this.previousVideoTagStatus&&(Hi(r)?this.adsScheduler.onSeekedWhileAdInProgress():e.isSeekedOverMidroll(t)&&this.adsScheduler.onSeekToAdOpportunity(e.getSeekedMidroll(t))),this.previousVideoTagStatus=n}}],[{key:"getVideoTagStatusDependencies",value:function(e){return[hn.videoTagStatus(e)]}},{key:"getClosestSkippedUnplayedMidroll",value:function(e,t){for(var n=t;n>0;n-=1)if(-1===e.indexOf(n))return n;return null}},{key:"getClosestLowerSeekedMidrollNumber",value:function(e,t){var n=In()(e).reverse().find(function(e){return e<=t});return e.indexOf(n)+1}},{key:"getSeekedSpecificMidroll",value:function(e,t,n,r){var i=this.getClosestLowerSeekedMidrollNumber(e,t),o=this.getClosestSkippedUnplayedMidroll(r,i);return{midrollNumber:o,currentTime:t,midrollTime:e[o-1],mediaId:n}}},{key:"isSeekedOverSpecificMidroll",value:function(e,t,n){if(jn(e))return!1;var r=this.getClosestLowerSeekedMidrollNumber(e,n);return null!==this.getClosestSkippedUnplayedMidroll(t,r)}},{key:"getSeekedReoccuringMidroll",value:function(e,t,n,r){var i=Math.floor(t/e),o=this.getClosestSkippedUnplayedMidroll(r,i);return{midrollTime:o*e,currentTime:t,midrollNumber:o,mediaId:n}}},{key:"isSeekedOverReoccuringMidroll",value:function(e,t,n){if(jn(e))return!1;var r=Math.floor(n/e);return null!==this.getClosestSkippedUnplayedMidroll(t,r)}},{key:"getSeekedMidroll",value:function(e){var t=_i.playedMidrolls(e),n=hn.currentVideoTime(e),r=bi.midrolls(e),i=r.every,o=r.on,a=Cn.mediaId(e);return this.isSeekedOverReoccuringMidroll(i,t,n)?this.getSeekedReoccuringMidroll(i,n,a,t):this.isSeekedOverSpecificMidroll(o,t,n)?this.getSeekedSpecificMidroll(o,n,a,t):null}},{key:"isSeekedOverMidroll",value:function(e){var t=_i.playedMidrolls(e),n=hn.currentVideoTime(e),r=bi.midrolls(e),i=r.every,o=r.on,a=o;return bi.prerollEnabled(e)&&(a=o.filter(function(e){return 0!==e})),this.isSeekedOverReoccuringMidroll(i,t,n)||this.isSeekedOverSpecificMidroll(a,t,n)}}]),e}(),Gi=function e(t,n){var r=this;Ai()(this,e),f()(this,"pendingAdStatusStoreSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"adsScheduler",void 0),f()(this,"onPendingAdStatusChanged",function(e){var t=_i.pendingAdStatus(e).type,n=_i.adStatus(e);if("playPreroll"===t&&!Hi(n)){var i=Cn.activeVideoIndex(e),o=Dn.mediaId(e);r.adsScheduler.onPrerollReached(o,i+1)}}),this.adsScheduler=n,this.pendingAdStatusStoreSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getPendingAdStatusDependencies,this.onPendingAdStatusChanged.bind(this))};f()(Gi,"getPendingAdStatusDependencies",function(e){return[_i.pendingAdStatus(e)]});var $i=3,Ki=function e(t,n){var r=this;Ai()(this,e),f()(this,"videoTimeSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"videoSeekSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"adTagGenerator",void 0),f()(this,"monetization",void 0),f()(this,"lastRequestedMidroll",null),f()(this,"prerollScheduler",void 0),f()(this,"generateMidrollTag",function(e){var t="midroll".concat(e.midrollNumber);return r.adTagGenerator.generate(t,e.mediaId)}),f()(this,"generatePrerollTag",function(e,t){var n="preroll".concat(t);return r.adTagGenerator.generate(n,e)}),f()(this,"onAdTimeReached",function(){r.monetization.onMidrollAdOpportunity()}),f()(this,"onPreAdTimeReached",function(e){r.onPreMidrollAdOpportunity(e)}),f()(this,"onSeekToAdOpportunity",function(e){r.onPreMidrollAdOpportunity(e)}),f()(this,"isMidrollAlreadyRequested",function(e){return e.midrollNumber===r.lastRequestedMidroll.midrollNumber&&e.mediaId===r.lastRequestedMidroll.mediaId&&e.midrollTime===r.lastRequestedMidroll.midrollTime}),f()(this,"onPreMidrollAdOpportunity",function(e){if(Un(r.lastRequestedMidroll)||!r.isMidrollAlreadyRequested(e)){r.lastRequestedMidroll=e;var t=r.generateMidrollTag(e);r.monetization.onPreMidrollAdOpportunity(e,t)}}),f()(this,"onPrerollReached",function(e,t){var n=r.generatePrerollTag(e,t);r.monetization.onPrerollAdOpportunity(n)}),f()(this,"onSeekedWhileAdInProgress",function(){r.monetization.onMidrollAdOpportunity()});var i=t.getState;this.monetization=n,this.videoTimeSubscriber=new qi(t,this),this.videoSeekSubscriber=new zi(t,this),this.prerollScheduler=new Gi(t,this);var o=_i.adTagUrlTemplate(i());this.adTagGenerator=new Wi(o)},Yi=function(){function e(){Ai()(this,e)}return Vi()(e,null,[{key:"generateAdRequest",value:function(e,t,n){var r=new google.ima.AdsRequest;return r.adTagUrl=e,Fn()||r.setAdWillPlayMuted(t),r.vastLoadTimeout=n,r}}]),e}(),Zi=function(e){return function(t){t({type:"[MONETIZATION] change ad status",payload:e})}},Xi=function(e){return function(t){t({type:"[COMMON] set pending video status",payload:{pendingStatusObject:{type:e,value:""}}})}},Ji=function(e){return function(t){t({type:"[MONETIZATION] change loading ad status",payload:e})}},Qi=function(e){return function(t){t({type:"[MONETIZATION] update ad muted",payload:e})}},eo=function(e){return function(t){t({type:"[MONETIZATION] change ad volume",payload:e})}},to=function e(t,n,r,i,o){var a=this;Ai()(this,e),f()(this,"store",void 0),f()(this,"IMAAdManager",void 0),f()(this,"adsLoader",void 0),f()(this,"adDisplayContainer",void 0),f()(this,"adVideoElement",void 0),f()(this,"videoPlayerElement",void 0),f()(this,"adContainerElement",void 0),f()(this,"anchorStatusStoreSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"totalAdRequestMadeAmount",0),f()(this,"registerForWindowResize",function(){var e=zn(a.onResize.bind(a),80);window.addEventListener("resize",e)}),f()(this,"initMutationObserver",function(e){new MutationObserver(a.onResize).observe(e,{attributes:!0,childList:!1,subtree:!1})}),f()(this,"loadNewAd",function(e,t){var n=a.store.dispatch;a.clearOldAdManagerIfExist();var r=a.createAdRequest(e);try{a.validateAdRequestCorrectness(r),a.adsLoader.requestAds(r),function(e,t){return function(n){n({type:"[MONETIZATION] change ad tag",payload:{adTag:e,adUnit:t}})}}(e,t)(n),Zi("requested")(n),a.totalAdRequestMadeAmount+=1}catch(i){a.onAdError(a.getLoadingError(r))}}),f()(this,"playAd",function(){try{a.IMAAdManager.init(a.videoPlayerElement.clientWidth,a.videoPlayerElement.clientHeight,google.ima.ViewMode.NORMAL);try{a.IMAAdManager.start()}catch(e){return!1}return!0}catch(t){return a.onAdError(a.getPlayAdError("ad manager start error")),!1}}),f()(this,"clearOldAdManagerIfExist",function(){Un(a.IMAAdManager)||(a.IMAAdManager.destroy(),a.IMAAdManager=null)}),f()(this,"createAdLoader",function(e,t){var n=bi.vpaidMode(e);a.adsLoader=new google.ima.AdsLoader(t),a.adsLoader.getSettings().setDisableCustomPlaybackForIOS10Plus(!0),a.adsLoader.getSettings().setVpaidMode(google.ima.ImaSdkSettings.VpaidMode[n]),a.adsLoader.addEventListener(google.ima.AdsManagerLoadedEvent.Type.ADS_MANAGER_LOADED,a.onIMAAdsManagerLoaded.bind(a),!1,a),a.adsLoader.addEventListener(google.ima.AdErrorEvent.Type.AD_ERROR,a.onAdError.bind(a),!1,a)}),f()(this,"createAdRequest",function(e){var t=a.store.getState,n=gn.muted(t()),r=bi.adRequestTimeout(t());return Yi.generateAdRequest(e,n,r)}),f()(this,"validateAdRequestCorrectness",function(e){e&&e.adTagUrl&&decodeURIComponent(e.adTagUrl.replace(/\+/g," "))}),f()(this,"getLoadingError",function(e){var t=function(){return"bad ad request ".concat(JSON.stringify(e))};return{getError:function(){return{getMessage:t}}}}),f()(this,"getPlayAdError",function(e){var t=function(){return"play ad error: ".concat(JSON.stringify(e))};return{getError:function(){return{getMessage:t}}}}),f()(this,"setAdVolume",function(e){var t=a.store,n=t.getState,r=t.dispatch,i=gn.volume(n());Bn()||gn.muted(n())?(e.setVolume(0),Qi(!0)(r)):(e.setVolume(gn.volume(n())),eo(i)(r),Qi(!1)(r))}),f()(this,"createIMAAdManager",function(t){a.IMAAdManager=t.getAdsManager(a.adVideoElement,e.getAdsRenderingSettings()),a.setAdVolume(a.IMAAdManager)}),f()(this,"registerToAdManagerEvents",function(){a.IMAAdManager.addEventListener(google.ima.AdErrorEvent.Type.AD_ERROR,a.onAdError),a.IMAAdManager.addEventListener(google.ima.AdEvent.Type.CONTENT_PAUSE_REQUESTED,a.onContentPauseRequested),a.IMAAdManager.addEventListener(google.ima.AdEvent.Type.CONTENT_RESUME_REQUESTED,a.onContentResumeRequested),a.IMAAdManager.addEventListener(google.ima.AdEvent.Type.STARTED,a.onAdStarted),a.IMAAdManager.addEventListener(google.ima.AdEvent.Type.IMPRESSION,a.onAdImpression),a.IMAAdManager.addEventListener(google.ima.AdEvent.Type.SKIPPED,a.onAdSkipped),a.IMAAdManager.addEventListener(google.ima.AdEvent.Type.COMPLETE,a.onAdCompleted),a.IMAAdManager.addEventListener(google.ima.AdEvent.Type.PAUSED,a.onAdPaused),a.IMAAdManager.addEventListener(google.ima.AdEvent.Type.RESUMED,a.onAdStarted),a.IMAAdManager.addEventListener(google.ima.AdEvent.Type.AD_PROGRESS,a.onAdProgressChanged),a.IMAAdManager.addEventListener(google.ima.AdEvent.Type.VOLUME_CHANGED,a.onVolumeChanged),a.IMAAdManager.addEventListener(google.ima.AdEvent.Type.VOLUME_MUTED,a.onAdVolumeMutedChanged),a.IMAAdManager.addEventListener(google.ima.AdEvent.Type.ALL_ADS_COMPLETED,a.onAdCompleted)}),f()(this,"onIMAAdsManagerLoaded",function(e){var t=a.store.dispatch;a.createIMAAdManager(e),a.registerToAdManagerEvents(),Zi("loaded")(t)}),f()(this,"onAdError",function(e){var t=a.store.dispatch;!function(e){return function(t){t({type:"[MONETIZATION] change ad error",payload:e})}}(e.getError().getMessage())(t),Ji(!1),a.continuePlayingContent()}),f()(this,"onAdImpression",function(e){var t=a.store.dispatch,n=!e.getAd().g.vpaid;a.setPodInfo(e),function(e){e({type:"[MONETIZATION] increase ad impression counter"})}(t),function(e){return function(t){t({type:"[MONETIZATION] update is vast ad",payload:e})}}(n)(t)}),f()(this,"onVolumeChanged",function(e){var t=a.store.dispatch;eo(e.target.getVolume())(t)}),f()(this,"onAdVolumeMutedChanged",function(e){var t=a.store.dispatch;0===e.target.getVolume()?Qi(!0)(t):Qi(!1)(t)}),f()(this,"continuePlayingContent",function(){var e=a.store,t=e.getState,n=e.dispatch,r=hn.videoTagStatus(t());Xi("idle"===r?"play":"resume")(n)}),f()(this,"stopPlayingContent",function(){var e=a.store.dispatch;Xi("pause")(e)}),f()(this,"onContentPauseRequested",function(){a.stopPlayingContent()}),f()(this,"onContentResumeRequested",function(){a.continuePlayingContent()}),f()(this,"onAdPaused",function(){var e=a.store.dispatch;Zi("paused")(e)}),f()(this,"setPodInfo",function(e){var t=e&&e.getAd()&&e.getAd().getAdPodInfo();if(!Un(t)){var n=a.store.dispatch;!function(e,t){return function(n){n({type:"[MONETIZATION] change pod info",payload:{slotNumber:e,podNumber:t}})}}(t.getAdPosition(),a.totalAdRequestMadeAmount)(n)}}),f()(this,"onAdStarted",function(){var e=a.store,t=e.dispatch,n=e.getState,r=gn.volume(n());Zi("playing")(t),0===a.IMAAdManager.getVolume()?a.IMAAdManager.setVolume(0):window.shouldPlayAdRule||a.IMAAdManager.setVolume(r),a.onResize()}),f()(this,"onAdCompleted",function(){var e=a.store.dispatch;Zi("completed")(e)}),f()(this,"onAdSkipped",function(){var e=a.store.dispatch;Zi("skipped")(e)}),f()(this,"onResize",function(){Un(a.IMAAdManager)||(a.IMAAdManager.resize(a.videoPlayerElement.clientWidth,a.videoPlayerElement.clientHeight,google.ima.ViewMode.NORMAL),a.adContainerElement.style.height="".concat(a.videoPlayerElement.clientHeight,"px"))}),f()(this,"onAdProgressChanged",function(e){var t,n,r=a.store,i=r.dispatch,o=r.getState,s=e.getAdData().currentTime,u=e.getAdData().duration,c=_i.adDuration(o());(t=s,function(e){e({type:"[MONETIZATION] change ad current time",payload:t})})(i),c!==u&&(n=u,function(e){e({type:"[MONETIZATION] change ad duration",payload:n})})(i)}),f()(this,"onAnchorStatusChanged",function(){var e=a.store.getState;"processing"!==Pr(e())&&a.onResize()}),f()(this,"changeAdVolume",function(e){Un(a.IMAAdManager)||a.IMAAdManager.setVolume(e)}),f()(this,"changeAdMuted",function(e,t){Un(a.IMAAdManager)||(t?a.IMAAdManager.setVolume(0):a.IMAAdManager.setVolume(e))}),f()(this,"changeAdStatus",function(e){Un(a.IMAAdManager)||("playing"===e&&a.IMAAdManager.resume(),"paused"===e&&a.IMAAdManager.pause())});var s=t.getState;this.store=t,this.adVideoElement=r,this.videoPlayerElement=i,this.adContainerElement=n,this.adDisplayContainer=new google.ima.AdDisplayContainer(n,r),this.createAdLoader(s(),this.adDisplayContainer),this.adDisplayContainer.initialize(),this.anchorStatusStoreSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getAnchorDependencies,this.onAnchorStatusChanged.bind(this)),this.registerForWindowResize(),this.initMutationObserver(o)};f()(to,"getAdsRenderingSettings",function(){var e=new google.ima.AdsRenderingSettings;return e.restoreCustomPlaybackStateOnAdBreakComplete=!0,e.enablePreloading=!1,e.uiElements=[],e.loadVideoTimeout=15e3,e}),f()(to,"getAnchorDependencies",function(e){return[Pr(e)]});var no=function e(t,n,r,i,o,a){var s=this;Ai()(this,e),f()(this,"store",void 0),f()(this,"playerId",void 0),f()(this,"adScheduler",void 0),f()(this,"adHandler",void 0),f()(this,"imaLoadingStatusSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"adStatusSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"videoTagStatusSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"adContainer",void 0),f()(this,"adVideoElement",void 0),f()(this,"videoPlayerElement",void 0),f()(this,"playerContainer",void 0),f()(this,"pendingMidrollAdPlay",!1),f()(this,"pendingPrerollAdPlay",!1),f()(this,"pendingPrerollAdTag",null),f()(this,"pendingMidrollNumber",null),f()(this,"pendingAdStatusStoreSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"adMutedStoreSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"adVolumeStoreSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"onMidrollAdOpportunity",function(){var e=s.store,t=e.dispatch,n=e.getState,r=_i.adStatus(n()),i=bi.continuePlayingWhileWaitingForAd(n());"loaded"===r?s.playAd(!0):"requested"===r&&(s.pendingMidrollAdPlay=!0,i||(Xi("pause")(t),Ji(!0)(t))),function(e){e({type:"[MONETIZATION] increase ad Opportunity counter"})}(t)}),f()(this,"onPrerollAdOpportunity",function(e){var t=s.store,n=t.getState,r=t.dispatch,i=Fi.loadingImaStatus(n());Un(s.adHandler)?"loading"!==i&&""!==i||(Ji(!0)(r),s.pendingPrerollAdPlay=!0,s.pendingPrerollAdTag=e):(s.pendingPrerollAdPlay=!0,Ji(!0)(r),s.adHandler.loadNewAd(e,"preroll"))}),f()(this,"onPreMidrollAdOpportunity",function(e,t){Un(s.adHandler)||(e.currentTime>=e.midrollTime&&(s.pendingMidrollAdPlay=!0),s.pendingMidrollNumber=e.midrollNumber,s.adHandler.loadNewAd(t,"midroll"))}),f()(this,"hasPendingAd",function(){return s.hasPendingMidrollAdPlay()||s.hasPendingPrerollAdPlay()}),f()(this,"onAdStatusChanged",function(e){var t=s.store.dispatch,n=_i.adStatus(e);"completed"===n&&Ji(!1)(t);var r=bi.continuePlayingWhileWaitingForAd(e),i=_i.loadingAd(e);"playing"!==n&&"error"!==n||r||!i||Ji(!1)(t),s.hasPendingAd()&&"loaded"===n?s.playAd(s.hasPendingMidrollAdPlay()):s.hasPendingAd()&&"error"===n?(Ji(!1),s.clearPendingMidroll(),s.clearPendingPreroll()):Hi(n)||(Ji(!1),function(e){e({type:"[MONETIZATION] clear ad data"})}(t))}),f()(this,"clearPendingMidroll",function(){s.pendingMidrollNumber=null,s.pendingMidrollAdPlay=!1}),f()(this,"clearPendingPreroll",function(){s.pendingPrerollAdPlay=!1,s.pendingPrerollAdTag=null}),f()(this,"onVideoTagStatusChanged",function(e){"complete"===hn.videoTagStatus(e)&&function(e){e({type:"[MONETIZATION] clear played midrolls"})}(s.store.dispatch)}),f()(this,"hasPendingMidrollAdPlay",function(){return s.pendingMidrollAdPlay}),f()(this,"hasPendingPrerollAdPlay",function(){return s.pendingPrerollAdPlay}),f()(this,"playAd",function(e){var t,n=s.store.dispatch,r=s.adHandler.playAd();e?((t=s.pendingMidrollNumber,function(e){e({type:"[MONETIZATION] add played midroll number",payload:t})})(n),s.clearPendingMidroll()):s.clearPendingPreroll(),r||(Ji(!1)(n),Xi("resume")(n))}),f()(this,"onIMALoadingStatusChanged",function(t){e.canUseIMA(t)&&(Un(s.adHandler)&&(s.adHandler=new to(s.store,s.adContainer,s.adVideoElement,s.videoPlayerElement,s.playerContainer)),s.hasPendingPrerollAdPlay()&&s.adHandler.loadNewAd(s.pendingPrerollAdTag,"preroll"))}),f()(this,"onPendingAdStatusChanged",function(){var e=s.store.getState,t=_i.pendingAdStatus(e()).type;switch(t){case"paused":case"playing":s.adHandler.changeAdStatus(t)}}),f()(this,"onAdMutedChanged",function(){var e=s.store.getState,t=_i.adMuted(e()),n=gn.volume(e());s.adHandler.changeAdMuted(n,t)}),f()(this,"onAdVolumeChanged",function(){var e=s.store.getState,t=_i.adVolume(e());s.adHandler.changeAdVolume(t)});var u=t.getState;this.store=t,this.playerId=n,this.adContainer=r,this.adVideoElement=i,this.videoPlayerElement=o,this.playerContainer=a,this.adScheduler=new Ki(t,this),this.adStatusSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getAdStatusDependencies,this.onAdStatusChanged.bind(this)),this.videoTagStatusSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getVideoTagStatusDependencies,this.onVideoTagStatusChanged.bind(this)),e.canUseIMA(u())?this.adHandler=new to(t,r,i,o,a):this.imaLoadingStatusSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getIMALoadingStatusDependencies,this.onIMALoadingStatusChanged.bind(this)),this.pendingAdStatusStoreSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getPendingAdStatusDependencies,this.onPendingAdStatusChanged.bind(this)),this.adMutedStoreSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getAdMutedDependencies,this.onAdMutedChanged.bind(this)),this.adVolumeStoreSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getAdVolumeDependencies,this.onAdVolumeChanged.bind(this))};f()(no,"getAdStatusDependencies",function(e){return[_i.adStatus(e)]}),f()(no,"getVideoTagStatusDependencies",function(e){return[hn.videoTagStatus(e)]}),f()(no,"getIMALoadingStatusDependencies",function(e){return[Fi.loadingImaStatus(e)]}),f()(no,"canUseIMA",function(e){return"success"===Fi.loadingImaStatus(e)}),f()(no,"getPendingAdStatusDependencies",function(e){return[_i.pendingAdStatus(e)]}),f()(no,"getAdMutedDependencies",function(e){return[_i.adMuted(e)]}),f()(no,"getAdVolumeDependencies",function(e){return[_i.adVolume(e)]});var ro=function(e,t){!function(e,t){var n=document.getElementById(vn(t));B(b(Li,{store:e,playerId:t}),n)}(e,t);var n=function(e){var t=Ri(e);return document.getElementById(t)}(t),r=function(e){var t=Bn()?Di(e):En(e);return document.getElementById(t)}(t),i=function(e){var t=En(e);return document.getElementById(t)}(t),o=function(e){var t=bn(e);return document.getElementById(t)}(t);return new no(e,t,n,r,i,o)},io=n(4),oo=n.n(io),ao=n(7),so=n.n(ao),uo=function(){function e(){Ai()(this,e),f()(this,"duration",void 0),f()(this,"position",void 0),f()(this,"previousPosition",void 0),f()(this,"loadTime",void 0),f()(this,"adOrder",void 0),f()(this,"adType",void 0),f()(this,"adDuration",void 0),f()(this,"errorMessage",void 0),f()(this,"adPodNumber",void 0),f()(this,"adSlotNumber",void 0)}return Vi()(e,[{key:"setDuration",value:function(e){return this.duration=e,this}},{key:"setPosition",value:function(e){return this.position=e,this}},{key:"setPreviousPosition",value:function(e){return this.previousPosition=e,this}},{key:"setLoadTime",value:function(e){return this.loadTime=e,this}},{key:"setAdOrder",value:function(e){return this.adOrder=e,this}},{key:"setAdType",value:function(e){return this.adType=e,this}},{key:"setAdDuration",value:function(e){return this.adDuration=e,this}},{key:"setErrorMessage",value:function(e){return this.errorMessage=e,this}},{key:"setAdPodNumber",value:function(e){return this.adPodNumber=e,this}},{key:"setAdSlotNumber",value:function(e){return this.adSlotNumber=e,this}},{key:"build",value:function(){var e=[];return jn(this.position)||e.push("video current position=".concat(Hn(this.position),"sec")),jn(this.duration)||e.push("video duration time=".concat(Hn(this.duration),"sec")),jn(this.loadTime)||e.push("video load time=".concat(this.loadTime,"milliseconds")),jn(this.previousPosition)||e.push("previous position=".concat(Hn(this.previousPosition),"sec")),jn(this.adOrder)||e.push("ad order=".concat(this.adOrder)),jn(this.adType)||e.push("ad type=".concat(this.adType)),jn(this.adDuration)||e.push("ad duration=".concat(Hn(Number(this.adDuration)),"sec")),jn(this.adPodNumber)||e.push("pod number=".concat(this.adPodNumber)),jn(this.adSlotNumber)||e.push("slot number=".concat(this.adSlotNumber)),jn(this.errorMessage)||e.push("error message=".concat(this.errorMessage)),e.join(";")}}]),e}(),co="mmPlus GTM data ready to GA",lo="mmPlus GTM event to GA",po={EMBED:"vplayer video player embed",FIRST_PLAY:"vplayer video first play",COMPLETION_25_PERCENTAGE:"vplayer video 25% complete",COMPLETION_50_PERCENTAGE:"vplayer video 50% complete",COMPLETION_75_PERCENTAGE:"vplayer video 75% complete",COMPLETION_90_PERCENTAGE:"vplayer video 90% complete",AD_BLOCK:"vplayer video ad block",AD_REQUEST:"vplayer video ad request",AD_IMPRESSION:"vplayer video ad impression",AD_ERROR:"vplayer video ad error",AD_VIEWABLE_IMPRESSION:"vplayer video ad viewable impression",AD_COMPLETE:"vplayer video ad complete",AD_SKIP:"vplayer video ad skip",AD_PAUSE:"vplayer video ad pause",VIDEO_COMPLETE:"vplayer video complete",FULLSCREEN_ON:"vplayer video fullscreen on",FULLSCREEN_OFF:"vplayer video fullscreen off",SEEK:"vplayer video position seeked",VIDEO_MUTE:"vplayer video mute",VIDEO_UNMUTE:"vplayer video unmute",CONTROLS_MUTE_OR_UNMUTE:"controls Mute/Unmute button click",CONTROLS_PLAY_OR_PAUSE:"controls Play/Pause button click",CONTROLS_FULLSCREEN:"controls Fullscreen button click",CONTROLS_NEXT_VIDEO:"controls Next Video button click",CONTROLS_SETTINGS:"controls Settings button click",MINI_PLAYER_CLOSE:"vplayer video miniplayer close",LOADING_CONTENT_ERROR:"vplayer video player error",PLAYER_ERROR:"vplayer general player error",CONTENT_VIEWABLE_IMPRESSION:"vplayer video content viewable impression"},fo="loading ad provider library failed",ho="mid",yo=function e(t,n){var r=this;Ai()(this,e),f()(this,"store",void 0),f()(this,"analyticsEventsCallbacks",void 0),f()(this,"videoMuteSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"videoFullscreenSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"onMuteStateChanged",function(e){var t=gn.muted(e),n=hn.currentVideoTimeFragment(e),i=(new uo).setPosition(n).build(),o=t?po.VIDEO_MUTE:po.VIDEO_UNMUTE;r.analyticsEventsCallbacks.onEvent(o,i)}),f()(this,"onFullsScreenStateChanged",function(e){var t=gn.fullscreen(e),n=hn.currentVideoTimeFragment(e),i=(new uo).setPosition(n).build(),o=t?po.FULLSCREEN_ON:po.FULLSCREEN_OFF;r.analyticsEventsCallbacks.onEvent(o,i)}),this.store=t,this.analyticsEventsCallbacks=n,this.videoMuteSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getVideoMuteDependencies,this.onMuteStateChanged.bind(this)),this.videoFullscreenSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getVideoFullscreenDependencies,this.onFullsScreenStateChanged.bind(this))};f()(yo,"getVideoMuteDependencies",function(e){return[gn.muted(e)]}),f()(yo,"getVideoFullscreenDependencies",function(e){return[gn.fullscreen(e)]});var go=n(3),vo=n.n(go),mo=n(8),bo=n.n(mo),Oo=n(9),_o=n.n(Oo),So=n(5),Eo=n.n(So);n(20);var wo={root:null,threshold:.5,rootMargin:"0px"},Po=function(){function e(t,n,r){Ai()(this,e),f()(this,"store",void 0),f()(this,"observableElement",void 0),f()(this,"callback",void 0),f()(this,"isViewableTimeoutHandler",null),f()(this,"observer",void 0),f()(this,"didReport",!1),this.store=t,this.observableElement=n,this.callback=r,this.observer=new IntersectionObserver(this.onIntersecting.bind(this),wo)}return Vi()(e,[{key:"unobserve",value:function(){this.observer.unobserve(this.observableElement),this.clearIsViewableTimeout()}},{key:"observe",value:function(){this.observer.observe(this.observableElement)}},{key:"onPlay",value:function(){this.didReport||this.observe()}},{key:"onPause",value:function(){this.unobserve()}},{key:"onComplete",value:function(){this.didReport=!1,this.unobserve()}},{key:"onIntersecting",value:function(e){var t=e[0].isIntersecting;null===this.isViewableTimeoutHandler&&t?this.isViewableTimeoutHandler=setTimeout(this.notifyViewable.bind(this),2e3):this.clearIsViewableTimeout()}},{key:"clearIsViewableTimeout",value:function(){clearTimeout(this.isViewableTimeoutHandler),this.isViewableTimeoutHandler=null}},{key:"notifyViewable",value:function(){var e=this.store.getState;this.didReport=!0,this.callback(e()),this.unobserve()}}]),e}();function To(e){var t=function(){if("undefined"===typeof Reflect||!Reflect.construct)return!1;if(Reflect.construct.sham)return!1;if("function"===typeof Proxy)return!0;try{return Date.prototype.toString.call(Reflect.construct(Date,[],function(){})),!0}catch(e){return!1}}();return function(){var n,r=Eo()(e);if(t){var i=Eo()(this).constructor;n=Reflect.construct(r,arguments,i)}else n=r.apply(this,arguments);return _o()(this,n)}}var Ao=function(e){bo()(n,e);var t=To(n);function n(e,r,i){var o;return Ai()(this,n),o=t.call(this,e,r,i),f()(vo()(o),"videoTagStatusSubscriber",void 0),f()(vo()(o),"onVideoTagStatusChanged",function(e){var t=hn.videoTagStatus(e);"playing"===t?o.onPlay():"paused"===t||"seeking"===t?o.onPause():"complete"!==t&&"error"!==t||o.onComplete()}),o.videoTagStatusSubscriber=new ji(e,n.getVideoTagStatusDependencies,o.onVideoTagStatusChanged.bind(vo()(o))),o}return n}(Po);f()(Ao,"getVideoTagStatusDependencies",function(e){return[hn.videoTagStatus(e)]});var Co=function e(t,n,r,i){var o=this;Ai()(this,e),f()(this,"store",void 0),f()(this,"videoTagStatusSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"analyticsEventsCallbacks",void 0),f()(this,"videoViewableImpressionObserver",void 0),f()(this,"firstPlayReportedIndex",-1),f()(this,"loadingStartTime",-1),f()(this,"registerVideoCallbacksIdNeeded",function(e,t){"none"!==hn.playbackMethod(e)&&document.getElementById(En(t)).addEventListener("loadstart",o.onLoadStart.bind(o),{once:!0})}),f()(this,"onLoadStart",function(){o.loadingStartTime=Date.now()}),f()(this,"getFirstPlayLabel",function(e){var t=hn.currentVideoTimeFragment(e),n=hn.currentVideoDuration(e),r=-1===o.loadingStartTime?-1:Date.now()-o.loadingStartTime;return(new uo).setPosition(t).setLoadTime(r).setDuration(n).build()}),f()(this,"onVideoTagStatusChanged",function(e){var t=hn.videoTagStatus(e),n=Cn.activeVideoIndex(e);if(!(n===o.firstPlayReportedIndex)&&"playing"===t){o.firstPlayReportedIndex=n;var r=o.getFirstPlayLabel(e);o.analyticsEventsCallbacks.onEvent(po.FIRST_PLAY,r)}}),f()(this,"reportVideoViewableImpression",function(e){var t=hn.currentVideoTimeFragment(e),n=(new uo).setPosition(t).build();o.analyticsEventsCallbacks.onEvent(po.CONTENT_VIEWABLE_IMPRESSION,n)}),this.store=t,this.analyticsEventsCallbacks=n,this.videoViewableImpressionObserver=new Ao(t,r,this.reportVideoViewableImpression.bind(this)),this.videoTagStatusSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getVideoTagDependencies,this.onVideoTagStatusChanged.bind(this)),this.registerVideoCallbacksIdNeeded(t.getState(),i)};f()(Co,"getVideoTagDependencies",function(e){return[hn.videoTagStatus(e)]});var Ro=[25,50,75,90],Do=function(){function e(){Ai()(this,e),f()(this,"lastReportedPercentage",void 0),this.lastReportedPercentage=0}return Vi()(e,[{key:"clear",value:function(){this.lastReportedPercentage=0}},{key:"updateConsumption",value:function(e,t){var n=e.position,r=e.duration,i=Math.round(n/r*100);i>this.lastReportedPercentage&&this.notifyReportableConsumption(e,i,t)}},{key:"notifyReportableConsumption",value:function(e,t,n){var r=this;Ro.filter(function(e){return e>r.lastReportedPercentage&&e<=t}).forEach(function(t){return n(t,e.position,e.duration)}),this.lastReportedPercentage=t}}]),e}(),Mo=function(){function e(t,n){var r=this;Ai()(this,e),f()(this,"store",void 0),f()(this,"analyticsEventsCallbacks",void 0),f()(this,"videoTimeSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"videoTagStatusSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"videoDataStoreSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"percentageConsumption",void 0),f()(this,"previousVideoTagStatus",void 0),f()(this,"lastPlayedPosition",void 0),f()(this,"getVideoPercentageAction",function(e){switch(e){case 25:return po.COMPLETION_25_PERCENTAGE;case 50:return po.COMPLETION_50_PERCENTAGE;case 75:return po.COMPLETION_75_PERCENTAGE;case 90:return po.COMPLETION_90_PERCENTAGE;default:return""}}),f()(this,"onVideoTimeChanged",function(e){var t=hn.currentVideoDuration(e),n=hn.currentVideoTimeFragment(e);r.percentageConsumption.updateConsumption({position:n,duration:t},r.onVideoTimeReport.bind(r))}),f()(this,"onVideoTagStatusChanged",function(e){var t=hn.videoTagStatus(e),n=hn.currentVideoTimeFragment(e),i=hn.currentVideoDuration(e);if("seeking"===r.previousVideoTagStatus){var o=(new uo).setPosition(n).setPreviousPosition(r.lastPlayedPosition).build();r.analyticsEventsCallbacks.onEvent(po.SEEK,o)}else"seeking"===t&&(r.lastPlayedPosition=n);r.previousVideoTagStatus=t,"complete"===t&&r.onComplete(i)}),f()(this,"onVideoDataChanged",function(){r.percentageConsumption.clear()}),f()(this,"onComplete",function(e){var t=(new uo).setDuration(e).build();r.analyticsEventsCallbacks.onEvent(po.VIDEO_COMPLETE,t),r.percentageConsumption.clear()}),this.store=t,this.analyticsEventsCallbacks=n,this.videoTimeSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getVideoTimeDependencies,this.onVideoTimeChanged.bind(this)),this.videoTagStatusSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getVideoTagStatusDependencies,this.onVideoTagStatusChanged.bind(this)),this.videoDataStoreSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getVideoDataDependencies,this.onVideoDataChanged.bind(this)),this.percentageConsumption=new Do}return Vi()(e,[{key:"onVideoTimeReport",value:function(e,t,n){var r=this.getVideoPercentageAction(e),i=(new uo).setPosition(t).setDuration(n).build();this.analyticsEventsCallbacks.onEvent(r,i)}}]),e}();f()(Mo,"getVideoTimeDependencies",function(e){return[hn.currentVideoTime(e)]}),f()(Mo,"getVideoTagStatusDependencies",function(e){return[hn.videoTagStatus(e)]}),f()(Mo,"getVideoDataDependencies",function(e){return[Cn.videoData(e)]});var Io=function e(t,n,r,i){Ai()(this,e),f()(this,"store",void 0),f()(this,"timeEvents",void 0),f()(this,"videoSettingsEvents",void 0),f()(this,"videoTagStatusEvents",void 0),this.store=t,this.timeEvents=new Mo(t,n),this.videoSettingsEvents=new yo(t,n),this.videoTagStatusEvents=new Co(t,n,r,i)},ko=n(13),No=n.n(ko),Lo=function(){function e(){Ai()(this,e),f()(this,"batteryLevel","N/A"),f()(this,"batteryChargingState",!1),f()(this,"connectionSpeed","N/A"),f()(this,"connectionType","N/A"),this.setConnectionParams()}return Vi()(e,[{key:"setConnectionParams",value:function(){var e=this,t=navigator.connection||navigator.mozConnection||navigator.webkitConnection;t&&(this.updateConnectionParams(t.effectiveType,t.downlink),t.addEventListener("change",function(){return e.updateConnectionParams(t.effectiveType,t.downlink)}))}},{key:"updateConnectionParams",value:function(e,t){this.connectionType=e,this.connectionSpeed=t}},{key:"attach",value:function(){var e=so()(oo.a.mark(function e(){var t,n=this;return oo.a.wrap(function(e){for(;;)switch(e.prev=e.next){case 0:if(!navigator.getBattery){e.next=7;break}return e.next=3,navigator.getBattery();case 3:t=e.sent,this.updateBatteryParams(t.level,t.charging),t.ondischargingtimechange=function(e){return n.updateBatteryParams(e.target.level,e.target.charging)},t.onchargingtimechange=function(e){return n.updateBatteryParams(e.target.level,e.target.charging)};case 7:case"end":return e.stop()}},e,this)}));return function(){return e.apply(this,arguments)}}()},{key:"updateBatteryParams",value:function(e,t){this.batteryLevel="".concat(100*e),this.batteryChargingState=t}},{key:"getBatteryLevel",value:function(){return this.batteryLevel}},{key:"getBatteryChargingState",value:function(){return this.batteryChargingState}},{key:"getConnectionSpeed",value:function(){return this.connectionSpeed}},{key:"getConnectionType",value:function(){return this.connectionType}}]),e}(),xo=function(){"undefined"===typeof window["$$mm-analytics"]&&(window["$$mm-analytics"]={});var e=window;return"undefined"===typeof e["$$mm-analytics"].firstEmbed&&(e["$$mm-analytics"].firstEmbed=Date.now()),e["$$mm-analytics"].firstEmbed},Vo=function(){return"time in process=".concat(Date.now()-xo()," milliseconds")},jo=function(){function e(t,n){var r=this;Ai()(this,e),f()(this,"videoTag",null),f()(this,"deviceParams",void 0),f()(this,"trafficDomain",void 0),f()(this,"adTimeoutDuration",void 0),f()(this,"playbackMethod",void 0),f()(this,"configName",void 0),f()(this,"playerUniqKey",void 0),f()(this,"attach",so()(oo.a.mark(function e(){return oo.a.wrap(function(e){for(;;)switch(e.prev=e.next){case 0:return r.deviceParams=new Lo,e.next=3,r.deviceParams.attach();case 3:case"end":return e.stop()}},e)}))),f()(this,"translatePlaybackMethod",function(e){switch(e){case"lazyplay":return"scroll to play";case"autoplay":return"auto play";case"autoplay_when_viewable":return"auto play when viewable";case"none":return"click to play";default:return"none"}}),f()(this,"getAdTimeout",function(e){return e?"".concat(qn(e)," sec"):"none"}),f()(this,"getVideoTag",function(){return null===r.videoTag&&(r.videoTag=document.getElementById(En(r.playerUniqKey))),r.videoTag}),f()(this,"getUnitSize",function(){var e=r.getVideoTag();return null!==e?"".concat(e.clientWidth,":").concat(e.clientHeight):"none"}),f()(this,"getBatteryCharging",function(){return r.deviceParams.getBatteryChargingState()?"yes":"no"});var i=t.publisher_contribution,o=t.monetization,a=t.playback_method,s=t.config_name;this.playerUniqKey=n,this.trafficDomain=i,this.adTimeoutDuration=o&&o.ad_request_timeout?this.getAdTimeout(o.ad_request_timeout):"none",this.playbackMethod=this.translatePlaybackMethod(a),this.configName=s}return Vi()(e,[{key:"getDimensions",value:function(){return{mmPlusConfigName:this.configName,mmPlusPlaybackMethod:this.playbackMethod,mmPlusPrePvTimeInProcess:Vo(),mmPlusTrafficDomain:this.trafficDomain,mmPlusUnitSize:this.getUnitSize(),mmPlusUnitType:"video",mmPlusUserBatteryCharging:this.getBatteryCharging(),mmPlusUserBatteryLevel:this.deviceParams.getBatteryLevel(),mmPlusUserConnectionSpeed:this.deviceParams.getConnectionSpeed(),mmPlusUserConnectionType:this.deviceParams.getConnectionType(),mmPlusAdTimeoutDuration:this.adTimeoutDuration}}}]),e}();function Uo(e,t){var n=Object.keys(e);if(Object.getOwnPropertySymbols){var r=Object.getOwnPropertySymbols(e);t&&(r=r.filter(function(t){return Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(e,t).enumerable})),n.push.apply(n,r)}return n}function Fo(e){for(var t=1;t<arguments.length;t++){var n=null!=arguments[t]?arguments[t]:{};t%2?Uo(Object(n),!0).forEach(function(t){f()(e,t,n[t])}):Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors?Object.defineProperties(e,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors(n)):Uo(Object(n)).forEach(function(t){Object.defineProperty(e,t,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(n,t))})}return e}function Bo(e){var t=function(){if("undefined"===typeof Reflect||!Reflect.construct)return!1;if(Reflect.construct.sham)return!1;if("function"===typeof Proxy)return!0;try{return Date.prototype.toString.call(Reflect.construct(Date,[],function(){})),!0}catch(e){return!1}}();return function(){var n,r=Eo()(e);if(t){var i=Eo()(this).constructor;n=Reflect.construct(r,arguments,i)}else n=r.apply(this,arguments);return _o()(this,n)}}var Ho=function(e){bo()(n,e);var t=Bo(n);function n(e,r){var i,o;return Ai()(this,n),o=t.call(this,e,r),f()(vo()(o),"domain",void 0),f()(vo()(o),"getDimensions",function(){return Fo(Fo({},No()((i=vo()(o),Eo()(n.prototype)),"getDimensions",i).call(i)),{},{mmPlusDomain:o.domain})}),o.domain=window.location.hostname,o}return n}(jo);function qo(e,t){var n=Object.keys(e);if(Object.getOwnPropertySymbols){var r=Object.getOwnPropertySymbols(e);t&&(r=r.filter(function(t){return Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(e,t).enumerable})),n.push.apply(n,r)}return n}function Wo(e){for(var t=1;t<arguments.length;t++){var n=null!=arguments[t]?arguments[t]:{};t%2?qo(Object(n),!0).forEach(function(t){f()(e,t,n[t])}):Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors?Object.defineProperties(e,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors(n)):qo(Object(n)).forEach(function(t){Object.defineProperty(e,t,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(n,t))})}return e}function zo(e){var t=function(){if("undefined"===typeof Reflect||!Reflect.construct)return!1;if(Reflect.construct.sham)return!1;if("function"===typeof Proxy)return!0;try{return Date.prototype.toString.call(Reflect.construct(Date,[],function(){})),!0}catch(e){return!1}}();return function(){var n,r=Eo()(e);if(t){var i=Eo()(this).constructor;n=Reflect.construct(r,arguments,i)}else n=r.apply(this,arguments);return _o()(this,n)}}var Go=function(e){bo()(n,e);var t=zo(n);function n(e,r,i){var o;Ai()(this,n),o=t.call(this,i,r),f()(vo()(o),"store",void 0),f()(vo()(o),"playerUniqId",void 0),f()(vo()(o),"autoPlay",void 0),f()(vo()(o),"streamType",void 0),f()(vo()(o),"gaProperty",void 0),f()(vo()(o),"booleanToYesOrNo",function(e){return e?"yes":"no"}),f()(vo()(o),"getAspectRatio",function(e,t){var r=n.getCommonDenominator(e,t),i=t/r;return"".concat(e/r,":").concat(i)}),f()(vo()(o),"getUnitRadio",function(){var e=o.getVideoTag();if(null!==e){var t=e.clientWidth,n=e.clientHeight;return o.getAspectRatio(t,n)}return"none"}),f()(vo()(o),"getVideoDuration",function(e){var t=Dn.duration(e);return t?"".concat(t," sec"):"none"});var a=i.playback_method,s=i.content_type,u=i.ga_property;return o.store=e,o.playerUniqId=r,o.autoPlay=o.booleanToYesOrNo("none"!==a),o.streamType=s,o.gaProperty=u||o.trafficDomain,o}return Vi()(n,[{key:"getDimensions",value:function(){var e=this.store.getState;return Wo(Wo({},No()(Eo()(n.prototype),"getDimensions",this).call(this)),{},{mmPlusVideoPlayerId:this.playerUniqId,mmPlusUnitId:Dn.mediaId(e())||"none",mmPlusUnitTitle:Dn.title(e())||"none",mmPlusVideoAutoplay:this.autoPlay,mmPlusVideoDuration:this.getVideoDuration(e()),mmPlusVideoStreamType:this.streamType,mmPlusVideoPlaylistId:Cn.mediaId(e())||"none",mmPlusVideoMuted:this.booleanToYesOrNo(gn.muted(e())),mmPlusUnitTags:Dn.tags(e()).toString()||"none",mmPlusUnitOrderNumber:Cn.activeVideoIndex(e()),mmPlusTrafficReferrer:document.referrer,mmPlusUnitProvider:Dn.provider(e())||"none",mmPlusUnitCreator:Dn.creator(e())||"none",mmPlusUnitExternalId:Dn.externalId(e())||"none",mmPlusUnitRatio:this.getUnitRadio(),mmPlusGaProperty:this.gaProperty,mmPlusVideoPlayerType:"vplayer",mmPlusVideoThumbnailsOn:"none",mmPlusVideoClosedCaptionsOn:"none",mmPlusVideoRecommendationsVisible:"none"})}}]),n}(jo);function $o(e){var t=function(){if("undefined"===typeof Reflect||!Reflect.construct)return!1;if(Reflect.construct.sham)return!1;if("function"===typeof Proxy)return!0;try{return Date.prototype.toString.call(Reflect.construct(Date,[],function(){})),!0}catch(e){return!1}}();return function(){var n,r=Eo()(e);if(t){var i=Eo()(this).constructor;n=Reflect.construct(r,arguments,i)}else n=r.apply(this,arguments);return _o()(this,n)}}f()(Go,"getCommonDenominator",function(e,t){for(var n=e,r=t;r;){var i=r;r=n%r,n=i}return n});var Ko=function(e){bo()(n,e);var t=$o(n);function n(e,r,i){var o;return Ai()(this,n),o=t.call(this,e,r,i),f()(vo()(o),"adStatusSubscriber",void 0),f()(vo()(o),"onAdStatusChanged",function(e){var t=_i.adStatus(e);"playing"===t?o.onPlay():"paused"===t?o.onPause():Hi(t)||o.onComplete()}),o.adStatusSubscriber=new ji(e,n.getAdStatusDependencies,o.onAdStatusChanged.bind(vo()(o))),o}return n}(Po);f()(Ko,"getAdStatusDependencies",function(e){return[_i.adStatus(e)]});var Yo=function e(t,n,r){var i=this;Ai()(this,e),f()(this,"store",void 0),f()(this,"analyticsCallback",void 0),f()(this,"adStatusSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"adImpressionSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"adProviderLoadingStatusSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"adViewableImpressionObserver",void 0),f()(this,"initAdBlockChecker",function(t){""===Fi.loadingImaStatus(t.getState())?i.adProviderLoadingStatusSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getAdProviderLoadingStatusDependencies,i.onAdProviderLoadingStatusChanged.bind(i)):i.onAdProviderLoadingStatusChanged(t.getState())}),f()(this,"getAdOrderFromPlayedMidrolls",function(e){return e&&e.length>0?e[e.length-1]:0}),f()(this,"getFullAdLabel",function(e){var t=hn.currentVideoTimeFragment(e),n=_i.adDuration(e),r=_i.playedMidrolls(e),o=_i.slotNumber(e),a=_i.podNumber(e),s=i.getAdOrderFromPlayedMidrolls(r);return(new uo).setAdType(ho).setPosition(t).setAdDuration(n).setAdOrder(s).setAdSlotNumber(o).setAdPodNumber(a).build()}),f()(this,"reportAdBlock",function(){i.analyticsCallback.onEvent(po.AD_BLOCK)}),f()(this,"reportError",function(e){var t=(new uo).setErrorMessage(e).build();i.analyticsCallback.onEvent(po.PLAYER_ERROR,t)}),f()(this,"reportAdRequested",function(e){var t=hn.currentVideoTimeFragment(e),n=_i.playedMidrolls(e),r=i.getAdOrderFromPlayedMidrolls(n),o=(new uo).setPosition(t).setAdOrder(r).build();i.analyticsCallback.onEvent(po.AD_REQUEST,o)}),f()(this,"reportAdImpression",function(e){var t=i.getFullAdLabel(e);i.analyticsCallback.onEvent(po.AD_IMPRESSION,t)}),f()(this,"reportAdError",function(e){var t=_i.adErrorMessage(e),n=_i.slotNumber(e),r=_i.podNumber(e),o=(new uo).setAdType(ho).setErrorMessage(t).setAdPodNumber(r).setAdSlotNumber(n).build();i.analyticsCallback.onEvent(po.AD_ERROR,o)}),f()(this,"reportAdComplete",function(e){var t=i.getFullAdLabel(e);i.analyticsCallback.onEvent(po.AD_COMPLETE,t)}),f()(this,"reportAdSkipped",function(e){var t=i.getFullAdLabel(e);i.analyticsCallback.onEvent(po.AD_SKIP,t)}),f()(this,"reportAdPaused",function(e){var t=i.getFullAdLabel(e);i.analyticsCallback.onEvent(po.AD_PAUSE,t)}),f()(this,"reportAdViewableImpression",function(e){var t=hn.currentVideoTimeFragment(e),n=_i.adDuration(e),r=_i.slotNumber(e),o=_i.podNumber(e),a=(new uo).setPosition(t).setAdType(ho).setAdDuration(n).setAdPodNumber(o).setAdSlotNumber(r).build();i.analyticsCallback.onEvent(po.AD_VIEWABLE_IMPRESSION,a)}),f()(this,"onAdStatusChanged",function(e){var t=_i.adStatus(e);"requested"===t&&i.reportAdRequested(e),"error"===t&&i.reportAdError(e),"completed"===t&&i.reportAdComplete(e),"skipped"===t&&i.reportAdSkipped(e),"paused"===t&&i.reportAdPaused(e)}),f()(this,"onAdProviderLoadingStatusChanged",function(e){var t=Fi.loadingImaStatus(e);"blocked"===t?i.reportAdBlock():"error"===t&&i.reportError(fo)}),f()(this,"onAdImpressionChanged",function(e){i.reportAdImpression(e)}),this.store=t,this.analyticsCallback=n,this.adStatusSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getAdStatusDependencies,this.onAdStatusChanged.bind(this)),this.adImpressionSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getAdImpressionDependencies,this.onAdImpressionChanged.bind(this)),this.adViewableImpressionObserver=new Ko(t,r,this.reportAdViewableImpression.bind(this)),this.initAdBlockChecker(t)};f()(Yo,"getAdProviderLoadingStatusDependencies",function(e){return[Fi.loadingImaStatus(e)]}),f()(Yo,"getAdStatusDependencies",function(e){return[_i.adStatus(e)]}),f()(Yo,"getAdImpressionDependencies",function(e){return[_i.adImpression(e)]});var Zo=pn.userInteraction,Xo=function(e){return Zo(e).userInteractionType},Jo=function e(t,n){var r=this;Ai()(this,e),f()(this,"store",void 0),f()(this,"analyticsCallbacks",void 0),f()(this,"userInteraction",void 0),f()(this,"getLabel",function(e){var t=hn.currentVideoTimeFragment(e);return(new uo).setPosition(t).build()}),f()(this,"reportMuteOrUnmute",function(e){r.analyticsCallbacks.onEvent(po.CONTROLS_MUTE_OR_UNMUTE,r.getLabel(e))}),f()(this,"reportFullscreen",function(e){r.analyticsCallbacks.onEvent(po.CONTROLS_FULLSCREEN,r.getLabel(e))}),f()(this,"reportPlayOrPause",function(e){r.analyticsCallbacks.onEvent(po.CONTROLS_PLAY_OR_PAUSE,r.getLabel(e))}),f()(this,"reportNextVideo",function(e){r.analyticsCallbacks.onEvent(po.CONTROLS_NEXT_VIDEO,r.getLabel(e))}),f()(this,"reportSettings",function(e){r.analyticsCallbacks.onEvent(po.CONTROLS_SETTINGS,r.getLabel(e))}),f()(this,"reportMiniPlayerClosed",function(e){r.analyticsCallbacks.onEvent(po.MINI_PLAYER_CLOSE,r.getLabel(e))}),f()(this,"onUserInteractionChanged",function(e){switch(Xo(e)){case"mute":case"unmute":r.reportMuteOrUnmute(e);break;case"fullscreen":r.reportFullscreen(e);break;case"play":case"pause":r.reportPlayOrPause(e);break;case"settings":r.reportSettings(e);break;case"nextVideo":r.reportNextVideo(e);break;case"closeMiniPlayer":r.reportMiniPlayerClosed(e)}}),this.store=t,this.analyticsCallbacks=n,this.userInteraction=new ji(t,e.getUserInteractionDependencies,this.onUserInteractionChanged.bind(this))};function Qo(e,t){var n=Object.keys(e);if(Object.getOwnPropertySymbols){var r=Object.getOwnPropertySymbols(e);t&&(r=r.filter(function(t){return Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(e,t).enumerable})),n.push.apply(n,r)}return n}function ea(e){for(var t=1;t<arguments.length;t++){var n=null!=arguments[t]?arguments[t]:{};t%2?Qo(Object(n),!0).forEach(function(t){f()(e,t,n[t])}):Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors?Object.defineProperties(e,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors(n)):Qo(Object(n)).forEach(function(t){Object.defineProperty(e,t,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(n,t))})}return e}f()(Jo,"getUserInteractionDependencies",function(e){return[Xo(e)]});var ta=function e(t,n,r,i){var o=this;Ai()(this,e),f()(this,"store",void 0),f()(this,"videoEventsReporter",void 0),f()(this,"monetizationEventsReporter",void 0),f()(this,"userInteractionReporter",void 0),f()(this,"eventCustomDimensions",void 0),f()(this,"dataLayer",null),f()(this,"attach",function(){var e=so()(oo.a.mark(function e(t,n,r){return oo.a.wrap(function(e){for(;;)switch(e.prev=e.next){case 0:return o.eventCustomDimensions=new Go(t,n,r),e.next=3,o.eventCustomDimensions.attach();case 3:return e.next=5,o.reportReady(t,n,r);case 5:o.reportEmbed();case 6:case"end":return e.stop()}},e)}));return function(t,n,r){return e.apply(this,arguments)}}()),f()(this,"initDataLayer",function(){var e;try{e=$n(window.parent)}catch(t){e=$n(window)}o.dataLayer=window[e]}),f()(this,"reportReady",function(){var e=so()(oo.a.mark(function e(t,n,r){var i;return oo.a.wrap(function(e){for(;;)switch(e.prev=e.next){case 0:return i=new Ho(r,n),e.next=3,i.attach();case 3:o.dataLayer.push(ea({event:co},i.getDimensions()));case 4:case"end":return e.stop()}},e)}));return function(t,n,r){return e.apply(this,arguments)}}()),f()(this,"reportEmbed",function(){o.onEvent(po.EMBED)}),f()(this,"onEvent",function(e,t,n,r){var i=r||o.eventCustomDimensions.getDimensions(),a=n||lo;o.dataLayer.push(ea(ea({event:a,mmPlusGA_event_action:e,mmPlusGA_event_label:t,mmPlusGA_event_category:"MMVideo"},i),{},{time:Date.now()}))}),this.store=t,this.initDataLayer(),this.videoEventsReporter=new Io(t,this,i,n),this.monetizationEventsReporter=new Yo(t,this,i),window.monetizationEventsReporter=this.monetizationEventsReporter,this.userInteractionReporter=new Jo(t,this),this.attach(t,n,r)},na=pn.discovery,ra=function(e){return na(e).nextVideo},ia=n(51).version,oa=function e(){var t=this;Ai()(this,e),f()(this,"controlsLoaded",void 0),f()(this,"loadInternalPlugins",function(e,n,r){var i=r.dev_config,o=e.getState,a=i&&!Un(i.pluginsPath)?i.pluginsPath:function(){try{return"https://www.oo-syringe.com/prod/players"}catch(w){return"./dist/bundle"}}();t.loadAnalyticsPlugin(e,n,r),t.loadActionsPlugin(n,a),t.loadMonetizationPlugin(e,n),t.loadControlsPlugin(n,a),t.loadAnchorPlugin(n,a),t.loadComscorePlugin(n,r,a),Sr(o())&&Un(Zn(o()))&&t.loadCloseBarPlugin(n,a),"none"!==ra(o())&&t.loadDiscoveryPlugin(n,a),Un(Zn(o()))||t.loadSplitViewPlugin(n,a)}),f()(this,"loadActionsPlugin",function(e,t){if(window.monti.dataset&&window.monti.dataset.plugins.actionsPlugin)window.monti.dataset.plugins.actionsPlugin(e);else{var n=document.createElement("script");n.setAttribute("type","text/javascript"),n.setAttribute("src","".concat(t,"/mplayer_actions_plugin.").concat(ia,".js")),n.addEventListener("load",function(){window.monti.dataset.plugins.actionsPlugin(e)}),n.addEventListener("error",function(){console.log("ACTIONS PLUGIN LOADER ERROR !")}),document.body.appendChild(n)}}),f()(this,"loadMonetizationPlugin",function(e,t){ro(e,t)}),f()(this,"loadControlsPlugin",function(e,t){if(window.monti.dataset&&window.monti.dataset.plugins.controlsPlugin)window.monti.dataset.plugins.controlsPlugin(e);else{var n=document.createElement("script");n.setAttribute("src","".concat(t,"/mplayer_controls_plugin.").concat(ia,".js")),n.setAttribute("type","text/javascript"),n.addEventListener("load",function(){window.monti.dataset.plugins.controlsPlugin(e)}),n.addEventListener("error",function(){console.log("CONTROLS PLUGIN LOADER ERROR !")}),document.body.appendChild(n)}}),f()(this,"loadAnchorPlugin",function(e,t){if(window.monti.dataset&&window.monti.dataset.plugins.anchorPlugin)window.monti.dataset.plugins.anchorPlugin(e);else{var n=document.createElement("script");n.setAttribute("src","".concat(t,"/mplayer_anchor_plugin.").concat(ia,".js")),n.setAttribute("type","text/javascript"),n.addEventListener("load",function(){window.monti.dataset.plugins.anchorPlugin(e)}),n.addEventListener("error",function(){console.log("ANCHOR PLUGIN LOADER ERROR !")}),document.body.appendChild(n)}}),f()(this,"loadSplitViewPlugin",function(e,t){if(window.monti.dataset&&window.monti.dataset.plugins.splitViewPlugin)window.monti.dataset.plugins.splitViewPlugin(e);else{var n=document.createElement("script");n.setAttribute("src","".concat(t,"/mplayer_split_view_plugin.").concat(ia,".js")),n.setAttribute("type","text/javascript"),n.addEventListener("load",function(){window.monti.dataset.plugins.splitViewPlugin(e)}),n.addEventListener("error",function(){console.log("SPLIT VIEW PLUGIN LOADER ERROR !")}),document.body.appendChild(n)}}),f()(this,"loadComscorePlugin",function(e,t,n){if(window.monti.dataset&&window.monti.dataset.plugins.comscorePlugin)window.monti.dataset.plugins.comscorePlugin(e,t);else{var r=document.createElement("script");r.setAttribute("src","".concat(n,"/mplayer_comscore_plugin.").concat(ia,".js")),r.setAttribute("type","text/javascript"),r.addEventListener("load",function(){window.monti.dataset.plugins.comscorePlugin(e,t)}),r.addEventListener("error",function(){console.log("COMSCORE PLUGIN LOADER ERROR !")}),document.body.appendChild(r)}}),f()(this,"loadAnalyticsPlugin",function(e,t,n){!function(e,t,n){var r=document.getElementById(bn(t));new ta(e,t,n,r)}(e,t,n)}),f()(this,"loadDiscoveryPlugin",function(e,t){if(window.monti.dataset&&window.monti.dataset.plugins.discoveryPlugin)window.monti.dataset.plugins.discoveryPlugin(e);else{var n=document.createElement("script");n.setAttribute("src","".concat(t,"/mplayer_discovery_plugin.").concat(ia,".js")),n.setAttribute("type","text/javascript"),n.addEventListener("load",function(){window.monti.dataset.plugins.discoveryPlugin(e)}),n.addEventListener("error",function(){console.log("DISCOVERY PLUGIN LOADER ERROR !")}),document.body.appendChild(n)}}),f()(this,"loadCloseBarPlugin",function(e,t){if(window.monti.dataset&&window.monti.dataset.plugins.closeBar)window.monti.dataset.plugins.closeBarPlugin(e);else{var n=document.createElement("script");n.setAttribute("src","".concat(t,"/mplayer_close_bar_plugin.").concat(ia,".js")),n.setAttribute("type","text/javascript"),n.addEventListener("load",function(){window.monti.dataset.plugins.closeBarPlugin(e)}),n.addEventListener("error",function(){console.log("CLOSE BAR PLUGIN LOADER ERROR !")}),document.body.appendChild(n)}}),this.controlsLoaded=!1};function aa(e){return function(t){var n=t.dispatch,r=t.getState;return function(t){return function(i){return"function"===typeof i?i(n,r,e):t(i)}}}}f()(oa,"instance",void 0),f()(oa,"getInstance",function(){return oa.instance||(oa.instance=new oa),oa.instance});var sa=aa();sa.withExtraArgument=aa;var ua=sa;function ca(e,t){var n=Object.keys(e);if(Object.getOwnPropertySymbols){var r=Object.getOwnPropertySymbols(e);t&&(r=r.filter(function(t){return Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(e,t).enumerable})),n.push.apply(n,r)}return n}function la(e){for(var t=1;t<arguments.length;t++){var n=null!=arguments[t]?arguments[t]:{};t%2?ca(Object(n),!0).forEach(function(t){f()(e,t,n[t])}):Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors?Object.defineProperties(e,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors(n)):ca(Object(n)).forEach(function(t){Object.defineProperty(e,t,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(n,t))})}return e}var da={loadingHLSStatus:"",loadingImaStatus:""};function pa(e,t){var n=Object.keys(e);if(Object.getOwnPropertySymbols){var r=Object.getOwnPropertySymbols(e);t&&(r=r.filter(function(t){return Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(e,t).enumerable})),n.push.apply(n,r)}return n}function fa(e){for(var t=1;t<arguments.length;t++){var n=null!=arguments[t]?arguments[t]:{};t%2?pa(Object(n),!0).forEach(function(t){f()(e,t,n[t])}):Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors?Object.defineProperties(e,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors(n)):pa(Object(n)).forEach(function(t){Object.defineProperty(e,t,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(n,t))})}return e}var ha={playerId:"",playerInstanceUniqId:null,playbackMethod:"none",videoTagStatus:"idle",pendingVideoTagStatus:{type:null,value:null},currentVideoTime:0,currentVideoTimeFragment:0,currentVideoBufferedTime:0,currentVideoDuration:0,playerMode:"desktop",playerVisibility:0,playerPlaceholderVisibility:0,playerSettings:{muted:!0,volume:.5,speed:1,quality:"Auto",selectedSettingsCategory:"",fullscreen:{pendingFullscreenRequest:"",isFullscreenOn:!1}},loader:{showBlackScreen:!1},playerSize:"",errorMessage:"",loadingPlayer:!1};function ya(e,t){var n=Object.keys(e);if(Object.getOwnPropertySymbols){var r=Object.getOwnPropertySymbols(e);t&&(r=r.filter(function(t){return Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(e,t).enumerable})),n.push.apply(n,r)}return n}function ga(e){for(var t=1;t<arguments.length;t++){var n=null!=arguments[t]?arguments[t]:{};t%2?ya(Object(n),!0).forEach(function(t){f()(e,t,n[t])}):Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors?Object.defineProperties(e,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors(n)):ya(Object(n)).forEach(function(t){Object.defineProperty(e,t,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(n,t))})}return e}var va={showVoltaxLogo:!0,brandingLogoSrc:"",brandingLogoUrl:"",brandingColor:null};function ma(e,t){var n=Object.keys(e);if(Object.getOwnPropertySymbols){var r=Object.getOwnPropertySymbols(e);t&&(r=r.filter(function(t){return Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(e,t).enumerable})),n.push.apply(n,r)}return n}function ba(e){for(var t=1;t<arguments.length;t++){var n=null!=arguments[t]?arguments[t]:{};t%2?ma(Object(n),!0).forEach(function(t){f()(e,t,n[t])}):Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors?Object.defineProperties(e,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors(n)):ma(Object(n)).forEach(function(t){Object.defineProperty(e,t,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(n,t))})}return e}var Oa={orientation:"right",canClose:!0,anchorData:{anchorEnabled:!1,anchorDisabledByUser:!1,anchorStatus:"inactive"},closableAd:!0,continueStreaming:!1,anchoringAppearance:"below",stickyBelowClassName:"",width:"",closeAfter:10,margins:{top:10,bottom:10,left:10,right:10}};function _a(e,t){var n=Object.keys(e);if(Object.getOwnPropertySymbols){var r=Object.getOwnPropertySymbols(e);t&&(r=r.filter(function(t){return Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(e,t).enumerable})),n.push.apply(n,r)}return n}function Sa(e){for(var t=1;t<arguments.length;t++){var n=null!=arguments[t]?arguments[t]:{};t%2?_a(Object(n),!0).forEach(function(t){f()(e,t,n[t])}):Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors?Object.defineProperties(e,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors(n)):_a(Object(n)).forEach(function(t){Object.defineProperty(e,t,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(n,t))})}return e}var Ea,wa={continuePlayingWhileWaitingForAd:!1,midrolls:{},prerollEnabled:!1,vpaidMode:"ENABLED",adRequestTimeout:1e4,adsData:{podNumber:0,slotNumber:0,adType:null,adTagUrlTemplate:null,adErrorMessage:null,adDuration:0,adCurrentTime:0,adOrder:0,adStatus:"",pendingAdStatus:{type:""},playedMidrolls:[],adImpression:0,adOpportunity:0,isVastAd:!1,adMuted:!0,adVolume:.5,currentAdTag:null,adUnit:"",loadingAd:!1}},Pa=n(32),Ta=n.n(Pa);!function(e){e.POST="post",e.GET="get",e.DELETE="delete",e.PUT="put",e.PATCH="patch",e.HEAD="head",e.OPTIONS="options"}(Ea||(Ea={}));var Aa=function(){function e(){Ai()(this,e)}return Vi()(e,null,[{key:"preformRequest",value:function(e,t){return new Promise(function(n,r){Ta()({method:e,url:t}).then(function(e){var t=e.data;n(t)}).catch(function(e){r(e)})})}},{key:"preformGet",value:function(e){return this.preformRequest(Ea.GET,e)}}]),e}();f()(Aa,"HTTP_STATUS_CODES",{OK:200,UNAUTHORIZED:401,INTERNAL_SERVER_ERROR:500,BAD_REQUEST:400,NOT_FOUND:404,CONFLICT:409});var Ca=function(){function e(){Ai()(this,e)}return Vi()(e,null,[{key:"getSemanticPlaylistUrl",value:function(e,t,n){if(n&&!Un(n.semantic_url))return n.semantic_url;var r=function(e){for(var t="",n=0,r=0;r<e.length;r+=1)n=e.charCodeAt(r)+2,t+=String.fromCharCode(n);return encodeURIComponent(encodeURIComponent(t))}(e.split("?")[0]),i="".concat(function(){try{return"https://semantic-matching.minutemediaservices.com/semantic-feed"}catch(w){return"https://semantic-matching.minutemediaservices.com/semantic-feed"}}(),"/").concat(r),o="?";return Un(t.minimumDateFactor)||(i="".concat(i).concat(o,"minimum_date_factor=").concat(t.minimumDateFactor),o="&"),Un(t.tags)||(i="".concat(i).concat(o,"tags=").concat(t.tags),o="&"),Un(t.scopedKeywords)||(i="".concat(i).concat(o,"scoped_keywords=").concat(t.scopedKeywords),o="&"),Un(t.scannedElement)||Un(t.scannedElementType)||(i="".concat(i).concat(o,"article_container=").concat(function(e,t){switch(t){case"id":return"".concat("#").concat(e);case"className":return e.split(" ").map(function(e){return"".concat(".").concat(e)}).join("");default:return"".concat("").concat(e)}}(t.scannedElement,t.scannedElementType)),o="&"),Un(t.scanImagesOnPage)||(i="".concat(i).concat(o,"scan_images=").concat(String(t.scanImagesOnPage))),i}},{key:"getSpecificPlaylistUrl",value:function(e,t){return t&&!Un(t.playlist_url)?t.playlist_url:"".concat("https://vms-videos.minutemediaservices.com","/").concat(e,"/").concat(e,".json")}}]),e}(),Ra=function(e,t){var n=e.findIndex(function(e){return e.type===t});return n>=0?e[n].file:""},Da=function(){function e(){Ai()(this,e)}return Vi()(e,null,[{key:"getSemanticPlaylist",value:function(e,t){var n=window.location.href,r=Ca.getSemanticPlaylistUrl(n,e,t);return Aa.preformGet(r)}},{key:"getSpecificPlaylist",value:function(e,t){var n=Ca.getSpecificPlaylistUrl(e,t);return Aa.preformGet(n)}}]),e}();f()(Da,"getPromotedVideos",function(){var e=so()(oo.a.mark(function e(t){var n;return oo.a.wrap(function(e){for(;;)switch(e.prev=e.next){case 0:return n=t.map(function(e){return Da.getSpecificPlaylist(e).then(function(e){return e.data}).catch(function(){return[]})}),e.abrupt("return",new Promise(function(e){Promise.all(n).then(function(t){var n=t.reduce(function(e,t){return e.concat(t)},[]);n.length>0&&e(n)})}));case 2:case"end":return e.stop()}},e)}));return function(t){return e.apply(this,arguments)}}());var Ma=function(e,t){return function(n){n({type:"[CORE] load video request success",payload:e.data.map(function(e,n){return{mediaId:e.payload_id,tags:e.tags,sources:e.sources,duration:e.duration,thumbnail:e.image,title:t?e.title:"",description:e.description,creator:e.creator,provider:e.video_provider,externalId:e.external_id,index:n}})})}},Ia=function(e){return function(t){t({type:"[CORE] load video request error",payload:e})}},ka=function(e,t,n){return function(){var r=so()(oo.a.mark(function r(i){var o;return oo.a.wrap(function(r){for(;;)switch(r.prev=r.next){case 0:return i({type:"[CORE] load video request"}),r.prev=1,r.next=4,Da.getSpecificPlaylist(e,n);case 4:o=r.sent,Ma(o,t)(i),r.next=11;break;case 8:r.prev=8,r.t0=r.catch(1),Ia(Xn.GENERAL_ERROR)(i);case 11:case"end":return r.stop()}},r,null,[[1,8]])}));return function(e){return r.apply(this,arguments)}}()},Na=function(e,t,n){return function(){var r=so()(oo.a.mark(function r(i){var o,a;return oo.a.wrap(function(r){for(;;)switch(r.prev=r.next){case 0:if(i({type:"[CORE] load video request"}),o=[],r.prev=2,!(Array.isArray(e.promotedVideos)&&e.promotedVideos.length>0)){r.next=7;break}return r.next=6,Da.getPromotedVideos(e.promotedVideos);case 6:o=r.sent;case 7:return r.next=9,Da.getSemanticPlaylist(e,n);case 9:(a=r.sent).data=o.concat(a.data),Ma(a,t)(i),r.next=17;break;case 14:r.prev=14,r.t0=r.catch(2),o.length>0?Ma({data:o},t)(i):Ia(Xn.GENERAL_ERROR)(i);case 17:case"end":return r.stop()}},r,null,[[2,14]])}));return function(e){return r.apply(this,arguments)}}()},La=function(e,t){return function(n){n({type:"[CORE] set current video",payload:{index:e,videoData:t}})}};function xa(e,t){var n=Object.keys(e);if(Object.getOwnPropertySymbols){var r=Object.getOwnPropertySymbols(e);t&&(r=r.filter(function(t){return Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(e,t).enumerable})),n.push.apply(n,r)}return n}function Va(e){for(var t=1;t<arguments.length;t++){var n=null!=arguments[t]?arguments[t]:{};t%2?xa(Object(n),!0).forEach(function(t){f()(e,t,n[t])}):Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors?Object.defineProperties(e,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors(n)):xa(Object(n)).forEach(function(t){Object.defineProperty(e,t,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(n,t))})}return e}var ja={activeVideoIndex:0,mediaType:"semantic",mediaId:"",loadingMedia:!1,mediaLoadingError:"",mediaRequest:{type:null,value:""},videoList:[],videoData:{mediaId:"",tags:[],sources:[],duration:0,thumbnail:"",title:"",showTitle:!0,description:"",creator:"",provider:"",externalId:"",index:0}};function Ua(e,t){var n=Object.keys(e);if(Object.getOwnPropertySymbols){var r=Object.getOwnPropertySymbols(e);t&&(r=r.filter(function(t){return Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(e,t).enumerable})),n.push.apply(n,r)}return n}function Fa(e){for(var t=1;t<arguments.length;t++){var n=null!=arguments[t]?arguments[t]:{};t%2?Ua(Object(n),!0).forEach(function(t){f()(e,t,n[t])}):Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors?Object.defineProperties(e,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors(n)):Ua(Object(n)).forEach(function(t){Object.defineProperty(e,t,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(n,t))})}return e}var Ba={promotedVideos:[],scannedElement:"",tags:"",scopedKeywords:"",minimumDateFactor:"",scannedElementType:null,scanImagesOnPage:!1};function Ha(e,t){var n=Object.keys(e);if(Object.getOwnPropertySymbols){var r=Object.getOwnPropertySymbols(e);t&&(r=r.filter(function(t){return Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(e,t).enumerable})),n.push.apply(n,r)}return n}function qa(e){for(var t=1;t<arguments.length;t++){var n=null!=arguments[t]?arguments[t]:{};t%2?Ha(Object(n),!0).forEach(function(t){f()(e,t,n[t])}):Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors?Object.defineProperties(e,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors(n)):Ha(Object(n)).forEach(function(t){Object.defineProperty(e,t,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(n,t))})}return e}var Wa={userInteractionType:""};function za(e,t){var n=Object.keys(e);if(Object.getOwnPropertySymbols){var r=Object.getOwnPropertySymbols(e);t&&(r=r.filter(function(t){return Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(e,t).enumerable})),n.push.apply(n,r)}return n}function Ga(e){for(var t=1;t<arguments.length;t++){var n=null!=arguments[t]?arguments[t]:{};t%2?za(Object(n),!0).forEach(function(t){f()(e,t,n[t])}):Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors?Object.defineProperties(e,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors(n)):za(Object(n)).forEach(function(t){Object.defineProperty(e,t,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(n,t))})}return e}var $a={splitViewRatio:null};function Ka(e,t){var n=Object.keys(e);if(Object.getOwnPropertySymbols){var r=Object.getOwnPropertySymbols(e);t&&(r=r.filter(function(t){return Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(e,t).enumerable})),n.push.apply(n,r)}return n}function Ya(e){for(var t=1;t<arguments.length;t++){var n=null!=arguments[t]?arguments[t]:{};t%2?Ka(Object(n),!0).forEach(function(t){f()(e,t,n[t])}):Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors?Object.defineProperties(e,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors(n)):Ka(Object(n)).forEach(function(t){Object.defineProperty(e,t,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(n,t))})}return e}var Za={nextVideo:"none",showUpNext:!1,showSkippableContent:!1},Xa=function(e){return"none"===e?"none":"up_next"===e?"upNext":"skippable_content"===e?"skippableContent":"none"},Ja=function(e){for(var t=Object.keys(e),n={},r=0;r<t.length;r++){var i=t[r];"function"===typeof e[i]&&(n[i]=e[i])}var o,a=Object.keys(n);try{!function(e){Object.keys(e).forEach(function(t){var n=e[t];if("undefined"===typeof n(void 0,{type:yt.INIT}))throw new Error('Reducer "'+t+"\" returned undefined during initialization. If the state passed to the reducer is undefined, you must explicitly return the initial state. The initial state may not be undefined. If you don't want to set a value for this reducer, you can use null instead of undefined.");if("undefined"===typeof n(void 0,{type:yt.PROBE_UNKNOWN_ACTION()}))throw new Error('Reducer "'+t+"\" returned undefined when probed with a random type. Don't try to handle "+yt.INIT+' or other actions in "redux/*" namespace. They are considered private. Instead, you must return the current state for any unknown actions, unless it is undefined, in which case you must return the initial state, regardless of the action type. The initial state may not be undefined, but can be null.')})}(n)}catch(s){o=s}return function(e,t){if(void 0===e&&(e={}),o)throw o;for(var r=!1,i={},s=0;s<a.length;s++){var u=a[s],c=n[u],l=e[u],d=c(l,t);if("undefined"===typeof d){var p=mt(u,t);throw new Error(p)}i[u]=d,r=r||d!==l}return(r=r||a.length!==Object.keys(e).length)?i:e}}({dependenciesLoadingStatus:function(){var e=arguments.length>0&&void 0!==arguments[0]?arguments[0]:da,t=arguments.length>1?arguments[1]:void 0;switch(t.type){case"[CORE] update hls status":return la(la({},e),{},{loadingHLSStatus:t.payload});case"[CORE] update ima status":return la(la({},e),{},{loadingImaStatus:t.payload});default:return e}},playerData:function(){var e=arguments.length>0&&void 0!==arguments[0]?arguments[0]:ha,t=arguments.length>1?arguments[1]:void 0;switch(t.type){case"[CORE] initiate store":var n=t.payload;return fa({},function(e,t,n){var r=t.playback_method,i=t.player_id;return fa(fa({},e),{},{playbackMethod:Un(r)?e.playbackMethod:r,playerId:Un(i)?e.playerId:i,playerInstanceUniqId:n,playerMode:Fn()?"mobile":"desktop"})}(e,n.initiateParams,n.playerInstanceUniqId));case"[CORE] reset player data time params":return fa(fa({},e),{},{currentVideoTimeFragment:0,currentVideoBufferedTime:0,currentVideoDuration:0,currentVideoTime:0});case"[COMMON] set mute video":return fa(fa({},e),{},{playerSettings:fa(fa({},e.playerSettings),{},{muted:t.payload})});case"[COMMON] set volume":return fa(fa({},e),{},{playerSettings:fa(fa({},e.playerSettings),{},{volume:t.payload})});case"[COMMON] change selected settings category":return fa(fa({},e),{},{playerSettings:fa(fa({},e.playerSettings),{},{selectedSettingsCategory:t.payload})});case"[COMMON] change settings speed":return fa(fa({},e),{},{playerSettings:fa(fa({},e.playerSettings),{},{speed:t.payload})});case"[COMMON] change settings quality":return fa(fa({},e),{},{playerSettings:fa(fa({},e.playerSettings),{},{quality:t.payload})});case"[COMMON] set fullscreen":return fa(fa({},e),{},{playerSettings:fa(fa({},e.playerSettings),{},{fullscreen:fa(fa({},e.playerSettings.fullscreen),{},{isFullscreenOn:t.payload,pendingFullscreenRequest:""})})});case"[COMMON] set fullscreen request":return fa(fa({},e),{},{playerSettings:fa(fa({},e.playerSettings),{},{fullscreen:fa(fa({},e.playerSettings.fullscreen),{},{pendingFullscreenRequest:t.payload})})});case"[COMMON] set pending video status":var r=t.payload.pendingStatusObject;return fa(fa({},e),{},{pendingVideoTagStatus:fa({},r)});case"[COMMON] set player mode":return fa(fa({},e),{},{playerMode:t.payload});case"[CORE] update video current fragment position":return fa(fa({},e),{},{currentVideoTimeFragment:t.payload});case"[CORE] update video current position":return fa(fa({},e),{},{currentVideoTime:t.payload});case"[CORE] update video current buffered time":return fa(fa({},e),{},{currentVideoBufferedTime:t.payload});case"[CORE] update video current duration":return fa(fa({},e),{},{currentVideoDuration:t.payload});case"[CORE] change video tag status":return fa(fa({},e),{},{videoTagStatus:t.payload});case"[CORE] update player visibility":return fa(fa({},e),{},{playerVisibility:t.payload});case"[CORE] update placeholder visibility":return fa(fa({},e),{},{playerPlaceholderVisibility:t.payload});case"[CORE] change loading player status":return fa(fa({},e),{},{loadingPlayer:t.payload});case"[COMMON] show black screen with loader":return fa(fa({},e),{},{loader:fa(fa({},e.loader),{},{showBlackScreen:t.payload})});case"[CORE] set player size":return fa(fa({},e),{},{playerSize:t.payload});case"[COMMON] set error message":return fa(fa({},e),{},{errorMessage:t.payload});default:return e}},brandingData:function(){var e=arguments.length>0&&void 0!==arguments[0]?arguments[0]:va,t=arguments.length>1?arguments[1]:void 0;switch(t.type){case"[CORE] initiate store":return ga({},function(e,t){var n=t.powered_by_strip,r=t.brand_logo,i=t.brand_logo_click_url,o=t.brand_color;return ga(ga({},e),{},{showVoltaxLogo:Un(n)?e.showVoltaxLogo:n,brandingLogoSrc:Un(r)?e.brandingLogoSrc:r,brandingLogoUrl:Un(i)?e.brandingLogoUrl:i,brandingColor:Un(o)?e.brandingColor:o})}(e,t.payload.initiateParams));default:return e}},anchorOptions:function(){var e=arguments.length>0&&void 0!==arguments[0]?arguments[0]:Oa,t=arguments.length>1?arguments[1]:void 0;switch(t.type){case"[CORE] initiate store":return ba({},function(e,t){var n=t.anchor_options;if(!Un(n)){var r=n.anchoring_appearance,i=n.can_close,o=n.closable_ad,a=n.close_after,s=n.continue_streaming,u=n.orientation,c=n.margins,l=n.sticky_below_class_name,d=n.width,p=Un(c)?e.margins:{top:Number.isInteger(c.top)?c.top:e.margins.top,bottom:Number.isInteger(c.bottom)?c.bottom:e.margins.bottom,left:Number.isInteger(c.left)?c.left:e.margins.left,right:Number.isInteger(c.right)?c.right:e.margins.right};return ba(ba({},e),{},{anchoringAppearance:r||e.anchoringAppearance,canClose:Un(i)?e.canClose:i,orientation:Un(u)?e.orientation:u,closableAd:Un(o)?e.closableAd:o,closeAfter:Un(a)?e.closeAfter:a,continueStreaming:Un(s)?e.continueStreaming:s,stickyBelowClassName:Un(l)?e.stickyBelowClassName:l,width:Un(d)?e.width:d,margins:p,anchorData:ba(ba({},e.anchorData),{},{anchorEnabled:!0})})}return e}(e,t.payload.initiateParams));case"[COMMON] set anchor enable":return ba(ba({},e),{},{anchorData:ba(ba({},e.anchorData),{},{anchorEnabled:t.payload})});case"[ANCHOR] update is anchor status":return ba(ba({},e),{},{anchorData:ba(ba({},e.anchorData),{},{anchorStatus:t.payload})});case"[COMMON] set anchor disabled by user":return ba(ba({},e),{},{anchorData:ba(ba({},e.anchorData),{},{anchorDisabledByUser:t.payload})});default:return e}},monetization:function(){var e=arguments.length>0&&void 0!==arguments[0]?arguments[0]:wa,t=arguments.length>1?arguments[1]:void 0;switch(t.type){case"[CORE] initiate store":return Sa({},function(e,t){var n=t.monetization;if(Un(n))return e;var r=n.ad_tag,i=n.ad_type,o=n.vpaid_mode,a=n.ad_request_timeout,s=n.continue_content_play_while_waiting_for_ad,u=n.midrolls,c=u&&u.on&&u.on.sort(Wn),l=Un(s)?e.continuePlayingWhileWaitingForAd:s,d=c?c.indexOf(0):-1,p=-1!==d&&!l;return p&&(c=c.splice(d,1)),Sa(Sa({},e),{},{midrolls:Sa(Sa({},e.midrolls),{},{every:u&&u.every,on:c}),prerollEnabled:p,adRequestTimeout:Un(a)?e.adRequestTimeout:parseInt(a,10),vpaidMode:Un(o)?e.vpaidMode:o,continuePlayingWhileWaitingForAd:l,adsData:Sa(Sa({},e.adsData),{},{adType:Un(i)?e.adsData.adType:i,adTagUrlTemplate:Un(r)?e.adsData.adTagUrlTemplate:r})})}(e,t.payload.initiateParams));case"[COMMON] set new ad tag url template":return Sa(Sa({},e),{},{adsData:Sa(Sa({},e.adsData),{},{adTagUrlTemplate:t.payload})});case"[MONETIZATION] change ad status":return Sa(Sa({},e),{},{adsData:Sa(Sa({},e.adsData),{},{adStatus:t.payload,adErrorMessage:null})});case"[MONETIZATION] change ad tag":var n=t.payload,r=n.adUnit,i=n.adTag;return Sa(Sa({},e),{},{adsData:Sa(Sa({},e.adsData),{},{currentAdTag:i,adUnit:r})});case"[MONETIZATION] change pending ad status":return Sa(Sa({},e),{},{adsData:Sa(Sa({},e.adsData),{},{pendingAdStatus:t.payload})});case"[MONETIZATION] change ad error":return Sa(Sa({},e),{},{adsData:Sa(Sa({},e.adsData),{},{adStatus:"error",adErrorMessage:t.payload})});case"[MONETIZATION] increase ad impression counter":var o=e.adsData.adImpression;return Sa(Sa({},e),{},{adsData:Sa(Sa({},e.adsData),{},{adImpression:o+1})});case"[MONETIZATION] increase ad Opportunity counter":var a=e.adsData.adOpportunity;return Sa(Sa({},e),{},{adsData:Sa(Sa({},e.adsData),{},{adOpportunity:a+1})});case"[MONETIZATION] add played midroll number":var s=e.adsData.playedMidrolls,u=In()(s);return u.push(t.payload),Sa(Sa({},e),{},{adsData:Sa(Sa({},e.adsData),{},{adOrder:t.payload,playedMidrolls:u})});case"[MONETIZATION] clear played midrolls":return Sa(Sa({},e),{},{adsData:Sa(Sa({},e.adsData),{},{playedMidrolls:[]})});case"[MONETIZATION] clear ad data":return Sa(Sa({},e),{},{adsData:Sa(Sa({},e.adsData),{},{adOrder:0,currentAdTag:null,adDuration:0,adUnit:""})});case"[MONETIZATION] change ad duration":return Sa(Sa({},e),{},{adsData:Sa(Sa({},e.adsData),{},{adDuration:t.payload})});case"[MONETIZATION] update is vast ad":return Sa(Sa({},e),{},{adsData:Sa(Sa({},e.adsData),{},{isVastAd:t.payload})});case"[MONETIZATION] change ad current time":return Sa(Sa({},e),{},{adsData:Sa(Sa({},e.adsData),{},{adCurrentTime:t.payload})});case"[MONETIZATION] update ad muted":return Sa(Sa({},e),{},{adsData:Sa(Sa({},e.adsData),{},{adMuted:t.payload})});case"[MONETIZATION] change ad volume":return Sa(Sa({},e),{},{adsData:Sa(Sa({},e.adsData),{},{adVolume:t.payload})});case"[MONETIZATION] change pod info":var c=t.payload,l=c.podNumber,d=c.slotNumber;return Sa(Sa({},e),{},{adsData:Sa(Sa({},e.adsData),{},{podNumber:l,slotNumber:d})});case"[MONETIZATION] change loading ad status":return Sa(Sa({},e),{},{adsData:Sa(Sa({},e.adsData),{},{loadingAd:t.payload})});default:return e}},mediaData:function(){var e=arguments.length>0&&void 0!==arguments[0]?arguments[0]:ja,t=arguments.length>1?arguments[1]:void 0;switch(t.type){case"[CORE] initiate store":return Va({},function(e,t){var n=t.content_type,r=t.media_id,i=t.display_title;return Va(Va({},e),{},{mediaType:Un(n)?e.mediaType:n,mediaId:Un(r)?e.mediaId:r,videoData:Va(Va({},e.videoData),{},{showTitle:!!Un(i)||i})})}(e,t.payload.initiateParams));case"[CORE] load video request":return Va(Va({},e),{},{loadingMedia:!0});case"[CORE] load video request success":return Va(Va({},e),{},{loadingMedia:!1,videoList:t.payload});case"[CORE] set current video":var n=t.payload,r=n.index,i=n.videoData;return Va(Va({},e),{},{activeVideoIndex:r,videoData:i});case"[CORE] load video request error":return Va(Va({},e),{},{loadingMedia:!1,mediaLoadingError:t.payload});case"[COMMON] media request":var o=t.payload.mediaRequestObject;return Va(Va({},e),{},{mediaRequest:Va({},o)});default:return e}},semanticOptions:function(){var e=arguments.length>0&&void 0!==arguments[0]?arguments[0]:Ba,t=arguments.length>1?arguments[1]:void 0;switch(t.type){case"[CORE] initiate store":return Fa({},function(e,t){var n=t.semantic_options;if(Un(n))return e;var r=n.minimum_date_factor,i=n.promoted_videos,o=n.scan_images_on_page,a=n.scanned_element,s=n.scanned_element_type,u=n.scoped_keywords,c=n.tags;return Fa(Fa({},e),{},{minimumDateFactor:Un(r)?e.minimumDateFactor:r,promotedVideos:Un(i)?e.promotedVideos:i,scanImagesOnPage:Un(o)?e.scanImagesOnPage:o,scannedElement:Un(a)?e.scannedElement:a,scannedElementType:Un(s)?e.scannedElementType:s,scopedKeywords:Un(u)?e.scopedKeywords:u,tags:Un(c)?e.tags:c})}(e,t.payload.initiateParams));default:return e}},userInteraction:function(){var e=arguments.length>0&&void 0!==arguments[0]?arguments[0]:Wa,t=arguments.length>1?arguments[1]:void 0;switch(t.type){case"[USER INTERACTION] change user interaction":return qa(qa({},e),{},{userInteractionType:t.payload});default:return e}},splitView:function(){var e=arguments.length>0&&void 0!==arguments[0]?arguments[0]:$a,t=arguments.length>1?arguments[1]:void 0;switch(t.type){case"[CORE] initiate store":return Ga({},function(e,t){var n=t.anchor_options;if(!Un(n)){var r=n.split_view,i=n.split_view_ratio;return Ga(Ga({},e),{},{splitViewRatio:Un(r)||!r||Un(i)?e.splitViewRatio:i})}return e}(e,t.payload.initiateParams));default:return e}},discovery:function(){var e=arguments.length>0&&void 0!==arguments[0]?arguments[0]:Za,t=arguments.length>1?arguments[1]:void 0;switch(t.type){case"[CORE] initiate store":return Ya({},function(e,t){var n=t.next_video;return Un(n)?e:Ya(Ya({},e),{},{nextVideo:Xa(n)})}(e,t.payload.initiateParams));case"[DISCOVERY] show up next":return Ya(Ya({},e),{},{showUpNext:t.payload});case"[DISCOVERY] show skippable content":return Ya(Ya({},e),{},{showSkippableContent:t.payload});default:return e}}}),Qa=[],es=!1,ts=function e(){return function(t){return function(n){if(es)return Qa.push(n),null;es=!0;var r=t(n);return es=!1,Qa.length>0&&e()(t)(Qa.shift()),r}}},ns=function(e){var t=[];if(function(e){return!Un(e)&&!Un(e.enable_redux_debugging)&&e.enable_redux_debugging}(e)){var n=window&&window.__REDUX_DEVTOOLS_EXTENSION__&&window.__REDUX_DEVTOOLS_EXTENSION__();"function"===typeof n&&t.push(n)}var r=Et.apply(void 0,[wt(ua,ts)].concat(t));return vt(Ja,r)},rs=function(){function e(t){Ai()(this,e),f()(this,"playerVisibilitySubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"videoTagStatusSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"shouldPlayIfLazyplay",!0),f()(this,"shouldPlayIfAutoplayWhenViewable",!0),f()(this,"videoPausedByObserver",!1),this.store=t,this.playerVisibilitySubscriber=null,this.videoTagStatusSubscriber=null,this.playAccordingToPlaybackMethod()}return Vi()(e,[{key:"lazyplayHandler",value:function(e){hn.playerVisibility(e)>=.5&&(this.playVideo(),this.shouldPlayIfLazyplay=!1)}},{key:"autoplayWhenViewableHandler",value:function(e){hn.playerVisibility(e)>=.5?this.playVideo():this.pauseVideo()}},{key:"onPlayerVisibilityChanged",value:function(e){var t=hn.playbackMethod(e);"lazyplay"===t&&this.shouldPlayIfLazyplay&&this.lazyplayHandler(e),"autoplay_when_viewable"===t&&this.shouldPlayIfAutoplayWhenViewable&&this.autoplayWhenViewableHandler(e)}},{key:"onVideoTagStatusChanged",value:function(e){var t=hn.videoTagStatus(e);"paused"!==t||this.videoPausedByObserver||(this.shouldPlayIfAutoplayWhenViewable=!1),"playing"===t&&(this.shouldPlayIfAutoplayWhenViewable=!0,this.videoPausedByObserver=!1)}},{key:"initiatePlayerVisibilitySubscriber",value:function(){this.playerVisibilitySubscriber=new ji(this.store,e.getPlayerVisibilityDependencies,this.onPlayerVisibilityChanged.bind(this))}},{key:"initiateVideoTagStatusSubscriber",value:function(){this.videoTagStatusSubscriber=new ji(this.store,e.getVideoTagStatusDependencies,this.onVideoTagStatusChanged.bind(this))}},{key:"playVideo",value:function(){var e=this.store,t=e.dispatch,n=e.getState;"idle"===hn.videoTagStatus(n())?on("play")(t):on("resume")(t)}},{key:"pauseVideo",value:function(){var e=this.store,t=e.dispatch,n=e.getState;"paused"!==hn.videoTagStatus(n())&&(this.videoPausedByObserver=!0,on("pause")(t))}},{key:"playAccordingToPlaybackMethod",value:function(){var e=this.store,t=e.dispatch,n=(0,e.getState)();switch(hn.playbackMethod(n)){case"autoplay":this.playVideo();break;case"lazyplay":this.initiatePlayerVisibilitySubscriber();break;case"autoplay_when_viewable":this.initiatePlayerVisibilitySubscriber(),this.initiateVideoTagStatusSubscriber();break;case"none":an(!1)(t)}}}],[{key:"getPlayerVisibilityDependencies",value:function(e){return[hn.playerVisibility(e)]}},{key:"getVideoTagStatusDependencies",value:function(e){return[hn.videoTagStatus(e)]}}]),e}(),is=function(){function e(t,n,r,i){var o=this;Ai()(this,e),f()(this,"videoStatusSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"videoListSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"mediaRequestSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"playerVisibilitySubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"playbackMethodManager",void 0),f()(this,"store",void 0),f()(this,"loadContent",function(e,t,n,r){o.loadMedia(t,n,r).then(function(){o.playbackMethodManager=new rs(e)})}),f()(this,"loadMedia",function(e,t,n){var r=o.store,i=r.dispatch,a=r.getState,s=Dn.showTitle(a());if("semantic"===e){var u=pn.semanticOptions(a());return Na(u,s,n)(i)}return ka(t,s,n)(i)}),this.store=t,this.videoStatusSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getVideoStatusDependencies,this.onVideoStatusChanged.bind(this)),this.videoListSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getVideoListDependencies,this.onVideoListChanged.bind(this)),this.mediaRequestSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getMediaRequestDependencies,this.onMediaRequestChanged.bind(this)),this.playerVisibilitySubscriber=null,this.loadContent(t,r,n,i)}return Vi()(e,null,[{key:"createInstance",value:function(t,n,r,i){return new e(t,n,r,i)}}]),Vi()(e,[{key:"playNextVideo",value:function(e){var t=this.store.dispatch,n=Cn.videoList(e),r=Cn.activeVideoIndex(e)+1;n.length>1&&r>=n.length&&(r=0),r<n.length&&(!function(e){e({type:"[CORE] reset player data time params"})}(t),La(r,n[r])(t),on("play")(t))}},{key:"playPreviousVideo",value:function(e){var t=this.store.dispatch,n=Cn.videoList(e),r=Cn.activeVideoIndex(e);if(r>0){var i=r-1;La(i,n[i])(t),on("play")(t)}}},{key:"onVideoStatusChanged",value:function(e){"complete"===hn.videoTagStatus(e)&&this.playNextVideo(e)}},{key:"onVideoListChanged",value:function(e){var t=this.store.dispatch,n=Cn.videoList(e);!jn(n)&&n.length>0&&La(0,n[0])(t)}},{key:"onMediaRequestChanged",value:function(e){var t=Cn.mediaRequest(e);switch(t.type){case"playNewVideo":this.loadMedia("specific",t.value);break;case"playNextVideo":this.playNextVideo(e);break;case"playPreviousVideo":this.playPreviousVideo(e)}}}],[{key:"getVideoStatusDependencies",value:function(e){return[hn.videoTagStatus(e)]}},{key:"getVideoListDependencies",value:function(e){return[Cn.videoList(e)]}},{key:"getMediaRequestDependencies",value:function(e){return[Cn.mediaRequest(e)]}}]),e}(),os=function e(t){var n=this;Ai()(this,e),f()(this,"store",void 0),f()(this,"onDependencyFailure",function(e,t){console.log("onDependencyFailure",e,t);var r=n.store,i=r.dispatch,o=r.getState;switch(e){case"ima":"blocked"!==Fi.loadingImaStatus(o())&&Qn("error")(i);break;case"hls":er("error")(i)}}),f()(this,"onDependencyReady",function(e){var t=n.store.dispatch;switch(e){case"ima":Qn("success")(t);break;case"hls":er("success")(t)}}),this.store=t},as=function(e){return function(t){t({type:"[COMMON] set fullscreen",payload:e})}},ss=function(){function e(t,n){var r=this;Ai()(this,e),f()(this,"store",void 0),f()(this,"videoTag",void 0),f()(this,"pendingFullscreenSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"adStatusSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"playerUniqId",void 0),f()(this,"onAdStatusChanged",function(e){var t=_i.adStatus(e),n=r.videoTag.webkitDisplayingFullscreen;"playing"===t&&Bn()&&n&&r.exitFullscreen(r.videoTag)}),f()(this,"isPlayerInFullscreen",function(){var e=document,t=Bn()?En(r.playerUniqId):bn(r.playerUniqId);return Un(e.fullscreenElement)?!Un(e.webkitFullscreenElement)&&0===e.webkitFullscreenElement.id.localeCompare(t):0===e.fullscreenElement.id.localeCompare(t)}),f()(this,"changePlayerWidth",function(e){r.videoTag.style.width=e?"100%":"auto"}),f()(this,"onFullscreenChanged",function(){var e=r.store.dispatch,t=r.isPlayerInFullscreen();r.changePlayerWidth(t),as(t)(e)}),f()(this,"onFullscreenChangedIos",function(){var e=r.store.dispatch,t=r.videoTag.webkitDisplayingFullscreen;t||on("resume")(e),r.changePlayerWidth(t),as(t)(e)}),f()(this,"onPendingFullscreenRequestChanged",function(e){var t=gn.pendingFullscreenRequest(e);"enter"===t?r.enterFullscreen(r.videoTag):"exit"===t&&r.exitFullscreen(r.videoTag)}),f()(this,"getFullScreenElement",function(e,t){var n=document.getElementById(bn(r.playerUniqId));return Bn()?t:e?document:n}),f()(this,"enterFullscreen",function(e){var t=r.getFullScreenElement(!1,e);Bn()?t.webkitEnterFullscreen():document.webkitExitFullscreen?t.webkitRequestFullscreen():document.webkitCancelFullScreen?t.webkitRequestFullScreen():document.mozCancelFullScreen?t.mozRequestFullScreen():document.msExitFullscreen&&t.msRequestFullscreen()}),f()(this,"exitFullscreen",function(e){var t=r.getFullScreenElement(!0,e);document.webkitExitFullscreen||Bn()?t.webkitExitFullscreen():document.webkitCancelFullScreen?t.webkitCancelFullScreen():document.mozCancelFullScreen?t.mozCancelFullScreen():document.msExitFullscreen&&t.msExitFullscreen()}),this.store=t,this.videoTag=document.getElementById(En(n)),this.playerUniqId=n,document.addEventListener("fullscreenchange",this.onFullscreenChanged.bind(this)),document.addEventListener("webkitfullscreenchange",this.onFullscreenChanged.bind(this)),Bn()&&(this.videoTag.addEventListener("webkitendfullscreen",this.onFullscreenChangedIos.bind(this)),this.videoTag.addEventListener("webkitbeginfullscreen",this.onFullscreenChangedIos.bind(this))),this.pendingFullscreenSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getPendingFullscreenDependencies,this.onPendingFullscreenRequestChanged.bind(this)),this.adStatusSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getAdStatusDependencies,this.onAdStatusChanged.bind(this))}return Vi()(e,null,[{key:"createInstance",value:function(t,n){return new e(t,n)}}]),Vi()(e,null,[{key:"getPendingFullscreenDependencies",value:function(e){return[gn.pendingFullscreenRequest(e)]}},{key:"getAdStatusDependencies",value:function(e){return[_i.adStatus(e)]}}]),e}();function us(e,t){var n=Object.keys(e);if(Object.getOwnPropertySymbols){var r=Object.getOwnPropertySymbols(e);t&&(r=r.filter(function(t){return Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(e,t).enumerable})),n.push.apply(n,r)}return n}function cs(e){for(var t=1;t<arguments.length;t++){var n=null!=arguments[t]?arguments[t]:{};t%2?us(Object(n),!0).forEach(function(t){f()(e,t,n[t])}):Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors?Object.defineProperties(e,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors(n)):us(Object(n)).forEach(function(t){Object.defineProperty(e,t,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(n,t))})}return e}var ls,ds=function(e){return function(e){return e&&window.monti.playerConfigs&&window.monti.playerConfigs[e]}(e)?function(e){return window.monti.playerConfigs[e]}(e):window.monti.playerConfigs?window.monti.playerConfigs&&window.monti.playerConfigs[Object.keys(window.monti.playerConfigs)[0]]:null},ps=function e(t){var n=this;Ai()(this,e),f()(this,"videoTag",void 0),f()(this,"isBufferError",void 0),f()(this,"hls",void 0),f()(this,"hlsSetup",function(e,t,r,i){n.initiateHls(e),n.loadHlsSource(e,t,r,i)}),f()(this,"detachMedia",function(){Un(n.hls)||(n.hls.detachMedia(),n.hls.destroy(),n.hls=null)}),f()(this,"initiateHls",function(e){n.hls=new e,n.hls.attachMedia(n.videoTag)}),f()(this,"loadHlsSource",function(e,t,r,i){n.hls.on(e.Events.MEDIA_ATTACHED,function(){n.hls.loadSource(t)}),n.hls.on(e.Events.ERROR,function(t,o){n.mapHlsToErrors(e,o,i),t.details===e.ErrorDetails.BUFFER_STALLED_ERROR&&(r(!0),n.isBufferError=!0)}),n.hls.on(e.Events.FRAG_BUFFERED,function(){n.isBufferError&&(r(!1),n.isBufferError=!1)})}),f()(this,"mapHlsToErrors",function(e,t,r){if(t.fatal)switch(t.type){case e.ErrorTypes.NETWORK_ERROR:r(Xn.GENERAL_ERROR),n.hls.startLoad();break;case e.ErrorTypes.MEDIA_ERROR:r(Xn.GENERAL_ERROR),n.hls.recoverMediaError();break;default:r(Xn.GENERAL_ERROR),n.hls.destroy()}}),this.hls=void 0,this.videoTag=t,this.isBufferError=!1},fs=function e(){var t=this;Ai()(this,e),f()(this,"videoStreaming",void 0),f()(this,"hlsLibrarySetup",function(e,n,r,i){Un(t.videoStreaming)||t.videoStreaming.detachMedia(),t.videoStreaming=new ps(e),t.videoStreaming.hlsSetup(ls,n,r,i)})};f()(fs,"shouldLoadVideoStreamingSrcDirectly",function(e,t,n){return"no-need"===n&&!(""===e.canPlayType("application/vnd.apple.mpegurl"))}),f()(fs,"shouldUseHlsLibrary",function(e,t){return"success"===t&&(ls=void 0!==window.Hls?Hls:mmHls).isSupported()}),f()(fs,"isValidHlsUrl",function(e){return!Un(e)&&!e.includes(".mp4")}),f()(fs,"suitableVideoSource",function(e,t,n){return fs.isValidHlsUrl(t)?fs.shouldUseHlsLibrary(t,n)?"m3u8 with hls":fs.shouldLoadVideoStreamingSrcDirectly(e,t,n)?"m3u8 directly":"loading"!==n?"mp4":"":"mp4"}),f()(fs,"loadHlsVideoDirectly",function(e,t){e.setAttribute("src",t),e.load()});var hs=function(e){return function(t){t({type:"[MONETIZATION] change pending ad status",payload:{type:e}})}},ys="video/mp4",gs="application/vnd.apple.mpegurl",vs=function(){function e(t,n){var r=this;Ai()(this,e),f()(this,"store",void 0),f()(this,"videoTag",void 0),f()(this,"prerollEnabled",void 0),f()(this,"pendingVideoStatusSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"videoStreamingManager",void 0),f()(this,"videoDataSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"hlsLoadingStatusSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"newVideoDataLoaded",void 0),f()(this,"onHlsLoadingStatusChanged",function(e){"success"===Fi.loadingHLSStatus(e)&&(r.newVideoDataLoaded=!0,r.onPendingVideoStatusChanged(e))}),f()(this,"onPendingVideoStatusChanged",function(e){var t=hn.pendingVideoTagStatus(e),n=Dn.sources(e),i=Fi.loadingHLSStatus(e),o="blocked"===Fi.loadingImaStatus(e);r.handlePendingVideoStatus(t,n,i,o)}),f()(this,"onVideoDataChanged",function(){r.newVideoDataLoaded=!0}),f()(this,"sendPrerollPlayRequest",function(){var e=r.store.dispatch;hs("playPreroll")(e)}),f()(this,"handlePlayRequest",function(e,t,n){var i=r.store.dispatch;if(e&&e.length>0){if(r.newVideoDataLoaded&&(r.loadVideoSource(r.videoTag,e,t),r.newVideoDataLoaded=!1,r.prerollEnabled&&!n))return void r.sendPrerollPlayRequest();r.videoTag.play().catch(function(e){return console.error("Error playing the video: ",e)})}else dn(Xn.VIDEO_ERROR)(i)}),f()(this,"handlePendingVideoStatus",function(e,t,n,i){switch(e.type){case"play":r.handlePlayRequest(t,n,i);break;case"resume":r.videoTag.play().catch(function(e){return console.error("Error resuming the video: ",e)});break;case"pause":r.videoTag.pause();break;case"replay":r.videoTag.currentTime=0,r.videoTag.play().catch(function(e){return console.error("Error replaying the video: ",e)});break;case"seekTo":r.videoTag.pause(),r.videoTag.currentTime=e.value}}),f()(this,"loadMp4Source",function(e,t,n){var r=Ra(t,ys);n.setAttribute("src",r),n.load()}),f()(this,"loadVideoSource",function(e,t,n){var i=r.store.dispatch,o=Ra(t,gs);switch(fs.suitableVideoSource(e,o,n)){case"mp4":r.loadMp4Source(n,t,e);break;case"m3u8 with hls":r.videoStreamingManager.hlsLibrarySetup(e,o,function(e){return un(e)(i)},function(e){return dn(e)(i)});break;case"m3u8 directly":fs.loadHlsVideoDirectly(e,o)}}),this.store=t;var i=t.getState;this.videoStreamingManager=new fs,this.videoTag=document.getElementById(En(n)),this.prerollEnabled=bi.prerollEnabled(i()),this.pendingVideoStatusSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getPendingVideoStatusDependencies,this.onPendingVideoStatusChanged.bind(this)),this.videoDataSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getVideoDataDependencies,this.onVideoDataChanged.bind(this)),this.hlsLoadingStatusSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getHLSLoadingStatusDependencies,this.onHlsLoadingStatusChanged.bind(this))}return Vi()(e,null,[{key:"createInstance",value:function(t,n){return new e(t,n)}}]),Vi()(e,null,[{key:"getHLSLoadingStatusDependencies",value:function(e){return[Fi.loadingHLSStatus(e)]}},{key:"getPendingVideoStatusDependencies",value:function(e){return[hn.pendingVideoTagStatus(e)]}},{key:"getVideoDataDependencies",value:function(e){return[Cn.videoData(e)]}}]),e}();function ms(e,t){var n=Object.keys(e);if(Object.getOwnPropertySymbols){var r=Object.getOwnPropertySymbols(e);t&&(r=r.filter(function(t){return Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(e,t).enumerable})),n.push.apply(n,r)}return n}function bs(e){for(var t=1;t<arguments.length;t++){var n=null!=arguments[t]?arguments[t]:{};t%2?ms(Object(n),!0).forEach(function(t){f()(e,t,n[t])}):Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors?Object.defineProperties(e,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors(n)):ms(Object(n)).forEach(function(t){Object.defineProperty(e,t,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(n,t))})}return e}var Os={READY_EVENT:"ready",PLAY_EVENT:"play",PAUSE_EVENT:"pause",TIME_EVENT:"time",SEEK_EVENT:"seek",COMPLETE_EVENT:"complete",VOLUME_EVENT:"volume",MUTE_EVENT:"mute"},_s=Object.values(Os),Ss={FULLSCREEN_EVENT:"fullscreen",ANCHOR_STATUS_EVENT:"anchorStatusChanged",ANCHOR_CLOSED_EVENT:"anchorClosed"},Es={AD_PLAY_EVENT:"adPlay",AD_PAUSE_EVENT:"adPause",AD_RESUME_EVENT:"adResume",AD_COMPLETE_EVENT:"adComplete",AD_TIME_EVENT:"adTime",AD_MUTE_EVENT:"adMute",AD_SKIPPED_EVENT:"adSkipped",AD_ERROR_EVENT:"adError",AD_BLOCK_EVENT:"adBlock",AD_REQUEST_EVENT:"adRequest",AD_OPPORTUNITY_EVENT:"adOpportunity",AD_IMPRESSION_EVENT:"adImpression"},ws=Object.values(Es),Ps=Object.values(bs(bs(bs({},Os),Es),Ss)),Ts=function(){function e(t,n){var r=this;Ai()(this,e),f()(this,"eventsCallbacksHandler",void 0),f()(this,"store",void 0),f()(this,"videoStatusSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"videoMuteSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"videoVolumeSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"videoTimeFragmentSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"videoListStoreSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"previousVideoTagStatus",void 0),f()(this,"startSeekTime",0),f()(this,"canHandleReady",function(e,t,n){if(t===Os.READY_EVENT){var r=Cn.videoList(e);if(Array.isArray(r)&&r.length>0)return n(),!0}return!1}),f()(this,"canBeHandled",function(e,t){var n=r.store.getState;return r.canHandleReady(n(),e,t)}),f()(this,"reportSeekEnd",function(e){var t={position:hn.currentVideoTimeFragment(e),offset:r.startSeekTime};r.eventsCallbacksHandler.onEvent(Os.SEEK_EVENT,t)}),f()(this,"onMuteStateChanged",function(e){var t=gn.muted(e);r.eventsCallbacksHandler.onEvent(Os.MUTE_EVENT,{state:t})}),f()(this,"onVolumeChanged",function(e){var t=gn.muted(e),n=gn.volume(e);r.eventsCallbacksHandler.onEvent(Os.VOLUME_EVENT,{level:t?0:n})}),f()(this,"onVideoTimeFragmentChanged",function(e){var t=hn.currentVideoTimeFragment(e),n=hn.currentVideoDuration(e);r.eventsCallbacksHandler.onEvent(Os.TIME_EVENT,{duration:n,position:t})}),f()(this,"onVideoListChanged",function(){r.eventsCallbacksHandler.onEvent(Os.READY_EVENT)}),this.store=t,this.eventsCallbacksHandler=n,this.videoStatusSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getVideoStatusDependencies,this.onVideoStatusChanged.bind(this)),this.videoMuteSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getVideoMuteDependencies,this.onMuteStateChanged.bind(this)),this.videoVolumeSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getVolumeDependencies,this.onVolumeChanged.bind(this)),this.videoTimeFragmentSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getVideoTimeDependencies,this.onVideoTimeFragmentChanged.bind(this)),this.videoListStoreSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getVideoListDependencies,this.onVideoListChanged.bind(this)),this.previousVideoTagStatus=hn.videoTagStatus(t.getState())}return Vi()(e,[{key:"onVideoStatusChanged",value:function(e){var t=hn.videoTagStatus(e);switch("seeking"===this.previousVideoTagStatus&&this.reportSeekEnd(e),t){case"paused":this.eventsCallbacksHandler.onEvent(Os.PAUSE_EVENT);break;case"seeking":this.startSeekTime=hn.currentVideoTimeFragment(e);break;case"complete":this.eventsCallbacksHandler.onEvent(Os.COMPLETE_EVENT);break;case"playing":this.eventsCallbacksHandler.onEvent(Os.PLAY_EVENT)}this.previousVideoTagStatus=t}}],[{key:"getVideoStatusDependencies",value:function(e){return[hn.videoTagStatus(e)]}}]),e}();f()(Ts,"getVideoMuteDependencies",function(e){return[gn.muted(e)]}),f()(Ts,"getVolumeDependencies",function(e){return[gn.volume(e)]}),f()(Ts,"getVideoTimeDependencies",function(e){return[hn.currentVideoTimeFragment(e)]}),f()(Ts,"getVideoListDependencies",function(e){return[Cn.videoList(e)]}),f()(Ts,"isContentEvent",function(e){return _s.some(function(t){return t===e})});var As=function e(t,n){var r=this;Ai()(this,e),f()(this,"eventsCallbacksHandler",void 0),f()(this,"store",void 0),f()(this,"fullscreenSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"anchorStatusSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"anchorDisabledByUserSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"onFullscreenChanged",function(e){var t=gn.isFullscreenOn(e);r.eventsCallbacksHandler.onEvent(Ss.FULLSCREEN_EVENT,{state:t})}),f()(this,"onAnchorStatusChanged",function(e){var t="active"===Pr(e)?"activated":"deactivated";r.eventsCallbacksHandler.onEvent(Ss.ANCHOR_STATUS_EVENT,{state:t})}),f()(this,"onAnchorDisabledByUser",function(e){if(wr(e)){var t=hn.currentVideoTimeFragment(e);r.eventsCallbacksHandler.onEvent(Ss.ANCHOR_CLOSED_EVENT,{position:t})}}),this.store=t,this.eventsCallbacksHandler=n,this.fullscreenSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getFullscreenDependencies,this.onFullscreenChanged.bind(this)),this.anchorStatusSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getAnchorStatusDependencies,this.onAnchorStatusChanged.bind(this)),this.anchorDisabledByUserSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getAnchorDisabledByUserDependencies,this.onAnchorDisabledByUser.bind(this))};f()(As,"getFullscreenDependencies",function(e){return[gn.isFullscreenOn(e)]}),f()(As,"getAnchorStatusDependencies",function(e){return[Pr(e)]}),f()(As,"getAnchorDisabledByUserDependencies",function(e){return[wr(e)]});var Cs=function(){function e(t,n){var r=this;Ai()(this,e),f()(this,"store",void 0),f()(this,"eventsCallbacksHandler",void 0),f()(this,"adStatusSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"adImpressionSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"adOpportunitySubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"adTimeSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"adMuteSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"adProviderLoadingStatusSubscriber",void 0),f()(this,"previousAdStatus",void 0),f()(this,"canBeHandled",function(e,t){var n=r.store.getState;switch(Fi.loadingImaStatus(n())){case"loading":return!1;case"success":case"error":return!0;case"blocked":return t(),!0;case"":default:return!1}}),f()(this,"onAdStatusChanged",function(e){var t=_i.adStatus(e),n=_i.currentAdTag(e);switch(t){case"requested":r.eventsCallbacksHandler.onEvent(Es.AD_REQUEST_EVENT,{tag:n});break;case"paused":r.eventsCallbacksHandler.onEvent(Es.AD_PAUSE_EVENT,{tag:n});break;case"completed":r.eventsCallbacksHandler.onEvent(Es.AD_COMPLETE_EVENT,{tag:n});break;case"skipped":r.eventsCallbacksHandler.onEvent(Es.AD_SKIPPED_EVENT,{tag:n});break;case"playing":"paused"===r.previousAdStatus?r.eventsCallbacksHandler.onEvent(Es.AD_RESUME_EVENT,{tag:n}):r.eventsCallbacksHandler.onEvent(Es.AD_PLAY_EVENT,{tag:n});break;case"error":var i=_i.adErrorMessage(e);r.eventsCallbacksHandler.onEvent(Es.AD_ERROR_EVENT,{tag:n,message:i})}r.previousAdStatus=t}),f()(this,"onAtTimeChanged",function(e){var t=_i.adCurrentTime(e),n=_i.currentAdTag(e),i=_i.adDuration(e);r.eventsCallbacksHandler.onEvent(Es.AD_TIME_EVENT,{position:t,tag:n,duration:i})}),f()(this,"onAdMuteChanged",function(e){var t=_i.adMuted(e);r.eventsCallbacksHandler.onEvent(Es.AD_MUTE_EVENT,{state:t})}),f()(this,"onAdProviderLoadingChanged",function(e){"blocked"===Fi.loadingImaStatus(e)&&r.eventsCallbacksHandler.onEvent(Es.AD_BLOCK_EVENT)}),f()(this,"onAdImpressionChanged",function(e){var t=_i.currentAdTag(e);r.eventsCallbacksHandler.onEvent(Es.AD_IMPRESSION_EVENT,{tag:t})}),f()(this,"onAdOpportunityChanged",function(e){var t=_i.currentAdTag(e);r.eventsCallbacksHandler.onEvent(Es.AD_OPPORTUNITY_EVENT,{tag:t})}),this.store=t,this.eventsCallbacksHandler=n,this.previousAdStatus=_i.adStatus(t.getState()),this.adStatusSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getAdStatusDependencies,this.onAdStatusChanged.bind(this)),this.adTimeSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getAdTimeDependencies,this.onAtTimeChanged.bind(this)),this.adMuteSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getAdMuteDependencies,this.onAdMuteChanged.bind(this)),this.adImpressionSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getAdImpressionDependencies,this.onAdImpressionChanged.bind(this)),this.adOpportunitySubscriber=new ji(t,e.getAdOpportunitySubscriberDependencies,this.onAdOpportunityChanged.bind(this)),this.adProviderLoadingStatusSubscriber=new ji(t,e.getAdProviderLoadingDependencies,this.onAdProviderLoadingChanged.bind(this))}return Vi()(e,null,[{key:"getAdStatusDependencies",value:function(e){return[_i.adStatus(e)]}},{key:"getAdTimeDependencies",value:function(e){return[_i.adCurrentTime(e)]}},{key:"getAdMuteDependencies",value:function(e){return[_i.adMuted(e)]}},{key:"getAdProviderLoadingDependencies",value:function(e){return[Fi.loadingImaStatus(e)]}}]),e}();f()(Cs,"isAdEvent",function(e){return ws.some(function(t){return t===e})}),f()(Cs,"getAdImpressionDependencies",function(e){return[_i.adImpression(e)]}),f()(Cs,"getAdOpportunitySubscriberDependencies",function(e){return[_i.adOpportunity(e)]});var Rs=function e(t){var n=this;Ai()(this,e),f()(this,"contentEvents",void 0),f()(this,"generalEvents",void 0),f()(this,"adEvents",void 0),f()(this,"subscribers",{}),f()(this,"onRegisterToEvent",function(e,t,r){n.isValidEvent(e)&&(Ts.isContentEvent(e)&&n.contentEvents.canBeHandled(e,t)||Cs.isAdEvent(e)&&n.adEvents.canBeHandled(e,t())||n.getEventSubscribersList(e).push({callback:t,once:r}))}),f()(this,"isValidEvent",function(e){return Ps.some(function(t){return t===e})}),f()(this,"getEventSubscribersList",function(e){return Array.isArray(n.subscribers[e])||(n.subscribers[e]=[]),n.subscribers[e]}),f()(this,"filterOutOnceCallbacks",function(e,t){n.subscribers[e]=t.filter(function(e){return!e.once})}),f()(this,"onEvent",function(e,t){var r=n.getEventSubscribersList(e);r.forEach(function(e){(0,e.callback)(t)}),n.filterOutOnceCallbacks(e,r)}),this.contentEvents=new Ts(t,this),this.generalEvents=new As(t,this),this.adEvents=new Cs(t,this)},Ds=function(){function e(t){Ai()(this,e),f()(this,"eventsHandler",void 0),this.eventsHandler=new Rs(t)}return Vi()(e,[{key:"on",value:function(e,t){this.eventsHandler.onRegisterToEvent(e,t,!1)}},{key:"once",value:function(e,t){this.eventsHandler.onRegisterToEvent(e,t,!0)}}]),e}();function Ms(e,t){var n=Object.keys(e);if(Object.getOwnPropertySymbols){var r=Object.getOwnPropertySymbols(e);t&&(r=r.filter(function(t){return Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(e,t).enumerable})),n.push.apply(n,r)}return n}function Is(e){for(var t=1;t<arguments.length;t++){var n=null!=arguments[t]?arguments[t]:{};t%2?Ms(Object(n),!0).forEach(function(t){f()(e,t,n[t])}):Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors?Object.defineProperties(e,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptors(n)):Ms(Object(n)).forEach(function(t){Object.defineProperty(e,t,Object.getOwnPropertyDescriptor(n,t))})}return e}var ks=function(){var e=window.monti.dataset;return jn(e)?(e={players:{},preact:Is(Is({},r),i),store:{},plugins:{}},window.monti.dataset=e,e):e},Ns=function(e){var t=function(){var e=(new Date).getTime(),t=performance&&performance.now&&1e3*performance.now()||0;return"xxxxxxxx-xxxx-4xxx-yxxx-xxxxxxxxxxxx".replace(/[xy]/g,function(n){var r=16*Math.random();return e>0?(r=(e+r)%16|0,e=Math.floor(e/16)):(r=(t+r)%16|0,t=Math.floor(t/16)),("x"===n?r:3&r|8).toString(16)})}(),n=function(e,t){var n=ns(e.dev_config),r=ks(),i=n.dispatch;return r.store[t]=n,function(e,t){return function(n){n({type:"[CORE] initiate store",payload:{initiateParams:e,playerInstanceUniqId:t}})}}(e,t)(i),n}(e,t);return function(e,t,n){B(b(Pi,{playerId:t,store:n,playerPosition:e}),e)}(e.player_pos,t,n),oa.getInstance().loadInternalPlugins(n,t,e),ss.createInstance(n,t),vs.createInstance(n,t),is.createInstance(n,e.media_id,e.content_type,e.dev_config),function(e){var t=e.dispatch;if(er(Ci.getInstance().getHLSLoadingStatus())(t),Qn(Ci.getInstance().getIMALoadingStatus())(t),!Ci.getInstance().isDependenciesReady()){var n=new os(e);Ci.getInstance().addDependenciesCallback(n)}}(n),function(e,t){ks().players[t]=new Ds(e)}(n,t),t},Ls=function(e){return console.log("player initiation start",e),new Promise(function(t,n){try{var r=function(e){var t=e.player_pos||document.currentScript.parentElement,n=e.media_id||e.content_id;return cs(cs({},e),{},{player_pos:t,media_id:n})}(function(e){var t=e.player_id,n=ds(t);return null===n?e:cs(cs({},e),n)}(e)),i=Ns(r);!function(e){var t=new CustomEvent("montiConfigLoaded",{detail:{playerKey:e}});window.dispatchEvent(t)}(i),t(i)}catch(o){console.error("Player initiation error",o),n(o)}})},xs=function(){return{initiate:Ls}};window.monti=xs,Ci.getInstance().loadExternalDependencies()}]); window.monti().initiate(Object.assign({player_pos: document.currentScript.parentElement}, {"is_conflicting_with_other_jw_players":false,"programmatic_play_with_sound_on_desktop":false,"referrer_id":"af93e181-b289-0560-a2bf-808e93bb05bc","width":"100","comscore_publisher_id":"18120612","monetization":{"ad_type":"static_tag","continue_content_play_while_waiting_for_ad":false,"strategy":"on_player_load","ad_request_timeout":"10000","midrolls":{"on":[0]},"vpaid_mode":"ENABLED","ad_tag":"https://pubads.g.doubleclick.net/gampad/ads?sz=400x300|640x480|480x270|640x360&iu=/175840252/MMPlus/smithsonianmag/Video&impl=s&gdfp_req=1&env=vp&output=vast&unviewed_position_start=1&url=##REFERRER_URL_UNESC##&description_url=##DESCRIPTION_URL_UNESC##&correlator=##CACHEBUSTER##&cust_params=mm_midroll%3D##MIDROLL_ORDER##%26video_ID%3D##VIDEO_ID##"},"sponsorship":false,"player_identifier":"mplayer","recommendation_id":null,"brand_color":"#FF9900","powered_by_strip":true,"platform":"buffy","type":"video","config_name":"MM+ | Smithsonianmag | Podding","player_id":"3v9g2u2f","playlist_id":"fSkmeWKF","playback_method":"autoplay","anchor_viewability_method":"none","player_version":"v4","playlist_type":"semantic","semantic_options":{"scan_images_on_page":true,"scanned_element":"","tags":"geogrophy,nature,animals,habitat,outdoors,science,history","minimum_date_factor":30,"scanned_element_type":"tag","scoped_keywords":"mentalfloss","promoted_videos":[]},"script_destination":"mm","publisher_contribution":"floor8","general_script_description":"","brand_logo":"","brand_logo_click_url":"","next_video":"none","uniq_key":"af93e181-b289-0560-a2bf-808e93bb05bc","content_id":"fSkmeWKF","content_type":"semantic"})); Finland has vastly improved in reading, math and science literacy over the past decade in large part because its teachers are trusted to do whatever it takes to turn young lives around. This 13-year-old, Besart Kabashi, received something akin to royal tutoring. “I took Besart on that year as my private student,” Louhivuori told me in his office, which boasted a Beatles “Yellow Submarine” poster on the wall and an electric guitar in the closet. When Besart was not studying science, geography and math, he was parked next to Louhivuori’s desk at the front of his class of 9- and 10-year- olds, cracking open books from a tall stack, slowly reading one, then another, then devouring them by the dozens. By the end of the year, the son of Kosovo war refugees had conquered his adopted country’s vowel-rich language and arrived at the realization that he could, in fact, learn. Years later, a 20-year-old Besart showed up at Kirkkojarvi’s Christmas party with a bottle of Cognac and a big grin. “You helped me,” he told his former teacher. Besart had opened his own car repair firm and a cleaning company. “No big fuss,” Louhivuori told me. “This is what we do every day, prepare kids for life.” This tale of a single rescued child hints at some of the reasons for the tiny Nordic nation’s staggering record of education success, a phenomenon that has inspired, baffled and even irked many of America’s parents and educators. Finnish schooling became an unlikely hot topic after the 2010 documentary film Waiting for “Superman” contrasted it with America’s troubled public schools. “Whatever it takes” is an attitude that drives not just Kirkkojarvi’s 30 teachers, but most of Finland’s 62,000 educators in 3,500 schools from Lapland to Turku—professionals selected from the top 10 percent of the nation’s graduates to earn a required master’s degree in education. Many schools are small enough so that teachers know every student. If one method fails, teachers consult with colleagues to try something else. They seem to relish the challenges. Nearly 30 percent of Finland’s children receive some kind of special help during their first nine years of school. The school where Louhivuori teaches served 240 first through ninth graders last year; and in contrast with Finland’s reputation for ethnic homogeneity, more than half of its 150 elementary-level students are immigrants—from Somalia, Iraq, Russia, Bangladesh, Estonia and Ethiopia, among other nations. “Children from wealthy families with lots of education can be taught by stupid teachers,” Louhivuori said, smiling. “We try to catch the weak students. It’s deep in our thinking.” Advertisement scroll for more The transformation of the Finns’ education system began some 40 years ago as the key propellent of the country’s economic recovery plan. Educators had little idea it was so successful until 2000, when the first results from the Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA), a standardized test given to 15-year-olds in more than 40 global venues, revealed Finnish youth to be the best young readers in the world. Three years later, they led in math. By 2006, Finland was first out of 57 countries (and a few cities) in science. In the 2009 PISA scores released last year, the nation came in second in science, third in reading and sixth in math among nearly half a million students worldwide. “I’m still surprised,” said Arjariita Heikkinen, principal of a Helsinki comprehensive school. “I didn’t realize we were that good.” In the United States, which has muddled along in the middle for the past decade, government officials have attempted to introduce marketplace competition into public schools. In recent years, a group of Wall Street financiers and philanthropists such as Bill Gates have put money behind private-sector ideas, such as vouchers, data-driven curriculum and charter schools, which have doubled in number in the past decade. President Obama, too, has apparently bet on compe­tition. His Race to the Top initiative invites states to compete for federal dollars using tests and other methods to measure teachers, a philosophy that would not fly in Finland. “I think, in fact, teachers would tear off their shirts,” said Timo Heikkinen, a Helsinki principal with 24 years of teaching experience. “If you only measure the statistics, you miss the human aspect.”

      The facts show that America has it all wrong in putting to much emphasis on national "data-driven" competition. These approaches take away from the unique aspects of each child.

    1. parrot imitating spring

      I think the parrot here is symbolizing El General himself. Parrot is not spring which brings joy and happiness. El General is a bad person, however, he is pretending to bring his people joy and justice from his palace.

      We can also think of the parrot at the cage as a symbol for the Haitian people who were considered slaves. Even though the parrot is not free and is caged, it is still imitating spring, by the same token, in spite of the oppression and injustice, the Haitian people are not only pretending that everything is ok, but rather that they are happy! Because being miserable may actually upset El General.

    1. O]ur image of children no longer considers them as isolated and egocentric, does not see them only engaged in action with objects, does not emphasize only the cog-nitive aspects,

      This makes me think of our role as educators and evolving beliefs that we have of children. Do we see children as passive or active contributors? What thing I know is that the image of the child I have today may change tomorrow as I believe it gets reshaped with time, experience, and reflection.

    1. This World is not Conclusion. A Species stands beyond — Invisible, as Music — But positive, as Sound —

      I think the author's first sentence has a strong personal point of view that allows the reader to know what she is thinking directly. Personally, I would be curious to know what she would say next, and what discoveries would lead her to such an opinion and conclusion.According to the author's verse, the Truth About many things is not always what it seems. What we see, hear or feel may have different meanings. There are many times when human beings may be wrong about something

    1. You neverowe me personal information about your health (mental or physical), or anything else•You are always welcome to talk to me about things that you are going through.•If I can’t help you, I may know somebody who can.•If you need extra help, or you need to miss class, or you need more time with something, just ask. I’ll work with you. I promise.

      As a student, I think we can all agree that this gives us a sense of relief and some breathing room as we are living in one of the most chaotic eras of our life. This is greatly appreciated.

    1. Author Response

      We would like to thank eLife editors and the reviewers for their time and effort in reviewing our manuscript, entitled: “Partial prion cross-seeding between fungal and mammalian amyloid signaling motifs” by Bardin et al. We considered carefully their comments and modified our preprint accordingly (new version posted here) and address the remarks and criticism of the reviewers in the response provided below.

      The editors’ summary of the review read as follows:

      Summary

      Bardin and colleagues identify and characterize a third prion system in P. anserina based on a cognate innate immunity signalosome comprised of PNT1/HELLP. The authors demonstrate that the three prion pathways operate orthogonally without cross-seeding; however, the newly identified PNT1/HELLP prion can be cross-seeded by the putatively homologous human necroptosis pathway when it is reconstituted in P. anserina, which further supports an evolutionary relationship between them. The review has identified substantive concerns, which limit the novelty of the work and would require significant new studies to address the mechanistic gaps. These concerns include prior work revealing several major tenets including prion activity for PNT1/HELLP in C. globosum and evolutionary conservation to the mammalian necroptosis pathway and the absence for robust experimental support for cross-seeding, or the absence thereof, membrane disruption as the cause of incompatibility, and for the relationship among toxicity, growth, protein state, and protein interaction. Concerns were also raised about the data presented, or absent, in terms of replicates, frequency of observations, and variability.

      It is our understanding that the editors and reviewers raise two types of concerns. One relates to the novelty of the work. The second type directly questions the experimental soundness of some of the presented results. We will briefly respond to the criticism regarding novelty and in detail to the methodological critique. We show the existence of a third PFD-based cell-death inducing system in Podospora, that human RHIM-motifs form prions in Podospora and that RHIM-prions partially cross-seed with PP-fungal prions. These results are nonetheless novel and do shed light on the biology of Podospora and the relation of fungal and mammalian amyloid signaling motifs. Regarding the second group of concerns, we think that by clarifying certain approaches and by giving experimental results in full detail, we are able to wave many of the criticisms. For the remaining points (essentially the question of the HELLP membrane interaction), we amend our preprint to point at the delineation of experimental results and interpretation explicitly. We gratefully acknowledge the editors and reviewers input as a mean to improve the quality of the preprint and realize in light of some of these comments that the manuscript lacked in clarity at place and that detailed results tables (that were summarized in the original preprint for the sake of conciseness) should indeed be included. But having said that, it is our intention to stand our ground regarding the central claims of the paper (as they appeared in the abstract of the preprint).

      Reviewer #1

      Bardin and colleagues identify and characterize a third prion system in P. anserina based on the PNT1/HELLP NLR-based signalosome based on the amyloid signaling motif PP from Chaetomium globosum. The C-terminal domain of HELLP is shown to exist in either soluble or aggregated states based on fluorescence microscopy of tagged protein in vivo, termed the [pi] state, and to form amyloid in vitro. These distinct states can be propagated independently and induce conversion of full-length HELLP upon cytoplasmic mixing, which leads to cell death. The PNT1 N-terminal domain also forms foci in vivo and can seed conversion of HELLP, also leading to cell death. The C-terminal domain of C. globosum HELLP and the RHIM regions of mammalian RIP1 and RIP3, which both contain PP motifs, can cross-seed HELLP conversion to the aggregated state but the other known P. anserina prions [Het-s] and [phi] are unable to do so.

      Support for the model proposed is generally qualitative in nature, with multiple instances of data described but not presented, including the timing of conversion to the aggregated state, revision of the aggregated state in meiotic progeny, the frequencies of conversion and co-localization, and the correlations between growth and prion phenotype. For the data presented, replicates, frequency of observations, and variability are not reported.

      It is unclear to us what is meant by “the model proposed”. It is not our understanding that we are proposing “a model” in this paper. The results that we claim are:

      -There is a third NLR/HELL protein pair involving amyloid signaling in Podospora

      -There is no cross-seeding between HELLP PFD and the two other Podospora PFDs (HET-s, HELLF)

      -RHIM can form a prion in Podospora

      -There is a partial prion cross-seeding between PP PFDs and mammalian RHIM in vivo in Podospora

      These are the statements made in the abstract of the preprint. It is our opinion that these central claims stand in face of the reviewers criticism. We shall attempt to provide whenever possible quantitative details regarding the points raised.

      Specifically:

      the timing of conversion to the aggregated state

      There are two types of experimental situations here. In certain sets of experiments, spontaneous conversion to the prion state is measured at different subculture durations (5, 11, 19 days of subculture) (as appears in Table 1). When induced conversion (cross-seeding) is assayed, the conversion process is measured at a single time point. Details of the timing of assay of the conversion are given in the material and methods section (and now given in Table 1).

      revision of the aggregated state in meiotic progeny

      Details of the progeny of a specific cross involving curing of the [π] prion are now given. Among 20 meiotic progeny containing the GFP-HELLP(214-271), 3 were cured.

      the frequencies of conversion

      Possibly the statement that the results are “generally qualitative” comes from the fact that several conversion experiments or barrage interaction results were presented in tables with a binary output (+ or -) in the original preprint. This presentation was chosen because the replicates of these experiments yielded only monotonous all-or-none results. All tested strains were either converted (+) or not (-). In all tables, the number of tested strains and the number of replicates per strain are now given (Table S1 to S6). This presentation results in quite boring tables but we think that this should eliminate this ambiguity.

      and co-localization

      For all co-localization experiments, in addition to representative micrographs, counts of independent observations for each phenotypes and of co-localizing dots are given in Tables S7 and S8.

      the correlations between growth and prion phenotype.

      As there is no toxic effect of prion itself in absence of HELL or HeLo containing proteins (published results for [Het-s] and [φ], and verified here for [π] and [Rhim]), this last remark appear to apply to RHIM/HELLP co-expression that results in growth defects. We observe that strains co-expressing RHIM and HELLP are affected in their growth when there are infected with [Rhim] prions. These results are presented in Table 2. We based the conclusion that the growth defect relates to acquisition of the prion phenotype because the growth defect occurs after contact with a prion infected strain. This increase in the number of strains with a growth defect requires presence of the corresponding PFD in the recipient strain. Finally, the same table presents as positive control a similar experiment with homotypic [π]/HELLP interactions.

      In addition, a mechanism is proposed to explain the toxicity associated with HELLP conversion to the aggregated state - membrane localization - but this model is not supported by robust data such as a marker for the membrane in the fluorescence images or a biochemical fractionation. Moreover, the absence of functional data, such as mutations that disrupt amyloid formation, leave the model with correlative observations to support it.

      We agree that we do not prove membrane association for HELLP. Considering the precedent of HET-S, it is however a plausible explanation for the documented cell-death inducing activity. We acknowledge that we do not provide experimental evidence based on biochemical fractionation or dual labeling that HELLP relocates to the membrane (this would probably require confocal microscopy). What we due claim however is that in this regard HELLP behaves analogously to HET-S, CgHELLP and HELLF. We have modified the text of the preprint to specifically make the statement that proof of membrane localization would require other approaches (in particular biochemical fractionation).

      The reviewer calls for mutations that disrupt amyloid formation and that should accordingly abolish HELLP toxicity. While this type of experiment is not lacking interest (this exact type of study has been made in the case of HET-S), we feel that at the present stage the fact that toxicity of HELLP is conditional and occurs specifically in interaction with [π] (not [π*] or other Podospora prions) is a sufficient support to legitimate the suggestion that HELLP functions analogously to HET-S, HELLF and CgHELLP by activation through amyloid templating.

      Finally, observations on the C. globosum system decrease the novelty of the observations.

      We address this comment below (response to substantive concern 1 of the reviewer #2).

      Reviewer #2

      This work reports the discovery of an amyloid-based cell death signaling pathway in the filamentous fungus, Podospora anserina. This makes the third such pathway in this fungus. As for the others, the amyloid in this case has prion-like activity, is selectively nucleated by a cognate innate immunity sensor protein, and results in activation of the membrane-disrupting activity of the protein. They show that all three pathways operate orthogonally - that is without cross-seeding. In contrast, cross-seeding did occur between this pathway and the putatively homologous human necroptosis pathway when it is reconstituted in P. anserina, which further supports an evolutionary relationship between them.

      Substantive concerns:

      1) The novelty of this finding is somewhat dampened by this group's prior demonstration of several of the major points of interest in previous papers. They had previously discovered and characterized the homologous pathway in a different fungus, and suggested an evolutionary link between fungal amyloid signalosomes and mammalian necroptosis using strong bioinformatic and structural evidence. In addition, they had shown that the two previously known amyloid signaling pathways in P. anserina operated orthogonally. Hence the major point of novelty, as reflected in the title, is the demonstration that this particular amyloid pathway can cross-seed the human necroptosis amyloids.

      We are honestly puzzled by this comment, shared indeed also by reviewer 1. At no place in the preprint do we claim that the discovery of the PP-motif is new, we build on preceding work on CgHELLP and claim novelty on distinct aspects. While argumenting on the significance of one’s work is somewhat of a vain enterprise, we shall nonetheless point the specific interest we see in these results. As part of our longstanding attention on Podospora as a model to study fungal PCD, we consider it of interest to document that this species contains three amyloid-activated HeLo/HELL-domain cell-death execution pathways. Bioinformatic surveys suggest the co-occurrence of several amyloid motifs in different fungal genomes, it is of interest we think to document this redundancy at a more functional level at least in one system. The present study is superior to the previous one on CgHELLP in the aspect that activity of the PP-motif proteins is being studied in their native context (not in a heterologous host that diverged from C. globosum tens of millions of years ago). Then, to our knowledge, RHIM-motifs have never been shown to behave as prions. There is a non-trivial relation of the concepts of amyloids and prions. The reviewer writes in a later paragraph that amyloids are inherently self-perpetuating but this does imply that all amyloids are prions (or vice versa for that matter). Showing that RHIM forms (like PP-motifs) a prion when expressed in Podospora, stresses we feel the functional similarity between the fungal and animal signaling motifs. The formation of the [Rhim] prions and their propagation in a fungal environment was not a foregone conclusion. It is our experience that not any amyloid sequence will form a prion in Podospora (Aβ, α-syn, etc..) and the reviewer is surely more than aware of the rich literature dealing with the amyloid/prion-relation in yeast models. The Podospora in vivo system might also be of use to others to study RHIM-assembly, for instance to screen for inhibitors of RHIM-assembly. As stated by the reviewer the major novelty is the demonstration of cross-seeding between fungal and human necroptosis pathways which has so far only been suggested on the basis of a sequence similarity on a minute motif of 5-10 amino acids in length. We feel that documenting cross-seeding does strengthen the hypothesis that these motifs are evolutionary related.

      2) Implications of "cross-seeding". The interspecific cross-seeding observed was modest; much lower than that for intraspecific templating between proteins of the same pathway. Specifically, it failed to induce a barrage, the puncta formed at different times, and colocalization was incomplete. More importantly, cross-seeding does not imply functional or evolutionary conservation. Consider the wide range of amyloid proteins that have been reported to cross-seed each other despite in some cases very different sequences, structures, and functions - for example the type-II diabetes peptide IAPP with the Alzheimer's peptide Aβ; the yeast prion protein Rnq1 with human Huntingtin; and the yeast prion Sup35 with human transthyretin. Although a direct comparison with the present data are not possible, these cross-seeding interactions appear comparably robust. The present demonstration of limited cross-seeding therefore seems not to add much additional support for an evolutionary relationship between necroptosis and fungal amyloid cell-death pathways.

      Cross-seeding is partial and not as efficient as in homotypic or intra-kingdom interactions. This is precisely our conclusion (see for instance line 470 to 473 of the original preprint). We point at this partial effect and state that it suggests both some level of structural similarity but also the existence of functionally important structural differences between RHIM and PP-amyloids. These results are in line with the fact that the consensus RHIM and PP-motifs while sharing some common position also markedly differ on others. The specificity of the cross interaction between [π] and [Rhim] prions is also supported by the absence of cross-reaction between [π] and the other Podospora prions (or between [Rhim] and [Het-s]). The same is true for the partial co-localization. These results serve as a functional context that will allow future structural data on the fold of the PP-motif to be meaningfully compared to the RHIM-structure. To insist on the partial nature of this cross-seeding underlying both relation and differences between PP and RHIM, we propose to modify the title of the manuscript to “Partial prion cross-seeding between fungal and mammalian amyloid signaling motifs”.

      The reviewer states : “More importantly, cross-seeding does not imply functional or evolutionary conservation”. Absolutely so. But when two amyloid forming regions show sequence similarity (not just composition bias) and both work as functional amyloid signaling motifs leading to necroptotic cell-death then cross-seeding is a further support (not proof) of evolutionary and functional conservation.

      3) Rigor of the fusion experiments. In all cases, despite having generated and validated the use of RFP- and GFP-labeled proteins, all fusion experiments to examine cell death microscopically (using Evans Blue staining) were between two GFP-expressing strains. This is frustrating because it makes it impossible to know from the images alone which of the two proteins is expressed in which cells, and in which cases of mycelia crossing paths is fusion occurring. I must therefore rely entirely on the labels provided, but they sometimes appear implausible. For example, the lower fusion event demarcated in Fig. 3C left panel would have been expected to allow GFP levels to equilibrate across the point of contact; instead there remains a sharp transition in GFP intensity between the two mycelia (third panel) indicating the cytoplasm is not being shared at the time of the image. In Fig. S8 top row, there is no apparent relationship between cell death and HELLP-GFP; moreover, cell death is seen occurring in mycelia containing either punctate or diffuse GFP-RIP3. While I appreciate that Evans Blue fluorescence may overlap with that of RFP (which should be stated) and preclude its visualization without multispectral imaging capabilities that may not be available to the authors, alternative viability stains and fluorescent proteins could in principle have been used to avoid this problem.

      Evans blue shows fluorescence that does indeed overlap with RFP fluorescence, which is the reason why we used GFP labeled proteins which is indeed less convenient to distinguish strains. But Evans blue staining allow clear and rapid identification of dead cells. Even with both strain labelled with GFP, strains can be identified based on diffuse versus dot-like fluorescence. Moreover, the fusion are observed in contact zone between the two strains under the microscope where the proportion of dead cells (stained cells) is drastically increased compared to the rest of the mycelium, the relative orientation and position of the filaments allows for strain identification. As for the concerns regarding equilibration levels of GFP or HELLP presence in heterokaryotic cells, it could be explained by the fact that necroptotic cell-death due to HELLP toxic effect, as for the others HeLo or HELL domain containing proteins (Seuring et al. 2012, Mathur et al. 2012, Daskalov et al. 2016, Daskalov et al. 2020), is associated with blocking of the septa to limit the spreading of cell-death through the entire mycelium. Fungal incompatibility is associated both with cell death and compartmentation of the mycelium.

      We thank the reviewer to bring to our attention the issues that may be encountered to clearly identify heterokaryotic cells on these images. Therefore, cell death imaging is presented in the new preprint using methylene blue allowing the use of RFP and GFP labeled proteins to identify unequivocally heterokaryotic cells.

      Minor Comments:

      1) The significance of these proteins forming "prions", as opposed to (merely) amyloids, should be articulated. This is important because prion-formation per se is irrelevant to the cell-level functions of the proteins, as nucleation of the amyloid state causes cell death and hence precludes their persistent/heritable propagation. Amyloid by nature is self-perpetuating at the molecular level and hence would seem to explain the properties of the protein. The discussion about possible exaptation of these pathways for allorecognition could be expanded or clarified in this regard.

      These are interesting points. Prion and amyloids are terms with different field of application. The term prion is only meaningful in vivo. We use it preferentially here, because for the most part we document prion propagation and only indirectly amyloid formation. We feel however that it might be premature to conclude that the prion-behaviour is totally irrelevant to the function of these proteins as signaling devices. This all depends (as for other prions) on the actual balance between toxicity and infectivity. It might well be that HELLP propagates part of the amyloid signal before it actually leads to cell death. Please note that even full length HET-S can be observed in certain growth condition in the form of dots and may thus partition between a toxic and an infectious fraction.

      2) Colocalization between two proteins does not imply that one has templated the other to form amyloid, even when both are capable of forming amyloid independently (see https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0611158104 ).

      We fully agree. We have corrected the labelling of the figures that document co-localization that were previously labelled as cross-seeding experiments.

      3) Statements of partial cross-seeding are supported by quantitation (Fig. 8). In contrast, the authors appear to use qualitative observations to support rather definitive statements about the "total absence of" (line 344) of cross-seeding between other pathways.

      Quantitative data are now given regarding the experiment presented line 344. It is true that the statement “total absence of” relates to the absence of detectable cross-seeding in the experimental setting that was use. Here in this specific case, no prion formation of [Het-s] was detected in a total of 18x2x3 infection attempts with [Rhim] prion donor strains (18 transformants for each [Rhim]-type in triplicate).

      4) Fig. S9. "Note that induction of [Rhim] in transformants leads to growth alteration to varying extent ranging from sublethal phenotype to more or less stunted growth." Can the authors suggest an explanation for this heterogeneity? From my limited perspective, it suggests the existence of amyloid polymorphisms (i.e. a prion strain phenomenon), which is quite unexpected given the lack of polymorphism among known functional amyloids in contrast to rampant polymorphism among pathological amyloids. Hence the phenomenon could be interpreted as suggesting that amyloid is not an evolved/functional state for the PP motif. In any case the phenomenon is interesting and merits further discussion.

      Phenotypic variability in this experiment can be explained by variation of expression levels of the transgene and prion curing. Transformation occurs through ectopic integration in these experiments (there are no autonomous plasmids available for Podospora). As a consequence in any given experiment, the transformants will display different copy number and integration sites of the transgene and hence variability in expression level. An additional cause of variety is “escape” a due to counter-selection when strain show self-incompatibility, fungal articles in which the transgene causing incompatibility is mutated or deleted will escape cell-death and resume growth. This is very typical of self-incompatible strains and has been largely documented and used as an experimental tool for mutant selection in Podospora and other filamentous fungi. This phenomenon typically leads to sector formation. Then in the specific case of experiments involving prion proteins in addition to these mechanisms leading to genetic variability, “escape” can also occur through prion curing. If a prion causes self-incompatibility, growth recovery occurs through prion curing (this has been largely studied in the case of the [Het-s]/HET-S interaction). We do not formally exclude the possibility that part of the variability may reflect prion strain formation but other explanations should probably be considered more likely, as indeed we have no evidence for strain formation for any of the wild –type functional prion motifs we have characterized so far in fungi.

      Reviewer #3

      Three distinct amyloid-based cell-death pathways in fungi have been reported. The authors of the current manuscript extend their previous work of the HELLP/SBP/PNT1 pathway in Chaetomium globosum and describe a similar system in P. anserina. It is shown that the amyloid signaling domain of PTN1 can form a prion in cells deleted of HELLP, which is otherwise activated by the prion to cause cell death. Using this artificial system, the authors test whether the related RHIM motif of the human RIP1 and RIP3 protein can also form a prion in P. anserina and whether RHIM amyloids as well as other fungal amyloid-forming motifs can cross-seed PTN1.

      The experiments are well executed and explained but I have a few suggestions:

      1) Amyloid cross seeding is usually assayed in vitro using purified protein fragments. The artificial genetic system used here is certainly clever but the expression level of different proteins needs to be measured for better comparison of cross-seeding efficiencies.

      We feel that the in vivo system presented here has important advantages, in particular is it less “artificial” than in vitro seeding in the sense that at least HELLP is in its native cellular context. Note also that the cross-seeding experiments are done with several distinct transformants which as explained above represent different expression levels of the transgene.

      2) Page 16, line 333-334 and Fig 8: How were recipient strains sampled? How random was it? How many samples?

      We thank the reviewer to bring this to our attention and to address these shortcomings, we added precisions on samples selection and numbers in results and in methods section.

      3) Jargons/abbreviations. Page 19, line 405; Page 20, line 429: What are PAMPs, MAMPs, and PCD?

      These abbreviations have been spelled out.

    1. While Mr. Turners ideas were of honorable intentions, ( so we think) , one could point out the mental status of this persons actions. I also would like to know who taught him the christian ways and who was apart of his upbringing and what their ideals were. As we know many christian's do not spare the rod; to include his enslavement, lack of social interactions, and a huge contributor may have been lack of proper nutrition with can lead to many aliments alone. Now whether his reasons for commiting the mass murders was indeed the voice of a spirit or just a story to cover his tracks , knowing that many at the time were very strong in their christian beliefs and the word of God, this does not excuse the fact that what his did was on his own terms. This story brings light to many current events and how we can study history to prevent future incidents. Thank you for the lesson and thought provoking reading material, Professor.

    1. While Scrum is structured, it is not entirely rigid.

      Being able to adapt, add meetings / remove meetings, or even change process as needed (ie: tiger team for critical fixes) is important.

      Once close to release - we added a "bug triage meeting" just to see if things were actually bugs or not, it served a purpose for a few weeks - then we killed it once it was no longer needed.

      I try to consider "what is the value I am getting out of X" - and how we get there may change over time.

      At Rangle, there was a "Rangle Flow" ---- I don't think any project every fully hit it, but it was a bit of an ideal that we strove for.

      The fact that every project wasn't run exactly the same really bothered some people, but it was something that I enjoyed.

      Different teams have different dynamics - and if something is working really well for one team, doesn't mean the same thing will work for another.

    1. 26 REPRESENTATSON: CULTURAL REPRESENTATIONS AND SIGNIFYING PRACTICESeffects. Bul the meaning depends, not on the material quality of íhe sign, butOH its symboMc fanction. l\s because a particular sound or word standsfor,symbolizes or represente a concepí that it can function, in language, as a signaad convey meaniog - or, as the construcíionists say, signify ísign-i-fy).1.5 The language of traffic lightsThe simples! exampie of this point, which is critical fbr an understanding ofhow languages funetion as represeníational sysíems, is the famous trafficlighis exampíe. A teafüc lighí is a machine which produces differentcoloured lights in sequenee. The effect of light of different wavelengths onthe eye - which is a natural and material phenomenon - produces íhesensation of differeBt colours, ISkrar these things certainly do exist in thematerial world. But ií is our cuitare which breaks íhe speetnim of light iníodifferent colours, disíiaguisaes them from one another and aítaches ñames -Red, Green, Yellow, Blue — to them, We use a way of classifying the colourspectruin ío créate colours which are different from one another. Werepresent or symbolize the different colours and cíassify thein according íodiíferest colour-concepís. This is íhe conceptual colour sysíem of ourculture. We say 'our culture' because, of courses other cultures may divide thecolour speeíram differeníly. Whaí's more, they certainly use diíferent actualwords or letters to ideníify different coiours: what we calí 'red', the French cali'rouge' and so on. This is the linguistíc code - íhe one which correlaíes certainwords (signs) wiíh certain colours (concepís), aad tfaus enables us tocommunicaíe about colours to oíher people, using 'the language of colours'.But how do we use this representational or symbolic sysíem to reguláis theíraffic? Colours do not have any 'true' or fixed meaniag in thaí sense. Reddoes noí mean 'Stop' in nature, any more than Green meansr 'Go'. In othersettings, Red may stand for, syroboíize or represent 'Blood' or 'Danger* or"ConuiíiHíism'; and Green may represent 'Ireland' or *The Countryside' or'Envkonmentalism'. Even these meanings can chasge. In íhe 'language ofelecíric plugs', Red used to mean 'the connection wiíh the positive charge'buí íhis was artóírarily and wiíhout explanation changad ío Browní Buí Lhenfor many years the producers of plugs had to attach a slip of paper teliingpeople that the code or convention had changed, otherwise how would íheyknow? Red and Green work in íhe language of traffic lighis because 'Stop' and'Go* are íhe meanings which have been assigned to them in our culture by thecode OF conventions governing íhis language, and this code is widely knownaad alraost universaíly obeyed in our culture and cultures Mee ours - thoughwe can well imagine oíher cultures which did not possess the code, in whichthis language would be a complete myster

      this example further helps me understand how powerful our associations can be as a society when thinking or naming things. we gave meaning to the colors red and green in the sense that they mean stop and go, because we associated those colors to colors of importance. it's a given now for everyone to think of those signals because we gave it importance, even though colors don't have definite purposes or definitions in itself.

    1. studying to deceive the wisdom of Zeus

      I found this statement very interesting because when we think of Zeus people often associate him as a person who has little to no weakness. Society views him as as someone who cannot be defied. This example of greek mythology shows how he is not as powerful as he may usually be portrayed.

    1. There may be no greater enemy to quality writing than the 5-paragraph essay.

      This alone is a statement each of us should have on our teaching desks. Sometimes we need this, especially when our students need structure in order to pass an exam or begin/jumpstart their writing for school (especially when we get them as high schoolers who have been trained not to write); BUT, a 5 paragraph essay isn't writing nor is it the teaching of writing. We have to think rhetorically and do better by our student writers.

    1. Note: This rebuttal was posted by the corresponding author to Review Commons. Content has not been altered except for formatting.

      Learn more at Review Commons


      Reply to the reviewers

      Point-by-point response to reviewers

      Reviewer #1 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity (Required)): **

      The authors constructed a virtually complete fitness landscape of the P1 extension region (4-base-paired helix) in the group I intron from Tetrahymena thermophila, using a kanamycin resistance reporter to evaluate the fold-change in fitness, which is related to self-splicing activity. This was a clever choice of system because it was known from earlier work that the P1 extension adopts two different conformations during self-splicing. The fitness of each variant was determined from the number of reads acquired from the sequencing data sets and analyzed through an extensive computational pipeline. The strength of the paper is that this machine learning approach can be used to calculate how individual variants contribute to the fitness landscape and assess the directions of epistasis across a large number of identified genotypes.

      We thank the reviewer for highlighting one of the key strengths of our manuscript, the fact that our analytical approach, using SHAP values, enables contributions of individual variants to be assessed in a genotype-specific manner. This approach provides for a sound, robust, and principled way of describing and understanding the fitness impact of one mutation in the context of (potentially many) others.

      The authors argue that machine learning more successfully models subtle effects that arise from interactions between RNA residues, and that the power to analyze deep mutational sequencing experiments can better rationalize fitness constraints arising from multiple conformational states.

      We do indeed argue that machine learning is likely to play an increasing role in making sense of deep mutational scanning data. These scans provide high-resolution information on how fitness maps onto genotype, but the molecular underpinnings of this relationship often remain obscure. It is these “hidden” underpinnings, including the effects of specific mutations on RNA/protein folding, structures, and dynamics, that machine learning approaches can help elucidate.

      The results are mostly consistent with previous studies even though the authors collected the data in a more advanced and complicated way. They are also able to rationalize complex phenotypes - for example, the observed fitness defects are more prevalent under an unfavorable growth condition (30ºC), because the lower temperature hinders conformational exchange. Although such cold sensitive effects are well known in RNA, it is gratifying that this can be captured in the fitness landscape.

      Finding temperature-related fitness effects that are consistent with impaired conformational exchange was also gratifying for us and we thank the reviewer for highlighting this finding.

      The results would be more convincing if the authors directly measure the self-splicing activity of a few key variants, such as the C2C21 mutant, to determine whether these mutations alter the self-splicing mechanism of the Tte-119(C20A) master sequence in the way that they infer from their model. In interpreting their results, they may want to consider misfolding of the intron core (coupled to base pairing of P1) and reverse self-splicing. Reversibility in the hairpin ribozyme, for example, turned out to be the key for understanding the effects of certain mutations.

      We appreciate that measurements of splicing activity for individual genotypes would complement and further strengthen our study. We will therefore aim to construct strains for a few key genotypes and assay self-splicing activity using RT-qPCR – an approach we previously used successfully to monitor splicing kinetics of self-splicing introns in yeast mitochondria (see Rudan et al. 2018 eLife 7:e35330). Specifically, we will quantify the fraction of spliced and unspliced transcripts using primers that span the exon-exon and the 3’ exon-intron junction, respectively (the 5’ intron-exon junction is genotypically diverse and would require genotype-specific primers). This will be done under non-selective (-kan) conditions, where the relative fraction of spliced and unspliced transcripts is a function of intrinsic splicing ability and not confounded by selection. We aim to include the master sequence, C2C21, G3C20 and its mirror genotype C3G20, U3 (which restores perfect complementarity in the master sequence), and G5 (inferred from the high-throughput experiment to make a strong negative contribution to fitness).

      In interpreting our results, we will consider different mechanisms of splicing failure, such as kinetic problems (slow dissociation of P1ex), misfolding of the intron core, reverse self-splicing, and the use of cryptic splice sites, which has previously been documented (see e.g. Woodson & Cech 1991 Biochemistry 30:2042-2050). We note, however that a precise mechanistic dissection of the splicing defects of individual variants is not the purpose of this manuscript and we therefore do not aim to establish genotype-specific defects in great molecular detail.

      Related to the point above, interesting conclusions regarding the relationships between base identity and epistasis that arise from metastability should be strengthened with additional examples. For example, the authors can explain why a reverse base-pairing variant (C3G20) exhibits negative epistasis but is not similar to that of the G3C20 construct. This would ideally use the data from the screen but also be validated by checking the self-splicing activity of a few individuals at low and high temperature.

      In measuring splicing activity and its link to fitness for a subset of key variants (see point #4), we will include at least one mirror example such as C3G20/G3C20. In addition, we will highlight additional examples of this mirror asymmetry based on the results from our high-throughput screen.

      They should validate the screen by showing that kanamycin resistance does indeed correlate strictly with self-splicing activity, and not some other feature such as RNA turnover. (It would also not be a bad idea to check this in the cell, which can be done by primer extension or Northern blotting.)

      This question (i.e. whether altered RNA stability rather than splicing efficiency explains differential KNT production and ultimately fitness) has previously been addressed by Guo & Cech (2002) when introducing the knt+intron reporter system. These authors found no difference in mRNA stability in constructs that displayed differential kanamycin resistance. To shore up this conclusion further, we will measure fitness (via colony counts, growth rate or more directly through competitive fitness assays) of the key variants for which we determine splicing activity (see point #4) and then correlate splicing and fitness.

      The benefit of the machine learning model is that it can extract signals that may be hard to detect otherwise. The downside is that it doesn't produce a physical model, as far as I am aware. The parameters are themselves not meaningful - except to the degree that trends in the fitness estimates can be explained after the fact. This is something that should ideally be explained more directly in the manuscript.

      The reviewer raises an interesting point, that indeed deserves further discussion/explanation. The reviewer is right that, at first sight, high-resolution fitness landscapes like ours do not directly produce a physical (structural) model of the molecule under investigation. They connect genotype and fitness, but the molecular intermediate – a biophysical structure – is not explicit. However, over the last few years, it has become apparent that deep mutational scanning experiments can – both in principle and in practice – yield information that can be leveraged to infer such a physical model. In short, covariation in fitness between residues in a protein or bases in an RNA can be used as inputs for constraint-based modelling of physical interactions. Notably, Schmiedel & Lehner (2019, Nature Genetics 51: 1177-1186) recently demonstrated that deep mutational scanning data can be used in this manner to reconstruct secondary and tertiary protein structure with high accuracy. In principle, the same approach can be used to reconstruct RNA structures. This will require more extensive, molecule-wide fitness data, but our study points towards just this future, even for data collected from structural ensembles.

      When we stated in the original manuscript that deconvolution of the fitness landscape might help to reverse engineer structures, this ability to interpolate between genotype and fitness to reveal hidden biophysical/structural relationships is what we refer to. We will revise the manuscript to make this connection more explicit.

      The authors claim that by evaluating a large number of sequences at two conditions, they can capture variants with intermediate phenotypes (Fig. 1). This is not necessarily true. If the original screen allows only the most active variants to survive on kan+ medium, then the signature of intermediate phenotypes may not be encoded in the original data, and thus not retrievable even with sophisticated algorithms, which may also be prone to overfitting. At what limit of stringency will the screen fail to yield information about intermediate fitness? How deeply must one sequence to recover this information, especially if noisy or degraded? Some discussion of these effects would be helpful.

      The capacity of any high-throughput sequencing-based DMS experiment to resolve intermediate phenotypes does indeed depend on a number of things. The reviewer highlights two of these: First, in screens where the phenotype is not binary (dead/alive) but fitness can be measured on a continuous scale, can we – and do we – capture phenotypes with intermediate fitness? What if only the fittest/most active variants survive? This is, ultimately, an empirical question, and one we can answer quite definitively: we do observe a large range of intermediate phenotypes, which – in our study – correspond to intermediate fold-change values. For each genotype, we can provide confidence limits and assess statistical significance. Table S1 provides this information. Our capacity to resolve these intermediate phenotypes is mainly based on three things. One is adequate sequencing depth, as highlighted by the reviewer. The second is the number of biological replicates (N=6) we analyse, which allows us to differentiate biological variability from noise for a large number of genotypes. This is an important aspect of DMS experiments that has often been overlooked (i.e. there are many other studies where only a single replicate is analysed and biological heterogeneity is not taken into account). With six replicates in hand, we can directly estimate variability (as done e.g. in our DESeq2 analysis) and quantify uncertainty so as to guard against overfitting. In our view, this is arguably more important than sequencing depth in deriving appropriate fitness estimates. Finally, we can resolve intermediate phenotypes because we keep the time lag between initial exposure to kanamycin and assaying genotype frequencies relatively short (overnight growth, see Methods). Our experiment is effectively a multi-genotype competition experiment, and we provide a snapshot across the genotype pool at a given time. If we had measured after several days of culture, genotypes with greater relative fitness would have spread further through the population, at the cost of less fit genotypes, many of which would likely have been eliminated. We kept measurement lag relatively short on purpose so that we could see a clear differential response to kanamycin while still being able to catch more than just a handful of the very fittest genotypes.

      In light of the above, it will be apparent that there are no simple answers to the reviewer’s questions about required sequencing depth, levels of stringency, etc. The ability to assign differential fitness across a large population of genotypes hinges on multiple interrelated considerations (sequencing depth, complexity of the final & starting pool, number of replicates). In revising the manuscript, we will highlight some of the key considerations just discussed, bearing in mind that the manuscript cannot possibly discuss all possible pitfalls and requirements of deep mutational scanning experiments in great detail.

      Lastly, the evolvability of RNA is fascinating and there is much to learn. However, the authors don't discuss the implications of their findings for molecular evolution although they throw the term around. It would be exciting if there is a trend in the fitness landscape that could help explain the trajectory of RNA evolution in nature.

      We agree with the reviewer that it would be exciting to link deep mutational scanning results more closely with observable patterns of RNA evolution. This is true both in relation to evolution of P1ex/group I introns specifically and evolution of dynamic RNA structures more generally. Regarding the latter, we note that selection against excess stability has previously been inferred for 5’ UTRs (see e.g. Gu et al. 2010 PLoS Comp Biol 6: e1000664), although our case is slightly different in that a helix still needs to form but be sufficiently unstable to enable swift dissociation. We also note that riboswitches might make for an excellent subject to study asymmetric constraint and selection against excess stability as they involve formation of competing helices (including participation of some but not all nucleotides in more than one helix), their structure/function is well understood, and many examples are known, providing opportunities for evolutionary analysis. We consider this outside the scope of the current study. We will, however, seek to analyse patterns of evolution in P1ex to establish whether they correspond in a meaningful way to the fitness trends we observe in the laboratory. To do so, we will analyse the distribution and evolutionary history of variants across orthologous introns in different Tetrahymena species/strains, with a focus on P1ex, P10 and the surrounding sequence. Fortunately for us, the 23S ribosomal RNA gene in which the intron is embedded has been used as a phylogenetic marker so that intron/exon sequence information is available for a reasonable number of species/strains (see Doerder 2018 J Eukaryot Microbiol 66:182-208). We will generate an alignment of these sequences and ask, for example, whether N2-N5 are subject to different constraints than N18-N21 mirroring our experimental findings. We have previously successfully quantified patterns of variation surrounding self-splicing introns in yeast mitochondria (Repar & Warnecke 2017 Genetics 205:1641-1648). Note here that extending this analysis beyond Tetrahymena is problematic. Specifically, the intron is absent from close relatives of Tetrahymena (Doerder 2018 J Eukaryot Microbiol 66:182-208) and P1-proximal structures of distant relatives are quite variable. In addition, we are looking at intronic regions that are not only adjacent to but also directly interact with exonic sequence. The exonic context in which the intron is embedded therefore matters but will be quite different for more distant group I introns. We therefore think that aligning and comparing distant orthologs has limited merit.

      The authors use the abbreviation DMS for deep mutational scanning; the RNA structure field uses the reagent dimethylsulfate that is also abbreviated DMS. They may want to choose a different acronym or just avoid an acronym altogether.

      We appreciate this point about false-friend acronyms. We will either find a different acronym or avoid it altogether.

      Reviewer #1 (Significance (Required)):

      As the importance of RNA structure for gene expression becomes more widely appreciated, interest in understanding the evolution of RNA structures is also increasing. Compared with the molecular evolution of proteins, evolution and fitness in RNA is far less understood, although the authors appropriately point to a number of recent studies on this topic. The main advance here is to use machine learning methods to analyze the results of a large genotypic screen, with the goal of more accurately capturing the fitness effects of sequences at varied distances from the parental sequence. The specific conclusions reached here such as the importance of metastability or the prominence of cold sensitive effects are not revolutionary, but the authors illustrate how such phenomena can be investigated more systematically and in more depth.

      We thank the reviewer for highlighting that our analytical approach showcases how deep mutational scanning data can be analysed in an unbiased and systematic manner to better understand the relationship between genotype, molecular phenotype (e.g. structure), and fitness. The reviewer also rightly points to specific results we obtain regarding temperature-related effects and metastability of P1ex/P10. However, we believe that the most important contribution of this work is a more general one, namely our proof-of-principle demonstration that deep mutational scanning data can capture multiple conformational states simultaneously, and that these states can be deconvoluted from a single fitness landscape to attribute the fitness impact of individual mutations to specific RNA conformations. To our knowledge this had not been explicitly demonstrated before and our work provides an important cornerstone for future studies looking to interpret mutational effects in either RNAs or proteins in the light of dynamic structures.

      In light of comments by reviewer #2 below, it is worth reiterating the proof-of-principle nature of this study. Many of the specific results we obtain (e.g. importance of avoiding excess stability in P1ex) are not revolutionary. Indeed, we would be worried if they were. We chose to investigate P1ex because substantial prior work exists that has furnished us with solid positive controls. This independent prior validation allows us to both have great confidence in the data we generate and demonstrate cogently that the two conformational states at the beginning and end of the splicing reaction are captured in the data.

      Finally, we believe our work, in covering a virtually complete genotype space, using multiple replicates to quantify uncertainty in fitness estimates, and using SHAP scores to interpret variant effects in genotype-specific context, sets a new high bar for this type of study and will provide valuable reference data and analytical recipes for future analyses. **

      Reviewer #2 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity (Required)):

      The manuscript by Soo et al probes the effect of mutations on the fitness of the Tetrahymena Group I self-splicing intron. They used high-throughput sequencing to simultaneously identify the effect of every possible sequence in a 4-bp helix. The approach is sound and the conclusions are generally supported. However, the analysis seems overly complicated given the dataset. Both the analysis and the accompanying writing make it difficult to understand what seems to be a fairly clear conclusion - that the relative stabilities of two alternative RNA helices are important for splicing.

      We thank the reviewer for testifying to the validity of our approach and the soundness of our conclusions. Regarding the complexity of the analysis, the reviewer is right in that – for the conclusion that the relative stabilities of two alternative helices are important for fitness – a simpler analysis would have sufficed. However, as elaborated in response to point #11 above, our objective here is not merely to draw specific conclusions about the relative stabilities of P1ex and P10, but more general: a) to demonstrate that a single fitness landscape can be deconvoluted to implicate multiple conformations in fitness defects and b) to provide a basic but powerful recipe for doing so in an unbiased, systematic manner using machine learning.

      We will strive to make the writing clearer so that readers can follow this reasoning and appreciate our analytical choices.

      • **Major Comments** *

      The authors state that this method can identify the impact of transient conformational states. However, the two conformational states in this study are not transient - in fact they are associated with two distinct chemical steps of splicing and are quite stable. It may be that the effect of important transient states would be observed, but this study does not demonstrate that.

      We used the word “transient” to describe two alternative RNA structures formed during the life cycle of the intron. Both states (characterized by P1ex and P10 formation) are transient in as much as they disappear as splicing proceeds. In retrospect, we agree with the reviewer that this usage is too loose (given how the term is generally used in the literature) and might evoke the wrong connotations. We will therefore revise the manuscript to eliminate references to P1ex and P10 as transient states, but rather describe them as alternative conformations. Of course, the general point remains true: that deep mutational scanning data should in principle capture all fitness-relevant structural states even if these are transient (in the strict sense of the word).

      "Fitness" ends up being on an arbitrary scale, which impairs some analysis. A similar high-throughput sequencing pipeline could have been used to directly monitor splicing of every mutant, though at this point that is outside the scope of this study. Even with the arbitrary units, it would be clearer if more time were spent comparing fitness to base-pair stability on an individual basis, rather than the broad analyses. (See minor comments for details.)

      The reviewer is right in saying that a high-throughput pipeline could have been designed to monitor splicing of each genotype directly (rather than assaying fitness of the cell population that represents a particular genotype).We chose not to do so. One reason for this is that monitoring splicing directly would have necessitated design of a more complicated assay. This is because, to monitor splicing efficiency, one would have to monitor both pre-mRNA and mRNA for different genotypes. The former is straightforward (using primers that span the exon-intron junction) but the latter is not: successful splicing removes the genotype-specific information from the mRNA (that information being solely encoded in the intron). This a solvable problem in principle. One might, for example, introduce barcodes of sufficient complexity in the mRNA that can be linked back to the intron genotype, but doing so would have introduced a further source of error and complicated analysis. We therefore opted for monitoring genotypic fitness by sequencing the plasmids from which the RNAs originate. This does mean that our measurements of fitness are not coupled to a specific molecular phenotype (such as splicing efficiency) – we presume (but are not entirely sure) this is what the reviewer refers to when talking about fitness being on an “arbitrary scale”. However, fitness derived in this manner has the advantage of providing information that does not start from a mechanistic preconception. We ask how variant affects survival and reproduction of the cell without presuming specific mechanism and the results can therefore capture any mechanism, including those that we did not consider initially. The challenge then becomes to tease out possibly multiple mechanisms from unbiased data.

      We will tackle the reviewer’s final comment, regarding analysis of base-pair stability, below in response to one of the minor comments (point #20).

      \*Minor Comments** *

      The sentence in the abstract beginning "Using an in vivo report system..." is very difficult to comprehend. This is due both to the length of the sentence and the word usage. The final sentence of the abstract is similarly difficult. In general, the writing overemphasizes complexity at the cost of clarity.

      We will revise the entire manuscript to make the writing both clearer and more concise.

      Analysis of results in terms of "epistasis" obscures what could be a straightforward observation. This is the same as saying that mutants are not independent, or that their energetic costs are not additive. This follows obviously from the observation that the nucleotides being mutated are base-paired.

      Making explicit reference to “epistasis” is a considered choice. Framing results in terms of epistasis might be less familiar to readers grounded in RNA or protein biophysics/biochemistry, but is very much at the heart of thinking about the genotype-phenotype relationship from an evolutionary perspective, where global descriptions of epistasis are commonplace and usually provide the starting point for thinking about genotype-phenotype relationships, evolution and evolvability. So what seems unnecessarily obscure when seen through the lens of one field, is natural when considered in the context of another. Importantly, it is also the central approach adopted by many if not most prior deep mutational scanning studies (see e.g. Hayden et al. 2011; Pressman et al. 2019; Zhang et al. 2009; Li et al. 2016; Puchta et al. 2016; Domingo et al. 2018; Li and Zhang 2018; Weinreich et al. 2013; Lalić and Elena 2015; Bendixsen et al. 2017 as cited on page 3 of the manuscript) so we think this framing is helpful to compare our results to prior work.

      We expect that the readership will include many researchers interest in mapping genotype-phenotype-fitness relationships who will expect to see global analyses and descriptors of the type we present. We will, however, revise the manuscript to ensure that our description of the findings remains accessible to readers from other fields.

      More specifically, we also note that the fact that mutations are not independent (i.e. epistasis exists) might be trivial from the fact that P1ex is a base-paired helix. The magnitude and direction (“sign”) of epistasis, however, are not. In fact, as we describe, contrary to prior DMS on RNA helices, we find a lot of positive epistasis, reflecting, as we argue, selection against excess stability of P1ex to allow subsequent formation of P10.

      The novel information is the sensitivity of fitness to base pairing. This is best shown in an analysis like Figure 3A (see below), not broad measures of epistasis.

      Please see responses to points #11, #12, and #16 above for an elaboration of what we consider to be the main merits of this study and why providing broad measures of epistasis is a sensible choice.

      Figure 1C isn't necessary for the reader to understand the process.

      We are happy to follow editorial guidance as to whether this panel is superfluous and should be removed or is worth including.

      It is unclear what figure 2C is showing. It appears that the replicates are similar to each other, that 30 deg C and 37 deg C are also similar, but that +/- Kan are different. This probably doesn't need a figure in the main text.

      This figure does indeed capture what the reviewer describes: genotype pools in +/-kan are least similar to each other, while 30/37ºC are similar but distinct in the +kan condition and effectively indistinguishable in the -kan condition, in line with expectations. We agree with the reviewer that this information per se is something that would typically be found in a supplementary figure. However, we would advocate for retention of this panel in the main manuscript in this instance because of the way in which it was derived: using the Bray-Curtis dissimilarity index. To our knowledge, this is the first time that Bray-Curtis dissimilarity has been used to quantify, in a principled way, the similarity between genotype pools. Borrowed from the ecology literature, the index captures both richness (number of different species/genotypes in the ecosystem/genotype pool) and relative abundance to provide an integrated measure of genotype diversity. We believe that this measure will be useful for future studies and rather than relegating the figure to the supplement, we would aim to briefly highlight its methodological novelty. *

      *

      Figure 3A could be the most informative part of the manuscript. However, predicted minimum free energy should be on the x-axis as the independent variable. The expectation then is that you would see a peak in fitness at some free energy, with fitness falling off both with increased and decreased stability. Furthermore, there should be more analysis along these lines. The authors should calculate helical stability for both P1ex and P10 for every mutant and compare with fitness. Mutations which affect both could also be separated out. Figure 4C comes the closest to this but views it only in terms of GC pairs; there is no reason not to quantify the energetic effects given that predictions of stability for helices is quite good. Deviations from a model invoking only helical stabilities would indicate another factor is involved (alternative base-pairing or tertiary structure, for example).

      We agree with the reviewer that the axes in Figure 3A should be flipped and we will do so in the revised manuscript. We also agree that, when it comes to helical stability of P1ex, the simple expectation would be to see a peak at a certain stability with drop-offs either side, as intimated by Figure 4C. We further agree with the reviewer that Figure 4C is rather indirect and can be made more quantitative by considering helical stability across all genotypes directly. To this end, we will use one of the many tools available that allow prediction of helical stability from primary sequence (e.g. the enf2 function in RNAStructure, as used by Torgerson et al 2018 RNA, see point #24 below) and replace Figure 4C with a more quantitative fitness landscape based on these computations. To provide added confidence in the computations of helical stabilities from primary sequence in the context of our structure, we will also calculate helical stabilities from molecular dynamics simulations for the subset of genotypes we considered previously (Figure 4E/F) and see how inferred stabilities compare.

      There appears to be a missing verb in the legend for figure 3A, second sentence.

      We will fix this error.

      Figure S5 appears to be redundant with Figure 1.

      At first glance, Figure S5 does indeed appear redundant with Figure 1 but it is not. Figure S5 shows the relevant sequence of the group I intron and bordering exons in its native context, i.e. when embedded in the 23S ribosomal RNA gene of Tetrahymena thermophila, whereas Figure 1 shows the genotype of the mutant intron embedded in knt. The sequences are different. We will revise the legend to Figure S5 to make this clearer.

      Figure S6 is a better analysis than what appears in the main text, and could be expanded to all base pairs.

      We will expand Figure S6 to include all base pairs as suggested. We disagree that this is a better analysis compared to what appears in the main text. Rather, it provides a complementary, hypothesis-driven view whereas the analysis in the main text is more systematic and unbiased in approach. *

      *

      Reviewer #2 (Significance (Required)):

      This manuscript largely focuses on the technical approach. The shift in analytic strategy described above would increase the conceptual impact. The conclusions are consistent with and fit in with recent uses of high-throughput sequencing to study RNA systems. For example Pitt & Ferré-D'Amaré, Science (2010) and Kobari et al, NAR (2015) describe fitness landscapes of the ligase and HDV ribozymes, respectively. Torgerson et al RNA (2018) make similar measurements on the glycine riboswitch, including a treatment of relative helix stability for two mutually exclusive conformations. The overall results are of interest to researchers in the field of noncoding RNA.

      We thank the reviewer for highlighting the paper by Torgerson et al, of which – embarrassingly – we were not aware. We will make reference to this paper in a revised manuscript and highlight that riboswitches might be a good model system to further explore asymmetric constraint and selection against excess stability in an evolutionary context (also see our response to point #9 above).

      As highlighted earlier, we think the main conceptual impact of our work lies not in the description of helical stabilities. Rather, it lies in a) providing a rigorous proof-of-principle that deep mutational scanning can capture multiple conformational states simultaneously, and b) that, using an unbiased machine learning approach, these states can be deconvoluted from a single fitness landscape to attribute the fitness impact of individual mutations to specific RNA conformations. A shift in analytical strategy to “cut to the chase” and narrowly focus on helical stability would be misguided in this context, as we seek to provide not only insights into the data at hand but also lay out a sound and general recipe for analysing similar datasets in the future.

  6. Sep 2020
    1. There will always be times in history where a movement can be polluted by outside issues. I think of this as similar to the visions seen by Joseph Smith. The second Great awakening was a moment in time where we can look back and see some positive things but then realize that there may have been a false profit that skewed the message. The same could be said for Nat Turner. I think this is an example of mental illness and the grandiose ideas that can accompany certain diagnosis. It is unfortunate that things like this can cast a shadow onto the good.

    2. It's unfortunate that so many men, women, and children were murdered during this rebellion. However, it is quite common for all living things to become casualties of war when there is an act of "war". I don't agree fully with Nat Turner murdering so many individuals. Nevertheless, he may have seen his rebellion as an act of God because of the way he was treated from birth. Sometimes you have to put yourself in another's shoes to understand why they say what they say and to understand their actions.

      Murder is condemned in the Bible but Nat felt God called upon him to rid the world of it's evils which so happened to be the many he killed. I think if the shoe were on the other foot and it had been us in present day in his shoes we may have done the same thing. The lengths an individual will go to for freedom can come in many forms when you have been mistreated and beaten and ripped of your dignity. Just as we experience psychological events now that cause trauma. We have to acknowledged the trauma he endured and his ancestors endured which led to his breaking point along with many other's who participated in the rebellion.

      Again, I'm not saying murder is okay and was the answer but I cannot make judgement on the situation when I did not live during his time, nor do I know what it is like to be stripped of your freedom and dignity. I cannot say my actions would have been of anything less than his and the others which is sad to say but truthful. All he had was his faith in God and he felt God chose him to lead and make a statement of his rebellion.

    3. As I was reading Nat Turner’s confession, I found myself horrified and deeply moved. I could not stop reading his statement, even though it was terrifying to read. What I found the most troubling is that Nat uses verses from the Bible and saying he heard from God to justify his violent behavior. Nowadays, I am afraid if someone did as Nat had done, we might label them as psychotic. While any murder is significant, I think it must be addressed that there were over fifty men, women, and children killed by Turner and his followers. This was not one or two lives that were murdered but over fifty lives. This was a massive massacre. Turner’s reputation of being able to hear from God aided in his credibility and made others more willing to follow his lead. When he talks with the attorney, Thomas Ruffin Gray, he openly admits that God made him do it (the murders). He was not doing what he himself wanted to do but what God wanted him to do. What a terrifying thought to justify one’s actions by claiming they did it because God had told them to. Another incredible thing to consider is that Nat Turner knew there would be consequences for his actions. He was a slave who was bold and brave enough to step up and take a stand against slaveholders. While most of us may not agree with the actions taken my Turner and his followers, I do not know how we can deny where their aggression stemmed from.

    1. They have improved his food, his clothing, his shelter; they have increased his security and released him partly from the bondage of bare existence.

      Humans are only at the top of the food chain because of their ability to make and manipulate technology - homo faber

    2. Consider a future device …  in which an individual stores all his books, records, and communications, and which is mechanized so that it may be consulted with exceeding speed and flexibility. It is an enlarged intimate supplement to his memory

      Sounds like IBM's Watson

    1. Note: This rebuttal was posted by the corresponding author to Review Commons. Content has not been altered except for formatting.

      Learn more at Review Commons


      Reply to the reviewers

      Dear reviewers,

      Thank you very much for your constructive and helpful remarks and suggestions!

      We marked the changes in the manuscript in yellow.

      Our replies to the specific points:

      Reviewer #1 In the Introduction the authors need to cite earlier work in Chlamydomonas which first showed that binding of specific proteins to the psbA 5'UTR is correlated with increased translation in the light (Danon et al. 1991).

      As suggested, we added the reference to the introduction.

      Reviewer #1 The paper could be improved by testing for protein binding to the footprint region in high vs low light. An obvious candidate is HCF173.

      We agree that HCF173 is an obvious candidate, although its interaction could be mediated via additional proteins. Alice Barkan’s group has demonstrated that in maize HCF173 binds to the same region upstream of the translation initiation region (McDermott et al., 2019) where we detected a footprint (Supplemental Figure S11A-D). Furthermore, McDermott et al showed that the binding sequence is conserved. We would like to analyze this question in more detail, but we have currently in the lab no approach available to specifically isolate psbA mRNA with its bound proteins for this analysis and therefore have to postpone the answer to this question to future studies.

      Reviewer #2: \*Important changes to make before full submission:** 1)It is becoming clear that the translation efficiency (TE) is often not a calculation of translational output from specific mRNAs but in fact is better to be described as ribosome association. There can be many reasons for increased ribosome association including ribosome stalling and increased translational engagement. It would be good for the authors to add a simple Western blot to demonstrate directly increased protein output from psbA during high light as compared to low light treatments. This figure could be added to Figure S1.*

      We want to stress that we have chosen a condition that is well known to increase psbA translation in higher plants as shown in the literature with different methods (e.g. Chotewutmontri and Barkan, 2018; Schuster et al., 2020). The protein encoded by psbA, the D1 subunit of photosystem II, has an increased turnover in high light, i.e. a higher amount of D1 has to be produced to compensate for the increased degradation of photodamaged D1 (Mulo et al., 2012; Li et al., 2018).

      Although there is a lot of evidence in the literature for good correlation of translation efficiency as determined by ribosome profiling and protein synthesis, the reviewer raised a valid concern. Ribosome pausing or even ribosome stalling could also cause increased ribosome binding and thereby increased amounts of ribosome footprints. Therefore, we analyzed ribosome pausing in selected genes including psbA and rbcL. The pattern of ribosome pausing was very similar in low and high light (new Supplemental Figure 14), which rules out any ribosome stalling at specific sites or drastic changes in ribosome pausing. To analyze if there is increased ribosome pausing, we determined the fraction of footprints at pause sites compared to the total number of footprints. We used two different pause scores as cutoffs to determine pause sites. To include as many pausing events as possible, we used a pause score of 1, i.e. everything higher than the mean ribosome density per nucleotide of the corresponding coding region (Gawronski et al., 2018). This fraction was unaltered in low and high light (new Supplemental Figure 14). With a more stringent pause score of 20 (20 times higher ribosome density than the mean), an increase of ribsome pausing in high light was detected for psbA, whereas we did not find differences between high and low light for rbcL and psaA. However, this increase in pausing at the psbA mRNA is insufficient to explain the increase in the total amounts of ribosome footprints. Additional pause scores were tested, the value for the psbA fraction with a pause score of 20 included in Supplemental Figure S14 showed the largest difference.

      Reviewer #2: \*Strongly suggested additions to the manuscript to improve its significance before publication** 1)Identifying the RNA-binding protein(s) (likey HCF173 which may be in a complex with other proteins) that interacts with the 5' UTR of psbA in a highlight dependent manner would increase the significance of this study. Finding that this protein binds to other plastid transcripts with weak Shine-Delgarno sequences would also be a nice addition to this study.*

      See comment to reviewer 1. McDermott et al. (2019) describe HCF173 as relatively specific for psbA. Therefore, we do not assume that other genes with weak Shine-Dalgarno sequences are regulated via HCF173 but via different proteins using a similar molecular mechanism to influence the mRNA secondary structure at the translation initiation region.

      Reviewer #2: \*Strongly suggested additions to the manuscript to improve its significance before publication** 2)Mutational analysis of the RBP binding site and also to change the secondary structure around the start codon based on the new structure maps to show the effects of these various changes on protein output would really provide important new findings on how important the RBP being as compared to the RNA secondary structure changes are for regulating protein output form psbA. It could also allow the demonstration of the dependence or independence of these two features on regulating translation from chloroplast mRNAs.*

      We agree with the reviewer that this would be a very interesting study. Unfortunately, it requires a larger collection of lines with mutated psbA sequences. Plastid transformation in Arabidopsis thaliana is still technically demanding and time consuming. Even in the case of Nicotiana tabacum, for which plastid transformation is well established, such a project would likely need several years. We therefore think that such a study is beyond the scope of the current manuscript.

      Reviewer #3 1.In this paper, author mentioned that DMS can modify four nucleotides under alkaline conditions. Because the chloroplast is slightly alkaline, the authors use DMS reactivity from 4 nucleotides to model RNA secondary structure. Based on Kevin Weeks' s paper, it shows that in cell-free condition, DMS has very limited ability to modify single-stranded G and U compared to A and C (Anthony M. Mustoe et al., 2019, PNAS 116: 24574. fig. 1B). In Lars B. Scharff' paper which is cited by the author, it is also mentioned that A and C is more reliable to model RNA secondary structure. The authors might need to calculate the correlation the DMS data and known RNA structure using G/U or all four nucleotides to show that DMS reactivity from G and U is also reliable to be used. Also in Fig. S3B, the reproducibility of G/U between replicates is not as good as A/C. I don' t think G and U can be used to predict RSS.

      We agree with the reviewer that DMS reactivities at G/U are less reliable than those at A/C. This was shown by Mustoe et al. (2019) and by us for chloroplast rRNAs (Gawronski et al., 2020, Plants). We included a correlation of the known 16S rRNA secondary structure and the DMS reactivities at the different nucleotides (Supplemental Figure S5A) that demonstrates that the DMS reactivities at G/U actually contain information about rRNA secondary structure. This analysis demonstrated again that the reactivities at G/U are less reliable than at A/C. Therefore, we added an analysis of the more reliable A/C for comparison with the results for all four nucleotides (Figure 1D-F, 3C-F).

      Reviewer #3 2.Is the 5'UTR the only region which has RSS change? If not, how do RSS changes in other region contribute to translation?

      Translation initiation in plastids is mainly influenced by the secondary structure of the translation initiation region, especially at the cis-elements required for the recognition of the start codon. In addition, we have analyzed different other regions, e.g. the coding regions, the coding regions without the sequences next to the start codon, the end of the coding region, and the complete 5’ UTR (Supplemental Figure S14). We added a more detailed analysis of the changes of secondary structure of the coding region of those genes we focus on (Supplemental Figure S16). This shows that the secondary structure changes of the complete coding region correlate negatively with translation efficiency (see also Supplemental Figure S14G). A similar observation was made in E. coli and explained to be caused by differences in translation initiation, which are mainly influenced by the secondary structure of the translation initiation region (Mustoe et al., 2018).

      Reviewer #3 3.In Fig. 2A and 2B, the DMS reactivities seem very similar under low light and high light. Why did the authors obtain significantly different RNA secondary structure? Are the parameter of low light and high light the same when modelling RNA structure?

      The parameters for the RNA secondary structure predictions in Figure 2 are not identical (see Figure legend). For all structure predictions, the DMS reactivities were used as constrains, but only for the high light structure the sequence of the RNA binding protein’s footprint was forced to be single-stranded. These structure predictions are included to illustrate the mRNA structures in the presence and absence of an RNA binding protein. These structures are based on the observation that the two halves of the stem loop structure have different DMS reactivities in response to high light. The sequence including the protein footprint has lower DMS reactivities in both low and high light. This is in agreement with both a double-stranded sequence as well as a protein-bound sequence. In contrast, the other half of the stem loop, the sequence including the cis-elements of the translation initiation region, has increased DMS reactivities in high light, indicating that it is single-stranded. This suggests that there is protein binding in high light preventing the formation of the inhibitory stem loop.

      Reviewer #3 4.In Fig. S12, the correlationship between HL and LL in ribo-seq and RNAseq is high, which means no significant changes upon light change. In this paper, psbA should have translation change under high light conditions. I suggest the authors to label the dot representing psbA.

      Thank you very much for this suggestion! We marked psbA in the correlation plots (Supplemental Figure 12). The changes in the transcript levels are really minor, whereas for some genes the translation efficiency changes (see Figure 4 and Supplemental Figure S13).

      Reviewer #3 5.I suggest to use plants at the same stage for DMS-MaPseq and SHAPE probing.

      The different plant material was chosen because of the different requirements during probing. In this context, we would like to point out that observing the same changes in the translation initiation region in response to high light in different developmental stages is a stronger confirmation than observing the same response at the same developmental stage. This indicates that the response is not specific for a developmental stage.

      Reviewer #3 6.In Huang's paper (Jianyan Huang et al., 2019, Cell Reports 29: 4186-4199), there are many differential express genes under high light for 0.5hr. However, in the RNAseq data here, the correlation between high light and low light conditions is very high (Fig. S12). Why? Also, it would be nice if the authors could label several DEG whose expression change under high light treatment in Fig. S12?

      Supplemental Figure S12 contains only plastid-encoded RNAs, whereas Huang et al. (2019) focused on nuclear-encoded mRNAs. We clarified the figure legend of Supplemental Figure S12 by adding “of the plastid-encoded genes”. The values for the individual genes can be seen in Supplemental Figure S13.

      Reviewer #3 7.For the MNase footprint method, is the as-SD region the only region show enrichment under high light conditions? Besides, please provide the detailed method of MNase footprint. Does it work for RNA footprinting?

      The used methods are described under “Ribosome profiling (Ribo-seq)” and “Processing of Ribo-seq and RNA-seq reads” in Material and Methods. The approach was very similar to the one used for ribosome profiling with the difference that also smaller read lengths were included in the analysis (18-40 nt instead of 28-40 nt). We did this, because many plastid RNA binding proteins have footprints that are smaller than a ribosomal footprint. The described footprint is the only one detected near the translation initiation region of psbA. Binding of HCF173 was detected by the Barkan group in the same region using a RIP-Seq Analysis combined with RNase I digestion (McDermott et al., 2019), which confirms that our approach is working. We added a reference to the method section in the results part to clarify which approach was chosen.

    1. Reviewer #1

      Thank you for inviting me to review this manuscript by Paquola and colleagues, in which the authors used a combination of high-resolution anatomical data, machine learning, spectral DCM and resting functional connectivity measures to interrogate the relationship between structural and functional gradients of organization within the mesial temporal lobe.

      The study is broken into four related sections. In the first section, the authors analysed vertices within a set of mesial temporal lobe structures using a random-forest algorithm, which identified a set of microstructural profiles across the structure. They then interrogated these profiles for evidence of an iso-to-allometric axis, which is a principle known to characterise the transition from 6-layered isocortex (in entorhinal cortex) to 3-layer allocortex (in the hippocampal formation). The authors found evidence consistent with this transition in the BigBrain data, particularly with respect to the skewness of the distribution of thickness across the layers.

      In the second section, the authors use Spectral DCM on resting state data from a group of 40 individuals. They then relate the results of the spectral DCM model to the gradients identified using structural anatomy. This section was well-motivated and conducted.

      In the third section, the authors compare the structural gradient to resting state functional connectivity with vertices within the cerebral cortex. The results here were quite compelling, showing a dissociation between the iso- and allo-cortical poles in the MTL in which the iso-cortex was correlated with fluctuations in the lateral dorsal attention and frontoparietal networks, whereas the allo-cortical pole was correlated with vertices in the default mode and medial occipital regions.

      In the final section, the authors conducted a number of checks of their analysis, including an SNR test to ensure that the temporal lobes (a notorious site for MRI signal dropout) were adequate, and a substantial replication analysis. They should be commended for these steps, and also for making their code freely available.

      Comments:

      1) Section 1: I wonder whether the manuscript might benefit from the unpacking of the random forest results. Is there an intuitive way to characterize skewness that may benefit the reader - such as a particularly uneven spread of thickness distributed across the layers? And is this finding something that we might expect, given the hypothesized gradient of iso-to-allocortex in the MTL?

      2) Section 1: Along these lines, is it fair to single out an individual measure from the random-forest regression as being the most salient? From my understanding (which might be mistaken), the weights on a particular variable in a regression need to be viewed in context of the performance of the whole model.

      3) Section 2: One minor comment is that it might be helpful for the reader if the "in" and "out" effective connectivity directions were incorporated into the matrix in Figure 2A.

      4) Section 2: I wasn't sure that I followed the logic of the experiment in which the authors split the MTL data into thirds to test for the consistency of their results. Were each of these sufficiently powered to allow for direct comparison with the main effect? Did the boundaries between these models cut across known regional areas? Perhaps a different way to achieve the same ends would be to use bootstrapping in order to provide a confidence interval around the relationship between structure and function?

      5) Section 3: Did the authors hypothesize the iso vs. allo-cortical relationship to resting state networks a priori, or was it discovered upon exploration of the data. Either is fine, in my opinion, but I think it would benefit the reader to have these results placed in the context of the known literature.

      6) Section 3: Do the authors expect that the patterns identified in the MTL will relate to subcortical gradients identified in other structures, such as the cerebellum (Guell et al., 2018), thalamus (Müller et al., 2020, and basal ganglia (Stanley et al., 2019)? See also Tian et al., 2020 for general subcortical gradients.

    1. Reviewer #2:

      General Assessment:

      The role of visual experience with faces in the formation of face-specific neural "modules" is tested in a deep convolutional neural network model of object recognition, AlexNet. A modified version of the ILSVRC-2012 training dataset was constructed by removing all images with primate faces, removing remaining categories with fewer than 640 images, and re-training the deprived network: d-Alexnet. d-Alexnet was compared to pre-trained Alexnet on classification performance, quality of fit to fMRI data, strength of face-selectivity, representational similarity, and learned receptive field properties. The authors argue that face-selectivity is significantly reduced, but not eliminated, with the deprivation, and that this reduction is consistent with an interpretation that d-Alexnet represents faces more similarly to objects than Alexnet. While this work is well-motivated and timely, there are substantial issues in the conceptual approach, the methods used, clarity of the results, and most importantly, the strength of the conclusions.

      Major Concerns:

      1) The validity of these results is uncertain due to a) insufficient reproducibility within this work and b) fragile definitions of face-selectivity.

      a) Given that small changes in weight initialization or training procedure can have a large effect on learned representations (see Mehrer et al. 2020, https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/2020.01.08.898288v1.abstract ), the authors must demonstrate that their results hold across multiple initializations of each network type. Several key results hinge on the number and identity of "face-selective" channels (Figure 2, 3c-e) and only a single instance of each model type is used. In particular, the result that 2/256 channels are "selective" in d-Alexnet compared to 4/256 in Alexnet is likely sensitive to small variations in the methods, including the choice of evaluation stimuli and the initialization of the weights. If the models were re-trained, could the ratio be 4 channels to 4 channels, 0 channels to 2 channels, or some other result? With only a single instance of each model and such a small (and potentially unstable) number of face-selective channels in each model, I am not convinced that these results support the claims made.

      SUGGESTION: Report results averaged across multiple initializations of each model to demonstrate robustness. Statistical tests should be conducted across models (as if they were individual subjects) to demonstrate the significance of any effects found.

      b) The definition of "selectivity" is potentially fragile and may not hold when tested with more standard evaluation sets. In the primate face-selectivity literature, functional localizers are used to compare face responses to non-face responses. These localizers have much stronger controls over low-level features than the stimuli used to evaluate selectivity in this work. I am especially concerned that the faces (from FITW) differ from non-face objects (from Caltech-256) in low-level properties such as image resolution, pose, background, contrast, luminance, and more. Furthermore, selectivity is typically defined in the field as a continuous quantity (e.g., t-contrast, d-prime, face-selectivity-index) and is not often assessed in a binary fashion by the number of units significantly more responsive to faces than the second-best category. Many of these continuous metrics also incorporate variance in responses as well as the mean of responses. Thus, the designation of channels as "selective" or "not-selective" in this work based on mean responses to only 2 of the 205 categories (L101) prevents the reader from understanding how the distribution of face-selectivity shifted under the deprivation, which is one of the primary claims. Instead, we only see the number of selective channels after a binary cutoff, which may be sensitive to initialize and the stimulus set used to evaluate selectivity.

      SUGGESTION: Compute selectivity using evaluation sets in which faces are better matched to non-face objects. Report the distribution of selectivity for each channel before and after deprivation.

      2) Because one model in the comparison is pre-trained and the other is trained from scratch, there is the possibility that all of the differences between the models are due to differences in the training that are independent from the content of the training images.

      a) In the regression analysis, is it the case that non-selective channels also show differences in R2? For example, if the d-Alexnet is worse on the training task (d-ImageNet) than Alexnet, we expect a general reduction in its ability to explain neural responses (see e.g. Yamins et al., 2014). The claims that face-selectivity is specifically impaired in d-Alexnet need to be supported by demonstration that non-selective channels are equally good (or poor) fits to vertices in face-selective regions. Furthermore, the authors do not demonstrate that face-selective channels are better than non-selective channels in either model type, which is useful context for understanding whether the correspondence between face-selective channels and face-selective brain regions is meaningful.

      SUGGESTION: report non-selective channel fits to the same vertices for each model type and compare to face-selective channel fits.

      b) L366: the authors write that "the d-Alexnet was initialized with values drawn from a uniform distribution". This is not standard practice; in fact, the kernel weights in the original AlexNet model were initialized from a Gaussian distribution. To make comparisons to the non-deprived model, the authors need to also retrain the non-deprived model to account for the potential confounds between their training/initialization procedure and that used in the pre-training.

      SUGGESTION: re-train the non-deprived AlexNet in-house, then compare that model to d-AlexNet.

      1. A major conceptual issue is in the definition of a "face module". Despite "face module" in the title, a working definition of "face module" is not clearly provided in the manuscript. Context clues suggest that the authors may consider any face-specific process evidence of a "face module", but the experiments performed indicate that a specific set of criteria were explored: selectivity for faces, different representations for faces and non-face objects, holistic processing, etc. Especially given that the results of this work indicate some residual face-selectivity, a clear definition of "face module" - grounded in the existing literature - is needed to evaluate the claims provided.

      SUGGESTION: clearly define what the "face module" is in the brain, then explain what the corresponding evidence for a "face module" would be in the DCNN.

      4) A number of analyses are not well-motivated or are lacking in detail

      a) The analysis of the "empirical receptive field" is lacking in detail and motivation, and the color-scale is both nonlinear and missing a label. Specific questions:

      i) How should this result be compared to data in primate face-selective regions?

      ii) Is this result a trivial consequence of the difference in number of activated units (panel D)?

      iii) What are the units of the colormap?

      iv) Why are only two channels shown for AlexNet if 4 channels are face-selective?

      v) Is the extent of the empirical receptive field quantified?

      vi) How should the reader think about empirical receptive fields in a weight-shared convolutional architecture?

      b) The evaluation of the face-inversion test is poorly motivated. The face-inversion effect indicates that human subjects are better at remembering upright faces than inverted faces. However, the analysis performed here evaluates the magnitude of the response of face-selective channels. If anything, a classification task is needed to compare to the human task, because the "face inversion effect" cited is not simply that face-selective units respond more strongly to upright than inverted faces, but that the activation of the units supports differences in classification between upright and inverted faces.

      SUGGESTION: At minimum, justify 1) why the magnitude of channel response is a good measure of the face inversion effect or 2) remove the claim that the models do/don't exhibit the behavioral effect.

    2. Nancy Kanwisher (Reviewer #1):

      Xu et al use deep nets to ask whether face selectivity, and face discrimination performance, can arise in a network that has never seen faces. By painstakingly removing all faces from the training set, and comparing Alexnet trained with and without faces, they claim to find, first, that the face-deprived network does not have deficits in face categorization or discrimination (relative to the same network trained with faces), second that the face-deprived network showed some face-selectivity, and third that face deprivation reduced face selectivity. They conclude that "domain-specificity may evolve from non-specific experience without genetic predisposition, and is further fine-tuned by domain-specific experience."

      I love the question and the general strategy behind this study, and indeed we have long discussed doing something much like this in my lab, and we presented a preliminary result of this kind at VSS years ago (https://jov.arvojournals.org/article.aspx?articleid=2433862 ). It is a great use of deep nets to ask what kinds of structures can in principle arise with different kinds of training diets. Xu et al are also to be congratulated for the huge effort they went to in curating a data set of stimuli with no faces, for which they are correct no current algorithm is adequate, requiring a huge amount of labor-intensive human effort.

      Nonetheless, despite my might enthusiasm for the question, the general logic of the study, and the major effort to create the training set, I do have a few significant concerns about the paper:

      1) The biggest problem in the paper in my view is that although regular Alexnet saw faces in the training set, it was not trained on face discrimination, and its performance on this task is very low (66%). That is above chance but very much lower than a network that is actually trained on face discrimination. In our studies, which are typical of this literature, we find that when Alexnet is trained on the VGG-Face dataset identification of novel faces is around 85% correct (top-1). So to say that the face-deprived network performed no differently from the face-experienced network on a face discrimination task, while true, is misleading, because really this reflects the fact that neither was trained on face discrimination and both do pretty badly. And perhaps more importantly, for faces humans have learned, their typical face recognition accuracy would be way higher than 66% correct. So, the face-deprived network really does very badly compared to a real face-trained network, or to humans, and does not represent a strong case of preserved face discrimination despite lack of face experience. Instead, it reflects the kind of face recognition performance one would expect from an object recognition system or a prosopagnosic patient: above chance but not very accurate. Thus, I think the behavioral data show not preservation of face perception abilities in a network trained without faces, but low performance at face discrimination, much like a network that has seen faces but not been trained to discriminate them.

      2) The claim that "face-selective channels already emerged in the d-AlexNet" is similarly overstated in my view, given that only two such units were found and the selectivity of the one we are shown (on the right in Figure 2a) is weak. Although the authors concede that the selectivity of these two units is lower than found in Alexnet trained with faces, that understates the case, as Figure 2a shows. The analysis in Figure 2b, correlating responses of face-selective channels from Alexnet to natural movies, with brain responses to the same movies, is interesting but doesn't tell us what we most need to know. Several public data sets include the magnitude of response of FFA and OFA to a set of 50-100 images, and I would find it more useful to compare those to the response of Alexnet face units to the same images.

      A small point: Only human and primate faces were removed from the dataset, but I would think other animal faces (e.g. cats and dogs) should produce some relevant training. Certainly face-selective regions in the human brain respond strongly to animal faces, as several studies have shown. This might be worth considering in the discussion when potential reasons for the emergence of face-selective channels are discussed (line 229-236).

      For the reasons above, I don't think the results of this study strongly support the conclusion that "the visual experience of faces was not necessary for an intelligent system to develop a face-selective module". At least the "face-specific module" so claimed is a far cry from the human face processing system in both neurally measured selectivity and behavioral performance.

    1. But indeed these things are nothing; if God should withdraw his hand, they would avail no more to keep you from falling

      Typically, we live life and abuse these "worldy things" given to us and we think we like to think we have everything we need but what I think Edwards is saying here is that yes we may be happy and think everything is going great but If you don't have a relationship with God, those worldly things are not going to save you. God is our gateway to heaven and if He takes his hand away, nothing is stopping us from going to hell. Edwards is basically saying if you are not living you're life for God as a christian, God will not give you his hand

    2. born again,

      The term "born again" is used a lot, both in the bible and in sermons and songs even to this day. To be born again in Christ is to have a new life driven by Him and moving past all the sins of your "old life." Considering how long ago this sermon was given, does anyone think that it may have had a different meaning at that point in time? If so, how has it matured into what we interpret it as today?

    1. Author Response

      We would like to thank the three reviewers for their efforts and the constructive feedback. Below, we describe how we will address the reviewers’ comments in an updated manuscript.

      Summary:

      All of the reviewers expressed concerns about the advance that the work described in the paper represents. These issues were a focus of the consultation among the reviewers. The main concern is that the work needs to go beyond demonstrating that some ganglion cells exhibit nonlinear integration for naturalistic inputs - as that point is quite well established in the literature. The comparison between natural stimuli and gratings could help in this regard, but several issues confound that comparison (e.g. differences in dynamics of the two types of stimuli). These concerns are detailed in the individual reviews below.

      Reviewer #1:

      This paper investigates how retinal ganglion cells integrate inputs across space, with a focus on natural images. Nonlinear spatial integration is a well-studied property of ganglion cells, but it has been largely characterized using grating stimuli. A few studies have extended this to look at spatial integration in the context of natural images, but we certainly lack a comprehensive treatment of that issue. The current paper has a number of strengths - notably using a number of complementary stimuli and analysis tools to study a large population of ganglion cells and linking properties of responses to artificial stimuli with those to natural stimuli. It also has a few weaknesses (some detailed carefully in the paper) - such as the inability to identify ganglion cell types (aside from a few), and to pinpoint specific circuit mechanisms. These are limitations of the techniques used. This is not a request as much as setting the context of the contribution of the paper. Generally the paper was in good shape, and the data supported the conclusions well. I do think there are a number of issues that could be strengthened. Those are listed below in rough order of importance.

      Statistical correlations in natural scenes:

      A number of analyses in the paper rely on estimating the spatial contrast from an image and comparing the dependence of various measures of the cells' responses on spatial contrast. A danger in this analysis is that spatial contrast is likely correlated with many other statistical properties of the image, so attributing a given response property to spatial contrast has some potential confounds. This issue should be discussed as a possible caveat, unless the authors can rule it out. The paper, accurately, describes the results in terms of correlations (and not causal relationships), but some discussion of the complexity of natural image statistics would be helpful.

      Spatial contrast is defined in our work via the variance of pixel intensity inside the receptive field. Indeed, spatial contrast may reflect different aspects of visual scenes, such as object boundaries, textures, or gradients in light intensity. Differences in the effects of these image features on a ganglion cell’s response will not be captured by our analysis. However, the goal of relating spatial contrast to spike count was primarily to analyze whether the spatial structure of light intensity inside the receptive field was related to the response of a given ganglion cell (beyond the mean illumination), and the pixel intensity variance provides a simple, straightforward measure of this spatial structure. To clarify this aspect and better relate it to the complexity of natural images, we will add a corresponding paragraph in the Discussion.

      Comparison of grating and natural scene spatial scale:

      The section starting around line 233 was confusing for several reasons. First, this section starts by measuring the spatial scale associated with the grating responses, and then comparing that to LN model performance for natural inputs. It's not clear why the spatial scale is the relevant aspect of the responses to gratings. Indeed, the next paragraph provides a measure of the relative sensitivity of the nonlinear and linear response components (via a comparison of F1 and F2 responses). It would be helpful to include some initial text to motivate the different measures of the grating responses and to anticipate that you will look at both spatial scale and sensitivity.

      A related issue that bears more directly on the scientific conclusions comes up later in the blurring experiments. The issue is whether it is valid to directly compare the apparent spatial scale of nonlinear responses to images (estimated via blurring) with that of the grating responses. Natural images should have much higher power at low spatial frequencies, and this may strongly impact the spatial scale identified with the blurring experiments.

      We agree that the writing may not have been entirely clear, and we will reorganize the material to discuss the extracted spatial scale and nonlinearity index in parallel as suggested. Regarding the difference in spatial scales from reversing gratings and blurred natural images: yes, it is also our interpretation that the power at low spatial frequencies plays a key role. Our main point here was to assess whether and to what degree the typical analyses of spatial nonlinearity as measured from reversing gratings translate to natural images despite the differences in spatial and temporal structure of the two stimulus classes. In a revised manuscript, we will make sure to earlier clarify the role of low spatial frequencies.

      Clustering of orientation-selective cells:

      An interesting suggestion in the paper is that the orientation-selective cells can be divided into two groups that differ in their spatial integration properties. Do these groups represent different orientations, as suggested in the text? That seems a simple piece of information to add. Related to this, I would suggest moving Figure S4 into the main text.

      We do not have information about the absolute preferred orientations of the orientation-selective (OS) cells, as we did not keep track of retinal orientation when placing the retinas on the multielectrode array. At this point, we can therefore only rely on indirect analyses of relative preferred orientations between pairs of OS cells in the same retina. These indicate that pairs of two nonlinear OS cells tend to have aligned preferred orientation (and similarly for pairs of linear OFF OS cells), but pairs of a linear and a nonlinear OFF cell tend to have divergent preferred orientations. This is shown in Fig. S4C. For a revised manuscript, we will consider integrating Fig. S4 into the main text, as suggested.

      Presentation of checkerboard stimuli and results:

      The checkerboard analysis, particularly how it isolates properties of spatial integration, could get introduced more thoroughly for a reader unfamiliar with it. A related issue is how well the chosen isoresponse contour captures structure in the full distribution of responses. In some cases that looks pretty good, but in others it is less clear. Could you add a supplementary figure or something similar that characterizes how consistent the isoresponse contours are for different response levels?

      These are good suggestions, and we will aim at clarifying the analysis as proposed and add information about the consistency of iso-response contours for different response levels. In the present analysis, the iso-response contours are used just for illustration, whereas the quantification of rectification and integration of preferred contrast are extracted from specific points in the stimulus-response space, which we found to work robustly for a population analysis without being strongly effected by threshold or saturation effects of the cells. We will explain this more clearly in a revised manuscript.

      Drift in responses over time:

      Some of the rasters - e.g. the bottom left in Figure 1C - show considerable drift over time. It is important that this drift not be interpreted as a failure of the LN model and hence indicative of nonlinear spatial integration. Can you test for drift like this across cells, and exclude any that seem potentially problematic? More generally, some assurance that the variability in the responses for a given generator signal value is real variability across images is needed.

      The presentation of all 300 natural images over ten trials takes about 50 minutes and some drift over this period seems unavoidable. To minimize systematic effects of experimental drift on the measured average responses for different images, we applied randomization within trials, which assured that all images were presented once in random order in each trial before the next trial started. In addition, to quantify the real variability over images of the average response for a given generator signal, we applied a goodness-of-fit measure (CCnorm) that takes into account variability over trials.

      We now also tested directly for the drift mentioned by the reviewer, but observed sizeable effects in only a small subset of cells that were included in the analysis. In most cases, drift corresponded to a global scaling that approximately affected responses to all images proportionally. This is reflected in a high correlation over images between the average responses of the first five and last five trials; 94% of analyzed cells had a correlation coefficient of at least 0.7. Such global scaling of responses does not affect the analysis of differences in average responses. In a revised manuscript, we will provide analyses of drift effects and exclude cells that contain drift effects that appear to deviate from global response scaling.

      Reviewer #2:

      Summary:

      Understanding how retinal ganglion cells respond to natural stimuli is a central but daunting question, which retinal neurophysiologists have begun to tackle recently. Here Karamanlis and Gollisch perform large-scale multi-electrode recordings in the mouse retina and demonstrate that the responses of many ganglion cells cannot be predicted by standard linear-nonlinear models (L-LN). They go on to test a variety of clever artificial stimuli that emphasize and allow for the quantification of the non-linear aspects of RGCs responses and convincingly demonstrate that non-linear processing is associated with sensitivity to fine spatial contrasts (subunits) and local rectification. While these aspects of RGC receptive fields have been previously described, demonstrating their applicability to natural vision is a significant advancement.

      Major Comments:

      My first main concern is with the way the paper is written. It does not highlight the significant advancements but rather emphasizes what is already known from other studies. For example, many of the conclusions of non-linear spatial integration & signal rectification arising in bipolar cells have been well described previously. By contrast, novel aspects like the sensitivity of reversal gratings being unrelated to LN model performance for natural scenes should be explained more in detail. The authors should more clearly state the major advancements that are being made here beyond what has already been shown previously (e.g. Turner and Rieke, 2016)

      It is possible that our efforts to provide context by relating our results to established findings in retinal signal integration overshadowed the novel aspects of our work. As suggested, we will aim at pointing out these aspects more clearly. For example, compared to the work of Turner and Rieke (2016), we a) focused on a different species with more diversity in accessible RGC types, b) generalized the connection of spatial integration and natural scene encoding to a wider range of cell types (e.g. including also spatially linear and nonlinear ON-OFF cells as well as cells that are inversely sensitive to spatial contrast), and c) developed methods to assess and quantitatively characterize subunit nonlinearities with multielectrode recordings of many cells in parallel, without the need for intracellular recordings or knowledge of the receptive field location.

      Second, the authors never include non-linear subunits in their model to demonstrate improved performance. Testing models with filters that incorporate rectification and convexity as experimentally determined will enable them to show their utility more convincingly. Without this, the reader is left with the conclusion that there are RGCs that exhibit non-linear or linear spatial integration (already known) and that non-linear integrators cause LN models to perform poorly with natural images (Turner and Rieke, 2016).

      The aim of the present work was to assess how well models with linear receptive fields account for responses to natural images in various cells of the mouse retina and whether the models’ shortcomings can be related to the cells’ spatial stimulus integration characteristics. While we agree that models with nonlinear subunits could help support the conclusions, fitting such models to recorded data is – we believe – beyond the scope of the current manuscript. The many parameters of nonlinear subunit models, such as the number, shape, and layout of subunits or their nonlinearity and weight, all likely vary considerably across the diverse population of cells in our recordings. To avoid extensive parameter fitting, simplified models with ad hoc selection of subunit layouts and nonlinearities could help assess whether spatial nonlinearities are important, as in the work by Turner and Rieke (2016). Instead, as an alternative, we chose to analyze the importance of spatial nonlinearities via the effect of spatial contrast in images with similar mean intensity in the receptive field (e.g. Fig. 2). For our data, an advantage of this approach is that it is directly applicable to cell types with diverse spatial integration characteristics, such as the cells that are inversely sensitive to spatial contrast, which wouldn’t be captured by a standard subunit model with rectifying subunit nonlinearities. In future work, however, we plan to analyze subunit models that can account for the diversity of observed response patterns.

      Third, I'm not sure how 'natural' their natural images are, given static images are flashed over the cell intermittently. While such stimuli might simulate some sort of saccadic eye movements, whether this is relevant for mouse vision is not clear. Would linear models be more predictive for responses to natural movies? Some discussion on this issue would be helpful.

      Rather than aiming for fully natural movie-like stimuli, we used flashed images in our work to focus on aspects of spatial integration. This indeed entails a simplification of the temporal structure of natural stimuli, which was intended, but it preserves natural spatial structure, such as the occurrence of objects, boundaries, textures, and intensity gradients, as well as continuously decreasing power for higher spatial frequencies. Nonlinear spatial integration in the presence of this natural spatial structure will likely also shape responses under natural movies. To clarify this approach, we will re-evaluate our wording regarding the application of natural stimuli in our work and discuss the simplification compared to natural movies, as suggested.

      Reviewer #3:

      The manuscript by Karamanlis and Gollisch examines the responses of mouse retinal ganglion cells (RGCs) to natural stimuli. The primary conclusion of the manuscript is that spatial integration of stimuli within the receptive field is nonlinear. This nonlinear integration is consistent with "local signal rectification". This results in a set of RGCs that are sensitive to spatial contrast within the RF. The Authors also note the presence of cells that are suppressed by contrast and cells that prefer uniform stimulation of the RF. To reach these conclusions the authors use multi-electrode array recordings from isolated mouse retina. Spatial RFs are estimated using white noise stimuli, which are then used to generate a null-model for linear spatial summation. They compare predictions of this null-model to the responses of the same RGCs to briefly flashed natural images. The authors find some RGCs that are consistent with this null model and many that are not consistent. The authors correlate deviations from linear spatial summation to deviations revealed by contrast reversing gratings. They also used a mixed-contrast, flashed-checkerboard paradigm to map the contrast tuning and rectification of RF subunits. Finally, the authors show that some of these results track with functionally distinct RGC types such as direction-selective and "IRS" RGCs.

      The data and analyses presented in this manuscript are high quality. However, I think the study is largely consistent with many previous studies that demonstrate nonlinear spatial integration among RGCs in the mammalian (including mouse) retina. I think the Authors view the use of natural stimuli as a major departure from previous work, but I'm not convinced of this for two reasons. First, I don't see a compelling reason to think that results using contrast reversing gratings or other 'textured stimuli' (e.g. Schwartz et al Nat Neuro 2012) would fail to generalize to flashed natural scenes. Second, the implicit claim here is that a 200ms flashed natural scene interleaved with an 800ms gray screen is a natural stimulus. I think this assumes a lot about the space-time separability of the RF mechanisms, and these assumptions are not well justified.

      Major Concerns:

      1) I think the introduction of the manuscript is building a straw man argument, suggesting that many (or most) scientists think the retina is predominantly linear. A pubmed search of 'retinal ganglion cell' and 'nonlinear' produced more than 300 studies. Specifying subunit nonlinearity produces 28 studies. The discovery of subunit nonlinearities is roughly 50 years old and many manuscripts demonstrate Y-like receptive fields are more common across RGC types than X-like receptive fields.

      The goal of our work was not to show that receptive fields of mouse retinal ganglion cell are (often) spatially nonlinear, but to test whether these nonlinearities matter for natural images. It is conceivable that spatial nonlinearities as measured with typical artificial stimuli such as spatial gratings or spatiotemporal white noise are not (as) relevant for natural images because the simultaneous occurrence of strong positive and negative contrast inside a receptive field is much rarer in natural images. Indeed, in our work we find that traditional measurements of spatial nonlinearities with reversing gratings do not provide a robust quantitative prediction of whether spatial nonlinearities matter under natural images for a given ganglion cell. As laid out in the Introduction, there is surprisingly little research yet on how spatial nonlinearities affect the encoding of natural images, and in a revised version of the manuscript, we will aim at clarifying that this is the focus of our work here.

      2) The authors seem to be arguing that the spatial nonlinearities engaged by the contrast reversing gratings are not the same as those engaged by their natural scenes (Figure 3). However, I think the authors are assuming too much that the spatial and temporal components of the RFs are separable. The flashed natural scenes are interleaved with relatively long gray screens. The contrast reverse granting are reversed in a square-wave fashion with no interleaved gray screen. These distinct spatiotemporal dynamics in the stimuli seem likely to explain the difference. This would also seem likely to explain why the flashed checkerboards in Figure 4 produced results more correlated to flashed scenes in Figure 1. In summary, I don't see a strong reason to think the authors are observing anything other than subunit rectification of the sort described by Hochstein and Shapley in the 1970s and followed up in many subsequent studies.

      We do not think that spatial nonlinearities as observed with reversing gratings or with natural stimuli are related to different mechanisms. The point of our analysis was rather to assess whether typical assessments of spatial nonlinearities with reversing gratings allow quantitative predictions about the relevance of spatial nonlinearities under flashed natural images, and we find that this is often not the case. We believe that this is largely due to the differences in spatial structure, in particular, the prevalence of high-contrast edges in the gratings. Yet, indeed, differences in temporal stimulus structure might also contribute. We actually tested flash-like presentations of gratings in some of our recordings, and results were quite similar to those obtained with contrast-reversing gratings and led to the same conclusions. We will describe this in the revised manuscript for clarification.

      3) It is not clear to this reviewer that flashed natural images interleaved by a gray screen is qualitative more natural than white noise, sinusoidal gratings, or square-wave gratings.

      The spatial structure of natural images is the focus of the present work. It is in this aspect that flashed photographs are more natural than typical artificial stimuli like spatiotemporal white noise or gratings. In particular, natural images contain a broad spectrum of spatial frequencies with relatively more power at smaller frequencies, and they combine occasional edges with intensity gradients and textures. Gratings, for example, are characterized by high power at large spatial frequencies, that is, high spatial contrast, which is well suited for triggering effects of spatial nonlinearities but occurs much more rarely in natural images. Thus, understanding whether spatial nonlinearities are important in a natural setting requires considering stimuli that match the natural spatial structure. It seems likely that nonlinear spatial integration observed under flashed presentation of natural images remains relevant when stimuli are supplemented with natural temporal structure, even though the latter may likely trigger additional effects that shape the responses (e.g. adaptation or nonlinear temporal integration).

      4) The null-model constructed by the authors in Figure 1 assumes the RF follows a specific functional form (e.g. Gaussian). However, many studies show that individual RFs frequently exhibit strong deviations from a Gaussian RF. To what extent are the deviations from the null model produced by deviations from linear summation or just linear mechanisms that deviate from the specific parametric form imposed by the model?

      Measuring the detailed structure of receptive fields (RFs) with high precision from time-limited experiments is a challenge, and using a fitted (elliptical) Gaussian profile is a standard procedure for limiting the effect of noise in the RF structure. We also tried using the pixel-wise spatial profile obtained from the reverse-correlation analysis as a spatial filter, but results were similar, yet often more noisy. We therefore settled on the standard procedure of using a Gaussian fit to the RF. Deviations from the Gaussian profile can indeed contribute to deviations of the model. Yet, for natural images, which have most of their power in low spatial frequencies, these deviations are likely to be small. Furthermore, our subsequent analyses show that the Gaussian RF model provides a useful baseline because it allows us to extract the relation between model deviations and image structure. In addition, the results from the model analysis were supported by the findings under presentation of blurred natural images, which did not require any assumptions about the underlying RF model. In a revised manuscript, we will point out that relying on Gaussian RFs is a choice that we make and that deviations of the receptive field structure may contribute to decreased model performance, but that the subsequent analyses support the usefulness of the applied Gaussian RF model.

      5) It was unclear how the authors rule out the contribution of differences in (nonlinear) temporal integration to the effects in this study. In general, RGC RFs are not space-time separable, and it seems that the analyses in the manuscript assume they are.

      Our choice of using flashed images as stimuli with no temporal structure beyond onset and offset and assessing responses via elicited spike counts was motivated by focusing on spatial stimulus integration and minimizing effects of temporal processing. Nonetheless, our extraction of receptive fields from measurements under spatiotemporal white-noise stimulation uses a space-time separation of the spike-triggered average. Thus, the lack of space-time separability of ganglion cell receptive fields can contribute to the putative underestimation of surround components, which we have discussed in the manuscript. In a revised manuscript, we will add an explicit reference to the issue of space-time separability.

      6) This study overlaps significantly with Cao, Merwine and Grzywacs (2011), 'Dependence of retinal Ganglion cell's responses on local textures of natural scenes', Journal of Vision. This article is not cited here, but in my view, the major conclusions are similar.

      Thank you for pointing us to this paper, which is indeed relevant for our work. Both the Cao et al. paper and our manuscript evaluate the effect of spatial contrast in natural images by relating spatial contrast to response deviations from a linear-RF model, albeit with different methods. An important difference, apart from the different species, is that our work then focuses on relating the identified effects of spatial contrast to functional characterizations of the specific nonlinear operations inside the receptive field (e.g. rectification). Furthermore, we also focus on the diversity of spatial-integration properties between cells and cell types, including the description of spatially linear cells and cells that are inversely sensitive to spatial contrast. In a revised manuscript, we will add a comparison to the methods and results from Cao et al.

      7) In my experience, the strength of subunit rectification can be labile during ex vivo experiments. What controls have the author's performed to ensure the effect they are studying remain stable over the duration of their recordings?

      Experimental rundown could, of course, affect subunit rectification as well as other response aspects, such as overall sensitivity. However, we observed that responses for different repeats of the same natural images were typically quite stable over the course of the hour-long stimulus. As also discussed in the response to Reviewer 1, we now analyzed how responses to late trials deviated from responses to early trials and found that only a small subset of cells displayed sizeable drift. Furthermore, those cases were mostly affected by a global drift in response size, keeping the relative responses for different images approximately constant. (For 94% of cells, the correlation of images was larger than 0.7 between average responses for the first five and for the last five trials; approximately on the level of estimated random trial-by-trial variability.) This indicates that the features of stimulus integration did not change substantially over the course of the experiment. In addition, nonlinearities as assessed with our flashed checkerboards were strongly correlated to nonlinearities under natural images, despite the fact that these stimuli were applied 1-2 hours apart. Thus, the strength of subunit rectification appears to be sufficiently stable to allow comparison over different stimuli.

    2. Reviewer #1:

      This paper investigates how retinal ganglion cells integrate inputs across space, with a focus on natural images. Nonlinear spatial integration is a well-studied property of ganglion cells, but it has been largely characterized using grating stimuli. A few studies have extended this to look at spatial integration in the context of natural images, but we certainly lack a comprehensive treatment of that issue. The current paper has a number of strengths - notably using a number of complementary stimuli and analysis tools to study a large population of ganglion cells and linking properties of responses to artificial stimuli with those to natural stimuli. It also has a few weaknesses (some detailed carefully in the paper) - such as the inability to identify ganglion cell types (aside from a few), and to pinpoint specific circuit mechanisms. These are limitations of the techniques used. This is not a request as much as setting the context of the contribution of the paper. Generally the paper was in good shape, and the data supported the conclusions well. I do think there are a number of issues that could be strengthened. Those are listed below in rough order of importance.

      Statistical correlations in natural scenes:

      A number of analyses in the paper rely on estimating the spatial contrast from an image and comparing the dependence of various measures of the cells' responses on spatial contrast. A danger in this analysis is that spatial contrast is likely correlated with many other statistical properties of the image, so attributing a given response property to spatial contrast has some potential confounds. This issue should be discussed as a possible caveat, unless the authors can rule it out. The paper, accurately, describes the results in terms of correlations (and not causal relationships), but some discussion of the complexity of natural image statistics would be helpful.

      Comparison of grating and natural scene spatial scale:

      The section starting around line 233 was confusing for several reasons. First, this section starts by measuring the spatial scale associated with the grating responses, and then comparing that to LN model performance for natural inputs. It's not clear why the spatial scale is the relevant aspect of the responses to gratings. Indeed, the next paragraph provides a measure of the relative sensitivity of the nonlinear and linear response components (via a comparison of F1 and F2 responses). It would be helpful to include some initial text to motivate the different measures of the grating responses and to anticipate that you will look at both spatial scale and sensitivity. A related issue that bears more directly on the scientific conclusions comes up later in the blurring experiments. The issue is whether it is valid to directly compare the apparent spatial scale of nonlinear responses to images (estimated via blurring) with that of the grating responses. Natural images should have much higher power at low spatial frequencies, and this may strongly impact the spatial scale identified with the blurring experiments.

      Clustering of orientation-selective cells:

      An interesting suggestion in the paper is that the orientation-selective cells can be divided into two groups that differ in their spatial integration properties. Do these groups represent different orientations, as suggested in the text? That seems a simple piece of information to add. Related to this, I would suggest moving Figure S4 into the main text.

      Presentation of checkerboard stimuli and results:

      The checkerboard analysis, particularly how it isolates properties of spatial integration, could get introduced more thoroughly for a reader unfamiliar with it. A related issue is how well the chosen isoresponse contour captures structure in the full distribution of responses. In some cases that looks pretty good, but in others it is less clear. Could you add a supplementary figure or something similar that characterizes how consistent the isoresponse contours are for different response levels?

      Drift in responses over time:

      Some of the rasters - e.g. the bottom left in Figure 1C - show considerable drift over time. It is important that this drift not be interpreted as a failure of the LN model and hence indicative of nonlinear spatial integration. Can you test for drift like this across cells, and exclude any that seem potentially problematic? More generally, some assurance that the variability in the responses for a given generator signal value is real variability across images is needed.

    1. ugly, or “bad” landscape, disgust also aids in the creation of a hierarchy, or scale of values, whereby different kinds of landscapes may be judged according to their proximity to, or distance from, either extreme. But once we begin to speak of landscapes as being ideal, or “good,” or as flawed, or “bad,” we have entered into the realm of moral judgement.

      Respond: I feel like Landscape Architects have a very different outlook on good and flawed landscapes compared to everyone else. The most clean cut lawns with a few crape myrtles seems perfect for most people in the south , but I think if you would suggest that to a landscape architect they would role their eyes. Landscape Architects find the beauty in things not everyone else would too. So can we really measure what a good landscape is if it is so relative?

    1. Yet the focus on negative effects of media consumption offers an incomplete picture. These accounts do not appropriately value the skills and knowledge young people are gaining through their involvement with new media, and, as a consequence, they may mislead us about the roles teachers and parents should play in helping children learn and grow

      Screen time has always been a point of contention in my family, but I think it's because we often think of it as "messing around" in an unproductive way. When you think of screen time as using media and programs as ways of creating, learning or engaging, than it becomes a bit more complex

    1. ‘‘Enhancement,’’ however, only captures some of thephenomena of interest here. In many cases, assessmentand detection technology such as brain scans may be used,which do not alter the person being scanned. In othercases, instead of enhancing some cognitive trait, ‘‘di-minishment’’ might be desired—for example, reducingone’s memory consolidation or temporarily reducingone’s empathy (cases we will discuss below).

      this is really cool to think about because it's stuff you would see in a sci-fi movie but its actually becoming our reality slowly but surely

    Annotators

    1. Good Brother Brooke, I often look, to hear of your return: But none can tell, if you be well, nor where you do sojourn: Which makes me fear, that I shall hear your health appairéd* is: impaired And oft I dread, that you are dead, or something goeth amiss. Yet when I think, you cannot shrink, 10 but must with Master be: I have good hope, when you have scope, you will repair to me. And so the fear, and deep despair, that I of you then had I drive away: and wish that day wherein we may be glad. Glad for to see, but else for me: will be no joy at all: For on my side, no luck will bide, 20 nor happy chance befall. As you shall know, for I will show, you more when we do speak, Than will I write, or yet recite, within this Paper weak. And so I end, and you commend, to him that guides the skies: Who grant you health, and send you wealth, no less than shall suffice.

      Very concerned for her brother well being

    1. he latest debate in the 2020 presidential race has exposed the limits of how far Democrats are willing to go on voting rights. It began with a question to Sen. Bernie Sanders (I-VT), from a volunteer with the American Civil Liberties Union at an Iowa town hall in April: Should people in prison be allowed to vote? Sanders said yes, and then doubled down on his answer. At a CNN town hall a couple weeks later, Sanders was asked if the Boston marathon bomber should be allowed to vote — and, again, said yes. In a USA Today op-ed, he defended his position, arguing that “the right to vote is an inalienable and universal principle that applies to all American citizens 18 years and older. Period.” Since Sanders was first asked, other Democratic candidates have been questioned about their stances. Most other candidates have yet to say that prisoners should be given the right to vote, instead defending the right to vote only for nonviolent offenders or people who completed their sentences. Some appear undecided. Only two states — Maine and Vermont, where Sanders is from — currently let all people vote while they’re in prison. Other states apply restrictions based on whether someone is in prison, on probation, on parole, or has completed a sentence. (They don’t typically make a distinction on whether a person’s crime was violent or not.) (function() { var l = function() { new pym.Parent( 'vox-felony-disenfranchisement-laws-by-state-3__graphic', 'https://apps.voxmedia.com/at/vox-felony-disenfranchisement-laws-by-state-3/'); }; if(typeof(pym) === 'undefined') { var h = document.getElementsByTagName('head')[0], s = document.createElement('script'); s.type = 'text/javascript'; s.src = 'https://pym.nprapps.org/pym.v1.min.js'; s.onload = l; h.appendChild(s); } else { l(); } })(); As of 2016, 6.1 million people were prevented from voting due to a felony conviction, and about 1.3 million were in state or federal prison, the Sentencing Project, an advocacy group, found. Since black Americans are more likely to go to prison, these laws have a disproportionate impact on black voters, in part reflecting their roots in the Jim Crow era: More than 20 percent of black voters were disenfranchised in Florida, Kentucky, Tennessee, and Virginia in 2016. There appears to be some support for giving people convicted of felonies their voting rights back. Last fall, Florida voted to let most people with felony records vote once they complete their sentences — giving the right to vote back to, potentially, more than 1 million people (although that’s now in question as Republican lawmakers place new restrictions on who can vote). What Sanders is calling for, though, goes much further, enfranchising literally hundreds of thousands or millions of people across the country — in a way that could especially benefit black voters. For some Democrats, who have paid more attention to voting rights in recent years, and for activists with the ACLU who are aiming to get candidates on the record on this topic, it’s a logical next step. But the discussion has shown there are limits in how far even some Democrats — let alone the public — are willing to go in expanding voting rights. The polls so far show that giving people in prison the right to vote is unpopular among the majority of voters and Democrats. And in a Democratic primary where so much of the attention, even beyond policy specifics, is going to finding the candidate who can beat President Donald Trump, that unpopularity is drawing concerns about whether a politically risky issue like this one should be discussed at all. Modern felony disenfranchisement laws have some roots in Jim Crow Felony disenfranchisement laws slotted into the push after the Civil War, particularly in the South, to limit civil rights gains following the end of slavery and ratification of constitutional amendments — the 13th, 14th, and 15th — protecting minority rights. The resistance to civil rights gains also included the Jim Crow laws behind legally enforced racial segregation and other limits on black voting power. It’s been a decades-long project for civil rights activists to undo all of these laws. After the civil right movement, Democrats have taken up the banner to protect minority voting rights. Passage of the Voting Rights Act of 1965 was fairly bipartisan, but since then Republicans in particular have moved to curtail access to the polls through voter ID laws, cuts to early voting, and purges of the voter rolls. Democrats have fought back against Republicans on these issues, but the Democratic efforts haven’t included much advocacy on prisoner disenfranchisement laws. Preventing people with criminal records from voting in the US goes back to the colonial era and the concept of “civil death” — the notion that some bad actions effectively left a person dead in terms of civic engagement. But there’s also a uniquely American and racist twist to this story, rooted in Jim Crow. After the South lost the Civil War, state lawmakers in Florida, for example, enacted laws — the Black Codes — to constrain black rights. They created crimes, such as disobedience and “disrespect to the employer,” that could be enforced in a way that would target and criminalize black people in particular, according to a 2016 report by the Brennan Center for Justice, an advocacy group. Then, when Florida was forced to write voting rights protections for men of all races into its state constitution, lawmakers added an exception that would exempt victims of the Black Codes: Article XIV, Section 2, imposed a lifetime voting ban for people with felony convictions. Section 4 of this same suffrage article directed the legislature to “enact the necessary laws to exclude from ... the right of suffrage, all persons convicted of bribery, perjury, larceny, or of infamous crime” — the same crimes the legislature had recently recognized and expanded through the Black Code. Brennan went on: “Shortly after the 1868 constitution was approved, a moderate Republican leader boasted that he had kept Florida from becoming ‘niggerized.’” Since then, Florida has changed its constitution and laws, Brennan noted, and the felony disenfranchisement law was reformed again after the report, in the 2018 elections. But the roots of its post–Civil War disenfranchisement laws linger. Florida was not alone. Journalists and historians have documented similar efforts in Virginia and other Southern states. And of course, the federal government had to enact the (now-weakened) Voting Rights Act of 1965 to shield black voters from state-level discrimination, as well as other civil rights laws to prohibit other forms of systemic racism. But the criminal justice system remains one path toward disenfranchising voters, with a criminal or felony record often costing people various legal rights and protections even after they get out of jail or prison. And this system is rife with racial disparities, as the Washington Post’s Radley Balko explained in his thorough breakdown of the research. “We use our criminal justice system to label people of color ‘criminals’ and then engage in all the practices we supposedly left behind,” Michelle Alexander argued in her influential (and at times criticized) book The New Jim Crow. “Today it is perfectly legal to discriminate against criminals in nearly all the ways that it was once legal to discriminate against African Americans.” Still, felony disenfranchisement laws have survived legal challenges. Courts, including the US Supreme Court, have generally upheld such voting restrictions under the US Constitution’s 14th Amendment, which suggests that the government may abridge the right to vote due to “participation in rebellion, or other crime.” Without the courts, the only real hope for these efforts is to turn influential politicians and public opinion around on the issue — which is what Sanders seems to be trying to do and what some activists are encouraging. This might have to trickle down to the state level too, because there’s some scholarly debate about whether Congress even has the power to end felony disenfranchisement at the federal level. Where Democrats stand on felony disenfranchisement With the debate about voting from prison, Democrats now have a chance to expand the broader debate about voting rights. But, besides Sanders, most other candidates haven’t committed to full voting rights for people in prison. Sanders has been very explicit in his case: He argues that voting is a right that should never be taken away from anyone in a democracy. And that means people, no matter how terrible they prove to be, should keep their right to vote. “Even if Trump’s former campaign manager and personal lawyer end up in jail, they should still be able to vote — regardless of who they cast their vote for,” he wrote in USA Today. He later added, “In my view, the crooks on Wall Street who caused the great recession of 2008 that hurt millions of Americans are not ‘good’ people. But they have the right to vote, and it should never be taken away.” Some journalists, pundits, and activists have sided with Sanders. Writing in New York magazine, Zak Cheney-Rice argued that people aren’t imprisoned just because of bad decisions or mistakes, but also due to systemic factors that led them on a wrong path. Denying these people the right to vote robs them of the opportunity to express how society has failed them, how society continues to fail them through torturous and unconstitutional prison environments, and how society should and can be corrected, he argued. “A society that expels from its conception of humanity so many people who are sick, or in pain, or who make mistakes based upon which their entire lives are suddenly deprived of the opportunity for redemption, is an immoral society,” Cheney-Rice wrote. “But most Americans will not see that unless prisoners have a voice in that society. Giving them the vote is not the whole answer, nor is it the only one. But it is an essential beginning.” He pointed out that, beyond Maine and Vermont, several countries let people vote from prison to varying degrees, including France, Israel, Japan, and Sweden. But so far, other Democratic candidates have mostly distanced themselves, to varying degrees, from Sanders’s proposal. South Bend, Indiana, Mayor Pete Buttigieg spoke in stark terms, arguing, “I do believe that when you are out, when you have served your sentence, then part of being restored to society is that you are part of the political life of this nation again — and one of the things that needs to be restored is your right to vote.” He added, “But part of the punishment when you’re convicted of a crime and you’re incarcerated is you lose certain rights, you lose your freedom. And I think during that period it does not make sense to have an exception for the right to vote.” Former Rep. Beto O’Rourke (D-TX), meanwhile, suggested that maybe there should be an exception for nonviolent offenders: “I would think especially for nonviolent offenders that we rethink removing the right to vote and allow everyone, or as many as possible, to participate in our democracy. For violent criminals, it’s much harder for me to reach that conclusion.” Other candidates suggested they’re undecided. Sen. Elizabeth Warren (D-MA), for example, said, “Once someone pays their debt to society, they’re out there expected to pay taxes, they’re expected to abide by the law, they’re expected to support themselves and their families. I think that means they’ve got a right to vote. While they’re still incarcerated, I think it’s a different question. And I think that’s something that we could have more conversation about.” Sen. Kamala Harris (D-CA) similarly said, “I think we should have that conversation.” It’s a philosophical question: Can someone at some point do something so terrible that he loses his right to vote? For Sanders, the answer is no. For others, the answer is yes, though just how terrible the act has to be before that right is lost, and how long the right is lost for, varies from candidate to candidate. Sanders’s stance is very unpopular One reason Sanders’s Democratic opponents may be reluctant to support his idea: politics. The idea of letting people in prison vote is very unpopular. A recent poll from the Hill and Harris X found that 69 percent of registered voters — and 61 percent of Democrats — said people who are incarcerated for a felony shouldn’t be allowed to vote. Another poll by YouGov found that 65 percent of Americans disagree with Sanders’s statement that all prisoners, including “terrible people” like the Boston Marathon bomber, should be allowed to vote. There is support for letting people vote after they complete their sentences. YouGov found that 65 percent of Americans agree that people convicted of a nonviolent felony should be allowed to vote after they have completed their sentences. And in Florida, voters approved an initiative, with nearly 65 percent in favor, letting people convicted of felonies vote once they’ve completed their sentences, with exceptions for those convicted of murder or felony sex offenses. But with the poll numbers on prisoners’ voting rights, it’s not hard to guess why a bunch of Democratic candidates might be cautious about supporting the idea. This has come up with some other issues in the Democratic primaries, like reparations. While some Democrats may truly believe that reparations are good policy, they’re still widely viewed as politically toxic. Sanders himself previously took this approach when asked about reparations, calling the idea “very divisive” in 2016. But in other instances, and seemingly with prisoner voting rights, Sanders has stuck his neck out in support of political causes. He is, after all, a self-described socialist willing to take on the establishment. His entire 2016 campaign against Hillary Clinton was widely considered a long shot, but he launched it anyway largely to move the party to the left on issues like college and health care. To Sanders’s credit, this worked. Today, Democrats are tripping over themselves in voicing their support for Medicare-for-all or at least some sort of expansion of public health insurance. It’s hard to see that happening, or the broader conversation about single-payer health care in general, without Sanders putting the issue at the front of his 2016 bid for the White House. For activists, this is what they want to see. As the ACLU explained, “If we can raise the volume on key issues like criminal justice reform, immigration, voting rights and reproductive freedom with presidential candidates before the 2020 primaries, we can make sure civil rights and civil liberties are front and center.” For many Democrats, though, this isn’t the time for stands purely based on principle. An overwhelming focus of the 2020 Democratic primaries is to find a candidate who can beat Trump. The notion of “electability” is one of the reasons that former Vice President Joe Biden is leading in the polls. So taking up a cause that is very unpopular and could help Democrats lose the 2020 election is a nonstarter for many, even those who may in theory support giving people in prison the right to vote nationwide. There may be some political incentives for Democrats to embrace Sanders’s views on prisoner voting rights, though. The research indicates that letting people convicted of felonies vote could disproportionately benefit Democrats. That’s made Republicans more resistant to the idea — Trump and Vice President Mike Pence criticized Sanders’s comments — but it could make Democrats more receptive, too. But, at least for now, most of the public, Democrats, and the presidential candidates are not on board. Content by Vox NEXT 360p 720p HD 1080p HD Auto (360p) About Connatix V60548 About Connatix V60548 Visit Advertiser website GO TO PAGE Skip 1/1 Vox’s guide to where 2020 Democrats stand on policy Health care 16 The metapolitics of Medicare-for-all Bernie Sanders’s Medicare-for-all plan, explained The Sanders-Warren dispute about how to pay for Medicare-for-all, explained Elizabeth Warren’s plan to pay for Medicare-for-all, explained Elizabeth Warren’s new Medicare-for-all plan starts out with a public option Joe Biden’s health care plan, explained Kamala Harris’s Medicare-for-all plan, explained Pete Buttigieg’s Medicare-for-all-who-want-it plan, explained Where 2020 Democrats agree and disagree on Medicare-for-all How the Democratic presidential candidates would combat the opioid epidemic Elizabeth Warren’s $100 billion plan to fight the opioid epidemic, explained Kamala Harris’s plan to reduce prescription drug costs, explained America’s first-ever public option, explained by Gov. Jay Inslee Elizabeth Warren’s ambitious new bill to lower generic drug prices, explained What 2020 Democrats would do about maternal mortality rates Democrats’ confused, and confusing, Medicare-for-all debate Show More Criminal justice 12 Bernie Sanders’s criminal justice reform plan, explained Elizabeth Warren’s criminal justice reform plan, explained Kamala Harris’s criminal justice reform plan, explained Cory Booker is a genuinely distinctive Democrat on one big issue: criminal justice Cory Booker has a plan to reform the criminal justice system — without Congress The controversial 1994 crime law that Joe Biden helped write, explained Amy Klobuchar’s record as a “tough on crime” prosecutor, explained Joe Biden’s long record supporting the war on drugs and mass incarceration, explained Kamala Harris just introduced a bill to decriminalize marijuana Amy Klobuchar has a plan to reverse the war on drugs — and doesn’t need Congress to do it Kamala Harris wants public defenders to get paid as much as prosecutors Thousands of rape kits are currently untested. Kamala Harris has a plan to change that. Show More Taxes and economics 12 Bernie Sanders’s wealth tax proposal, explained Elizabeth Warren’s proposed tax on enormous fortunes, explained Bernie Sanders’s new plan to supercharge the estate tax, explained Elizabeth Warren’s latest big idea is “economic patriotism” Bernie Sanders wants to tax companies that pay their CEOs way more than their workers Elizabeth Warren’s plan to expand Social Security, explained The big divide among 2020 Democrats over trade — and why it matters Presidential hopefuls are promising workers a $15 minimum wage Elizabeth Warren’s vision for changing America’s trade policy, explained Elizabeth Warren’s plan to make farming great again, explained Amy Klobuchar’s $1 trillion infrastructure plan, explained Michael Bennet’s plan to prevent and end recessions, explained Show More Immigration 6 The 2020 Democratic immigration debate, explained Bernie Sanders’s immigration plan puts the rights of workers into focus Elizabeth Warren’s immigration proposal goes much further than a pathway to citizenship Julián Castro wants to radically restrict immigration enforcement Beto O’Rourke’s immigration plan would go even further on executive power than Trump Elizabeth Warren has endorsed the most radical immigration idea in the 2020 primary Climate change 12 A guide to how 2020 Democrats plan to fight climate change Kamala Harris’s climate plan would take polluters to court Bernie Sanders’s Green New Deal, explained Pete Buttigieg wants the US to be carbon-neutral by the time he’s 68 Elizabeth Warren thinks corruption is why the US hasn’t acted on climate change Julián Castro’s Green New Deal frames climate as a civil rights issue Beto O’Rourke now has the most robust climate proposal of any 2020 presidential candidate Andrew Yang’s plan to tackle climate change, explained Jay Inslee promised serious climate policy and he is delivering Jay Inslee has a radical plan to phase out fossil fuel production in the US Kirsten Gillibrand wants the fossil fuel industry to pay for climate damages Jay Inslee is writing the climate plan the next president should adopt Show More Identity and social justice 12 The 2020 Democratic primary debate over reparations, explained Pete Buttigieg has a plan to win over women The one big policy change 2020 Democrats want to make for veterans, explained Julián Castro has an ambitious plan to fix American policing Marianne Williamson presents the 2020 Democratic primary’s first reparations plan Pete Buttigieg lays out his plan to help black Americans Julián Castro released an animal welfare plan. It’s good policy — and smart politics. Julián Castro’s indigenous communities plan is a 2020 first Elizabeth Warren has a plan to narrow the wage gap for women of color Amy Klobuchar releases plan to fight hate crimes in wake of El Paso shooting Study: Cory Booker’s baby bonds nearly close the racial wealth gap for young adults Beto O’Rourke just unveiled a comprehensive proposal to protect LGBTQ people Show More Education 8 Elizabeth Warren’s K-12 education plan, explained Elizabeth Warren’s free college plan, explained Bernie Sanders’s free college proposal just got a whole lot bigger Bernie Sanders is rolling out an education plan that cracks down on charter schools Kamala Harris’s plan to dramatically increase teacher salaries, explained Democrats’ ongoing argument about free college, explained Cory Booker’s massive overhaul of the Newark schools, explained Joe Biden’s plan to triple spending on low-income schools, explained Business, labor, and tech 12 Every 2020 frontrunner’s labor platform, explained Bernie Sanders’s plan to reshape corporate America, explained Elizabeth Warren wants to break up Google, Amazon, and Facebook Amy Klobuchar enters 2020 race ready to take on Big Tech Elizabeth Warren’s new plan to make sure Amazon (and other big companies) pays corporate tax, explained Elizabeth Warren wants CEOs to go to jail when their companies behave badly The 20-year argument between Joe Biden and Elizabeth Warren over bankruptcy, explained Bernie Sanders’s corporate employee ownership plan, explained Elizabeth Warren’s latest Wall Street enemy: private equity The big new plan to save unions endorsed by Bernie Sanders and Pete Buttigieg, explained Bernie Sanders’s ambitious plan to double union membership, explained Facial recognition tech is a problem. Here’s how the Democratic candidates plan to tackle it. Show More Family issues 6 We asked all the 2020 Democrats how they’d fix child care. Here’s what they said. Kamala Harris’s new paid family leave plan is the most generous yet Elizabeth Warren’s universal child care plan, explained Kamala Harris’s plan to close the gender wage gap, explained Kirsten Gillibrand is making paid family leave a defining issue in her 2020 run Kirsten Gillibrand’s new policy platform is about making parenting affordable Foreign policy 8 The 2020 Democrats’ foreign policy divide Democrats want to challenge Trump’s foreign policy in 2020. They’re still working out how. Democrats are increasingly critical of Israel. Not Pete Buttigieg. Bernie Sanders’s political revolution on foreign policy, explained John Delaney has a serious foreign policy plan Why Kirsten Gillibrand’s foreign policy plan is one of the strongest yet Why Joe Biden’s foreign policy experience is both a weakness and a strength in 2020 Joe Biden wants to restore the pre-Trump world order Guns 6 Here’s where every 2020 candidate stands on guns Joe Biden’s gun plan calls for universal background checks and an assault weapons ban Elizabeth Warren has a new plan to reduce gun violence by 80 percent Bernie Sanders’s record on gun control, explained Pete Buttigieg’s plan to combat domestic terrorists and pass gun control laws, explained Cory Booker’s ambitious new gun control plan, explained Abortion 4 Here’s where all the 2020 Democratic candidates stand on abortion rights Elizabeth Warren just announced her abortion platform. It’s aggressive. Kamala Harris has a plan to stop states from restricting abortion access Joe Biden’s evolution on abortion, explained Government reform and anti-corruption 8 Elizabeth Warren’s new remedy for corruption: a tax on lobbying Elizabeth Warren’s first priority as president: ending government corruption 2020 Democrats’ campaign finance pledges, explained Elizabeth Warren has a new plan to make voting easier The Democratic debate over filibuster reform, explained Elizabeth Warren says she wants to make it legal to indict presidents Kirsten Gillibrand’s plan to get more small donors into politics: give every voter $600 Elizabeth Warren’s new policy rollout targets Pentagon corruption Poverty and inequality 4 5 anti-poverty plans from 2020 Democratic presidential contenders, explained Kamala Harris’s new basic income-style bill is so frustratingly close to being great Why Andrew Yang wants to give a random Twitter follower a $12,000 basic income Elizabeth Warren’s plan to fix America’s digital divide, explained Housing 3 Bernie Sanders’s housing-for-all plan, explained Cory Booker’s plan to fix the housing crisis and make renting affordable Cory Booker and Kamala Harris’s affordable housing plans, explained Most Read <img class="c-dynamic-image " alt="A cartoon of a woman wearing a shirt that reads “super mom” sitting at a desk looking at an open laptop and a tablet on a stand." data-chorus-optimize-field="main_image" src="data:image/gif;base64,R0lGODlhAQABAIAAAAUEBAAAACwAAAAAAQABAAACAkQBADs" data-cid="site/dynamic_size_image-1601047081_1441_33898" data-cdata='{"image_id":67450083,"ratio":"standard"}'> <noscript><img alt="A cartoon of a woman wearing a shirt that reads “super mom” sitting at a desk looking at an open laptop and a tablet on a stand." src="https://cdn.vox-cdn.com/uploads/chorus_image/image/67450083/Header.0.jpg"></noscript> The hell that is remote learning, explained in a comic Here’s what one week of online school is like for my 7- and 5-year-old kids.

      I have recently heard many debates on this and some people ask " why dont people in prison vote" and my opinion on that is that they should be allowed everyone has the right to vote even with their bad actions. I dont think its right that some states allow and dont allow prisoners to vote, whether or not they commited a crime or it wasnt as severe.

    1. Note: This rebuttal was posted by the corresponding author to Review Commons. Content has not been altered except for formatting.

      Learn more at Review Commons


      Reply to the reviewers

      Reviewer #1 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity (Required)):

      Molenaars et al., describe a protocol to extract and quantify a wide range of polar and apolar metabolites from the same C. elegans sample using methanol-chloroform based phase separation. The authors assess the method across different input amounts, in comparison to a 1-phase extraction method and through metabolic perturbations using RNAi against several metabolic enzymes. Finally, they provide a metabolomics analysis of metabolite variation across several C. elegans strains. The data are of overall high quality and presented in a clearly written manuscript.

      We really appreciate the positive words from the reviewer.

      To help assessing the value of the method to other approaches, several controls are suggested below:

      1.Fig.1: Metabolite abundance in the polar phase should be compared to 1-phase extraction methods (analogous to Fig. 2I, which compares metabolites in the apolar phase to 1-phase extraction)

      We acknowledge the apparent asymmetry in the text; comparing our two-phase method to a single phase lipidomics method indeed suggests a similar comparison for metabolomics. However, our established polar metabolomics method has always been based on this exact two-phase extraction. The current method exclusively asks whether it is possible to integrate our dedicated lipidomics platform into our established two-phase polar metabolomics method, by utilizing the apolar phase that is usually discarded. This way, the method enables comprehensive metabolomics/lipidomics screening while limiting the need of culturing twice the amount of material.

      Our manuscript does not necessarily ask the more fundamental question of the advantages of a one-phase vs two-phase extraction for polar metabolites. Interestingly, the one-phase vs two-phase metabolomics methods have been compared previously and the authors show here that the two-phase method achieved broader metabolite coverage, satisfactory extraction reproducibility, acceptable recovery and safety (DOI: 10.1038/srep38885). This is most probably due to the cHILIC column being sensitive for contamination and therefore excluding lipids from your samples is beneficial for measuring polar metabolites. We hence believe that developing a single phase polar method would appear superfluous for the purpose of this study.

      2.Are polar metabolites also detected in the apolar phase? Can the less hydrophobic lipids missing from the apolar phase detected in the polar phase?

      This is an interesting question that mostly relates to the lyso-lipids that are not detected in the lipid phase of our two-phase extraction. The first point to make is that sample solvents that are used at the final stage of extraction are not compatible between methods. In other words, the solvent we normally use for the lipids phase (xxx) cannot be injected on the cHILIC column. So, in a practical sense, we would not be able to measure these compounds, even if they would technically be dissolved in the other layer. However, we tried a few different alternative approaches to get more information on this point:

      We have attempted to integrate the lyso-lipids in the cHILIC measurements, in the polar layer, using the polar sample solvents. This was unsuccessful; no reproducible peaks, not even the internal standards, were measured. We will include a note on these results in our manuscript. We have, albeit for a different sample matrix, attempted to dissolve both layers of the two-phase extraction in the cHILIC sample solvents. While we cannot guarantee this for all metabolites, it appears that most polar metabolites are exclusively found in the polar layer. We were not able to integrate even a single peak from any of the sugar, amino acids, nucleotides, etc in the apolar layer dissolved in polar solvents. We have reconstituted both the polar and apolar layer of our two-phase extraction in 50:50 methanol:chloroform and analyzed them on the lipidomics platform. We did find some of the lipid internal standards partition to the polar phase, especially LPG (and to a lesser extent LPE and LPA) compared to for instance PE, SM, PG and PC that all end up in the apolar phase. We will include these data in the revised manuscript as a supplemental figure as it demonstrates that the lyso-lipids are poorly measured in the two-phase extraction. This is also why in the text we advise to use the dedicated one-phase extraction when interested primarily in these species.

      3.Fig.3l-n: The authors claim that extracting metabolites from the polar and apolar phases of the same sample leads to better cross-correlation than if metabolites are extracted from different samples using methods optimized for the respective metabolite classes. To provide experimental evidence, metabolite abundance should be compared directly when metabolites are extracted from the same or from different samples using suitable methods.

      We agree with this point. We will amend the text to not overstate these advantages.

      Reviewer #1 (Significance (Required)):

      The methodological and conceptual advancement of the present study is rather incremental. The authors essentially use the classical chloroform/methanol/water phase separation protocols developed by Bligh & Dyer and Folch, which have been used extensively for lipid extraction for many decades now. However, the effort to carefully measure the metabolites contained in the aqueous phase is laudable. For method validation, the authors use well-understood perturbations that yield predictable results. Overall, I consider the study more appropriate for a publication as a methods protocol, which could be of interest to the metabolomics community, rather than as a research paper.

      We agree; our goal was indeed to create and share a method, we will make sure to emphasize this in our cover letter.

      While the extraction method we use is not novel per se and based on classical extraction procedures, it is important to underscore that we are only now able to use these extractions in combination with high-resolution mass spectrometry. This opens new opportunities for basic discovery. The efficiency we achieve by using both phases of the two-phase procedure makes our method highly attractive for hypothesis generation, especially in sample sets where limited amounts of material are available.

      Reviewer #2 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity (Required)):

      The authors provide a detailed description of a method to analyse both polar as well as lipophilic metabolites from the same nematode sample. This provides significant advantages over methods using individual samples. Moreover and by using internal standards they establish an extremely good correlation of individual metabolites. This paper is of immediate importance for the worms community and beyond.

      We are very grateful to receive this positive response from the reviewer and for highlighting the advantages of our described method also beyond the worm community.

      **Major comments:**

      none **Minor comments:**

      The correction process using internal standards could be described a bit more detailed.

      In our revised manuscript, we will describe the internal standard use and corrections in more detail in the text. In summary: internal standards are selected for specific metabolites based on their Pearson correlation and %CV. Subsequently, metabolite peak areas were divided by the area of the appropriate internal standard. This corrects for any loss of sample during sample prep, for instance during the isolation of the two layers.

      Jenni Watts has written a nice Worm Book chapter on lipids which may be cited in addition to reference 17, since it covers many of the metabolites and related enzymes contained in this manuscript

      We will include a reference to this Worm book chapter reviewing fat regulation in C. elegans in our paper, thank you for the suggestion.

      Reviewer #2 (Significance (Required)):

      see above

      Reviewer #3 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity (Required)):

      The manuscript is well written and consider. However, there is room for further improvements:

      We thank the reviewer for the positive response and for the suggestions raised.

      1) Author need to write exactly how many metabolites not just >, semi-quantitative analysis of >100 polar (metabolomics) and >1000 apolar (lipidomics) metabolites in C. elegans, for example they did with other papers in Table 1

      We understand that this might appear vague. The notation was a compromise, based on the following considerations:

      1. The maximum number of reported metabolites can be different to the number of analyzed metabolites in a specific experiment or even a specific sample. For instance, our method is perfectly capable of measuring creatine metabolism –we have standards for these metabolites and they can be reliably measured–, however we have not yet been able to detect these metabolites in elegans. Some mutants also lose abundance of a certain metabolite to the point of it not being reliably measurable, which means they are filtered out in the bioinformatics.
      2. Since the initial draft of our manuscript we have been able, and will continue to be able, to add new metabolites to our analysis, as we perform a full scan over the range of m/z 50-1200. Because of this, we felt it more accurate to state that we can measure >100 metabolites, instead of a specific number.

        2) Authors also need to clarify on number of samples in the result section while describing the statistical analysis.

      We understand this point raised by the reviewer and will specify not only the number of samples, but also that they are indeed biological replicates. This will be included in the figure legends.

      Reviewer #3 (Significance (Required)):

      This might be interesting paper for the research community who work with C.elegans (metabolism or in general)

      Thank you, we are in fact utilizing this double extraction for other non-worm samples such as mice an human tissues and we believe this could also benefit the research community beyond the model organism C. elegans.

      The authors must deposit the raw data and make it available for the public, so they could also benefit from this good work.

      It is our full intention to share our data in a convenient and standardized way through for instance the MetaboLights database (https://www.ebi.ac.uk/metabolights/). We agree and changes will be implemented as suggested.

      Reviewer #4 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity (Required)):

      **Summary:** The authors present a method for extraction of both lipid and polar metabolites from the model organism C. elegans. This extraction method is based on the well-established Blyth and Dyer method, with a slight modification to retain and utilize both the organic and non-polar fractions for LCMS analysis. They applied and tested this method against a monophasic extraction utilizing the same solvent system. They report that there is a loss of metabolites in the non-polar fraction to the polar fraction (of more polar metabolites) and small differences between the monophasic and biphasic extractions. They also expanded on the linearity of the extraction efficiency by increasing the number of worms. Further they applied the single extraction method to both knockdown mutants of C. elegans and Recombinant Inbred Lines derived from N2 and the natural isolate CB4856 to determine whether this method would still be able to differentiate the metabolome between the genetically different C. elegans populations.

      We thank the reviewer for their comments and suggestions.

      **Major comments:**

      *Are the key conclusions convincing?*

      As a whole the conclusions are convincing and valid.

      We appreciate that the reviewer considers our work convincing and valid.

      *Should the authors qualify some of their claims as preliminary or speculative, or remove them altogether?*

      The use of the adjective "robust" is, to an extent, erroneous. As defined, a robust method implies that the method is capable of withstanding small (deliberate or not) changes or variations. In this case the robustness of the method was not assessed and not clear how replication was carried out.

      We have in fact performed analysis on both biological replicates and repeated injections of pooled samples to determine robustness. We will clarify the biological replicates in the text and will place the pooled QC samples in the main text with additional explanation and relevant statistics such as % coefficient of variance (%CV) between them. For clarity, we plotted %CV of all polar as well as apolar metabolites. For polar metabolites 97% of the metabolites had a %CV lower than 30. For apolar metabolites 86% of the metabolites had a %CV lower than 30.

      *Would additional experiments be essential to support the claims of the paper? Request additional experiments only where necessary for the paper as it is, and do not ask authors to open new lines of experimentation.*

      Reproducibility would need to be assessed/quantified to establish how robust the method is. Even though linearity with an increase in the number of worms is a good indication, it does not satisfactorily establish the robustness of the method. The use of replicates to assess the agreement between measurements (i.e. bland-Altman plots), linearity as well as coefficients of variation (included in the sup material but not clear in the body of the manuscript) would characterize the methods best. The isolation of each variance originating from instrumental (pooled quality controls), biological (biological replication) and sample preparation (multiple extractions from the same biological source) is critical.

      We have these data and will elaborate on this in our revised manuscript. We will discuss the quality control samples more prominently in the main body of the manuscript, and show one or more figures that specifically address both analytical and biological variance (see rebuttal figure 2). In summary, we assessed this variance using (a) a repeated injection of a pooled QC sample, and (b) biological replicates prepared individually. Especially the latter condition, in which we assess biological variance is representative for the actual method application. The %CV under these conditions is ≤20% for the majority of metabolites, which is why we consider our method robust.

      *Are the suggested experiments realistic in terms of time and resources? It would help if you could add an estimated cost and time investment for substantial experiments.*

      The suggested experiments are in-fact just further analysis with the already collected data. There would be no need for further experiments, however it is not clear whether pooled QCs/or reference materials were used and the number of replicates per experimental design.

      All the data are available. These analyses will be included in the revision.

      *Are the data and the methods presented in such a way that they can be reproduced?*

      The methods are very well described. My only comment is to address how the replicates were grown/created and how many per strain/group. If the replicate measurements were done on the same samples (repeated injections), I believe that would weaken the findings (if not invalidate them altogether), however if these were biological replicates from independent starting populations the findings are valid and convincing.

      We performed bona fide biological replicates. We will explicitly mention this in the paper together with the other descriptions of our validation protocols.

      *Are the experiments adequately replicated and statistical analysis adequate?*

      As per my above comments.

      **Minor comments:**

      *Specific experimental issues that are easily addressable.*

      It is not clear how the sample preparation process was carried out (randomization, run order, QCs etc). As per the guidelines widely accepted from –Broadhurst, D., Goodacre, R., Reinke, S.N. et al. Guidelines and considerations for the use of system suitability and quality control samples in mass spectrometry assays applied in untargeted clinical metabolomic studies. Metabolomics 14, 72 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11306-018-1367-3.

      We will provide details on the analysis itself in a table. In summary: Samples were measured in a random order, with blanks and QC samples throughout the run.

      *Are prior studies referenced appropriately?*

      A major reference that has applied this extraction method before in the same model organism is missing:

      Castro, C., Sar, F., Shaw, W.R. et al. A metabolomic strategy defines the regulation of lipid content and global metabolism by Δ9 desaturases in Caenorhabditis elegans. BMC Genomics 13, 36 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2164-13-36

      We will include this paper in our references. We would like to note though that this method requires not just an LC system to analyze lipids, but also GC with additional derivatization steps. Our method achieves comprehensive lipidomics using a single technique and no additional derivatization.

      Further a recent publication that goes beyond the work described by the authors using similar approach: MPLEx: a Robust and Universal Protocol for Single-Sample Integrative Proteomic, Metabolomic, and Lipidomic Analyses. Ernesto S. Nakayasu, Carrie D. Nicora, Amy C. Sims, Kristin E. Burnum-Johnson, Young-Mo Kim, Jennifer E. Kyle, Melissa M. Matzke, Anil K. Shukla, Rosalie K. Chu, Athena A. Schepmoes, Jon M. Jacobs, Ralph S. Baric, Bobbie-Jo Webb-Robertson, Richard D. Smith, Thomas O. Metz mSystems May 2016, 1 (3) e00043-16; DOI: 10.1128/mSystems.00043-16

      We will also include this paper, reporting 51 polar metabolites and 84 lipid species, in our references. While we recognize that they also make use of both phases and the protein pellet, we think our method is much more practical in several key ways:

      Our metabolomics platform provides twice as many species and our lipids platform exceeds their analytical capabilities 10 fold. This means a far better coverage of differences within metabolite and lipid classes, allowing for far more intricate patterns to be detected. We show this for instance in our plots comparing carbon chain length to degree of saturation (Fig 4 and S2 in original manuscript); a comparison that is only possible with the data density that our method offers. The MPLEx metabolomics method also requires the use of a GC system and derivatization steps, while our method does not, making it much more user friendly and requiring only a single analytical system.

      *Are the text and figures clear and accurate?*

      Yes *Do you have suggestions that would help the authors improve the presentation of their data and conclusions? *

      The figures, overall are of exceptional quality.

      As per current scientific consensus, Box plots should also be overlaid with the actual datapoints (which was aptly done for the bar charts and other plots).

      The supplementary data even though comprehensive is hard to understand. A "readme" file detailing what data each file contains would improve readability and comply with FAIR principles.

      We agree that a readme file would make the supplemental data more understandable. We will provide such a file. For the box plots we will show the actual data points in our revised manuscript.

      Reviewer #4 (Significance (Required)):

      Even though the approach is not novel and has long been used in Natural Products Chemistry and in other organisms, it's highly significant to set an extraction method standard for the field of C. elegans metabolomics (including myself doing metabolomics and natural products chemistry with LCMS and NMR). However, this manuscript does not cover the technical aspects of the method with sufficient depth to hallmark this method as the standard for the field. Further information is needed to fill the missing gaps (as highlighted by the authors). Ratios between solvent and biological material amounts, reproducibility, recovery rates (even though buried in the supplementary files) and metabolite coverage are still missing.

      As a side note, the disparity between the monophasic and biphasic extractions could be overcome by a sequential extraction of the same sample, with no incurred cost on performance (and removing the much-dreaded pipetting uncertainty near the line between solvents). The second aspect of the manuscript, which initially was a welcoming idea (and important), became >50% of the manuscript creating a disconnect between the information set by the abstract and introduction and the results/conclusion. The work is extremely relevant in both sections of the manuscript, but the technical aspect is still lacking details and/or analysis.

      Strongly suggested: explicit compliance with the minimum reporting standards as per the Metabolomics Standards Initiative (MSI) and deposition of the data to a metabolomics repository (i.e. Metabolights or Metabolomics Workbench). These are internationally accepted requirements for metabolomics publications.

      We are aware that the extraction itself is an analytical chemistry staple. However, it is precisely in this fact that we find novelty. It should be noted that both of the other papers mentioned by the reviewers that have attempted to integrate lipidomics and metabolomics have had to resort to labor intensive (as well as possibly expensive and destructive) derivatization steps and a separate analysis on GC. Our method does not have these requirements. It is indeed a single and very common extraction, after which each dried phase is reconstituted and immediately injected. But this simplicity is not a concession, as our metabolome coverage is easily more comprehensive than the other mentioned methods. We therefore feel that this simplicity should not discount our currently presented method, but be considered an additional advantage.

      Sequential extractions may be an option to consider. However, we feel like they are less user friendly and unneeded. Because we use internal standards, it is never an issue to pipet slightly more or less of any particular sample; making it easy to avoid the line between solvents.

      We will explicitly clarify where we already comply with the standards (such as the analysis of biological replicates and repeated injection of a QC sample) and are confident we can add figures and further information such as deposition of our data to comply with the rest.

      REFEREES CROSS-COMMENTING

      Completely agree with reviewer #1 comments, they are on point and I completely missed it. Relevant and should be addressed.

      Reviewers #2 points out work worth acknowledging, the internal standard work was quite thorough and well designed.

      Reviewer #3 and my comments overlap nicely, the need for further description of samples/replication and deposition of data in a metabolomics repository.

      Further work is required to make this a good publication and standard for the field, without this extra work addressing the reviewers comments I feel this work could be to certain degree misleading and/or incomplete putting in cause its publication potential.

    1. Note: This rebuttal was posted by the corresponding author to Review Commons. Content has not been altered except for formatting.

      Learn more at Review Commons


      Reply to the reviewers

      Note: This preprint has been reviewed by subject experts for Review Commons. Content has not been altered except for formatting.

      We thank the reviewers for their feedback and encouragement. We have now fully revised the manuscript to address all comments. Our specific responses are provided below and we have highlighted changes in the text. The major additions are:

      • analysis of simulated time-courses with lower temporal resolution
      • analysis of ex vivo PER2::LUCIFERASE SCN recordings
      • analysis of simulated time-courses with Poisson distributions of noise
      • plotted summary statistics for several figures
      • mathematical formula and explanation in the Methods Overall, these revisions have strengthened our findings and improved the manuscript, particularly in demonstrating that the issues with the chi-square periodogram are not specific to sampling interval or data type.

      Reviewer #1 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity (Required)):

      **Summary:**

      Tackenberg & Hughey investigate the reliability of a popular period estimation algorithm, the chi-square periodogram. They find a bias in the estimation, and through careful investigation identify the cause. This is a well executed and well presented study.

      **Comments:**

      In Figs 2+3 the authors show that the discontinuity in periodogram coincides with the number of complete cycles, K. However, in Fig 2C there are several other positions where K abruptly changes, but little effect on the chi-squared statistic is observed. Can the authors offer an explanation as to why the magnitude of the discontinuities differ?

      We have taken a closer look at how each component of the chi-square statistic calculation changes at points where K decreases, and have found that discontinuities do always occur at these points. In addition to the obvious effect of the K * N term on the sudden decreases, we found that the sum of squares of the column means alone (the primary component of the numerator) also changes abruptly at each transition point of K. As a result, the discontinuity magnitude is likely roughly proportional to the amplitude of the chi-square statistic at that point.

      An important claim is that the discontinuity is observed in multiple software implementations. However, the plots of Supplementary Fig 1C,D are presented too small to evaluate this claim.

      In Supplemental Fig. 1C-D, the critical information is the shape of the periodogram and the presence of a discontinuity, so we believe the plot sizes are appropriate.

      It may be of interest to apply the algorithms to a single-cell experimental data set which are qualitatively different (e.g., oscillation shape, damping).

      We have created a new supplemental figure (Supplemental Fig. 8) by applying the strategy and visualization used in Fig. 6 to SCN PER2::LUC recordings instead of wheel-running data, and have updated the text accordingly.

      Reviewer #1 (Significance (Required)):

      It has been previously shown that the chi-square periodogram algorithm has performance shortcomings for the analysis of circadian data (e.g. Zielinski et al., 2004). However, this study demonstrates exactly why, giving more conclusive evidence to support the conclusion that it should be avoided. This will be useful to many in the mammalian circadian community. It should be noted however that other algorithms are already favoured by other ciock communities (e.g. plant), even if a rigorous understanding of the biases were lacking.

      The methods developed here will be valuable for future comparisons of circadian algorithms. Of particular importance will be comparing algorithms for analysis of single-cell rhythms or non-stationary rhythms.

      Reviewer #2 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity (Required)):

      Chi-squared periodograms (CSP) are routinely used in circadian biology. In particular, this test has been used to determine circadian period in behavioral data (e.g. actigraphy) in mammals, flies and other species. This paper suggests that CSP, in some circumstances (e.g. where there are discontinuities), that CSP could be improved by changing the algorithm. They propose different steps to do this (e.g. using their greedy CSP code) and/or by using alternative tests such as Lomb-Scargle.

      The authors use simulated data to demonstrate their findings, and whilst I can see the benefits of this, it would be useful to benchmark the algorithms on actual real world circadian data (e.g. actograms from mouse or fly experiments). Although these types of data may not be publicly available, it would be highly likely to be available from multiple labs in the circadian field. In particular, fly datasets will be abundant in many clock labs. This would aid the utility of the papers findings for the field.

      Fig. 6 is entirely based on real-world circadian data (mouse wheel-running activity), as is the newly added Supplemental Fig. 8.

      Reviewer #2 (Significance (Required)):

      The paper is helpful for the circadian field when dealing with datasets that may contain discontinuities.

      It appears that the paper will be primarily useful for behavioral data, rather than, for example, transcriptomic time courses, since these tend to be much shorter and less sample intensive. Thus, it would be useful for circadian (and other) researchers analysing activity data in particular.

      My expertise is in circadian rhythms, both behavioural and molecular (e.g. sequencing) level analyses. Thus, I would be a possible end-user for the algorithms in this paper.

      Reviewer #3 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity (Required)):

      **Summary:**

      The authors identify a serious flaw in a popular method called Chi-squared periodogram (CSP) for period estimation in circadian rhythms. They systematically get to the source of the problem -- a discontinuity in the test statistic. This flaw leads to a bias in the period estimate. They present two modifications to the CSP, one of which they prefer. Nevertheless, they show that other more flexible methods such as Lomb-Scargle Periodogram work well without this discontinuity (bias) issue.

      **Major Comments:**

      1.One thing the authors do not include is timeseries lengths of non-integer days. Would it not be an interesting suggestion to choose a non-integer length time course, which is not a multiple of the periods of interest, and still continue using CSP as is ? This is also rather counter-intuitive.

      Figs. 3A and 6 and newly added Supplemental Fig. 8 use non-integer (24-h) days.

      2.I suppose the authors use a sampling resolution of 6min with wheel-running activity in mind. But it would be worth it in the interest of completeness to also consider a lower resolution. There is nothing in this study that ties it to the specific application, is it not?

      Although a sampling resolution of 6 minutes is not specific to wheel-running activity, we have added an analysis identical to that of Fig. 5 but with a resolution of 20 minutes (Supplemental Fig. 5). Additionally, the PER2::LUC SCN recordings analyzed in Supplemental Fig. 8 have a sampling resolution of 20 minutes.

      3.The authors discuss only the mean absolute error in the text but isn't the direction (sign) of the error also of interest. As far as I can see in Fig 5, conservative CSP overestimates and greedy CSP generally underestimates periods.

      We discuss both the error (references to Fig. 5A) and absolute error (references to Fig. 5B) in the text. We feel the interpretation suggested by the reviewer may be too reliant on the results of 3-day simulations, as the apparent underestimation by greedy appears far less substantial in simulations of 6 and 12 days.

      **Minor Comments:**

      1.I would like to see the formulae for the ratio of variances and p-values to be clear about how the authors computed the CSP. They describe it in words already, but I think some mathematics is warranted here.

      We have added the formula for the standard chi-square periodogram to the Methods section.

      2.It is nice to the see the raw data in the plots. But I would like to see the plot of the summary statistics (mean and variance/st. dev) for each of scatter plots to judge the size of bias. It is not easy to do this with the Excel sheet.

      We have overlaid a black circle representing the median and a vertical black line representing the 5th-95th percentile range onto Fig. 5 and Supplemental Figs. 3-7.

      Reviewer #3 (Significance (Required)):

      The authors present a sobering perspective on the chi-squared periodogram, which is still very popular among empirical biologists. They plainly show using artificial data that it is better to avoid the CSP when possible, although they suggest improvements to the CSP. The authors provide an R package to perform the analysis.

      There have been previous work that have highlighted other limitations of the CSP. This might be considered one more nail in the coffin of the CSP.

      I think this paper would be interest to both computational biologists and wet-lab biologists, but I think it ought to have a greater influence on the latter as the former already resort to more sophisticated approaches.

      My expertise is in Computational and Theoretical biology.

    1. from the beginning of the file

      Perhaps change "file" to "disk" as suggested by fat16.h. This part was very confusing.

      Also, there was no mentioning in the instructions that the root directory entries may not be exactly located as described in the picture, so perhaps change "78*512" to something like "depending on bpb".

      I'm not sure if these choices were intended to make sure that we carefully looked at the code documentations instead of just the instructions, but I think these parts should be adjusted so that other students aren't confused.

    1. For them, the notion of the paradigm neatly encapsulates the idea that truth and knowledge are relative, and depend upon the larger system of assumptions and relations from which they emerge.

      I think that it's very valuable for human kind to consider knowledge and truth as relative. Considering language itself limits our communication, what we believe to be truth could potentially limit our understandings of the world. All of this philosophical talk reminds me of Plato's Allegory of the Cave in which those who live in the cave know nothing but the cave. We, as well, may rely too heavily on what we know in the world.

    1. “enlarge knowledge by observation and experiment … so that nature being known, it may be mastered, managed, and used in the services of humane life.”

      The concept of a second creation and the Edenic recovery has a long history. Cicero in De natura deorum (45 B.C.) conceived of a second nature that humans create by channeling the rivers to suit their needs, by sowing and fertilizing the soil of the plains and the mountains to bear fruit and wheat, and by planting trees to shade their gardens and parks. Medieval Catholic and later Protestant theologians believed that Christians possessed a divine creative spark that would enable them to design tools and machines capable of transforming the land into a new Garden of Eden.

      Hughes, T. P. (2004). How to Think about Technology and Culture. In 944899466 737787191 T. Hughes (Ed.), Human Built World (pp. 17-27). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

      The chosen passage by Merchant points to the visions and views of Bacon specific to his revelation of the sciences role in nature. The involvement and exploitation of science in relation to the manipulation of nature is reinforced in Bacon's and some philosophers' frame of mind during this time. This is important to note because of the 'new organon' and that being resolved as scientific methods combined with mechanical methods to obtain a unified knowledge of material power and dominion over nature.

      This establishment of human dominion and the mechanical arts (teché) are what is described during this time as man operating nature to create something new. This is not far fetched from what we view design as today, the main difference being our acknowledging to our part in the manipulation of the land and the goal to not achieve dominion but balance. (in modern day landscape architecture practices) Bacon's objective in offering the significance of science and mechanical tools, point to achieving the "mastering or management of nature to obtain services for human life"; disagrees with these modern day views/practices.

      Hughes discusses the managing and human egocentrism as well, indicating the manipulation of water typologies such as rivers for the channelization of water, or tending the land for soil suitable for our chosen species of growth. Bacon seems to give reason for his bound of nature for our own use and advantage, in that in order to search the depths and beginnings of the Earth there must be a dissection of nature. From Merchant's critique of science as a manipulation method, we can begin to pull direct connections from Bacon's look at exploration into the remoter nature and its relation to to current day transgressions still in place. An example could be tree deforestation and the destruction and manipulation of nature to achieve a human-use or function.

    1. when light-wing'd toysOf feather'd Cupid seal with wanton dullnessMy speculative and officed instruments,

      Here in the use of Cupid we see that call back to the classics. Later on in the play I believe Othello refers to Desdemona as like a Cherub, another little angel baby from the classics. I think this sort of alludes to that sort of idealistic Love that may be associated with the classics. I think Desdemona and Othello did have an almost archetypal love story befitting that capital "L" Love we have talked about, but it was eventually corrupted.

    1. Some books have been attacked merely for being “controversial,” suggesting that for some people the purpose of education is not the investigation of ideas but rather the indoctrination of a certain set of beliefs and standards.

      This is a great statement right here. It is stating the true meaning of education and how it should be in society today and yet we have people around the world who think that just because something doesn't align with their ideas and may have children questioning their beliefs (like how regular adult conversation should have the right to) it is not right and should be kept in the dark from students. And yet, these ideas are found by students anyway, just in resources other than school and these resources can sadly steer students away from correct information (although schools can do that as well).

    2. ensuring students the freedom to choose to read any text and opposing “efforts of individuals or groups to limit the freedom of choice of others.”

      What do we think about this statement? I definitely agree that something like this is important, but I'm wondering what responsibility we, as teachers, have to filter out ideas in our classrooms given that some students may have difficulty with some of the ideas presented in these texts.

    1. Author Response

      Summary:

      As you will see the reviewers agreed that the premise behind this manuscript is important and timely both in the context of basic auditory science and for informing technology. However, they raised largely consistent concerns about the generalizability of your observations to other auditory stimuli and to more naturalistic listening conditions.

      We appreciate the reviewers’ positive assessment underpinning the significance and timeliness of our present research endeavours. We assume generalizability of our findings to more naturalistic listening conditions because the proposed model framework successfully explained the outcomes of experiments that were conducted under listening conditions differing in reverberation and source stimuli. Those differences, however, only occurred across but not within experiments and thus were not considered in the model explicitly. The set of experiments and relevant cues was chosen such that the investigation of decision strategies for the combination or selection of cues in the context of perceptual externalization could be conducted on a limited but still divers set of cues. The proposed framework allows to easily extend the set of cues. For example, in another work (see Li et al., in press), we successfully modelled the impact of situational changes of the amount of reverberation on externalization perception by extending the framework to reverberation-related cues. This further strengthens our assumption that our findings can be generalized. Nevertheless, we understand that more direct evidence for this generalizability would further increase the confidence in the conclusions we draw.

      Reviewer #1:

      I agree with the authors that the question at the basis of this work is timely and important both from the point of view of understanding auditory perception and for informing technology. However I am not convinced that the findings here will necessarily generalize to other stimuli/listening situations.

      I think the biggest limiting factor here is that the primary data on which the modelling is based are drawn from many different studies which used different stimuli, different tasks, different presentation environments and different equipment). I can see how testing the model on existing data is an important first step, but I would think that a critical next step is to form a set of (contrasting) predictions to be tested on a single stimulus set, within a single group of participants, as a way of confirming model validity. In this experiment I would also avoid using static non-reverberant environments since we know that these factors greatly affect spatial perception.

      We do not follow the reasoning why the above mentioned diversity of experimental paradigms is a limitation. On the contrary, in our opinion, the diversity of the considered experiments demonstrates robustness of our findings for a variety of experimental procedures. We agree that an additional validation experiment would further strengthen our study, but we question its necessity and still believe that the present modelling work is extensive and compelling enough to warrant publication.

      Other comments:

      1) The title greatly overstates the main findings, it would be toned down.

      In the title, we aimed at describing the research topic in general terms accessible to a broad readership. We take your comment as an advice to state the main findings instead.

      2) Intro, line 30-33 this statement is misleading. As written it appears to claim temporal aspects of auditory perception are based on short term regularity, whilst spatial perception is based on long term effects. This is not correct see e,g Ulanovsky 2004.

      Agreed. We will remove the sentence or rephrase it in more general terms because the misleading distinction is actually irrelevant to our study.

      3) As a reader not highly familiar with the auditory spatial processing literature I found the results section very dense and hard to follow. If you are targeting a general audience it is important to clarify concepts, avoid using abbreviations where possible etc.

      Thank you for your advice. We will aim to increase the level of abstraction within the results section.

      4) When discussing the various decision strategies which you tested, consider explaining how they might be implemented by the auditory system, at which stage of processing etc.

      Our study approached the problem from an algorithmic point of view and did not touch upon the more detailed level of neural implementation. While the cue processing has a clear neurophysiological basis in the subcortical layers of the auditory system, we will include some speculation about the involved cortical networks in a revised version of the manuscript.

      5) It is very difficult to evaluate your results without more information about the stimuli and studies from which they were taken. Whilst you do provide references, I think the paper would be much clearer if you provide a more complete description of the stimuli (even in table form; paradigms etc).

      We appreciate your advice and will provide more details about the simulated experiments in a table.

      Reviewer #2:

      The current study compares four decision rules, factoring in seven potential acoustic cues, for predicting perceived sound externalization for single-source binaural sound with stationary interaural cues. Test stimuli included a harmonic vowel complex, noise and speech. Results show that monaural and binaural cues shape externalization. However, how listeners weighted these cues varied across the tested conditions. The authors consider the fact that some of these cues covary acoustically, by additionally testing their model on subsets of two of these cues only. No single externalization cue emerged as a clear predictor for perceived externalization. However, overall, a static cue weighting strategy tended to outperform dynamic cue weighting for predicting externalization.

      Major concerns dampen enthusiasm for the current work.

      1) It is unclear what neural mechanism is being tested. A premise of the current approach is that perceived sound externalization is primarily driven by acoustic cues. However, we know this not to be true. Context matters. As pointed out by the authors (l370-372), when listening to sounds processed with head related transfer functions (HRTFs) over headphones, listeners can externalize sound better when the context of the test room matches the room where HRTFs were recorded (Werner and Klein 2014).

      Sound externalization is an auditory percept and as such primarily driven by acoustic cues. How those cues are used for perceptual inference is certainly context dependent. From the present study, we conclude that the auditory system evaluates deviations from a small set of expected acoustic cues in a fixed weighted (and not selective) manner. We further explain that these expectations, which are represented as templates in the model, must be adaptive to the context. This is well in line with your example of room divergence (Werner and Klein, 2004): listeners are thought to establish expectations about reverberation-related acoustic cues and evaluate incoming sensory information against those expectations with a fixed weighting between cues. If expectations are not met (i.e., acoustic cues deviate from their templates), perceptual externalization degrades.

      2) Most external sounds are neither anechoic nor stationary. Therefore, any neural decision metric on externalization must have been shaped by lifelong experience with dynamic, reverberant cues for interpreting externalization. The current work mostly models stationary single source sound that was either anechoic or mildly reverberant, providing pristine spatial cues. I do not follow the author's point that this would not matter (l498-502): "While the constant reverberation and visual information may or may not have stabilized auditory externalization, they certainly did not prevent the tested signal modifications to be effective within the tested condition. In our study, we thus assumed that such differences in experimental procedures do not modulate our effects of interest." That is an untested assumption.

      Others showed that the type of spectral manipulations we considered remain effective also if reverberation is present (e.g. Hassager et al., 2013) and if listeners are exposed to dynamic cues by moving their heads or the sound source (Brimijoin et al., 2013). We used the above-mentioned argument in order to motivate why we ignored certain differences across studies in the first place and the high explanatory power obtained with the proposed model framework suggests that this simplification was adequate. We agree that the above-mentioned sentence can be easily misunderstood and we will modify it by including the explanation stated here.

      3) Many of the current test stimuli are perceived as ambiguous - providing 50% externalization ratings - and thus do not provide a sensitive test of brain mechanisms of sound externalization.

      The field mostly agrees that auditory externalization is not a binary phenomenon but a matter of degree – we very recently published a review article that discusses this issue in detail (Best, et al., 2020). Hence, the experimental outcomes, denoted as externalization scores, ranging from 0 to 1 indicate the degree of externalization that is considered to mediate perceived egocentric distance. The externalization scores do not indicate the level of perceptual ambiguity.

      We will include this explanation in the manuscript in order to prevent further misunderstanding.

      4) Reverberation enhances perceived externalization, but this cannot be predicted by any of the tested decision metrics which only consider stationary monaural or binaural cues.

      True, there are also other cues potentially affecting the degree of auditory externalization. Reverberation-related acoustic cues are one of them. The main purpose of our study was to identify the basic functional mechanisms that integrates or selects between various cues – the purpose was not the identification of all possible cues that may affect auditory externalization. Thus, we chose a set of experiments that can be narrowed down a priori, particularly allowing to ignore reverberation-related cues.

      For the effect of reverberation-related cues, we point interested readers to another modelling study (Li et al., in press) that we conducted in parallel, in which we applied the here proposed framework also to reverberation-related cues and obtained good predictions.

      On balance, this reviewer is unconvinced that the current work will generalize to realistic dynamic and reverberant conditions.

      We agree with the reviewer that our study does not address dynamic and variable reverberant conditions. It was by-design limited to static conditions with fixed reverberation because we had no reason to believe that the targeted decision strategies applied to combine or select cues would be fundamentally different in more complex conditions.

      S. Werner and F. Klein, "Influence of Context Dependent Quality Parameters on the Perception of Externalization and Direction of an Auditory Event," presented at the AES 55th International Conference: Spatial Audio (2014 Aug.), conference paper 6-4.

      Reviewer #3:

      The manuscript "Decision making in auditory externalization perception" aims to identify cues that create/hinder an auditory externalization percept by using a template-based modeling approach. The approach as well as the findings are very interesting, and the study is thoroughly conducted. However, the manuscript adds little new knowledge to the field. Furthermore, a critical discussion is missing. The authors use a template-based model, but do not discuss the possible problems with such an approach. Particularly as each condition uses another model fit. This potentially allows the model to use cues that the auditory system cannot or does not consider. Nevertheless, the approach can still teach us which cues are potentially important for auditory externalization.

      1) The title seems inappropriate as the main work seems to be on the identification and combination of cues for externalization but not on the decision making.

      In combination with Reviewer #1’s first comment, we understand that the title could have been more specific. We will change the title accordingly.

      2) The model needs a more detailed explanation in the introduction. Otherwise the result section is not understandable without consulting the methods section.

      We will carefully re-evaluate which methodological details are necessary to understand the results section on a more abstract level.

      3) Add a Discussion on template-based models and fitting conditions. The risk of mathematical inspired models is that features are exploited that the auditory system cannot access. A more sophisticated front-end than a gammatone filterbank might reduce this risk. Alternatively, the use of physiologically inspired front-ends as in Scheidiger et al. (2018) might be interesting to consider. Nevertheless, I acknowledge that some of the features used in this study are backed by physiological and psychoacoustical studies.

      We agree with the concern behind the use of efficient functional approximations of the auditory periphery. Interestingly, however, we are very confident that this particular approximation does not provide spurious cues, especially in the context of monaural spectral shapes, because we did cross-validate the effectiveness of those cues with a physiologically more accurate model (Zilany et al., 2014) in previous work (Baumgartner et al., 2016).

      We will incorporate a corresponding explanation in the manuscript.

      4) It is known that the monaural spectral shape is important for externalization, for example from the studies that you have used. Thus, I partly question the novelty of the findings.

      We partly agree. It has also been suggested that interaural spectral cues are important for externalization perception. Further, it is also known that other cues contribute (e.g., reverberation-related cues as already discussed in response to the comments of Reviewer #2). Now, which cues contribute to which degree and how are they integrated? This is the main research question behind our study, with the ultimate goal to better understand the mechanisms of cue integration in the context of a perceptual inference task.

      5) I am not too familiar with template based models but I wonder if there is a problem if you use your models to fit and test with the same datasets?

      Cross-validation (i.e., using separate data sets for fitting/training, validating, and testing) is particularly important for complex models that allow overfitting. Such models can often be very closely fit to comparably small sets of data and thus the goodness of fit is not discriminative between those models. Here, in contrast, we compared the goodness of fit for models that contained a rather small and equal number of model parameters and this goodness of fit did strongly differ across models and was therefore informative for model selection in itself. If we separated the data sets, we would need to jointly assess the differences in initial model fits (to training data) together with the differences in predictive power (for testing data).

      References:

      Baumgartner, R., Majdak, P., & Laback, B. (2016). Modeling the effects of sensorineural hearing loss on sound localization in the median plane. Trends in Hearing, 20, 2331216516662003.

      Best, V., Baumgartner, R., Lavandier, M., Majdak, P., & Kopčo, N. (2020). Sound Externalization: A Review of Recent Research. Trends in Hearing, 24, 2331216520948390.

      Brimijoin, W. O., Boyd, A. W., & Akeroyd, M. A. (2013). The contribution of head movement to the externalization and internalization of sounds. PloS one, 8(12), e83068.

      Li, S., Baumgartner, R., & Peissig, J. (in press). Modeling perceived externalization of a static, lateral sound image. Acta Acustica.

      Zilany, M. S., Bruce, I. C., & Carney, L. H. (2014). Updated parameters and expanded simulation options for a model of the auditory periphery. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 135(1), 283-286.

    1. Millions of Americans were shocked and alarmed when presidential hopeful, and leading Republican presidential candidate, Donald Trump, not only announced his intent to build a wall along the United States-Mexican border to keep out “criminals and rapists,” but also demanded a ban on Muslim immigrants, even Syrian refugees, from entering the United States.16

      I think that it is racism, also he is afraid from Islam as he sees that is most of Muslims may cause danger, because of this stereotype that we discussed before that Muslims can be terrorists I think

    1. but between instructional design issues and the theories of human learning.

      I think this is a great field of study because we know so much about the psychology of humans that we're able to create proper ways of learning that will be successful. We see the issues in the design of learning and teaching and we can find the best solution based off of unique situation of each person. I think this is great for people who may have felt like a minority such as people who are dyslexic, deaf, have learning disabilities or even young children. There are experts who are meeting the needs of everyone to help us get the most out of learning.

    1. Federal Title IV grants and loans accounted for 71 percent of revenue at for-profit colleges

      When I read "for profit" institutions I immediately think of ITT Tech and how it came under fire for questionable practices--as may of their students fell under crippling debt.

      I think a question we should be asking is whether or not these "for profit"institutions should exist--these institutions' goal is to make revenue and would therefore have little incentive to provide anything else other than the programs they offer. They are however, able to offer flexibility that perhaps a "normal" (non-profit) 2-tear/4-year can't offer (night classes/or online classes) to people who work in the day; there may be some "non-profits" who can also offer this, but it is often at a very limited basis. After doing a quick google search, it seems as though many of these "for profit" schools are focused on trades, offering programs in automotive, nursing, etc.(there is also K-12 "for profit" education"). What would happen if these "for profit" institutions that are backed by private corporations and investors were instead backed by federal/state funding? Of course, that'd be a whole other discussion/debate and possible policy brief--but just a thought! Also it can be argued that even "non-profit" colleges/universities can be seen as "for profit" as they are often run as businesses (offer services, advertisement etc) and there is a reliance on tuition etc. to help pay for facilities, professors, etc. Key factors that differentiate these institutions from the "for-profits" is that they offer a space to create networks/socialize and at times produce knowledge/research that is shared with the world.

    1. now if it is uncertain whether god gave it, we rightly ask whether it was well given. Then if we find that (a) it was well given, we also find that it was given by him from whom all goods are given to the soul; or if we find that (b) it was not well given, then we realize it was not given by him Whom it is blasphemous to blame.[2] on the other hand, if it is certain that god gave it, then, no matter how it was given, we must recognize that it should neither (a) not have been given, nor (b) have been given otherwise than it was given. for it was given by him Whose deed cannot be faulted in any way

      This concept is interesting because it is a paradox. If it was given by God, then free will must simply be good because God is good. However, does that make sinning good? If sinning isn't good, does that mean that God made a mistake? According to Christian belief, anything that God gives cannot be bad and God does not make mistakes. If God didn't give free will, then who did? There is no answer to this because no one is considered as powerful as God is and no one would be able to give humans the ability to make their own choices. I'm not sure what the answer to this question is, but I believe that God designed us fully and gave us free will. I think that free will is a good thing because it gives us the ability to make our own choices and learn from our mistakes. Because of this a lot of good can come from making the wrong choices. While this doesn't always happen when one makes a mistake, it is a potential positive aspect. So, while sins/mistakes aren't always good, there may be silver linings.

    1. Author Response

      Reviewer #1:

      This manuscript provides evidence that drug administration during a reconsolidation window does not necessarily prevent memory recall, as has been shown by many groups. The authors attempted to replicate several published experiments and despite demonstrating that the drugs had other effects on the animals' behavior and physiology (e.g. weight gain), no effects on memory were observed.

      The paper is nicely prepared.

      We sincerely thank the reviewer for these kind words and the support to publish our replication efforts.

      Reviewer #2:

      General assessment:

      In this study, Luyten et al. aimed to replicate post-retrieval amnesia of auditory fear memories reported numerous times in the literature. They used a variety of behavioural approaches combined with systemic pharmacological treatments (propranolol, rapamycin, anisomycin, cycloheximide) after reactivation of fear memories. Interestingly, none of the treatments induced a significant decrease of freezing responses during subsequent retrieval tests. Authors strengthened their null results by using Bayesian statistics, confirming the absence of drug-induced amnesia.

      Overall, the study is really interesting. Experiments and analyses are very well designed and bring some important findings to the debated topic of post-retrieval amnesia and its clinical relevance.

      We are grateful that the reviewer appreciates our work and recognizes the general importance of our null findings. We genuinely thank them for the time that they took to evaluate our paper in detail and hope to provide some clarifications in our responses below.

      I have nevertheless several comments for the authors to consider.

      -Despite being very detailed, the authors should clarify and uniformize their Methods section and Supplemental information (e.g. number of CS, contexts used...) to improve the understanding of the different approaches. Similarly, methods for the reinstatement protocol (Exp 2) are missing.

      We understand that the information in the main text is quite dense, but we explicitly chose to focus on the central message here, i.e., that we applied standard procedures that should have allowed us to detect amnestic effects in consideration of most of the published literature. In addition, the crucial overview of the number of training and test trials, as well as the context that was used for each session is depicted in Fig. 1-3, immediately above the results of the respective experiments.

      In the Supplement, we provide a more extensive (and repetitive) report of the experimental procedures. The idea is that the reader can find the most important information in the main text, and all additional details in the Supplement (or in our preregistrations on the Open Science Framework: https://osf.io/j5dgx ). For example, in the main text, it is mentioned that reinstatement in Experiment 2 consisted of two US presentations in context A, one day before the final test (see p. 6 and Fig. 1C). The Supplement (p. 1) adds that the reinstatement session started with 300 s of acclimation, followed by the first US and 180 s later by the second US, and that the rat was removed from the context 120 s after last US onset. For all phases of Experiment 2, the US was a 0.7-mA, 1-s shock.

      • In exp 5, tests 1 and 2 are supposed to have 12 CS each. However, only 8 dots are represented on the graph. Did the authors average some freezing values after the initial 4 first CS presentations?

      Thank you for noticing this. We did not average freezing values, but just did not measure freezing on all trials, as we were not specifically interested in the concrete freezing levels on each trial, but rather in the overall extinction curve. As mentioned in the legend of Fig. 2, freezing during CS5-7-9-11 was not measured (and hence also not shown). In other words, the 8 dots on the graph represent CS1-2-3-4-6-8-10-12.

      -There is an obvious difference in baseline freezing response before the test in Exp 7 (Figure 5A-B). Discussion of these differences is an important point and was thoroughly discussed by the authors in the Supplement.

      Thank you for pointing this out.

      -Ln 384-387: "... additional Bayesian analyses were carried out that collectively suggested substantial evidence for the absence of an amnestic effect". Despite the "substantial effect" given by the meta-analysis, I am a bit confused by the meaning of an "anecdotal evidence against drug < control" reported in half of the experiments. How do the authors interpret these results?

      In short, Bayesian analyses provide evidence that is categorized starting from ‘no evidence’, to ‘anecdotal’, ‘substantial’, ‘strong’, etc. depending on the obtained Bayes factor. Grouping studies with anecdotal and substantial evidence in a meta-analysis can result in overall substantial evidence, which is what we observed here.

      Addressing this remark in more detail, we want to point out that the use of frequentist analyses (ANOVAs and t-tests) allowed us to conclude that we could not replicate the amnestic effects of previously published studies – we did not obtain a statistically significant amnestic effect although we had sufficient power to detect the effect sizes that had been previously reported. However, those analyses do not permit us to make inferences about the evidence against an amnestic effect. Bayesian analyses, on the other hand, do allow us to quantify the obtained evidence against an amnestic effect (i.e., the null hypothesis) for each single experiment or by combining the results of several studies. When a single study suggests only anecdotal evidence against an amnestic effect, this implies that we cannot conclude based on that study alone that we have proper evidence for the absence of an effect. Rather, we can only conclude that we have no evidence for the presence of an amnestic effect and weak (‘anecdotal’) evidence for its absence. However, a collective analysis of our studies does lead to the conclusion of substantial evidence for the absence of an amnestic effect overall.

      -The effect of cycloheximide on memory consolidation is indeed unexpected. Even if beyond the scope of the current study, what is the authors' hypothesis to explain that cycloheximide in their conditions induced a pro-mnesic effects on the consolidation of fear memories but altered the consolidation of extinction?

      As indicated by the reviewer, this is beyond the scope of the current study. We have no additional data on this effect and can only guess at its meaning. Also note that the effect was rather small and disappeared quickly during the test under extinction.

      One purely speculative hypothesis is that the injection with cycloheximide was more arousing than the vehicle injection, either due to sensations caused by the substance during injection or due to the rapidly emerging malaise it induced (or a combination of both), which we have documented in the Supplement (p. 5).

      In line with work by McGaugh, Roozendaal and colleagues, such arousal around the time of training could, in theory, enhance consolidation of a fearful memory, and thus explain greater fear memory during test (see e.g., Roozendaal & McGaugh (2011), https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026187 ). Then again, a similar argument could be made for improved consolidation of the extinction memory (de Quervain et al. (2019), https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-018-5116-0 ), which we did not observe. One could suggest that – assuming that we have observed ‘true’ effects here – the arousal component had the upper hand during the consolidation of the fear memory, while the protein synthesis inhibition overruled such effects during consolidation of the extinction memory. As this is all highly speculative, we prefer to not add this to the Discussion.

      -Cycloheximide seemed to induced post reconsolidation amnesia of fear memory after extinction training (Exp 8, Fig 3G) but not after single CS reactivation. Can the authors please develop this point? Is it possible that several presentations of the CS is required to destabilise the initial memory trace?

      First of all, it is important to emphasize that cycloheximide-treated rats in Experiment 8 (Fig. 3G) froze more during the CSs of Test 2 than control animals, arguing against a drug-induced reconsolidation blockade of the initial fear memory. Furthermore, the obvious within-session extinction during Test 1 in Experiment 8 suggests that it did not function as a typical reactivation-without-extinction session (Merlo et al. (2014), https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4001-13.2014 ).

      In light of the current literature, reactivation with a single CS is by far the most common way to destabilize a memory trace that was formed with one or three CS-US pairings. As mentioned in our paper, this should provide an appropriate degree of prediction error for the memory to become malleable (p. 12).

      Theoretically, it is indeed possible that more than one (e.g., two) CS presentations could allow for destabilization of the memory trace, although others who have used reactivation sessions with more than one CS presentation did not find the amnestic effects that they did observe with a single CS (Merlo et al. (2014); Sevenster et al. (2014), https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.035493.114 ).

      Reviewer #3:

      Luyten et al's study examines the phenomenon of drug-induced post-retrieval amnesia for auditory fear memories in rats, and report that after several experiments using Propranolol, Rapamycin, Anisomycin or Cycloheximide that they essentially observe no disruption of reconsolidation, (i.e., no amnesia). This is a well-executed, written and meticulous study examining an important phenomenon. The author's lack of observing amnesia using these "reconsolidation blockers" highlights an important fact that systemic administration of these drugs at the time of memory retrieval may not robustly influence reconsolidation processes despite what the existing literature may collectively indicate. The author's data clearly indicate this point and it is important the scientific community be made aware of these difficulties in blocking reconsolidation using systemic administration of these drugs.

      We are thankful for these generous comments and value the reviewer’s thorough and thoughtful assessment of our work. We also appreciate the reviewer’s position that it is important to get this message across to the scientific community.

      This group has previously published similar studies disputing similar phenomena. First highlighting a lack of amnesia following the reconsolidation-extinction paradigm and then more recently demonstrating a lack of amnesia attempting to block the reconsolidation of context fear memories. This is now their third installment focusing on Cued fear memories. Certainly, these findings are important, but arguably the novelty of such findings may be diminished a bit.

      We appreciate that the reviewer is well aware of some of our other work in this domain that supports a more general and widespread reproducibility crisis in this field.

      Regarding the novelty, one key point to stress here, which is also articulated in the paper (p. 3, 13), is that the current rodent findings (which we could not replicate) are the ones that provide the most direct basis for the clinical translations that have been proposed (e.g., by giving patients a propranolol pill after retrieval of a traumatic or phobic memory, see e.g., https://kindtclinics.com/en/ or Kindt & van Emmerik (2016), https://doi.org/10.1177/2045125316644541 ), and are therefore critical in their own right, not only because of their fundamental scientific relevance, but certainly also in light of their clinical reach.

      In one of the "control" experiments where the experimenters administer anisomycin immediately post training, they observe a paradoxical result - they observe memory strengthening instead of the expected blockade of consolidation and amnesia. This result highlights a number of things to consider when we interpret these overall results. For one protein synthesis inhibitors(PSIs) are toxic and when administered systemically usually result in inducing the animals to have diarrhea and generally just makes them sick. This of course will make the animals stressed and agitated and result in increasing their stress and likely amygdala activity. All of this could likely be the reason why the animals exhibited memory strengthening or no impairment in consolidation even with a PSI on board. See PMCID: PMC7147976. Figure 6. In this study, they could rescue the impairment of PSI on consolidation by increasing BLA principal neuron firing. Thus an important take away is something like this could easily be happening in the reconsolidation experiments - that there is no blockade because the animals are stressed either due to PSI on board or because some issues with experimenter/animal interactions, etc lead to higher BLA neural activity and rescue of the reconsolidation process.

      We agree that (systemic) protein synthesis inhibitors can induce signs of sickness in the animals (particularly in the first hours after injection) and have provided a detailed description of our relevant observations in the Supplement (p. 4-5). The reviewer is completely correct in stating that this may cause some amygdala activation which could interfere with the amnestic effects that we expected to see, as described in the paper by Shrestha, Ayata et al. (2020), and in line with our reply to Reviewer #2’s first comment regarding our cycloheximide experiment. Yet, effective induction of amnesia with these drugs has repeatedly been reported in the literature.

      Nevertheless, although relevant, the current remark has relatively little implications for our findings. In the large majority of our experiments, we did not use these toxic protein synthesis inhibitors (PSIs) (such as cycloheximide and anisomycin), but drugs that have generally been administered systemically throughout the literature (with successful amnestic effects). Furthermore, in the experiments where we did administer systemic cycloheximide or anisomycin, we observed no differences compared to vehicle-treated rats in contextual freezing (e.g., 9% on average in Experiment 7) immediately prior to the crucial test tones (Test 1, 24h after injection) – which argues against high levels of stress or agitation. Moreover, a blinded experimenter could not tell the difference between PSI-treated versus vehicle-treated animals while handling the animals for the test session, and observed no behavioral abnormalities, nor signs of pain or distress, as mentioned in the Supplement. We acknowledge that these experimenter observations may not entirely reflect what is happening in the animals’ amygdala, but they at least go against the notion that PSI-treated animals would be too sick to be tested properly.

      I don't think the authors go far enough articulating the important differences between systemic and intra-cranial administration of these drugs. Time is a potential factor. Immediate administration of the drug at high concentration in the target brain region (BLA) versus many minutes until the drug gets to the target region with uncertain concentration levels that may not mirror levels reached with intracranial administration. It's unfortunate the authors were not able to include intra-BLA administration of these drugs in this study. I do not necessarily expect them to do such experiments, since they have already done so much and it is not clear the laboratory has the appropriate expertise to conduct such experiments, but this comparison would be helpful.

      We fully agree that our results do not provide any information about the replicability of intracranial administration of drugs to induce post-retrieval amnesia of cued fear memories. We had already clearly acknowledged this in the first version of the paper (p. 11), but have now added an extra section to the Discussion (p. 13) to highlight this point in the new version posted on BioRxiv (Version 2). Notwithstanding the expertise of our laboratory to carry out intracranial infusions, we agree with the reviewer that such experiments are beyond the scope of this article.

      It is, however, noteworthy that the drugs that we used in 6 experiments did not necessarily rely on intracranial administration in prior successful studies. Rapamycin, for example, has generally been used systemically (not intracranially). Propranolol has been used either systemically or intracranially in rodents and always systemically in human subjects (healthy and patients). Bearing in mind the timing issue that was raised by the reviewer, we moreover included an experiment with pre-reactivation administration of propranolol (Experiment 4), where the drug was injected 5-8 minutes before the rats heard the reactivation tone.

      I think it is important that the authors make some statement of training conditions on cannulated versus cannulated rats. For example, every animal in Nader's 2000 study was bilaterally cannulated targeting the BLA. In contrast every animal in this study underwent no such surgery. I think this is relevant. In my experience non cannulated animals are a bit smarter than cannulated animals and the training conditions across these two differing groups may not equate to the same level of learning. And of course, differences in learning levels can lead to differences in the ability of the retrieved memory to destabilize.

      Thank you for pointing this out. We are aware that there may be differences between operated and non-operated animals and already briefly discussed this matter in the Supplement (p. 4). We have now also added this issue to the Discussion in the new section (p. 13) where we emphasize the differences between systemic and intracranial drug administration in relation to the previous comment.

      That being said, the comment regarding (non-)cannulated rats only really applies to Experiment 7 where we tested the effects of systemic anisomycin or cycloheximide. Prior cued fear conditioning studies indeed used intracranial administration of these drugs. The argument does not hold for Experiments 1-6, as systemic propranolol and rapamycin have repeatedly been reported to have amnestic effects in non-operated rats, with procedures identical to or closely resembling ours.

      The authors mention possibly examining markers of memory destabilization. GluR1 phosphorylation, Glur2 surface levels, protein degradation/ubiquitination have all been used to assess if destabilization has occurred. I do not fully agree with their reasons for not performing such experiments. They could examine some or one of these phenomena across differing training conditions between retrieval, no-retrieval animals. This likely could be informative. However, the authors may not possess the necessary expertise to conduct such experiments, so I'm not stating these experiments need to be completed, but certainly the study could be strengthened with such data.

      We agree that including yet more control experiments, using different experimental approaches could further strengthen the study. Nevertheless, the main conclusion of our paper – i.e., reconsolidation blockade using systemic administration of several drugs is considerably more difficult to reproduce than what the literature collectively indicates – is strongly and sufficiently supported by the data that we already report here. Overall, we believe that our conclusion does not require such additional controls. Moreover, even though the comparisons suggested by the reviewer could indeed be scientifically interesting, it is still unclear whether such experiments would provide sufficiently clear cut-offs as to which experimental condition would then allow for adequate memory destabilization and interference.

      Experiment 3E - Propranolol without reactivation. I don't see any data for this on the graphs. Am I missing something?

      Our apologies for the confusion. The legend shown next to Fig. 1F applies to all panels of Fig. 1, but only Experiment 1 (shown in Fig. 1A-B) contained a no-reactivation group as an additional control. Experiment 3 (shown in Fig. 1E-F) did not. We have moved the legend to the bottom of Fig. 1 to clarify this.

      The authors should probably cite this paper too, PMID: 21688892. The authors in this study find no evidence that propranolol inhibits cued fear memory reconsolidation.

      Thank you for bringing this to our attention. We were aware of this paper, but it had slipped through the cracks. We have cited it in the new version of the paper (p. 11).

    1. When adults assume attitudes that are in any way dismissive, judgmental, or all-knowing, we may silence all but the most outspoken of children and risk that even those brave voices fall on deaf ears.

      This is something important to remember because it changes the way a child perceives their own learning and capabilities. I think sometimes where this comes out is me treating children too casually I spend a lot of time with (my own child is a great example) instead of valuing their ideas as valid and at least exploring questions. While this is hard to do every time - the awareness helps us to better shape the moments we spend with children.

    1. Researchers who engage in CES often decrease their legitimacy within the academy since the positivist tradition, which they are challenging, is quite powerful. Thus, in many ways, they join the ranks of the “marginalized.” In short, issues of power and privilege are multilayered. Second, we need to understand, to the best of our ability, how systems of power shape the understanding and actions of individuals, including our own. Third, we need to be open in our communications and unafraid to ask questions that take us close to the central nervous system of racial difference, while also understanding why our questions may go unanswered. Open and frank conversations about race in our society are few and far between, leaving most of us woefully unprepared for how to productively and sensitively engage in such conversations. Fourth, we need to distinguish between individuals and institutions. While many urban universities have been prime movers in gentrifying their surrounding communities, many of the faculty who teach in those places have strongly opposed such actions. Creating spaces for those academics to come together to share experiences, lessons learned, and suggestions for moving forward, as URBAN has done, provides some comfort and support. Fifth, and on a proactive note, researchers need to be mindful of the privileges that often do accrue by virtue of working in a university and, if we are white, the privileges that come from our racial standing. Acting as empowerment agents, we can leverage this privilege in our work with community and youth partners. Finally, we need to accept and live with the knowledge that some tensions will never be resolved. However, if these tensions push us to further interrogate our roles, our work, our status, and our place in the larger fight for social justice, they may ultimately strengthen our collaborative work, enhancing our ability to effect positive social change

      I think this entire conclusion is incredibly strong and overall provides good discussion and analysis based upon the metonyms presented.

    1. When we buy new clothes not to keep ourselves warm but to look "well-dressed" we are not providing for any impor- tant need. We would not be sacrificing anything significant if we were to continue to wear our old clothes, and give the money to famine relief. By doing so, we would be preventing another person from starv- ing. It follows from what I have said earlier that we ought to give money away, rather than spend it on clothes which we do not need to keep us warm.

      I think the author does not take into account human behavior when making this argument, and he basically defines human necessities as food, shelter, water. He neglects to consider how gaining power and resources to increase fitness is woven into our genetic makeup. Social dominance is a primary characteristic of how humans function in societies, and quality of clothes are often an indication of high social status. I think that the author has faith in human selflessness, but I believe that inherent selfishness will prevent the idea of everyone living at a point of marginal utility from ever becoming a reality. Singer assumes that the majority of the human population has attained an elevated level of moral reasoning that, to be frank, very few people reach. I think that in general, people act in ways that are beneficial or mutually beneficial in terms of elevating status (social status, wealth, etc). I think it is difficult for people to discern a direct material benefit from helping others that may never be able to return the favor. Because of this, we are less likely to help.

    1. The strangeness of the past enables us to step back and look at our society and ourselves from a new perspective

      This quote is totally one of the key thoughts and philosophy of History its self. 'Strangeness' has a really interesting connotation to it, when somethings strange or different often times I want to know more about it. Just to see if it really is as strange as I thought? Because what's strange to you or me might be the "normal' for someone else. So if we view History's past it may seem strange because we are looking through present perspective. But if we alter the way we think just a little bit you can understand way that was normal at the time and even maybe start seeing similarities of past and present.

    1. Something is activated in us, perhaps even actualized, as we move.

      Is this suggesting that "we" evolve and find new solutions as we develop?

      I do think that this is maybe a bit of a romanticism of "turns", sometimes turns are brutal and, though Rogoff may not like it, technocratic or hard to adjust to. It seems as if Rogoff is suggesting we acquiesce to these easily.

    1. “Now it’s cool.”

      Pronouns, like other grammar tools, are very important. We've been using them for so long that they have become a serious part in our conversations about people other than ourselves. I wouldn't necessarily say that they've just suddenly become cool. I would say our interest has recently increased towards the subject when it comes to using the correct pronoun, as it shows our understanding of the identity of the person we are speaking about. Because someone's identity is important, their pronoun should be just as important. I think looking at pronouns as simply just a "part of speech" as Baron says is only looking at the basic layer of what a pronoun means. Messing up someone's pronoun can spark some conflicts, depending on the individual mentioned.

      But when you look at "they" (singular) versus they (plural), their doesn't seem to be an event where conflict would arise. I may have used they in reference to a singular being before, but I don't think there has been a time where it's crossed my mind to contemplate if what i'm saying is weird. Then again, I can't really think of many clear examples where I've used they as singular. I typically use he or she, since I tend to know the correct pronoun for the identity of the person i'm referring too. I guess myself, like others, may not see the distinction between the two as such an important topic, so we overlook it. But, since we've discussed how important semicolons and oxford commas are, maybe I'll start paying attention a little more.

    2. that this is a perfectly respectable option — and so unconscious that even those whocondemn it invoke it without noticing

      I think this kind of argument is the strongest I have heard in support of the singular "they". While people may not view that as "technically correct" while editing written work, it can often go unnoticed verbally. If someone left trash at the table, I feel like most people would say "someone left their trash." and not say something like "someone left his or her trash." And as we have discussed, grammar rules change (like the oxford comma). Who is to say that they/their is "incorrect"?

  7. lifeitself.us lifeitself.us
    1. s conditions of the residency: You will need to agree that Life Itself is not liable for any damage to you or your property.You are expected to participate in the community and care for the home ( eg: cooking, cleaning). Each home may have additional fees (eg: contribution of communal meals).Life Itself provides bedcover, sheets, pillow cover and towel.When leaving your residency we ask you to not leave any trace unless agreed, and put things back where you found them; to not colonise the space nor information (books, dvd’s etc.), and keep information, materials, equipment and spaces available as you found them. We will not pick you up at train stations or airport, that means that you have to come by your own means.Hubs can not take animals.Residencies are for individuals and we don’t offer it to partners. If you want to come with a partner please contact us and we can discuss this.Payment is by bank transfer or Paypal within two weeks of receipt of your acceptance to the residency (if this is a problem this can be discussed).You are responsible for obtaining any relevant travel permissions including visas. (Life Itself may be able to provide you with a letter of support).

      should be somewhere else I think

    1. Reviewer #2:

      In the manuscript by Galstyn et al on "Proofreading through spatial gradients", the authors proposed and studied a new kinetic proofreading (KP) model/scheme based on having a spatial gradient of the substrate (both "correct" and "wrong" ones) and the diffusive transport of the substrate-bound enzyme molecules to a spatially localized production site. The authors did an excellent job in explaining their new model and its connection and difference w.r.t. the classical Hopfield-Ninos KP mechanism. The key insight is that with spatial inhomogeneity, e.g., in the presence of a persistent spatial gradient for the enzyme or the substrate, one can consider spatial location as a state-variable. By having the substrate and product (or production site) at different spatial locations, these spatial degrees of freedom of the enzyme, i.e., enzymes at different physical location, can be considered as the intermediate states that are necessary for kinetic proofreading - each intermediate state contributes a certain probability for error-correction. In the original Hopfield-Ninos KP scheme, the intermediate state is provided by additional enzyme(s), whereas in this new KP scheme, it depends on having a spatial gradient, which the authors argue is more tunable. I like the theory for its simplicity and elegance. I have only a few mostly technical questions/comments.

      My main concern for this study, however, is about how relevant this mechanism is for realistic biological systems. The original Hopfield-Ninos KP mechanism was motivated by specific and important biological problems (puzzles), namely the unusually high fidelity in biochemical synthesis process (in comparison with its equilibrium value). In this MS, the theory is developed without a specific biological system or specific biological question in mind. It is true that spatial gradient exists across biological systems and the authors also showed that typical kinetic rates may fall in the functional range of this new gradient-dependent KP mechanism. But, what is the function of the original system that such a kinetic proofreading process can help improve? Is it biochemical synthesis? Do the authors envision "correct" and "wrong" biomolecules being produced at the production site (x=L) like in the original setting of Hopfield-Ninos? Or is it signaling like in the T-cell signaling case? If so, do the authors envision that both the correct signaling molecule and the incorrect signaling molecule have a spatial gradient and they can both be carried by the same enzyme to their functional sites? I am not asking for a detailed comparison with a specific system, but I think a known but unsolved biological phenomenon that may be explained by this new mechanism would really help motivate a biologist audience. Furthermore, a connection to a specific biological system could also lead to testable predictions that would ultimately verify (or falsify) the existence of this mechanism.

      Questions related to the model/theory:

      1) In this study, there is a production r for the enzymatic reaction at x=L where the enzyme is active. However, the effect of this reaction, which change ES-->E+P, is not considered in the model equations (1-3). Is it because r is considered to be small? If so, smaller than what? Since speed is directly related to r, how does the value of r affect the speed and the speed-accuracy trade-off?

      2) The nonmonotonic dependence of fidelity on the diffusion time for finite gradient as shown in Fig. 3c is intriguing. What determines the optimal diffusion constant (or diffusion time) when the fidelity is maximum for a given gradient length scale?

      3) The study of trade-off among energy dissipation, speed, and fidelity is quite nice and adds to a growing list of study on performance trade-off's in nonequilibrium systems. For example, a similar energy-speed-accuracy (ESA) trade-off was studied systematically in the context of adaptation in bacterial chemotaxis (Lan et al, Nature Physics 8, 422-428, 2012) and chemosensory adaptation in eukaryotic cells (Lan and Tu, J R Soc Interface 10 (87), 2013). In particular, the exponential dependence of the fidelity on power consumption (energy dissipation) shown in Fig. 4 in this MS agrees well with results in these earlier studies (see Fig. 3c and Eq. 5 in Lan et al, 2012; Fig. 4 in Lan&Tu, 2103). It would be informative to discuss the trade-off found here for the gradient-dependent KP scheme in comparison with similar trade-off relations in other systems.

      4) The power dissipation P is computed by Eq.8 in this MS. Where does Eq. 8 come from? What's the physical meaning of P? The standard way to compute energy dissipation is by computing the entropy production rate S', which is well defined. Then by assuming the internal energy does not change with time in steady state, we equate energy dissipation with kT*S'. The form of entropy production rate is known and can be found in text book (such as those from T. Hill) and papers (e.g., those from H. Qian and collaborators; and from U. Seifert and collaborators), and the formula given in Eq. 8 does not seem to be consistent with the known form of entropy production. In particular, for a given reaction with forward flux J+ and backward flux J-, the entropy production rate is: (J+-J-)ln(J+/J-), which can be easily shown to be positive definite and only =0 when detailed balance J+=J- is satisfied.

      Overall, the MS provided a new gradient-dependent scheme for error correction in chemical systems. The study of trade-off among energy dissipation, speed, and fidelity (accuracy) in this new mechanism is also valuable for the general study of cost-performance relation in non-equilibrium systems. My main concern is the lack of examples of specific biological systems where this gradient-dependent error correction mechanism could be at work to enhance the specific biological functions of these systems.

    1. Author Response

      We thank the editors for considering our manuscript for publication in eLife and the reviewers for their work. However, we would like to discuss several of their comments.

      The key issue seems to be a lack of novelty of our work, which is not correct in our opinion.

      We would like to quickly reiterate why we think that our findings are novel and have very broad implications.

      The importance of polygenic adaptation is becoming increasingly clear. Unfortunately, it is widely assumed that polygenic adaptation is very difficult, if not impossible, to study in natural populations, because the associated allele frequency shifts are too small to be experimentally characterized (Pritchard et al., 2010). Hence, typically the collective response of many loci are considered, which frequently results in wrong results due to population stratification (Berg et al., 2019; Sohail et al., 2019).

      Therefore, we have used experimental evolution to characterize polygenic adaptation. Experimental evolution is widely recognized as a powerful tool because of the possibility to replicate experiments. Here, we expand the power of experimental evolution by an hitherto unrecognized aspect: the impact of linkage disequilibrium - we demonstrate that two founder populations with different levels of linkage disequilibrium (LD) result in entirely different selection responses. The consequence of different LD structures is shown by our observation that the same population (i.e. identical LD structure) evolving in two different environments shows the same selection response, but a different population with different LD structure in the same environment shows different selection responses.

      This result has important implications for all studies of polygenic adaptation in natural populations because LD is not accounted for in studies of polygenic adaptation, but like in our study, haplotype blocks with multiple loci could result in a strongly selected allele. Hence, LD will determine the likelihood of this to occur. Furthermore, accounting for linkage provides the opportunity to study polygenic adaptation also in natural populations - a substantial change to the current testing paradigms.

      The second key result of our study is that we demonstrate that selection in hot and cold environments does not fit the simple model of polygenic adaptation, where the same set of loci is responding in different directions, when opposing selection regimes are applied. As pointed out by reviewer #2, this is particularly important as it shows that current models of polygenic adaptation are not well-suited to understand adaptation imposed by contrasting ecological factors. We show that there is almost no overlap between the haplotype blocks selected in the hot and cold environment. Most importantly, this is not a matter of power as we show that the blocks responding in one selection regime are not changing their frequency in the opposite direction in the other selection regime. We anticipate that this insight will have a profound impact on theoretical models of polygenic adaptation. Furthermore, as we studied temperature adaptation, our results will have also important consequences for the battery of ongoing studies aiming to link selection signatures to response to climate change.

      In brief, we think that very minor clarifications in our manuscript can solve the technical issues identified by the reviewers and will provide a clearer picture about the general implications of our findings.

      A detailed response to the comments of the reviewers is given below.

      Reviewer #1:

      Otte et al. used an evolve and re-sequence strategy to explore "the genetic architecture of adaptive phenotypes". The authors previously found different genetic architectures across different founder populations evolving in a common hot environment. The authors chose one of these founder populations for replicated experimental evolution (5 replicate populations) in a cold environment for 50 generations. The authors were surprised to discover the same number of loci evolve under strong selection between the hot-evolved and cold-evolved replicate populations, though the 20-ish loci are largely non-overlapping. The distribution of selection coefficients was also similar. They interpret this commonality as evidence that the founder population history has a larger effect on adaptive architecture than the selection regime.

      The study demonstrates a comprehensive effort to discover the number of genome regions and distribution of selection coefficients that emerge from a highly controlled experimental evolution project. The experienced team applies a sophisticated toolkit to this powerful experimental design - a toolkit that grows ever more sophisticated with each new experimental run that they perform. However, the authors set me up to learn why such different adaptive architectures emerge from different founder populations. Ultimately, the researchers acknowledge that they "cannot pinpoint the cause for the differences in the inferred adaptive architecture..."

      Here, the reviewer correctly identified one of the main new questions that arose from the new experiment we performed in this study. In a large part of the discussion and the associated analyses we are providing answers to this question, i.e. possible alternative explanations for the different observed architectures in the Portugal vs. the Florida population. We can indeed not pinpoint "the" cause for the differences that the reviewer seems to request here as a definite answer, but we favour one of the explanations that has not yet been discussed in literature previously (LD).

      Some results simply recapitulated the previous Portugal E&R study and other results recapitulated a D. melanogaster E&R study.

      This statement about "some results" is ignoring the main new experiment of this study, which is the Portugal population evolving in a cold temperature. For this, we carried out a new selection experiment in a new environment, which finds different selection targets than the previously published experiments. This new experiment therefore does not recapitulate the previous results. We then compare this new experiment to a previous one, and this comparison raises a set of new questions that we address in this manuscript. Only for the purpose of making that comparison, we indeed "simply recapitulated" "some results" of the previous study. The statement is therefore misleading in the way it is put here. Furthermore, the D. melanogaster study is also not recapitulated: in that study, it was not possible to identify selected haplotypes. The D. melanogaster study was therefore unable to determine how many selection targets were shared between the hot and cold selection regimes. The identification of selected haplotypes was a major improvement in this study, which made it possible only now to determine how many targets are shared and to evaluate whether selection targets behave as predicted by the trait optimum model.

      I did not find the "common adaptive architecture" across different selection regimes to be a particularly compelling discovery of sufficiently broad interest.

      This is a very subjective opinion and it would be good if the reviewer had explained why this is no interesting discovery to her/him. We feel that this statement simply reflects that the reviewer does not fully appreciate the complexity of polygenic adaptation. We would like to point out again, that this result has important implications for the interpretation of selection signatures in natural populations.

      Other concerns and questions can be found below:

      Major concerns:

      1) Pg. 4: It is my understanding that the power of multiple populations from a single founder evolving in parallel allows for more rigorous identification of loci targeted by selection. I found it surprising to discover that if a lack of replication emerges from an experimental evolution study, this outcome is interpreted as "genetic redundancy." First, genetic redundancy has a precise definition in genetics that muddles the author's meaning. And second this interpretation seems rather post-hoc.

      This statement shows that the reviewer is disregarding the work of Barghi et al (2019, PLoS Biology) and the definition of redundancy in the context of polygenic adaptation as discussed by Laruson et al. (2020) or Barghi et al 2020 (Nature Reviews Genetics). In any case, this is a semantic issue and should not be considered as a major issue with our manuscript.

      2) To "shed more light on the different selection responses" is a weak motivation. The introduction sets me up to understand why selection responses are so different but no major insights into the "why" emerge from the cold-adaptation experiment.

      We modestly disagree - we clearly discuss different explanations of “why” and favor one of them (LD)

      3) More explanation of figure 1 in the main text is needed. Does each point correspond to a SNP that consistently changes across all five populations? Or is this the union?

      The reviewer does not seem to be familiar with the statistical analyses that have been used in our study in the same way as it is common practice in the field. Despite the common use of this test, we still provided a detailed explanation in M&M and explicitly mentioned the test in the figure legend. But this can easily be detailed even further and should not be a major issue with this manuscript.

      4) Line 210: How did the researchers define "stress" and determine that the degree of stress is equivalent across two temperature regimes? The absence of these data undermine the potency of the comparison.

      It is not clear why the reviewer requires a more elaborate definition of temperature stress - the concept of extreme temperatures imposing stress is well established and we cite the relevant literature for Drosophila in the text. Furthermore, it is not apparent why the reviewer requests the degree of stress to be equivalent between the two temperature regimes.

      5) How can the authors be sure that the only difference between the hot and cold populations was temperature? Was competition/population size/etc held constant? Might the lack of overlap between hot and cold adapted loci stem from one such regime selecting for a different phenotype? (i.e., not temperature tolerance)

      As clearly stated in M&M, the culture conditions were the same with the exception of temperature.

      6) Line 237: The authors assert that most alleles show a temperature-specific response - a discovery with precedent in the literature, including from this team of researchers. The authors attribute the absence of common loci between temperature regimes to the high number of generations (50) compared to the number across seasons cited in Bergland et al. The researcher could easily look for common targets at earlier time points of experimental evolution to test this idea.

      This is an interesting suggestion, but the reviewer fails to explain why the analysis of early generations should be more informative than the analysis of later generations. Several studies have already documented the opposite.

      7) Line 292-293: This section reads as disingenuous - the researchers could have explored overlap between Portugal and Florida founders using only the selected loci coordinates and look for non-random overlap using simulations/resampling tests.

      The reviewer seems to assume that we could easily apply the same test for overlap that we used for the hot vs. cold comparison within the Portugal population to the Portugal hot vs. Florida hot comparison. But this is not feasible, and we clearly explain why the comparison of selected haplotype blocks between different founder populations is not helpful (low LD results in different haplotype blocks - even with the same target)

      8) Discussion: The speculation about why such different architectures emerged across Portugal and Florida was diluted by the absence of initial fitness estimation upon subjection to a cold environment (which would have offered evidence for different initial "optima" across founder populations) as well as the change in fitness from generation 0 to generation 50.

      It is not apparent why the reviewer requests a fitness estimate at the cold environment. Our analysis only included a single population in the cold environment. Hence, the only informative comparison is the one in the hot environment which has been done for both populations and is referenced in the manuscript.

      9) The simulations and corresponding discussion would make for an interesting review/opinion piece but not as new results for this manuscript.

      Unlike the reviewer, we think that a good discussion puts the results into perspective with different hypotheses on how to explain it and link this to the current literature.

      Minor Comments:

      1) Pg. 3. The recurrent citation of Barghi et al. in the Introduction undermined the reader's impression that fundamental questions are being addressed in this article.

      Maybe it escaped the reviewer’s attention that we cited three different Barghi et al. papers and only one reports experimental data (cited only once), while the others are required to describe the theoretical framework, including the concept of "redundancy" which the reviewer misunderstood. New fundamental questions in this current manuscript are addressed using the Portugal population, which was selected in a cold temperature regime (not hot-evolved Florida, which was the topic of Barghi et al. 2019).

      2) Lines 33-39: The argument that parallel signatures of selection across distinct natural populations are insufficient to address the polygenic basis of adaptive phenotypes, and so comparatively more contrived E&R studies are required, was unconvincing.

      Unfortunately, the reviewer does not provide support for this strong statement. In fact, we find the statement of “contrived E&R studies” not as objective as we would have liked to see in a scientific discourse.

      3) Line 158: Confusing. Should "among" actually be "within"?

      The reviewer is not right - the correct wording is "among" not within: multiple different haplotypes can carry the actual target of selection, and they can differ by additional variants which themselves are not selected for. Multiple haplotypes with the selection target are also experiencing more pronounced frequency changes than expected under neutrality. The correlation of their allele frequency trajectories depends, however, on the extent that hitchhiking SNPs are shared among these haplotypes. To account for this, we used a less stringent correlation cutoff.

      4) Line 486: I believe that the authors would be hard-pressed to find in the literature a paper declaring that "single population...[is] sufficient to understand the genetic basis of adaptive traits".

      In fact, many selection tests are targeting only a single population and most studies only apply them to a single population.

      Reviewer #2:

      This reviewer mainly asks us to discuss some of his/her ideas - this can be done, but since reviewer#1 felt already that there is too much discussion in our manuscript this is a bit of a mixed message.

      Overall Review: This is another commendable study from the Schloterer lab that features next generation genome-wide sequencing of multiple evolving populations. It compares results obtained with two different selection regimes, one hot and one cold, and two different founding populations of Drosophila simulans, one from Portugal and one from Florida. The results reveal a lack of consistency among selection regimes and founding populations. Temperature-dependent adaptation is shown to be "local" or "contingent," rather than globally consistent. My chief recommendations concern the experimental and theoretical contexts within which this study should be interpreted.

      Major points:

      1) I do not require any additional data collection or statistical revision. My comments are organized in terms of experimental paradigm (A) and theoretical significance (B).

      A.

      2) The typical paradigm for experimental evolution in this and many other labs is the use of hybrid populations created from isofemale lines. This method for founding experimental populations can be expected to generate some degree of random "historicity" as the isofemale lines approach fixation of specific genotypes with high stochasticity. Then there are further stochastic and historical effects which arise when such lines are hybridized. The strengths and limitations of this paradigm should be addressed. Most importantly, such stochastic historical effects might be the source of the discrepancy between the replicate lines derived from Portugal and Florida.

      We would like to emphasize that we were using freshly established isofemale lines kept in the laboratory for at most 10 generations, as stated in the M&M section.

      3) As the authors themselves point out, there is a comparative difficulty arising from the different scales of replication used for the Florida versus Portugal experiments.

      The reviewer is correct, and since we were aware of this, we performed statistical tests to account for this.

      A further question for large-scale experimentation is whether a larger and uniform level of replication might produce more similar results, such as 20 evolving populations from each source. Or indeed, three sets of ten evolving populations from three distinct founders from the two sources, with a total of 60 evolving experimental lineages. The authors should discuss whether they believe that their findings would hold up with such an expanded experimental protocol.

      This is an interesting thought of its own, but we feel that it does not contribute much to our current study.

      4) The authors themselves point out at one point that their experiments might have benefitted from some phenotypic characterization of the presumed temperature adaptation. That raises the more general question of how the field of experimental evolution can progress with some labs just doing phenotypes and other labs just doing genome-wide sequencing. Surely this and other studies would be strengthened by combining the two types of assay. Furthermore, genomic evolution might be usefully analyzed in terms of the degree to which specific genomic changes can be associated with specific phenotypic changes, as that is the foundation for adaptation itself.

      We would like to draw the attention to the fact that we performed a laboratory natural selection experiment, for which the environmental factor is known, but not the actually selected phenotype - hence the phenotyping is not as trivial as implied by the reviewer.

      B.

      5) This is yet another study that finds difficulties with the invocation of noroptimal selection along a one-dimensional functional gradient. Such models have been long-standing favorites of evolutionary theorists, such as Kimura and Lande. But that preference may arise more from the ease with which these models can be formulated and analyzed by theoreticians. Actual evolving populations don't seem to embody the precepts of such theory, whether the issue is the maintenance of genetic variation (see the work of Turelli, for example) or the evolution of closely studied populations, as illustrated by this study. An alternative point of view that the authors should discuss is that such models are indeed NOT usually correct.

      It is very interesting that this reviewer feels that our data demonstrate that the prevailing model of polygenic adaptation is wrong, but our manuscript is still considered to be of insufficient novelty.

      6) There are alternative theoretical frameworks that address the maintenance of genetic variation and the response to selection. Among these are schemes of protected polymorphism arising from overdominance, epistasis, and frequency-dependent selection. If the thrust of the preceding point 4 is accepted, then it would be theoretically salient for the authors to suggest what type of underlying population genetic machinery would best account for their findings, in place of the noroptimal selection-mutation balance model.

      We thank the reviewer for these interesting suggestions. However, their predictions are not at all trivial to test. For this reason, generations of population geneticists tried to test them, so we feel that this task is well beyond the scope of this manuscript.

      Reviewer #3:

      In their manuscript 'The adaptive architecture is shaped by population ancestry and not by selection regime,' Otte and colleagues use an evolve and resequence strategy to examine the response of a Portugal population of D. simulans responds to cold temperature. The authors identify putative targets of selection and compare the number of targets, their location, and the distribution of selection coefficients to previous work on the same population exposed to hot temperatures as well as a different population exposed to hot temperatures. The topic is of general interest, the work is sound and the writing is clear and concise.

      1) It is not clear what the novel contribution of this manuscript is. The title indicates that the key finding is that population of origin mediates response to selection rather than the selection regime. However, the authors fail to provide compelling data to support that. The data are from 1 population under two selection regimes and a second population under one of those regimes. There simply aren't enough comparisons to infer that population ancestry plays a bigger role than selection regime in adaptive evolution.

      We disagree with the reviewer and would like to repeat the logic of our experiment:

      Comparison 1: contrast of different populations in the same environment -> different architecture

      Comparison 2: contrast of the same population in different environments -> same architecture

      With this simple design it is possible to reach the conclusion that the architecture is affected by population history more than by selection regime and no more populations are needed to reach this conclusion. This insight has not been reported before.

      2) The authors also seem to argue that a contribution of this paper is that it illustrates that temperature adaptation is not a single trait. This was the major finding of a 2014 paper from the same group in D. melanogaster- a single founder population was exposed to hot and cold temperatures and the authors found almost no overlap between the putatively selected variants in the two different temperature regimes.

      We would like to point out that the analysis of Tobler et al. (2014) is on the basis of individual SNPs, which is difficult to interpret because of the many segregating inversions in D. melanogaster. All the complications of these data and the implications for the interpretation can be found in the discussion of Tobler et al. (2014). In the current study, we are identifying selected haplotype blocks, which is mandatory to compare the architectures and selection responses.

      3) Beyond the limited impact of the current work, there are some additional specific issues. The authors note that it was 'remarkable' that the distribution of selection coefficients and the number of inferred selection targets between the hot and cold experiments was 'highly similar.' What is the null expectation? Where does the null come from?

      This is a minor semantic issue. Naturally, there is no null model for the number of selection targets, but if two populations selected for the same trait provide different architectures, different selection regimes should be even more likely to generate different architectures.

      4) The discussion is somewhat unsatisfying and largely speculative. The 'different trait optima' section reads as straw man; this could be reframed to better guide the reader.

      Naturally, the discussion intends to put the results in a broader context. It would have been helpful to read how s/he envisions a reframing that would improve the manuscript.

      There is little support for the 'differences in adaptive variation' hypothesis.

      It would have been helpful to read which kind of support the reviewer would have expected beyond the evidence we have already provided.

      The section on LD was interesting, but the simulation findings should reside in the results section.

      This could be easily moved, but we feel that it is well-placed in the discussion as we use the simulations to compensate for the lack of literature on this field (again demonstrating the novelty of our manuscript).

      References:

      Barghi, N., R. Tobler, V. Nolte, A. M. Jakšić, F. Mallard, K. A. Otte, M. Dolezal, T. Taus, R. Kofler, & C. Schlötterer (2019). Genetic redundancy fuels polygenic adaptation in Drosophila. PLOS Biology 17: e3000128.

      Barghi, N., J. Hermisson, & C. Schlötterer (2020). Polygenic adaptation: a unifying framework to understand positive selection. Nature Reviews Genetics . Berg, J.J., Harpak, A., Sinnott-Armstrong, N., Joergensen, A.M., Mostafavi, H., Field, Y., Boyle, E.A., Zhang, X., Racimo, F., Pritchard, J.K., et al. (2019). Reduced signal for polygenic adaptation of height in UK Biobank. Elife 8.

      Bergland, A. O., E. L. Behrman, K. R. O’Brien, P. S. Schmidt, & D. A. Petrov (2014). Genomic Evidence of Rapid and Stable Adaptive Oscillations over Seasonal Time Scales in Drosophila. PLoS Genetics 10, e1004775.

      Láruson, Á. J., S. Yeaman, & K. E. Lotterhos (2020). The Importance of Genetic Redundancy in Evolution. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 35: 809–822. Pritchard, J.K., Pickrell, J.K., and Coop, G. (2010). The genetics of human adaptation: hard sweeps, soft sweeps, and polygenic adaptation. Current biology : CB 20, R208-215.

      Sohail, M., Maier, R.M., Ganna, A., Bloemendal, A., Martin, A.R., Turchin, M.C., Chiang, C.W., Hirschhorn, J., Daly, M.J., Patterson, N., et al. (2019). Polygenic adaptation on height is overestimated due to uncorrected stratification in genome-wide association studies. Elife 8.

    1. Children have a right to pretend being dead and think about what it means

      This right really stood out to me as my children do talk a lot about death and, initially, I recognised feelings of discomfort within me when they did. I am continuing to learn how important it is for children to have the right to discuss or express topics which may sometimes be 'taboo' or just difficult, through words and play. I love that this is inside the charter. We read through the charter in class today!

    1. We have to limit fortunes. Our present plan is that we will allow no one man to own more that $50,000,000. We think that with that limit we will be able to carry out the balance of the program. It may be necessary that we limit it to less than $50,000,000. It may be necessary, in working out of the plans that no man’s fortune would be more than $10,000,000 or $15,000,000. But be that as it may, it will still be more than any one man, or any one man and his children and their children, will be able to spend in their lifetimes; and it is not necessary or reasonable to have wealth piled up beyond that point where we cannot prevent poverty among the masses.

      Huey P. Long's vision of wealth distribution was a bold proposition for his time. Especially after the onset of the great depression. I understand how he feels that wealth should be spread throughout the working classes however I feel he walks a fine line when regulating how someone can earn or spend their money. He refers often to the Deceleration of independence when mentioning the pursuit of happiness but how can one person limit another persons ability to earn money and still live by those words? Other forms of regulating such as higher taxes or creating laws to prevent monopolizing the market I think are a more reasonable approach than putting a cap on how much you can earn. Especially in a country which allows its people simple liberties such as earning a living with no government resistance.

    2. Now, we have organized a society, and we call it “Share Our Wealth Society,” a society with the motto “Every Man a King.” Every man a king, so there would be no such thing as a man or woman who did not have the necessities of life, who would not be dependent upon the whims and caprices and ipsi dixit [unproved assertion] of the financial barons for a living. What do we propose by this society? We propose to limit the wealth of big men in the country. There is an average of $15,000 in wealth to every family in America. That is right here today. We do not propose to divide it up equally. We do not propose a division of wealth, but we propose to limit poverty that we will allow to be inflicted upon any man’s family. We will not say we are going to try to guarantee any equality, or $15,000 to a family. No; but we do say that one third of the average is low enough for any one family to hold, that there should be a guarantee of a family wealth of around $5,000; enough for a home, an automobile, a radio, and the ordinary conveniences, and the opportunity to educate their children; a fair share of the income of this land thereafter to that family so there will be no such thing as merely the select to have those things, and so there will be no such thing as a family living in poverty and distress. We have to limit fortunes. Our present plan is that we will allow no one man to own more that $50,000,000. We think that with that limit we will be able to carry out the balance of the program. It may be necessary that we limit it to less than $50,000,000. It may be necessary, in working out of the plans that no man’s fortune would be more than $10,000,000 or $15,000,000. But be that as it may, it will still be more than any one man, or any one man and his children and their children, will be able to spend in their lifetimes; and it is not necessary or reasonable to have wealth piled up beyond that point where we cannot prevent poverty among the masses.

      Provides a response to the proposed issues mentioned in previous paragraph. Agrees that every man should be a king and not long for basic human needs. Proposes made fortunes made should be capped to allows equal playing field for generations to come. This appeals to his listeners and may establish support.

    3. We have to limit fortunes. Our present plan is that we will allow no one man to own more that $50,000,000. We think that with that limit we will be able to carry out the balance of the program. It may be necessary that we limit it to less than $50,000,000. It may be necessary, in working out of the plans that no man’s fortune would be more than $10,000,000 or $15,000,000. But be that as it may, it will still be more than any one man, or any one man and his children and their children, will be able to spend in their lifetimes; and it is not necessary or reasonable to have wealth piled up beyond that point where we cannot prevent poverty among the masses.

      I think Huey P. Long's heart was in the right place to try and eliminate poverty after the Great Depression. The "Every Man a King" plan seemed like a Robin Hood plan to take from the wealthy and give to the poor. No doubt every American deserves the right to life, liberty, and pursuit of happiness as described in the Declaration of Independence but that shouldn't be at the detriment of the more wealthy Americans. By allowing there to be a cap on the accumulated wealth of Americans only hurts the country as a whole. The American Dream would die with this type of Economic plan. Socialism and welfare could still be accomplished without limiting the success of other Americans.

    4. We have to limit fortunes. Our present plan is that we will allow no one man to own more that $50,000,000. We think that with that limit we will be able to carry out the balance of the program. It may be necessary that we limit it to less than $50,000,000. It may be necessary, in working out of the plans that no man’s fortune would be more than $10,000,000 or $15,000,000. But be that as it may, it will still be more than any one man, or any one man and his children and their children, will be able to spend in their lifetimes; and it is not necessary or reasonable to have wealth piled up beyond that point where we cannot prevent poverty among the masses.

      Long proposes to limit fortunes, he alluded to people hoarding, people having too much in stark contract with the rest living in poverty. Long said this not reasonable and it is a complete waste. Long's idea is to spread the wealth around. The rich has a tendency to amass more riches, while the poor gets deeper into poverty

    5. We have a marvelous love for this Government of ours; in fact, it is almost a religion, and it is well that it should be, because we have a splendid form of government and we have a splendid set of laws. We have everything here that we need, except that we have neglected the fundamentals upon which the American Government was principally predicated.

      Huey wants the government to be treated equally and everyone should get a share in the same pot. Even though he may sound bias, as putting his government thoughts ahead of what others think, he still wants equal rights for everyone. Even in present day, people are not created equal. Things are not shared equally, the rich gets richer and the poor gets poorer.

    6. Here is the whole sum and substance of the share-our-wealth movement: Every family to be furnished by the Government a homestead allowance, free of debt, of not less than one-third the average family wealth of the country, which means, at the lowest, that every family shall have the reasonable comforts of life up to a value of from $5,000 to $6,000. No person to have a fortune of more than 100 to 300 times the average family fortune, which means that the limit to fortunes is between $1,500,000 and $5,000,000, with annual capital levy taxes imposed on all above $1,000,000. The yearly income of every family shall be not less than one-third of the average family income, which means that, according to the estimates of the statisticians of the United States Government and Wall Street, no family’s annual income would be less than from $2,000 to $2,500. No yearly income shall be allowed to any person larger than from 100 to 300 times the size of the average family income, which means; that no person would be allowed to earn in any year more than from $600,000 to $1,800,000, all to be subject to present income-tax laws. To limit or regulate the hours of work to such an extent as to prevent overproduction; the most modern and efficient machinery would be encouraged, so that as much would be produced as possible so as to satisfy all demands of the people, but to also allow the maximum time to the workers for recreation, convenience, education, and luxuries of life. An old-age pension to the persons of 60. To balance agricultural production with what can be consumed according to the laws of God, which includes the preserving and storage of surplus commodities to be paid for and held by the Government for the emergencies when such are needed. … To pay the veterans of our wars what we owe them and to care for their disabled. Education and training for all children to be equal in opportunity in all schools, colleges, universities, and other institutions for training in the professions and vocations of life; to be regulated on the capacity of children to learn, and not on the ability of parents to pay the costs. Training for life’s work to be as much universal and thorough for all walks in life as has been the training in the arts of killing. The raising of revenue and taxes for the support of this program to come from the reduction of swollen fortunes from the top, as well as for the support of public works to give employment whenever there may be any slackening necessary in private enterprise.

      Huey Long had many good ideas. I liked his idea of a pension for people to retire at 60. This would be hard to achieve, with growing populations. I did like his idea of taking care of veterans and their disabled. I feel that this needs to be improved upon even today. I am a firm believer in education and access to it and liked Long's ideas for universal education of children and professional and vocational training access later. I think some of these ideas are great, but many do not like paying more in to provide for others.

    7. We have a marvelous love for this Government of ours; in fact, it is almost a religion, and it is well that it should be, because we have a splendid form of government and we have a splendid set of laws. We have everything here that we need, except that we have neglected the fundamentals upon which the American Government was principally predicated.

      Huey P. Long think that everyone should be treated equal regardless of their background. Whether you are poor, rich or in the middle. He think that the wealth should be spreaded to all types of people in all walks of life. If you are poor the the fortunate should share with you. That is not how the system work in america. If you are born into a family of riches and good fortune then yes you are a heir to their fortune. If you are born into poverty then yes you inherit poverty but you will be taught how to survive being less fortunate. Your family will teach you how to be independent as you get older. The rich just doesn't give away their fortune. They may give to the less fortunate through some type of organization. If you are born to the king and queen then yes you will inherit their fortune. If it is more than one siblings to the king and queen they are also included in the fortune. Money just don't grow on trees.

    8. We have to limit fortunes. Our present plan is that we will allow no one man to own more that $50,000,000. We think that with that limit we will be able to carry out the balance of the program. It may be necessary that we limit it to less than $50,000,000. It may be necessary, in working out of the plans that no man’s fortune would be more than $10,000,000 or $15,000,000. But be that as it may, it will still be more than any one man, or any one man and his children and their children, will be able to spend in their lifetimes; and it is not necessary or reasonable to have wealth piled up beyond that point where we cannot prevent poverty among the masses. …

      My over all take from reading both pieces is everyone should be equal and have some type of balance and structure. Why should one have so much, and the other so little.Why is it the more money you have the power it gives you.To treat others as if they are beneath you.Would if we all at the same opportunities as the next man. Look how are veterans are being treated after serving this country. I do believe if you work hard and were able to enjoy the fruits of your labor, but when you pass money does not go with you, especially if that money can help with children being hungry,people living on the streets, I don't feel a balance would benefit us as a society. I know myself I buy frivolous things all the time,and when asked if I want to donate to some charity sometime I will but majority I won't, this was a real eye opener, I don't feel we should all have the same things but it should be fair.